Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T21:10:57.015Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Authoritarianism, perceptions of security threats, and the COVID-19 pandemic: A new perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 October 2023

Daniel Stevens*
Affiliation:
University of Exeter, Exeter, UK
Susan Banducci
Affiliation:
University of Exeter, Exeter, UK
Laszlo Horvath
Affiliation:
Birkbeck University of London, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Daniel Stevens; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

This article offers a new perspective on when and why individual-level authoritarian perceptions of security threats change. We reexamine claims that authoritarian members of the public responded to the COVID-19 pandemic in a counterintuitive fashion. The response was counterintuitive in that, rather than a desire for a stronger government with the ability to impose measures to address the pandemic and its consequences, authoritarian individuals rejected a stronger government response and embraced individual autonomy. The article draws on perceptions of security threats—issues that directly or indirectly harm personal or collective safety and welfare—from surveys in two different contexts in England: 2012, when perceptions of the threat from infectious disease was low relative to most other security threats, and 2020, when perceptions of the personal and collective threat of COVID-19 superseded all other security threats. We argue that the authoritarian response was not counterintuitive once we account for the type of threat it represented.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Association for Politics and the Life Sciences

Perceptions of threats to security are a central influence on political attitudes and behavior. They affect social and political tolerance (Chanley, Reference Chanley1994; Peffley et al., Reference Peffley, Hutchison and Shamir2015), intergroup bias (Obadi et al., Reference Obadi, Kunst, Kteily, Thomsen and Sidanius2018), conservatism (Thorisdottir & Jost, Reference Thorisdottir and Jost2011), closed-mindedness (Kruglanski, Reference Kruglanski2004), prejudice (Das et al., Reference Das, Bushman, Bezemer, Kerkhof and Vermeulen2009; Echebarra-Echabe & Fernandez-Guede, Reference Echebarra-Echabe and Fernandez-Guede2006), support for right-wing parties (Devos et al., Reference Devos, Spini and Schwartz2002), support for policies involving conflict or aggression (Hetherington & Weiler, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009; Hirsch-Hoefler et al., Reference Hirsch-Hoefler, Canetti, Rapaport and Hobfoll2016), support for democracy (Fernandez & Kuenzi, Reference Fernandez and Kuenzi2010; Norris & Inglehart, Reference Norris and Inglehart2019), trust in government (Viklund, Reference Viklund2003), participation in politics and elections (Miller & Krosnick, Reference Miller and Krosnick2004; Montalvo, Reference Montalvo2011), satisfaction with democracy (Fernandez & Kuenzi, Reference Fernandez and Kuenzi2010), and perceptions of the desirable attributes of leaders (Merolla et al., Reference Merolla, Ramos and Zechmeister2007), as well as attitudes toward civil liberties (Lahav & Courtemanche, Reference Lahav and Courtemanche2012), immigration (Davis & Silver, Reference Davis and Silver2004; Hopkins, Reference Hopkins2010; Lahav & Courtemanche, Reference Lahav and Courtemanche2012; Stevens & Vaughan-Williams, Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016), and the environment (Arikan & Gunay, Reference Arikan and Gunay2021). In an era when “citizen stakeholders” are part of “a new mode of governance reliant on the conscription of ordinary individuals into the state’s traditional apparatuses and projects” (Jarvis & Lister, Reference Jarvis and Lister2010, p. 183), what citizens perceive as security threats and why is more important now than ever.

While we know a lot about perceptions of particular security threats at particular moments in time, we know surprisingly little about perceptions of a range of security threats over time.Footnote 1 This is especially surprising regarding threats from infectious diseases, given that “emerging infectious diseases have been increasing in frequency over the past five decades” (Daszak et al., Reference Daszak, Keusch, Phelan, Johnson and Osterholm2021, p. 204), most dramatically with COVID-19. Previous research attributes perceptions of particular threats at particular moments in time to a combination of contextual and individual-level influences. It is thus often focused on the consequences of dramatic contextual change—“shocks” such as a terrorist attack or health pandemic, or longer-term changes such as increases in immigration (Hirsch-Hoefler et al., Reference Hirsch-Hoefler, Canetti, Rapaport and Hobfoll2016; Hopkins, Reference Hopkins2010; Sniderman et al., Reference Sniderman, Hagendoorn and Prior2004)—and individual-level influences including demographics such as sex, education, age, and, a major focus of contemporary research and of this article, authoritarian values (Hetherington & Weiler, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2018; Norris & Inglehart, Reference Norris and Inglehart2019; Stenner & Haidt, Reference Stenner, Haidt and Sunstein2018; Stevens & Banducci, Reference Stevens and Banducci2022; Tillman, Reference Tillman2021).

Indeed, individual-level authoritarian predispositions toward “the prudent and just balance between group authority and individual autonomy” that are marked on a scale “at one end by preference for uniformity and insistence upon group authority, and at the other end by preference for difference and insistence upon individual autonomy” (Stenner, Reference Stenner2005, pp.17, 15)— as shorthand, we refer to individuals who prefer uniformity as “authoritarians” and individuals who prefer individual autonomy as “libertarians”—are perhaps the preeminent variable in explaining perceptions of security threats and their impact on illiberal and antidemocratic policy preferences among the public (Hetherington & Weiler, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009; Norris & Inglehart, Reference Norris and Inglehart2019; Stenner & Haidt, Reference Stenner, Haidt and Sunstein2018; Stevens & Banducci, Reference Stevens and Banducci2022; Tillman, Reference Tillman2021). For example, Norris and Inglehart (Reference Norris and Inglehart2019) say that “authoritarian values prioritize … the importance of security against risks of instability and disorder (foreigners stealing our jobs, immigrants attacking our women, terrorists threatening our safety)” (p. 6), whereas libertarians, who do not prioritize these values, regard the risks as less serious and favor individual autonomy over security. Thus, authoritarian individuals are most likely to perceive security threats and to favor an antidemocratic politics to address them.

The literature has traditionally associated increased security threats with more authoritarian responses—illiberalism—among individuals with authoritarian predispositions (Altemeyer, Reference Altemeyer1996; Feldman & Stenner, Reference Feldman and Stenner1997; Stenner, Reference Stenner2005). But revisionist claims have argued that it is libertarians who adopt more authoritarian responses in contexts of increased security threats (Hetherington & Weiler, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009); in these accounts, authoritarians are on a permanent state of alert, meaning that contexts of increased security threat have little impact on their preferences for government actions and measures.

While recent studies have argued that both claims have validity (Arikan, Reference Arikan2023; Stevens & Banducci, Reference Stevens and Banducci2022), the COVID-19 pandemic added an additional wrinkle to the debate. This shock event, seemingly with implications for security that are likely to elicit a response among authoritarians, did not increase their support for more authoritarian measures to address the threat (e.g., Heller et al., Reference Heller, Decker, Clemens, Fegert, Heiner, Brahler and Schmidt2022; Hibbing, Reference Hibbing2022; Vowles, Reference Vowles2022). Although at first glance, this may seem to confirm the revisionist theory—indeed, it was libertarians who were more supportive of such measures—neither did the response to COVID-19 appear to reflect the permanent state of alert among authoritarians claimed by Hetherington and Weiler (Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009); rather, authoritarians appeared to diminish the threat of COVID-19.

The COVID-19 pandemic therefore invites us to reconsider the different theoretical perspectives on authoritarianism and contextual changes in security threats. This article undertakes this exercise by analyzing changes in perceptions of security threats and their relationship with authoritarian predispositions. Using surveys of perceptions of security threats on the same 10 issues (e.g., terrorism, climate change) in England eight years apart, in 2012 and 2020, we examine changes in perceptions of threats as a function of changes in context—which we define here in terms of changes in national newspaper attention to them.

Overall, we find that authoritarians exhibit a disproportionate increase in perceptions of threat only for threats pertaining to issues related to out-groups. We do not find an increase in perceptions of the threat of infectious disease among authoritarians, as represented by the health pandemics of avian flu (2012) and COVID-19 (2020); indeed, there is more evidence of a decrease in perceptions of this and other threats that we suggest share similar attributes. We argue that lower perceptions of threats among authoritarians on these issues occur because authoritarians do not regard them as especially problematic (e.g., environmental issues).

Unlike an issue such as immigration, these issues are not regarded as a threat to social norms. We conclude by discussing our findings about the kinds of individuals who are likely to respond to more threatening contexts with perceptions of increased threat and their implications for research on security threats, infectious diseases, and authoritarianism.

