Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T11:58:00.597Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations between residual depressive symptoms, cognition, and functioning in patients with euthymic bipolar disorder: results from the FACE-BD cohort

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Paul Roux*
Affiliation:
Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie d'Adultes, Centre Hospitalier de Versailles, Le Chesnay, Laboratoire HandiRESP, EA4047, UFR des Sciences de la Santé Simone Veil, Université de Versailles Saint-Quentin-En-Yvelines, Montigny-le-Bretonneux and Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, France
Aurélie Raust
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Henri Mondor, DHU Pepsy, Pôle de Psychiatrie et d'Addictologie, Créteil, Université Paris Est, Faculté de Médecine, Créteil and Inserm, U955, Equipe Psychiatrie Translationnelle, Créteil, France
Anne-Sophie Cannavo
Affiliation:
Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie d'Adultes, Centre Hospitalier de Versailles, Le Chesnay, Laboratoire HandiRESP, EA4047, UFR des Sciences de la Santé Simone Veil, Université de Versailles Saint-Quentin-En-Yvelines, Montigny-le-Bretonneux and Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, France
Valérie Aubin
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil and Pôle de Psychiatrie, Centre Hospitalier Princesse Grace, Monaco, France
Bruno Aouizerate
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil and Centre Expert Trouble Bipolaire, Pôle de Psychiatrie Générale et Universitaire (3/4/7), Hôpital Charles Perrens, Bordeaux
Jean-Michel Azorin
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil and AP-HM, Hôpital Sainte-Marguerite, Pôle de Psychiatrie, Marseille, France
Frank Bellivier
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, AP-HP, GH Saint-Louis - Lariboisière - Fernand widal, Pôle Neurosciences, Paris and Université Paris Diderot, UMR-S, Paris
Raoul Belzeaux
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil and AP-HM, Hôpital Sainte-Marguerite, Pôle de Psychiatrie, Marseille, France
Thierry Bougerol
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, University Grenoble Alpes, Grenoble, CHU de Grenoble et des Alpes, Grenoble and Grenoble Institut des Neurosciences (GIN) Inserm U 836, La Tranche, France
Iréna Cussac
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil and Pôle de Psychiatrie, Centre Hospitalier Princesse Grace, Monaco, France
Philippe Courtet
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, Department of Emergency Psychiatry & Post Acute Care, Academic hospital of Montpellier, Montpellier University, Montpellier and 16 INSERM U1061, Montpellier, France
Bruno Etain
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, AP-HP, GH Saint-Louis - Lariboisière - Fernand widal, Pôle Neurosciences, Paris, 11 Université Paris Diderot, UMR-S, Paris, France and Centre for Affective Disorders, Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Sébastien Gard
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil and Centre Expert Trouble Bipolaire, Pôle de Psychiatrie Générale et Universitaire (3/4/7), Hôpital Charles Perrens, Bordeaux
Sophie Job
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil and Pôle de Psychiatrie et Psychologie Clinique - Centre Psychothéapique de Nancy, Laxou, France
Jean-Pierre Kahn
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, Pôle de Psychiatrie et Psychologie Clinique - Centre Psychothérapique de Nancy, Laxou and Université de Lorraine, Nancy, France
Marion Leboyer
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Henri Mondor, DHU Pepsy, Pôle de Psychiatrie et d'Addictologie, Créteil, Universite Paris Est, Faculté de Médecine, Créteil and Inserm, U955, Equipe Psychiatrie Translationnelle, Créteil, France
Emilie Olié
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, Department of Emergency Psychiatry & Post Acute Care, Academic hospital of Montpellier, Montpellier University, Montpellier and 16 INSERM U1061, Montpellier, France
Chantal Henry
Affiliation:
Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, AP-HP, Hopitaux Universitaires Henri Mondor, DHU Pepsy, Pôle de Psychiatrie et d'Addictologie, Créteil, Université Paris Est, Faculté de Médecine, Créteil, Inserm, U955, Equipe Psychiatrie Translationnelle, Créteil and Institut Pasteur, Unité Perception et Mémoire, Paris, France
Christine Passerieux
Affiliation:
Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie d'Adultes, Centre Hospitalier de Versailles, Le Chesnay, Laboratoire HandiRESP, EA4047, UFR des Sciences de la Santé Simone Veil, Université de Versailles Saint-Quentin-En-Yvelines, Montigny-le-Bretonneux and Fondation Fondamental, Créteil, France
*
Paul Roux, MD, PhD, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie d'Adultes, Centre Hospitalier de Versailles, 177 rue de Versailles, 78157 Le Chesnay, France. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The relationship between residual depressive symptoms, cognition and functioning in patients with euthymic bipolar disorder is a subject of debate.