Previous research

Perceptions of security threats are a function of the probability of a threatening event occurring combined with the severity of its consequences (HM Government, 2010, p. 37; Sjoberg, Reference Sjoberg1999). Thus, a security threat with a high probability of occurrence may still be regarded as less serious than a security threat with a low probability of occurrence if the consequences of the former are regarded as far less serious than the consequences of the latter. Perceptions of probabilities and consequences, in turn, are driven by contextual changes, including changes in an issue’s salience and framing (Haider-Markel & Vieux, Reference Haider-Markel and Vieux2008), and individual-level factors such as education, media habits, and authoritarian predisposition. Examples of contextual change, such as terrorist acts like 9/11, have the capacity to shift public opinion as a result of the shock of death and destruction and the shifts in media attention that follow (Berrebi & Klor, Reference Berrebi and Klor2008; Croft & Moore, Reference Croft and Moore2010; Davis & Silver, Reference Davis and Silver2004).Footnote 2 These influences, in turn, are moderated by the individual-level attributes through which information is filtered, interpreted, and experienced, with authoritarianism being the key moderator according to contemporary research (e.g., Hetherington & Weiler, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2018; Huddy et al., Reference Huddy, Feldman, Taber and Lahav2005; Norris & Inglehart, Reference Norris and Inglehart2019; Stevens & Vaughan-Williams, Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016).

While infectious disease may also be regarded as a security threat (Stevens & Vaughan-Williams, Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016)—indeed, there is “an entire scientific literature on how infectious diseases promote authoritarianism” (Kealey, Reference Kealey2021)—the focus of much of that literature is on its relationships with authoritarian attitudes and practices at the aggregate rather than at the individual level, with countries that have a high incidence of infectious disease being more authoritarian. Parasite- or pathogen-stress theory (Thornhill & Fincher, Reference Thornhill and Fincher2014) finds a link between authoritarian governance and infectious disease over space and time (Pericas, Reference Pericas2020) that may be due both to evolutionary psychology and to the need for conformity in cultures to limit its spread (Tybur et al., Reference Tybur, Inbar, Aarøe, Barclay, Barlow, Barra, Becker, Borovoi, Choi, Choi, Consedine, Conway, Conway, Conway, Adoric, Demirci, Fernández, Ferreira, Ishii and Žeželj2016), and mediated by the behavioral immune system (Helzer & Pizarro, Reference Helzer and Pizarro2011; Murray & Schaller, Reference Murray and Schaller2012; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Schaller and Suedfeld2013). Zmigrod et al. (Reference Zmigrod, Ebert, Götz and Rentfrow2021) confirm a relationship between authoritarian attitudes and infectious disease in the 47 countries they examine, but they make an additional connection to voting behavior, with data from the United States showing a path from prevalence of infectious disease through authoritarian attitudes to voting for Donald Trump. Mixing individual-level survey data with county-level data on mortality rates, Hinckley (Reference Hinckley2021) also finds a relationship between existential threat and positive views of Donald Trump. However, other studies question the causal explanations in pathogen-stress theory (Bromham et al., Reference Bromham, Hua, Cardillo, Schneemann and Greenhill2018; Pollet, Reference Pollet2014) or suggest that the relationships are conditional on other societal-level factors such as economic wealth (Kusano & Kemmelmeier, Reference Kusano and Kemmelmeier2018).

More importantly for this article, most of the research on authoritarianism and infectious disease leaves questions about the micro-level processes open. We know little about how changes in context, such as increased threat of infectious disease over time, are related to individual-level perceptions of threat or their interaction with individual-level authoritarianism. Thus, they, too, cannot solve the puzzle of why the COVID-19 pandemic did not increase authoritarian support for more authoritarian measures.

How might contextual changes in security threats such as terrorism or infectious diseases combine with authoritarianism?Footnote 3 Stenner’s (Reference Stenner2005) answer is that authoritarians are highly sensitive to changes in context that have implications for threats to social norms or societal division: she describes authoritarians as “relentlessly sociotropic boundary maintainers” (p. 32). Indeed, authoritarianism has been identified as contributing to emergent cleavages in British public opinion, the focus of this article, because of perceptions of threats to social norms from immigration, which are then reflected in right-wing vote preferences in elections (Chrisp & Pearce, Reference Chrisp and Pearce2019; Fox & Pearce, Reference Fox and Pearce2018; Golec de Zavala et al., Reference Golec de Zavala, Guerra and Simao2017; Hetherington & Weiler, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2018; Kaufman, Reference Kaufman2016; Norris & Inglehart, Reference Norris and Inglehart2019; Peitz et al., Reference Peitz, Dhont and Seyd2018). Tillman (Reference Tillman2021) argues that as a result of immigration, the predisposition toward authoritarianism increasingly divides other publics in Western Europe, and Hetherington and Weiler (Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2018) that it divides Republicans from Democrats in the United States.

However, little of this research has examined (1) the claim that authoritarians are especially sensitive to changes in context that have implications for social norms; (2) the corollary that perceptions of other threats over time—those without implications for social norms—are more stable for authoritarians; or (3) the kinds of contextual change, beyond shocks such as terrorist attacks, that may affect authoritarians. This reflects two problems. First, there is a tendency toward a narrow focus on security issues related to conflict, terrorism, and prejudice. This tells us neither about other threats to welfare, such as those from infectious diseases, nor about the “everyday threats,” such as crime that individuals experience, and whether there are differences in responses to these types of threats as opposed to the threats to social norms to which Stenner (Reference Stenner2005) refers. Second, we possess knowledge of perceptions of various different security threats in a series of changing contexts, such as after a terrorist attack, and their relationship with authoritarian predispositions (e.g., Stevens & Banducci, Reference Stevens and Banducci2022), but we know less about the same security threats in different contexts, such as the threat of terrorism when there is little discernible threat, and their relationship with authoritarian predispositions. We also know about the relationship between authoritarian attitudes and practices and the threat from infectious diseases, but neither how the threat from infectious diseases is actually perceived nor its relationship with authoritarian predispositions.

Theory and hypotheses

If perceptions of threat are a function of perceptions of the probability of an event occurring and the likely severity of its consequences, perceptions of threat will increase given (a) perceptions of an increase in the likelihood that an event will occur (e.g., another terrorist attack after 9/11) or (b) perceptions of an increase in the severity of a threat’s consequences (e.g., environmental degradation or fallout from an economic depression). Perceptions of the probability and severity of a threat will be affected not only by the real world but also by signals such as media attention to an issue (regardless of whether those signals reflect real-world change): as Ridout et al. (Reference Ridout, Grosse and Appleton2008) put it, “individuals adjust their perception of reality to fit the image of the world around them” (p. 576), rather than vice versa. Media agenda-setting research tells us that the amount of media attention to an issue is taken as a signal of its importance. With regard to security threats, such changes in attention alone may be taken to signal changes in the probability and/or consequences of threats, but particularly if the framing is negative: both point to a context of increased threat.

The question then becomes how authoritarian predispositions interact with such changes in context. Previous research offers four answers.

1. Authoritarians are impervious to contextual change. Authoritarians are in a permanently elevated state of threat, which, in turn, implies that they are relatively unresponsive to changes in security threats. It is libertarians who are more likely to respond to increased attention to an issue with perceptions of elevated threat (Hetherington & Weiler, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009) because perceptions of high levels of threat are built into authoritarians’ worldviews.

2. Authoritarians are responsive to any contextual change. Altemeyer (Reference Altemeyer1996) argues that “authoritarians stand about ten steps closer to the panic button than the rest of the population” (p. 100) rather than being in a permanently elevated state of threat. This implies that in contexts of increased threat, authoritarian perceptions of threat should increase regardless of the issue.

3. Authoritarians are responsive to changes in security threats with implications for “normative threat” but impervious to contextual changes to security threats that do not have implications for normative threat. Stenner (Reference Stenner2005) suggests a more dynamic relationship between perceptions of threats and authoritarianism. She argues that while authoritarians are more likely to see the world as dangerous and “are inclined towards this peculiar fear of a dangerous world under any conditions” (Stenner, Reference Stenner2005, p. 69; italics in original), they are particularly attentive to “normative threat,” which can be defined as “threats to unity and consensus, or ‘oneness and sameness’” (Stenner & Haidt, Reference Stenner, Haidt and Sunstein2018, p. 180). Moreover, in stable contexts, Stenner suggests “somewhat wishful thinking” among authoritarians in which perceptions of normative threat are lower than for libertarians. But, at the same time, authoritarians have a “hypersensitivity” to changes with implications for normative threat (Stenner, Reference Stenner2005, pp. 69–70). The combination of “a dangerous world” with respect to perceptions of threat in general and wishful thinking and hypersensitivity to normative threat suggests that authoritarians are in a permanently elevated state of threat on most issues, but not those issues that have implications for normative threat. Therefore, we should expect greater responsiveness to changes in security threats with implications for normative threat among authoritarians; for security issues without implications for normative threat, authoritarians should have consistently elevated perceptions of threat that are impervious to contextual change.

4. Authoritarians are responsive to changes in security threats with implications for normative threat. They are also responsive to changes in security threats that do not have implications for normative threat but may respond to increased attention to them with decreased perceptions of those threats. A fourth set of research findings suggests that while Stenner (Reference Stenner2005) may be correct about normative threat, the notion that authoritarians simply perceive “a dangerous world,” implying a lack of sensitivity to other threats, also needs refining. Kahan et al. (Reference Kahan, Braman, Gastil, Slovic and Mertz2007) contend that “individuals selectively credit and dismiss asserted dangers in a manner supportive of their cultural identities” (p. 465, emphasis added; see also Hibbing, Reference Hibbing2020), implying that authoritarians should discount threats to difference and diversity (i.e., to individual autonomy). In addition, Duckitt and Sibley (Reference Duckitt and Sibley2010) argue that authoritarians are relatively unperturbed by security threats linked to inequality or scarcity because they tend not to impinge on authoritarians’ most fundamental concerns about group conformity and social norms (p. 1869). This also suggests that authoritarians may diminish threats that affect subgroups of society, such as hate crimes, rather than the whole of society. Finally, Choma et al. (Reference Choma, Hanoch, Gummerum and Hodson2013) argue that another salient characteristic of threats such as the environment is that they have diffuse rather than personal risks. It is not clear, however, that the threats that do seem to exercise high authoritarians, such as immigration, are perceived as personal (Stevens & Vaughan-Williams, Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016).

These perspectives imply that while there may be some consensus that an issue like the environment is a growing security threat, to the extent that environmentalism is associated with resource scarcity, authoritarians will be more likely to downplay the threat relative to libertarians. Even an existential threat like the COVID-19 health pandemic, while likely to be identified as a threat by the vast majority of people given its prominence, may be viewed as less of a threat by authoritarians if the emphasis is on its unequal economic effects or on mortality among out-groups that is unlikely to threaten social norms. We discuss this in more detail later. Similarly, signals of change in threat from crimes that affect all groups (e.g., burglary) may be viewed differently by authoritarians than threats from crimes that affect subgroups (e.g., hate crimes and crimes against women).

In sum, this fourth perspective suggests that for some issues beyond normative threat, rather than perceiving a dangerous world or being in a permanently elevated state of threat, authoritarians may respond to changes in context signaling increased threat from an issue by minimizing that threat. Authoritarians will respond to signals with implications for heightened normative threat with increased perceptions of threat but diminish other types of threat, such as those with diffuse risks or implications for inequality or scarcity.

A further important variable that may moderate perceptions of threats for authoritarians is whether they are sociotropic or personal. The corollary of Stenner’s (Reference Stenner2005) description of authoritarians as relentlessly sociotropic boundary maintainers is her claim that they are relatively unconcerned with personal threats. This is because personal threats have implications for the exercise of individual autonomy rather than the social norms and sociotropic boundary maintenance that authoritarians care most about (see also Feldman, Reference Feldman2013). However, Asbrock and Fritsche (Reference Asbrock and Fritsche2013) argue that perceptions of the personal more than the national threat of terrorism provoke authoritarian expression and suggest that “authoritarian responses may operate as a group-level coping strategy for a threat to the personal self” (p. 35). But their argument is based on two experiments rather than over-time analysis. More promisingly, Stevens and Banducci (Reference Stevens and Banducci2022) use survey data before and after a terrorist attack to claim that increases in perceptions of personal threat from terrorism increase perceptions of normative threat for authoritarians. But their claims are limited to a single issue over a short space of time.

In sum, we have gleaned three different categories of threat from the literature on threat and authoritarianism:

  1. 1. Threats to social norms and the in-group, such as threats from immigration or perceptions of weak border control

  2. 2. Threats with implications for inequality and resources, such as threats from the economy or infectious diseases such as COVID-19

  3. 3. Threats from crime, with an additional distinction between those that affect everyone (e.g., burglary) and those that affect subgroups (e.g., hate crimes and crimes against women), as well as between threats that are national versus personal.

Threats from infectious diseases could have implications for social norms. Indeed, with COVID-19, there was some initial focus on potential threats to social norms such as food hoarding or from a severe economic depression. There was also evidence of growing racial antipathy in countries like the United Kingdom and the United States, manifesting in increased hate crimes (Borkowska & Laurence, Reference Borkowska and Laurence2021; Strassle et al., Reference Strassle, Stewart, Quintero, Bonilla, Alhomsi, Santana-Ufret, Maldonado, Forde and Napoles2022), perhaps fed by policies that closed borders to out-groups. However, over time, the emphasis shifted to other subjects, including the drag on resources affecting the health service and the economy and the unequal death rates by race (i.e., to resources and inequality). In addition, Ollerenshaw (Reference Ollerenshaw2022) and Geana et al. (Reference Geana, Rabb and Sloman2021) argue that right-wing opinion leaders in the United States, such as Donald Trump, diminished the threat from COVID-19—the overall message was “don’t worry about this” (Geana et al., Reference Geana, Rabb and Sloman2021, p. 6)—and that the perceptions of their supporters, who tend to be more authoritarian, were influenced by this messaging (Geana et al., Reference Geana, Rabb and Sloman2021; Ollerenshaw, Reference Ollerenshaw2022; see also Hibbing, Reference Hibbing2022). Similarly, in the United Kingdom, the right-wing prime minister, Boris Johnson, initially diminished the threat of COVID-19. While he then introduced measures such as lockdowns, these were largely attributed to “following the science” (Andreouli & Brice, Reference Andreouli and Brice2022), and the government subsequently transitioned to initiatives such as “eat out to help out” (a government-subsidized scheme in which people could get up to a 50% reduction on food and drinks at restaurants, cafés, and pubs) that appear to have contributed to a second wave of infections and deaths toward the end of 2020 (Morales, Reference Morales, Ring, Hutton and Paton2020; Parker & Payne, Reference Parker and Payne2021; Lintern, Reference Lintern2021). Finally, Pretus and Villarroya (Reference Pretus and Vilarroya2022) point out that the absence of a clearly defined “enemy” with respect to COVID-19 and other such catastrophes and natural disasters may reduce the influence of group threat.

Thus, perceptions of threats from COVID-19 to social norms or from out-groups were likely lessened in the United Kingdom (as in the United States) by right-wing elites’ efforts to minimize the overall threat of the disease. Indeed, our analysis here confirms that COVID-19 was viewed by the public as a threat more like the environment or problems of resource scarcity than terrorism or immigration.

Following from our categorization of threats to social norms, threats with implications for inequality and resources, and threats from crime, we test the following hypotheses:

H1: In contexts indicative of increased security threats from out-groups, increases in perceptions of these issues as threats are greater for authoritarians than for libertarians.

H2: In contexts indicative of increased security threats pertaining to (a) inequality or resources or (b) crimes that affect subgroups rather than being universal, increases in perceptions of these issues as threats are smaller (or negative) for authoritarians than for libertarians.

H1 implies a positive interaction between authoritarianism and signals of increased threat for issues pertaining to out-groups, whereas H2 implies a negative interaction between authoritarianism and signals of increased threat pertaining to inequality, resource scarcity, or crimes against subgroups in society. H1 implies an interaction between authoritarianism and out-group threat that is consistent with the third and fourth possibilities suggested by the literature and inconsistent with the first (which implies a negative interaction) and second (which implies main effects of authoritarianism and context but no interaction). H2 implies an interaction between authoritarianism and threats pertaining to inequality, resources, and crimes against subgroups in society that is only consistent with the fourth.

Although H1 and H2 do not distinguish between perceptions of national and personal security threats, there is additional disagreement about whether authoritarians are concerned mainly by national (sociotropic) security threats (Feldman, Reference Feldman2013) or whether perceptions of personal security threats are paramount (Asbrock & Fritsche, Reference Asbrock and Fritsche2013). Therefore, we test two additional, mutually exclusive hypotheses:

H3a: Contexts of increased national security threats have greater effects on authoritarians than contexts of increased personal security threats.

H3b: Contexts of increased personal security threats have greater effects on authoritarians than contexts of increased national security threats.

What if there is less media attention to an issue over time—that is, a signal of decreasing threat? All else being equal, in this context, we would expect perceptions of threat to decrease for both authoritarians and libertarians. But for issues with implications for normative threat, Stenner (Reference Stenner2005) would lead us to expect such “normative reassurance” to lower threat levels for authoritarians more than libertarians.

We summarize the combinations of expectations around different kinds of security threats and changes in their prominence in Table 1: 0 represents no moderating influence of authoritarian predispositions, + a positive moderating effect, and – a negative moderating effect. We also indicate whether there is an expected main effect of authoritarian predispositions.

Table 1. Expectations of interactive relationships between authoritarian predispositions and attention to threats

Research design

The ideal test of our hypotheses would examine within-person changes in perceptions of security threats and their context with longitudinal panel data. However, longitudinal panels such as Understanding Society do not ask about security threats. The British Election Study (BES) regularly asks “most important issue” questions, but these do not necessarily elicit answers related to threat, and the BES does not regularly ask about perceptions of security threats.Footnote 4 To test our hypotheses without panel data, we thus draw inferences from two cross-sectional surveys that are unique in asking samples of (English)Footnote 5 respondents identical questions about perceptions of 10 security threats at two points in time: the June 2012 Perceptions of Security in an Age of Austerity online survey conducted by ICM for Stevens and Vaughan-Williams (Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016)Footnote 6 and an online survey conducted for the authors by Opinion Research Business (ORB) in July 2020.