Aims

To assess whether cognition mediates the association between residual depressive symptoms and functioning in patients with bipolar disorder who were euthymic.

Method

We included 241 adults with euthymic bipolar disorder in a multicentre cross-sectional study. We used a battery of tests to assess six cognition domains. A path analysis was then used to perform a mediation analysis of the relationship between residual depressive symptoms, cognitive components and functioning.

Results

Only verbal and working memory were significantly associated with better functioning. Residual depressive symptoms were associated with poorer functioning. No significant relationship was found between residual depressive symptoms and any cognitive component.

Conclusions

Cognition and residual depressive symptoms appear to be two independent sources of variation in the functioning of people with euthymic bipolar disorder.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2017 

Bipolar disorders are highly disabling Reference Rosa, Franco, Martinez-Aran, Sanchez-Moreno, Reinares and Salamero1 and prevalent. Reference Merikangas, Jin, He, Kessler, Lee and Sampson2 More than half of individuals with bipolar disorder experience significant functional impairment in several domains, such as family and social life and work, outside the acute phases of the illness. Reference Sanchez-Moreno, Martinez-Aran and Vieta3 In the past decade, the focus of research in bipolar disorder has changed from clinical remission to functional recovery. Reference Sanchez-Moreno, Martinez-Aran and Vieta3 However, the sources of high variation observed in the psychosocial functioning of individuals with bipolar disorder are still poorly understood. Cognitive impairment is an important determinant of functional impairment in bipolar disorder. Reference Martinez-Aran, Vieta, Torrent, Sanchez-Moreno, Goikolea and Salamero4 Because the strength of association between poor social functioning and cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder is similar to that seen in schizophrenia, Reference Depp, Mausbach, Harmell, savla, Bowie and Harvey5 it seems crucial to characterise the underlying architecture of cognitive performance in bipolar disorder. Previous studies investigating multiple cognitive areas in bipolar disorder have usually focused on domains a priori that were subjectively defined and selected. However, the validity of these domains is questionable, as it relies on the assumption that the latent organisation of human cognition is similar in people with bipolar disorder and healthy controls. This crucial assumption has received little experimental evidence. Reference Schretlen, Pena, Aretouli, Orue, Cascella and Pearlson6 It thus remains possible that the same neuropsychological tests might evaluate different cognitive competencies in these two groups of participants if there is a discrepancy in how cognitive measures relate to one another between individuals with and without bipolar disorder. This lack of equivalence between the constructs could lead to artefacts in the observed differences in cognitive test performances. A few studies have explored the latent cognitive structure in bipolar disorder and found that cognitive functioning was best described by multifactorial models. Reference Schretlen, Pena, Aretouli, Orue, Cascella and Pearlson6Reference Gallagher, Gray and Watson8 However, some of these studies included patients who were symptomatic. Reference Czobor, Jaeger, Berns, Gonzalez and Loftus7,Reference Gallagher, Gray and Watson8 Here, we have measured performance for a broad range of cognitive domains in euthymic bipolar disorder using a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests. We have examined the component structure of bipolar disorder cognitive processes using a principal component analysis (PCA), which allows the data-driven reduction of multiple cognitive measures, avoids arbitrary and a priori categorisation of several tests into domains, and results in a reliable estimate of underlying cognitive constructs in bipolar disorder.