The Perceptions of Security in an Age of Austerity survey asked whether 22 issues were a serious security threat to “(1) the world, (2) UK, (3) community in which you live, (4) you and your family at the moment.” The issues ranged from terrorism and the threat of Russia and China to burglary and online fraud. We replicated these questions in an online survey administered by ORB in July 2020, with four differences. First, we did not ask about community-level threats because Stevens and Vaughan-Williams (Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016) found minimal differences with perceptions of personal threats. Second, we randomly split the sample and asked different subsamples of respondents about their perceptions of threats to the United Kingdom or to “you and your family” to guard against possible effects of respondent fatigue—in the ICM survey, each respondent was asked four times about 22 issues. Third, because of additional concerns about respondent fatigue, we asked about a subset of the original 22 issues, dropping those such as “the far right” and “Islamophobia” that had been identified as threats by few respondents in 2012. And fourth, our sample in 2020 was from England rather than Great Britain. The analysis that follows excludes from the 2012 data respondents who resided in Scotland or Wales. In addition, given that our hypotheses include claims about perceptions of minority out-groups, we exclude non-White respondents,Footnote 7 providing a sample size of 1,414 for the 2012 survey and 2,926 in total for the 2020 survey, but with the latter sample split between respondents asked about national or personal threats rather than both.

The surveys captured authoritarian predispositions using Feldman’s (Reference Feldman and Stenner1997) four-question child-rearing values measure, in which respondents are given a choice of attributes and asked which is preferable in children—independence/respect for elders, obedience/self-reliance, curiosity/good manners, and considerate/well behaved. The premise of the scale is that child-rearing values reflect fundamental orientations toward conformity or autonomy—they are not affected, for example, by actual child-rearing practices (Hetherington & Weiler Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009, p. 50; Stenner, Reference Stenner2005, p. 24). The scale has been used widely to capture authoritarian predispositions, both in other research (e.g., Arikan, Reference Arikan2023; Hetherington & Weiler, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009; Stenner, Reference Stenner2005; Zmigrod, Reference Zmigrod, Ebert, Götz and Rentfrow2021) and in major election studies in the United States, Britain, France, New Zealand, and Switzerland. Respondents with the highest authoritarian predispositions choose respect for elders, obedience, good manners, and well behaved. Respondents with lower levels of authoritarian predisposition choose one to three of these values, while libertarians choose independence, self-reliance, curiosity, and considerate. Both surveys also include the control variables for mortality salience, television news consumption, sex, age and education employed in Stevens and Vaughan-Williams’s (Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016) analysis.Footnote 8

For the comparison of the two surveys to be valid, we checked that the samples were similar on the observed demographics—and they were on age, sex, and education (see Table A1 in the Appendix). We also examined the mean levels of mortality salience and authoritarianism in the two surveys, for completeness rather than because we would expect them to be the same; there are differences of one-quarter to one-third of a standard deviation, with mean levels of authoritarianism slightly higher in 2012 and mean levels of mortality salience slightly lower.

The analysis that follows focuses on 10 issues that appear in both surveys and capture the kinds of security threats outlined in H1 and H2: immigration; terrorism; weak border control; health pandemics, such as avian flu (2012) or COVID-19 (2020); environmental issues, such as global warming or greenhouse gas effects; resource scarcity; economic depression, financial crisis, and unemployment; burglary; crimes against women; and racial or religious hate crimes.

Table 2 presents summary data on the proportions of the samples identifying each issue as a security threat. It shows that at the national level, perceptions of the top five issues of terrorism, the economy, immigration, weak border control, and racial or religious hate crimes as threats are quite stable across the two surveys, while the new issues of health pandemic, environmental issues, and racial and religious hate crimes loom larger in the later survey. At the same time, Table 2 indicates increases in perceptions of almost all issues as personal threats compared to 2012—only burglary shows a small decrease. While the economy and terrorism are among the top-ranked personal threats in both years, some of the increases in perceptions of threats from other issues are large, particularly a health pandemic and environmental issues. Reasons for the increase in the personal threat from terrorism could be related to the number of relatively small-scale attacks that were not a feature of Islamic terrorism in the United Kingdom up to 2012, such as those at Westminster, the Manchester Arena, and London’s Borough Market in 2017, as opposed to the larger-scale terrorism and loss of life of 9/11 and 7/7.Footnote 9 As we show later, media attention to the personal threat of terrorism was much greater in 2020 than in 2012.

Table 2. Perceptions of threats in 2012 and 2020

Note: Figures for 2012 and 2020 are percentages.

It is worth noting some of the differences between the threat presented by avian flu in 2012 and COVID-19 in 2020 that are reported in Table 2, as well as that while the survey question gave avian flu as an example of a health pandemic in 2012, there had been an outbreak of swine flu in 2009. According to the Office for National Statistics (2022), COVID-19 was the leading cause of death in England and Wales in 2020, with roughly 75,000 deaths within 28 days of a COVID-19 test by the end of the year, of which about 40,000 had occurred by the time the ORB survey was in the field. In October 2020, the reproduction rate (R0) of the coronavirus, or the average number of additional people infected from a single infection case, was estimated to be 1.40 to 6.49, compared to 1.30 to 1.71 for other flu viruses, with a case fatality rate (CFR) of 1.40% to 3.67% (Bai & Tao, Reference Bai and Tao2021). Infection rates for the swine flu in 2009 were high, with an estimated 11% to 21% of the world’s population contracting the virus, but the CFR was much lower at 0.1% to 0.5%. The estimated global number of deaths varies widely, but even the highest of more than 1 million is a fraction of the deaths from COVID-19 (Roser, Reference Roser2020). In contrast with the high case rate of swine flu, in the 20 years from 2003 to 2023, there were 878 cases of avian flu reported globally, of which 458 were fatal (World Health Organization, 2023)—it had a higher CFR, estimated at 20% to 40%, but person-to-person transmission was limited to nonexistent (FutureLearn, 2020).

Table 3 presents the factor analysis of the 10 issues as national and personal threats. Given that the data are categorical—an issue is identified as a threat or not—the factor analyses are of the polychoric correlation matrices for national and personal threats. Three factors have eigenvalues greater than 1 in each factor analysis. Table 3 highlights the factors (with orthogonal varimax rotation) on which the issues have the highest factor loading. The issues in each factor are the same for national and personal threats; they reflect threats from out-groups, such as those from immigration, terrorism, and weak border control; threats from crime, such as burglary and hate crimes; and threats to health and resources, such as those from health pandemics, the environment, and the economy. Thus, the factor analysis confirms that these 10 issues cover the threat types described in our hypotheses and that health pandemics load with issues such as the environment rather than threats from out-groups.Footnote 10

Table 3. Exploratory factor analysis of the 10 security threats

We capture changing national and personal security threat contexts on these 10 issues by comparing newspaper attention to them in the year prior to the survey, using Nexis. We conducted a keyword and thematic search of the 10 issues in national newspaper articles (see the Appendix for a description of the keywords and themes), then used Nexis’s filters to focus only on “negative news” stories (i.e., stories that are threatening).Footnote 11 Nexis also allows separation of “personal news” stories—that is, stories that are more relevant to perceptions of individual-level threat—from all other stories (which we term “national-level” stories as shorthand for national and international stories on these topics). Table 4 shows the analysis of the 10 issues for national and personal news stories. A context of increased threat for an issue is signified by a substantial increase in the number of such newspaper stories.Footnote 12 By this criterion, 2020 was a context of increased threat from health pandemics, environmental issues, resource scarcity, immigration, and crimes against women and of unchanged or reduced threat from the other five issues.

Table 4. Coverage in national newspapers one year before June 2012 and July 2020 surveys

Note: Figures represent the number of stories resulting from theme and keyword searches in Nexis in the 12 months before the first day of each survey; see the Appendix for details

Large increases or decreases in attention to threats from a national frame also tend to be accompanied by large decreases or increases in attention to threats from a personal frame. They move together in direction and size of change: exceptions are crimes against women, for which coverage with a more personal frame increased more than coverage with a national frame, although both increased, and terrorism, for which coverage with a national frame decreased by 12%, while more personal stories pertaining to terrorism increased by 12%. Overall, among the three categories of threats outlined in Table 1, we have examples of issues that grew or declined in prominence for all three categories, allowing for satisfactory tests of our hypotheses.

Analysis of perceptions of security threats

To test the hypotheses, we pool the 2012 and 2020 surveys. We estimate two sets of models for perceptions of national and personal security threats: (1) initial examinations of the probability of identifying each of the 10 issues as a threat as a function of authoritarian predispositions, a dummy variable for 2020, and the interaction between the two; (2) the probability of identifying the three security threat types in Table 1—out-groups, inequality/resources, and crime—treating each individual’s response on each issue as a separate observation by stacking the data and including dummy variables for two of three threat types, and an interaction with the 2020 dummy variable.