Among clinical factors, residual depressive symptoms have been reported to be the strongest predictor of functional impairment Reference Bonnin, Martinez-Aran, Torrent, Pacchiarotti, Rosa and Franco9 and quality of life Reference Dias, Brissos, Frey and Kapczinski10 in euthymic bipolar disorder. They are also associated with lower adherence to medication in individuals with bipolar disorder. Reference Belzeaux, Correard, Boyer, Etain, Loftus and Bellivier11 In contrast, residual hypomanic symptoms have no impact on functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder. Reference Martinez-Aran, Vieta, Torrent, Sanchez-Moreno, Goikolea and Salamero4,Reference Bonnin, Martinez-Aran, Torrent, Pacchiarotti, Rosa and Franco9,Reference Allen, Bello and Thaler12Reference Martino, Marengo, Igoa, Scapola, Ais and Perinot14 Whereas residual hypomanic symptoms are not related to cognition, Reference Bourne, Bilderbeck, Drennan, Atkinson, Price and Geddes15 it is less clear whether subthreshold depressive symptoms negatively affect neuropsychological performance in euthymic bipolar disorder or not. The relationship between residual depressive symptoms and cognition is mixed, showing a small impact of subthreshold depression on only a few cognitive components, such as verbal memory, speed, and executive function, but not for others. Reference Bourne, Aydemir, Balanza-Martinez, Bora, Brissos and Cavanagh16 A few studies have explored the role of cognition in mediating the relationship between depressive symptoms and functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder, with inconsistent results. One study showed that verbal memory partially mediated the relationship between subthreshold depressive symptoms and functional outcome in people with bipolar disorder who were euthymic. Reference del Mar Bonnin, González-Pinto, Solé, Reinares, González-Ortega and Alberich17 However, the sample size was not large enough to include other cognitive moderators in the model. In contrast, another study reported that cognition did not mediate the relationship between depressive symptoms and social competencies in bipolar disorder. Reference Bowie, Depp, McGrath, Wolyniec, Mausbach and Thornquist18 Again, only one global neurocognitive composite score was included as the cognitive mediator in the model, because of the limited sample size. There have been no studies, to date, that have investigated simultaneous mediation between subdepressive symptoms and functioning by multiple cognitive components in bipolar disorder. In this study, the cognitive domains obtained with PCA were entered in a path analysis model, as potential mediators between residual depressive symptoms and functioning.

Method

Study design and recruiting network characteristics

This multicentre, cross-sectional study included patients recruited into the FACE-BD (FondaMental Academic Centers of Expertise for Bipolar Disorders) cohort by a French national network of nine bipolar disorder expert centres (Bordeaux, Créteil, Grenoble, Marseille, Monaco, Montpellier, Nancy, Paris and Versailles). This network was set up by the FondaMental Foundation (www.fondation-fondamental.org) and funded by the French Ministry of Research and the French Ministry of Health to build an infrastructure and provide resources to follow clinical cohorts and comparative-effectiveness research on a representative patient population.

Participants

Bipolar disorder was diagnosed based on a structured clinical interview that assessed the DSM-IV-TR criteria. 19 Out-patients of 18 to 65 years of age with type I, II, or not otherwise specified (NOS, including cyclothymia) bipolar disorder were eligible. All patients were euthymic when they were tested according to the DSM-IV-TR criteria, with a Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) Reference Montgomery and Asberg20 of less than 10 and a Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) Reference Young, Biggs, Ziegler and Meyer21 of less than 10. Exclusion criteria were a history of neurological or sensory disorders, dyslexia, dysorthographia, dyscalculia, dysphasia, dyspraxia, language delay, substance-related disorders in the previous month and electro-convulsive therapy in the past year. The ethics committee (Comité de Protection des Personnes lie de France IX) approved the study protocol on 18 January 2010. Although the committee waived the requirement for written informed consent, the patients received a letter informing them of the study and asking whether they agreed to participate.

Assessment tools

Clinical assessments

The age at onset; number of previous mixed, hypomanic, manic and major depressive episodes; subtype of bipolar disorder; and history of psychotic symptoms were recorded. We used the yes/no format for recording whether the patient was taking lithium carbonate, anticonvulsants, antipsychotics, antidepressants or anxiolytics at the time of the evaluation. Finally, three sociodemo-graphic characteristics were collected: gender, age and educational level.