We control for the same influences as Stevens and Vaughan-Williams (Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016) with the exception of religion and religiosity: mortality salience, television news consumption, sex, age, and education. We also interact these variables with the dummy variable for survey year to account for any changes in their relationships with threats.Footnote 13

Table 5 shows the estimates for the key independent variables for the hypotheses, indicating whether the sign and significance of the interactions are consistent with H1 and H2 given the changes in media attention shown in Table 4. The results are consistent with the hypotheses for 7 of the 10 issues (3 of 5 for which media attention increased, and 4 of 5 for which media attention stayed the same or decreased). All three issues pertaining to out-group threat accord with expectations. Immigration, for which there was increased threat in the context of the 2020 survey according to our analysis, was disproportionately more likely to be seen as a threat by authoritarians than in 2012. In contrast, the issues of terrorism and border control, for which there was little or no change in threat according to the analysis, show only main effects of authoritarian predispositions—that is, authoritarians are more likely to identify them as a threat, but there is no difference between 2012 and 2020.

Table 5. Perceptions of threat, authoritarianism and media coverage in 2012 and 2020—individual logit models

* p < .05.

Notes: Estimates are from logit models. All models control for mortality salience, media consumption, sex, education and age, along with their interactions with the survey year.

The estimates for the threats pertaining to inequality and resource scarcity confirm the hypotheses for two of the four issues—the economy and the environment. Both show authoritarians less likely to identify them as a threat than libertarians. For the environment, there is an additional, and expected, negative impact for authoritarians in 2020, when the analysis in Table 4 indicates a context of increased threat compared to 2012. Of the other two issues, perceptions of the threat of a health pandemic increased enormously in 2020: the sign on the interaction with authoritarianism is negative and close to statistical significance, in line with the expectation that authoritarians will be more resistant to conceding that COVID-19 is a national threat.

The three issues pertaining to crime also conform to expectations for burglary and racial and religious hate crimes: for the former, which affects everybody, authoritarians are more likely to identify it as a threat—and with little change in media attention in 2020, that relationship does not shift. At the same time, however, authoritarians are less likely to identify crimes against women as a threat—and the relationship is somewhat stronger in 2020, when the issue had garnered somewhat more media attention. Perceptions of hate crimes show a similar relationship, but this does not conform to expectations because the interaction term shows a stronger relationship with authoritarianism in 2020, even though the issue had receded a little in prominence.

With respect to personal threats, we tested competing hypotheses regarding whether changed national (H3a) or personal (H3b) threat contexts should have stronger relationships with authoritarianism. Table 5 suggests patterns that are generally similar. There are few interactions with different signs, but they are somewhat weaker for personal threat; there is one statistically significant interaction compared to four for national-level threat, indicating some support for H3a.

In the second set of tests, we stack the data, treating perceptions of threat for each of the 10 issues as a separate observation, meaning that our observations increase 10-fold (we cluster the standard errors to account for the nonindependence of observations for the same respondent). We analyze perceptions of threat as a function of authoritarian predispositions, the threat type—out-group, inequality/resources, or crime—and the interactions between threat type, authoritarian predispositions, and year.

Combining the issues into threat types also necessitates that we reconsider the implications of media coverage for increased or decreased threat. Using the net change in the percentage of stories with a national frame in Table 4 within the three different threat types to test H1 and H2 indicates little change in out-group threat—a net increase of 6%—and a large increase in threat from issues pertaining to inequality/resources, leading us to expect a negative interaction between authoritarian predispositions and the year dummy. The context of crimes against subgroups is mixed, with reports of crimes against women up in 2020 but of racial and religious hate crimes down by a larger percentage. On this basis, we would expect negative main effects of authoritarianism that are not moderated by year—because these are issues that are of lesser concern to authoritarians, and decreased coverage should not change that. Media coverage of the personal threat of the three threat types in Table 4 shows some variation from the context of national threat. Steep increases in stories with a more personal frame on terrorism and on crimes against women signal increases in out-group threat and crimes against subgroups rather than decreasing or staying the same as at the national level.

The estimates for national and personal threat are shown in the Appendix in Table A3.Footnote 14 We focus here on the relationships as represented in Figure 1, which shows simulations of perceptions of national and personal threat for the three threat types in 2012 and 2020, conditional on authoritarian predispositions, based on the model estimates.Footnote 15 The relationships are in the expected direction for perceptions of national threat for two of the three threat types. First, authoritarians are more likely to identify out-group threats. This is true in both 2012 and 2020. With the context of out-group threats changing little in terms of media attention with a national frame, there is also no change in the slope of the relationship. Similarly, for inequality/resource scarcity, Figure 1 shows the negative relationship suggested by H2b and in previous research by Duckitt and Sibley (Reference Duckitt and Sibley2010), in which authoritarians are less likely to identify such issues as threats. While the increased threat posed by COVID-19 in 2020 raised threat levels on these issues for all respondents, there is the expected negative relationship between authoritarian predispositions and the 2020 context—that is, authoritarians’ perceptions of these threats do not increase as much as libertarians; indeed, the marginal effect of authoritarianism in 2020 is significantly more negative than in 2012, in line with expectations. For crimes against subgroups, however, there is a statistically significant and unexpected negative interaction with year. This is not because decreased coverage of crime unexpectedly lowers perceptions of threat for authoritarians, however. Authoritarian perceptions of the threat change little, as expected, but libertarian perceptions of the threat from crime against subgroups are considerably higher in 2020 than in 2012.

Figure 1. Relationships between authoritarian predispositions, media attention, and identifying issues as threats.

Turning to H3a and H3b, the lower half of Figure 1 depicts the relationships between personal threat context, authoritarian predispositions, and perceptions of personal threat. If change in personal threat context has effects on perceptions like those outlined in Table 1, we would expect a positive interaction between authoritarianism and out-group threat and negative interactions with inequality/resources and crimes against subgroups. We see these relationships for out-group and inequality/resources threat but, again, unexpected results for crimes against subgroups in that there is not a negative interaction with authoritarianism.

Conclusion

The COVID-19 pandemic has illustrated the need to reconsider the relationship between authoritarianism and perceptions of security threats in order to account for the counterintuitive relationships between authoritarianism and opposition to strict COVID-19 policy measures in disparate studies from New Zealand to the United States. Existing research on the prevalence of infectious diseases and authoritarian attitudes, which tells us that authoritarian attitudes should increase (but not among which kinds of individuals), falls short. Revisionist claims about authoritarian/libertarian responses to increased threat also fail to explain why authoritarian perceptions of the threat were lower than those of libertarians.

The seemingly counterintuitive effects of COVID-19 have thus been attributed to factors such as elite cues, the ideology of the incumbent government, or political culture (Ollerenshaw, Reference Ollerenshaw2022; Vowles, Reference Vowles2022). But these explanations are limited and do not generalize. In this article, we have suggested that the authoritarian response to COVID-19 makes sense if we recognize that it belongs to a distinct category of threats with which authoritarians are less concerned and that they may even minimize.

Using the context of the COVID-19 pandemic to examine perceptions of a health pandemic and other security threats compared to perceptions of the same threats in the different context of 2012, we have drawn on the existing literature to test four competing theories of the relationship between changing threat contexts and authoritarian predispositions: (1) authoritarian perceptions of threats are permanently high and impervious to contextual change; (2) authoritarian perceptions of threats are responsive to any contextual change; (3) authoritarian perceptions of threats from social norms, or normative threat, are hypersensitive to contextual change, while their perceptions of other threats are permanently high, implying imperviousness to contextual change for those; and (4) authoritarian perceptions of normative threat are hypersensitive to contextual change, but authoritarians will also respond to signals of change in other kinds of threat (e.g., inequality and resource scarcity), with decreased perceptions of those threats.

We examined the four theories by studying the relationships between perceptions of 10 threats, authoritarian predispositions, and different contexts of those threats as signaled by changes in (negative) media attention. Using two surveys that asked about perceptions of the same threats at the national and personal levels in England in 2012 and 2020, our findings are most favorable to the third and fourth accounts of how authoritarians respond to changes in threat contexts; they are least favorable to the idea that authoritarians simply perceive a dangerous world and are unresponsive to contextual change (Hetherington & Weiler, Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009) or that they respond to any signal of increased threat (Altemeyer, Reference Altemeyer1996).

We confirmed that authoritarians are most responsive to changes in contexts pertaining to out-group threat, which have implications for the social norms that authoritarians care most about. The findings for threats such as COVID-19, which impinged on issues of health, resources, and inequality and did not seem to disproportionately affect authoritarian concern about social divisions in British society (see note 11), and for crimes against subgroups, on the other hand, suggest that there is either less authoritarian than libertarian sensitivity to contextual change on these issues or that high authoritarians minimize the threat. We also examined differences in perceptions of national and personal threats. We showed that the relationships are generally as evident for personal as for national threats and contexts—indeed for personal threat from out-groups the relationship with contextual change is stronger for authoritarians. Media coverage of terrorism with a more personal frame increased considerably in 2020 over 2012 according to our analysis. It may be that this change in context resonated with authoritarians in a way that indicates their concerns about out-groups go beyond the normative threat identified by Stenner (Reference Stenner2005). Perhaps they are the precursor to increased normative threat as suggested by Stevens and Banducci (Reference Stevens and Banducci2022), or out-group threats operate as suggested by Asbrock and Fritsche (Reference Asbrock and Fritsche2013), with personal threats exerting influence as a group-level coping strategy. More research is needed to understand the relationships between national and personal threats.

There are of course caveats to these findings. First, we compared two cross-sectional surveys; it would be preferable to track perceptions of the same respondents over time. It may be, for example, that the COVID-19 pandemic aroused authoritarian concern about social norms at the outset, but these concerns died down over time. Second, with regard to health pandemics, the degree of consensus that COVID-19 was a threat—and that avian flu was not—limited the variation in perceptions that can be explained and may have influenced our findings. Third, our measure of contextual change as increased or decreased newspaper attention is blunt—other indicators could be examined, such as Google search terms and real-world indicators—and it does not account for potential moderating factors such as how much attention a respondent pays to news. But our interest is in media coverage as a signal of changing threat contexts rather than its influence via exposure.Footnote 16 Nevertheless, future research should develop more refined measures of contextual change and how it is perceived.

Our findings show that despite the wealth of research on authoritarianism, its relationship with threats, and the consequences for phenomena such as conservative shifts and democratic backsliding, on some fairly basic questions of the dynamics of perceptions of threats we know little. Security studies has been enriched by efforts to go beyond elite perceptions and framings of threats to examine everyday or vernacular understandings of security threats (Downing et al., Reference Downing, Gerwens and Dron2022; Jarvis, Reference Jarvis2019; Jarvis & Lister, Reference Jarvis and Lister2010; Nyman, Reference Nyman2021). While this turn has provided a wealth of new insights about perceptions of security threats, the influences on them, and their consequences, it has also raised questions about changing contexts, changing perceptions, and the extent to which different security threats should be treated as alike. The new perspective we have outlined in this article on when and why individual-level authoritarian perceptions of security threats change begins to answer these questions.

This article shows that authoritarian perceptions of threats are more responsive to contextual change pertaining to several types of threats than most previous studies have examined, but not in a single direction in which indications of increased threat are associated with increased authoritarian concern about them—different security threats should not be treated as alike. For some threats, such as those pertaining to health and resources from infectious diseases such as avian flu and COVID-19, we have shown that indications of increased threat are associated with no change or decreased authoritarian concern about them—and that rather than being a function of elite cues or political culture we should expect that given the nature of the threat. This account also suggests, however, that under conditions in which a health pandemic clearly threatened social norms or damaged social cohesion authoritarians would respond differently—and as pointed out at the beginning of this article, with emerging infectious diseases increasing in frequency over the past five decades such situations could develop. It complicates answers to questions of the individual-level causes of relationships between the prevalence of infectious diseases and authoritarian attitudes. More broadly for security studies, it shows that to understand perceptions of everyday security threats there is a need to be more sensitive to which kinds of individuals respond to contexts of increased threat with changes such as an authoritarian shift in attitudes. This will require additional studies examining perceptions over time.

Data availability statement

All data and replication code for this study are available at ORE Open Research Exeter at: https://doi.org/10.24378/exe.4725. This study was not preregistered.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/pls.2023.12.

Footnotes

This article was awarded Open Data and Open Materials badges for open scientific practices. For details, see the data availability statement.

1 What do we mean by “security threat”? In the international relations literature, securitization describes a process in which issues become security threats, as opposed to having intrinsic and objective security-related properties, as a result of a “speech act” by a “securitizing actor” (Balzacq, Reference Balzacq2010). These are usually articulated as national security threats to the state by elites. But international relations scholars are also increasingly interested in “vernacular” definitions of everyday security threats by nonelites that go beyond threats to the state (e.g., Vaughan-Williams & Stevens, Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016). Indeed, ordinary people tend to define “security” more broadly in terms of “feeling physically safe where you are” and as the absence of threats, meaning that for ordinary people, crimes such as burglary are a security threat, although they are unlikely to be securitized. We draw on this broader definition: a security threat is realistic rather than symbolic, and involves harm (or its potential) to a person or group’s welfare.

2 “Shocks” may not inevitably raise threat levels, however. According to Marshall et al. (Reference Marshall, Bryant, Amsel, Suh, Cook and Neria2007), for that to happen, there needs to be a sense that what led to the shock is ongoing rather than a one-off—that is, the probability of recurrence is significantly different from zero. Elites may also frame issues as serious or emerging threats, regardless of any objective change, to promote more stringent security policies, expand government power into new spheres, or win votes, rather than in response to events, as when Donald Trump focused attention on the caravan of immigrants approaching the U.S. border in the days before the 2018 midterm elections, only to drop the subject soon after (Buzan et al., Reference Buzan, Waever and de Wilde1998; Fuchs, Reference Fuchs2013; Huijboom & Bodea, Reference Huijboom and Bodea2015; Schneier, Reference Schneier2003, Reference Schneier2008; Strauβ, Reference Strauβ, Friedewald, Burgess, Čas, Bellanova and Peissl2017).

3 We share the perspective of Feldman (Reference Feldman2013) and Stenner (Reference Stenner2005) that authoritarianism is a predisposition—“any pre-existing and relatively stable tendency to respond in a particular way to certain events” (Stenner, Reference Stenner2005, p. 14)—and thus relatively stable; indeed, the British Election Study (BES), which has asked Feldman’s authoritarianism questions in four waves, shows high correlations between waves and a barely changing mean.

4 Wave 4 of the 2014–2023 BES panel included a question about the government’s success in dealing with threats, but this was not asked in other waves of the survey. The annual Pew Global Attitudes Survey includes the United Kingdom, but there are four problems. First, the issues it asks about vary somewhat over time—for example, in 2012, they were limited to various types of economic threats, and in 2014, they were not asked at all. Second, Pew does not ask questions about individual-level characteristics in their surveys beyond demographics—that is, nothing that captures authoritarianism or media habits. Third, Pew does not ask about personal threats. Fourth, Pew asks whether up to eight “international concerns” are a “major threat,” “minor threat,” or “not a threat,” putting the onus on the respondent to say that an issue is “not a threat” to the country, whereas the question wording in the surveys we use ask respondents which of a list of issues is a threat at the moment. Pew indicates high perceptions of threat on almost all the issues it asks about: if we take major or minor threat responses as indicators of threat, an average of more than six of the eight issues were identified as major or minor threats by respondents in each of the 2013, 2016, 2017, and 2018 (data for 2019 and 2020 are not yet available) surveys that ask about the same issues as the surveys we use. Factor analysis also shows that these perceptions always load onto a single factor, suggesting that the question wording does not effectively capture variation in perceptions of threat. Nevertheless, we might expect the Pew surveys to show similar patterns of change over time—and they do. They show increases in perceptions of threats from the environment and little change or a slight decrease in perceptions of the threat from ISIS/terrorism, for example. Perceptions of the overall number of threats also show an increase, as in our surveys.

5 We said earlier that the focus of this article is Britain: 87% of the British population resides in England.

6 The data can be found in the UK Data Service repository at http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-851004.

7 The decision of whether to exclude non-White respondents from analysis depends on the research question. It is common to exclude Blacks or Latinos in trying to understand racial attitudes in the United States and Canada (Harell et al., Reference Harell, Soroka, Iyengar and Valentino2012; Hartman et al., Reference Hartman, Newman and Bell2014), Similarly with British data, in trying to understand the impact of the ethnicity of the candidate on voting, Fisher et al. (Reference Fisher, Heath, Sanders and Sobolewska2015) focus only on White respondents in some of their analysis. If the research question is broader, one might include all respondents and control for race. In our case, the threats from immigration and racial hate crimes in particular are different for minorities and likely to have different relationships with authoritarian predispositions and context. We therefore exclude non-White respondents from the analysis. Tables A2 and A4 in the Appendix show the analysis with non-White respondents included and a control variable for race. The substantive implications of the analysis do not change.

8 These were coded in the same way and recalculated to a 0–1 scale, with the exception of news consumption. In the 2012 survey, news consumption was operationalized by Stevens and Vaughan-Williams (Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016) as the number of hours spent watching television news as a proportion of the total hours spent watching television. In the 2020 survey, respondents were asked how often they watched BBC news, other national television news stations, and local television news. We employ z-scores from each of the two measures to make them comparable. The results do not change if we exclude this variable.

9 The increase in the number of issues identified as threats in 2020 at the national and personal levels may be partly a function of the research design differences discussed earlier. COVID-19 may also account for some of the increase because its effects were so universally felt. But there seems to have been an overall increase in perceptions of threats that is not explained by research design or COVID-19. The Pew surveys (from 2013 to 2018) referred to in Footnote note 4 show several similar patterns of increases to those we find in our surveys for national threats.

10 In additional analysis of perceptions of the threat of COVID-19 in 2020, we examined the relationship between authoritarian predispositions and perceptions of increased personal threat from COVID-19 (personal threat) and of increased divisions in British society as a result of COVID-19 (normative threat) (these were part of an experiment: we focus on the 500 respondents in the control group). Given that high authoritarians are particularly sensitive to threats that “violate traditional social conventions” (30), if COVID-19 was viewed in these terms—and the factor analysis suggests it was not—we would expect high authoritarians to be more likely to agree that it had increased societal divisions. However, the data show high authoritarians no more likely than libertarians to say that British society had become more divided. This relationship does not change with controls for partisanship (which were a more powerful influence on these perceptions). We did find a conditional relationship between authoritarian predispositions, media use, and perceptions of the personal threat of COVID-19 that implied greater consumption of media among high authoritarians was associated with reduced perceptions of threat. This is consistent with Ollerenshaw’s (Reference Ollerenshaw2022) evidence from the United States, which also focuses on the personal threat of COVID. We also looked at British Election Study Internet Panel (BESIP) data gathered during the pandemic. These data indicate that high authoritarians were consistently more likely to say that the government had handled the pandemic well. They were also less worried about the impact of the pandemic on the economy.

12 Of primary interest to us is comparing coverage of the same issue in the two periods under examination in order to gauge whether the threat had increased, decreased or stayed the same since 2012, rather than comparing the numbers of stories across different issues—because we cannot be certain that the keyword and thematic searches represent the universe of stories pertaining to each issue.

13 Reestimating the original 2012 models without those controls makes no difference to any of the substantive conclusions Stevens and Vaughan-Williams (Reference Stevens and Vaughan-Williams2016) drew about influences on perceptions of threats.

14 Table A4 in the Appendix shows the estimates with non-White respondents included.

15 All other variables are set at their mean or mode.

16 We examined the interactions between our measures of media exposure, authoritarian predispositions, and contextual change as a check. We found no systematic moderating effects of media exposure—which may be unsurprising, given that our measure of contextual change is media attention to an issue for the 12 months prior to the survey, a length of time for changing contexts to be noticed by individuals regardless of media consumption. We also examined models with two-way interactions between media exposure and authoritarian predispositions as a check that the year dummy variable is not merely picking up exposure effects. In contrast with Table 5, we found that these interactions were not statistically significant beyond chance.

References

Altemeyer, B. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Andreouli, E., & Brice, E. (2022). Citizenship under COVID-19: An analysis of UK political rhetoric during the first wave of the 2020 pandemic. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 32(3), 555572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arikan, G. (2023). Sociotropic and personal threats and authoritarian reactions during Covid-19. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 49(3), 447459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arikan, G., & Gunay, D. (2021). Public attitudes towards climate change: A cross-country analysis. British Journal of Politics and International Relations, 23(1), 158174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asbrock, F., & Fritsche, I. (2013). Authoritarian reactions to terrorist threat: Who is being threatened, the me or the we? International Journal of Psychology, 48(1), 3549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bai, Y., & Tao, X. (2021). Comparison of COVID-19 and influenza characteristics. Journal of Zhejiang University Science B, 22(2), 8798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balzacq, T. (Éd.). (2010). Securitization Theory. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203868508CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berrebi, C., & Klor, E. (2008). Are voters sensitive to terrorism? Direct evidence from the Israeli electorate. American Political Science Review, 102(3), 279301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borkowska, M., & Laurence, J. (2021). Coming together or coming apart? Changes in social cohesion during the Covid-19 pandemic in England. European Societies, 23(S1), S618–636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bromham, L., Hua, X., Cardillo, M., Schneemann, H., & Greenhill, S. (2018). Parasites and politics: Why cross-cultural studies must control for relatedness, proximity and covariation. Royal Society Open Science, 5(8), 181100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buzan, B., Waever, O., & de Wilde, J. (1998). Security: A new framework for analysis. Lynne Rienner.Google Scholar
Chanley, V. (1994). Commitment to political tolerance: Situational and activity-based differences. Political Behavior, 16(3), 343363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choma, B., Hanoch, Y., Gummerum, M., & Hodson, G. (2013). Relations between risk perceptions and socio-political ideology are domain- and ideology-dependent. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(1), 2934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chrisp, J., & Pearce, N. (2019). Grey power: Towards a political economy of older voters in the UK. Political Quarterly, 90(4), 743756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, S., & Moore, C. (2010). The evolution of threat narratives in the age of terror: Understanding terrorist threats in Britain. International Affairs, 86(4), 821835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Das, E., Bushman, B., Bezemer, M., Kerkhof, P., & Vermeulen, I. (2009). How terrorism news reports increase prejudice against outgroups: A terror management account. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 453459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daszak, P., Keusch, G. T., Phelan, A. L., Johnson, C. K., & Osterholm, M. T. (2021). Infectious disease threats: A rebound to resilience. Health Affairs, 40(2), 204211. https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.2020.01544CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, D., & Silver, B. (2004). Civil liberties vs. security: Public opinion in the context of the terrorist attacks on America. American Journal of Political Science, 48(1), 2846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devos, T., Spini, D., & Schwartz, S. (2002). Conflicts among human values and trust in institutions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 41(4), 481494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Downing, J., Gerwens, S., & Dron, R. (2022). Tweeting terrorism: Vernacular conceptions of Muslims and terror in the wake of the Manchester bombing on Twitter. Critical Studies on Terrorism, 15(2), 239266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duckitt, J., & Sibley, C. (2010). Personality, ideology, prejudice, and politics: A dual-process motivational model. Journal of Personality, 78(6), 18611893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Echebarra-Echabe, A., & Fernandez-Guede, E. (2006). Effects of terrorism on attitudes and ideological orientation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36(2), 259265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, S. (2013). Comments on: Authoritarianism in social context: The role of threat. International Journal of Psychology, 48(1), 5559.Google ScholarPubMed
Feldman, S., & Stenner, K. (1997). Perceived threat and authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 18(4), 741770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernandez, K., & Kuenzi, M. (2010). Crime and support for democracy in Africa and Latin America. Political Studies, 58(3), 450471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, S., Heath, A., Sanders, D., & Sobolewska, M. (2015). Candidate ethnicity and vote choice in Britain. British Journal of Political Science, 45(4), 883905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, S., & Pearce, S. (2018). The generational decay of Euroscepticism in the UK and the EU referendum. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties, 28(1), 1937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, C. (2013). Political economy and surveillance theory. Critical Sociology, 39(5), 671687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FutureLearn. (2020, March 10). How does coronavirus compare to other outbreaks? https://www.futurelearn.com/info/blog/covid-19-how-does-coronavirus-compare-to-other-outbreaksGoogle Scholar
Geana, M., Rabb, N., & Sloman, S. (2021). Walking the party line: The growing role of political ideology in shaping health behavior in the United States. SSM - Population Health, 16, 100950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golec de Zavala, A., Guerra, R., & Simao, C. (2017). The relationship between the Brexit vote and individual predictors of prejudice: Collective narcissism, right wing authoritarianism, social dominance orientation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haider-Markel, D., & Vieux, A. (2008). Gender and conditional support for torture in the war on terror. Politics & Gender, 4(1), 533.Google Scholar
Harell, A., Soroka, S., Iyengar, S., & Valentino, N. (2012). The impact of economic and cultural cues on support for immigration in Canada and the United States. Canadian Journal of Political Science, 45(3), 499530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartman, T., Newman, B., & Bell, C. S. (2014). Decoding prejudice toward Hispanics: Group cues and public reactions to threatening immigrant behavior. Political Behavior, 36(1), 143166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heller, A., Decker, O., Clemens, V., Fegert, J., Heiner, S., Brahler, E., & Schmidt, P. (2022). Changes in authoritarianism before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: Comparisons of latent means across East and West Germany, gender, age, and education. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 941466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helzer, E., & Pizarro, D. (2011). Dirty liberals! Reminders of physical cleanliness influence moral and political attitudes. Psychological Science, 22(4), 517522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hetherington, M., & Weiler, J. (2009). Authoritarianism and polarization in American politics. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hetherington, M., & Weiler, J. (2018). Prius or pickup? How the answers to four simple questions explain America’s great divide. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Hibbing, J. (2020). The securitarian personality: What really motivates Trump’s base and why it matters for the post-Trump era. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hibbing, J. (2022). Why do Trump’s authoritarian followers resist COVID-19 authorities? Because they are not really authoritarian followers. Frontiers in Political Science, 4, 880798). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpos.2022.880798CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinckley, R. (2021). Local existential threat, authoritarianism, and support for right-wing populism. Social Science Journal. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/03623319.2020.1859816CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsch-Hoefler, S., Canetti, D., Rapaport, C., & Hobfoll, S. (2016). Conflict will harden your heart: Exposure to violence, psychological distress, and peace barriers in Israel and Palestine. British Journal of Political Science, 46(4), 845859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HM Government. (2010, October). A strong Britain in an age of uncertainty: The national security strategy (Cm 7953). https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/61936/national-security-strategy.pdfGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, D. (2010). Politicized places: Explaining where and when immigrants provoke local opposition. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 4060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddy, L., Feldman, S., Taber, C., & Lahav, G. (2005). Threat, anxiety, and support of antiterrorism policies. American Journal of Political Science, 49(3), 593608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huijboom, N., & Bodea, G. (2015). Understanding the political PNR-debate in Europe: A Discourse analytical perspective. Perspectives on European Politics and Society, 16(2), 241255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarvis, L. (2019). Toward a vernacular security studies: Origins, interlocutors, contributions, and challenges. International Studies Review, 21(1), 107126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarvis, L., & Lister, M. (2010). Stakeholder security: The new Western way of counter-terrorism? Contemporary Politics, 16(2), 173188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahan, D., Braman, D., Gastil, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. K. (2007). Culture and identity-protective cognition: Explaining the White-male effect in risk perception. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 4(3), 465505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, E. (2016, July 7). It’s NOT the economy stupid: Brexit as a story of personal values. LSE British Politics and Policy Blog. https://blogs.lse.ac.uk/politicsandpolicy/personal-values-brexit-vote/Google Scholar
Kealey, T. (2021, August 31). The connection between disease and authoritarianism. The Dispatch. https://thedispatch.com/article/the-connection-between-disease-and/Google Scholar
Kruglanski, A. (2004). The psychology of closed mindedness. Routledge.Google Scholar
Kusano, K., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2018). Ecology of freedom: Competitive tests of the role of pathogens, climate, and natural disasters in the development of socio-political freedom. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahav, G., & Courtemanche, M. (2012). The ideological effects of framing threat on immigration and civil liberties. Political Behavior, 34(3), 477505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lintern, S. (2021, February 22.). The threat of a third coronavirus wave has forced Boris Johnson to learn from his mistakes. The Independent. https://www.independent.co.uk/news/health/coronavirus-third-wave-lockdown-roadmap-boris-johnson-b1805810.htmlGoogle Scholar
Marshall, R., Bryant, R., Amsel, L., Suh, E. J., Cook, J., & Neria, Y. (2007). The psychology of ongoing threat: Relative risk appraisal, the September 11 attacks, and terrorism-related fears. American Psychologist, 62(4), 304316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merolla, J., Ramos, J., & Zechmeister, E. (2007). Crisis, charisma, and consequences: Evidence from the 2004 U.S. presidential election. The Journal of Politics, 69(1), 3042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J., & Krosnick, J. (2004). Threat as a motivator of political activism: A field experiment. Political Psychology, 25(4), 507523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montalvo, J. (2011). Voting after the bombings: A natural experiment on the effect of terrorist attacks on democratic elections. Review of Economics and Statistics, 93(4), 11461154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Ring, S., Hutton, R., & Paton, J. (2020, April 24). How the alarm went off too late in Britain’s virus response. Bloomberg. https://www.bloomberg.com/news/features/2020-04-24/coronavirus-uk-how-boris-johnson-s-government-let-virus-get-away johnson-s-government-let-virus-get-awayGoogle Scholar
Murray, D., & Schaller, M. (2012). Threat(s) and conformity deconstructed: Perceived threat of infectious disease and its implications for conformist attitudes and behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(1), 180188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, D., Schaller, M., & Suedfeld, P. (2013). Pathogens and politics: Further evidence that parasite prevalence predicts authoritarianism. PLOS ONE, 8(5), e62275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2019). Cultural backlash: Trump, Brexit, and authoritarian populism. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyman, J. (2021). The everyday life of security: Capturing space, practice, and affect. International Political Sociology, 15(3), 313337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obadi, M., Kunst, J., Kteily, N., Thomsen, L., & Sidanius, J. (2018). Living under threat: Mutual threat perception drives anti-Muslim and anti-Western hostility in the age of terrorism. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(5), 567584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Office for National Statistics. (2022, May 23). How coronavirus (COVID-19) compares with flu and pneumonia as a cause of death. https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/healthandsocialcare/conditionsanddiseases/articles/howcoronaviruscovid19compareswithfluasacauseofdeath/2022-05-23Google Scholar
Ollerenshaw, T. (2022). The conditional effects of authoritarianism on COVID‑19 pandemic health behaviors and policy preferences. Political Behavior. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-022-09828-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, G., & Payne, S. (2021, March 19). “The best and worst of Boris”: Johnson’s stewardship of Covid crisis. Financial Times.Google Scholar
Peffley, M., Hutchison, M., & Shamir, M. (2015). The impact of persistent terrorism on political tolerance: Israel, 1980 to 2011. American Political Science Review, 109(4), 817832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peitz, L., Dhont, K., & Seyd, B. (2018). The psychology of supranationalism: Its ideological correlates and implications for EU attitudes and post-Brexit preferences. Political Psychology, 39(6), 13051322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pericas, J. (2020). Authoritarianism and the threat of infectious diseases. The Lancet, 395(10230), 11111112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollet, T. (2014). A re-analysis of the relationship between “parasite stress” and authoritarianism. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pretus, C., & Vilarroya, Ó (2022). Social norms (not threat) mediate willingness to sacrifice in individuals fused with the nation: Insights from the COVID-19 pandemic. European Journal of Social Psychology, 52(4), 772781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridout, T., Grosse, A., & Appleton, A. (2008). News media use and Americans’ perceptions of global threat. British Journal of Political Science, 38(4), 575593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roser, M. (2020, March 4). The Spanish flu: The global impact of the largest influenza pandemic in history. Our World in Statistics. https://ourworldindata.org/spanish-flu-largest-influenza-pandemic-in-historyGoogle Scholar
Schneier, B. (2003). Beyond fear: Thinking sensibly about security in an uncertain world. Copernicus Books.Google Scholar
Schneier, B. (2008). The psychology of security. In Progress in cryptology—AFRICACRYPT Conference proceedings (pp. 1114). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sjoberg, L. (1999). Consequences of perceived risk: Demand for mitigation. Journal of Risk Research, 2(2), 129149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sniderman, P., Hagendoorn, L., & Prior, M. (2004). Predisposing factors and situational triggers: Exclusionary reactions to immigrant minorities. American Political Science Review, 98(1), 3549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stenner, K. (2005). The authoritarian dynamic. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stenner, K., & Haidt, J. (2018). Authoritarianism is not a momentary madness, but an eternal dynamic within liberal democracies. In Sunstein, C. R. (Ed.), Can it happen here? Authoritarianism in America (pp. 175219). HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Stevens, D., & Banducci, S. (2022). What are you afraid of? Authoritarianism, terrorism and threat. Political Psychology, 43(6), 10811100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, D., & Vaughan-Williams, N. (2016). Everyday security threats: Perceptions, experiences and consequences. Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
Strassle, P., Stewart, A., Quintero, S., Bonilla, J., Alhomsi, A., Santana-Ufret, V., Maldonado, A., Forde, A., & Napoles, A. M. (2022). COVID-19–related discrimination among racial/ethnic minorities and other marginalized communities in the United States. American Journal of Public Health, 112(3), 453466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauβ, S. (2017). A game of hide and seek? Unscrambling the tradeoff. In Friedewald, M., Burgess, J. P., Čas, J., Bellanova, R., & Peissl, W. (Eds.), Surveillance privacy and security: Citizens’ perspectives (pp. 255272). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorisdottir, H., & Jost, J. (2011). Motivated closed-mindedness mediates the effect of threat on political conservatism. Political Psychology, 32(5), 785811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. (2014). The parasite-stress theory of values and sociality: Infectious disease, history and human values worldwide. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tillman, E. (2021). Authoritarianism and the evolution of West European electoral politics. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J., Inbar, Y., Aarøe, L., Barclay, P., Barlow, F. K., Barra, M. D., Becker, D. V., Borovoi, L., Choi, I., Choi, J. A., Consedine, N., Conway, A., Conway, J. R., Conway, P., Adoric, V. C., Demirci, D. E., Fernández, A. M., Ferreira, D. C. S., Ishii, K., … Žeželj, I. (2016). Parasite stress and pathogen avoidance relate to distinct dimensions of political ideology across 30 nations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(44), 1240812413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viklund, M. (2003). Trust and risk perception in Western Europe: A cross-national study. Risk Analysis, 23(4), 727738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vowles, J. (2022). Authoritarianism and mass political preferences in times of COVID-19: The 2020 New Zealand general election. Frontiers in Political Science, 4https://doi.org/10.3389/fpos.2022.885299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (2023). Cumulative number of confirmed human cases for avian influenza A(H5N1) reported to WHO, 2003-2023, 14 July 2023. https://www.who.int/publications/m/item/cumulative-number-of-confirmed-human-cases-for-avian-influenza-a(h5n1)-reported-to-who--2003-2023-14-july-2023Google Scholar
Zmigrod, L., Ebert, T., Götz, F., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2021). The psychological and socio-political consequences of infectious diseases: Authoritarianism, governance, and nonzoonotic (human-to-human) infection transmission. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 9(2), 456474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Expectations of interactive relationships between authoritarian predispositions and attention to threats

Figure 1

Table 2. Perceptions of threats in 2012 and 2020

Figure 2

Table 3. Exploratory factor analysis of the 10 security threats

Figure 3

Table 4. Coverage in national newspapers one year before June 2012 and July 2020 surveys

Figure 4

Table 5. Perceptions of threat, authoritarianism and media coverage in 2012 and 2020—individual logit models

Figure 5

Figure 1. Relationships between authoritarian predispositions, media attention, and identifying issues as threats.

Supplementary material: PDF

Stevens et al. supplementary material

Appendix

Download Stevens et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 192.9 KB