Psychosocial functioning was measured using the Functioning Assessment Short Test (FAST), Reference Rosa, Sanchez-Moreno, Martinez-Aran, Salamero, Torrent and Reinares22 which covers six functioning domains: autonomy, occupational functioning, cognitive functioning, financial issues, interpersonal relationships and leisure time. The higher the score, the greater the disability. FAST has good internal consistency (Cronbach alpha between 0.87 and 0.96) and test–retest reliability (Pearson correlation coefficient between 0.90 and 0.97; intraclass correlation coefficient between 0.90 and 0.98, see online supplement DS1 for a bibliography of FAST psychometric properties).

Battery of cognitive tests

Experienced psychologists administered the tests in a fixed order. Testing lasted a total of 120 min, including a 5–10 min break. The standardised test battery complied with the recommendations issued by the International Society for Bipolar Disorders. Reference Yatham, Torres, Malhi, Frangou, Glahn and Bearden23 It included 11 tests and evaluated the following six cognitive domains:

  1. (a) motor speed with the digit symbol coding and symbol search subtests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) version III; Reference Wechsler24

  2. (b) attention with the Conners' Continuous Performance Test II (CPT-II) Reference Conners and Staff25 and the Trail Making Test (TMT); Reference Reitan26

  3. (c) executive functions with the Stroop colour and word test and verbal fluency; Reference Lezak27

  4. (d) verbal memory with the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT); Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober28

  5. (e) working memory with the digit span subtest from the WAIS-III and the spatial span subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale version III; and

  6. (f) intellectual functioning with the vocabulary and matrix reasoning subtests from the WAIS-III.

Some of the current cognitive data obtained with this battery have been published previously. Reference Roux, Raust, Cannavo, Aubin, Aouizerate and Azorin29

Statistical analyses

PCA

PCA is a powerful statistical method for identifying the underlying organisation of multiple variables such as cognitive measures and allows data reduction necessary to avoid type I errors from multiple comparisons. The data-set for the PCA included 22 raw cognitive variables: number of correct answers for the digit symbol coding and symbol search tests, percentages of omissions and commission errors for the CPT-II, reaction time (ms) for hits in the CPT, time (s) to complete the TMT-A and -B, number of responses in the colour, word, and colour-word conditions of the Stroop test, number of correct words for phonemic and semantic verbal fluency, number of recalled words in the immediate recall, short and long delay free recall of the CVLT, number of total correct recognised words for the CVLT, span lengths for the forward and backward conditions of the spatial and digit span tests, number of correct answers for matrix reasoning, and total score for vocabulary.

Sampling adequacy was evaluated using the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure Reference Dziuban and Shirkey30 for the overall cognitive data-set and Bartlett's test of sphericity. Reference Bartlett31 The number of components to be extracted in the PCA was determined by Horn's parallel analysis. Reference Horn32 This method contrasts eigenvalues produced through a parallel PCA on 1000 random data-sets, with the same number of variables and observations as the observational data-set, to generate eigenvalues for components that are adjusted for sample error-induced inflation. Adjusted eigenvalues > 1 indicate dimensions to retain.

We ran a PCA on cognitive variables followed by an Oblimin rotation. The rotation was performed to simplify the component structure. We used an oblique rotation because the cognitive components were believed to be correlated with each other. In PCA, the usual standard for sample size is a participant-to-variable ratio > 5, Reference Bryant, Yarnold, Grimm and Yarnold33 and therefore required 110 participants for the current study.

Path and mediation analyses

Zero-order correlations between MADRS, cognitive components, FAST scores, age, gender and education were calculated using Pearson correlation coefficients. A path analysis was performed using MADRS, cognitive components and FAST scores to test whether cognitive components mediated the relationship between residual depressive symptoms and functioning. The model tested in the path analysis did not include the YRMS score, as we expected it to correlate with neither cognition nor functioning. The model allowed the residual variances of the cognitive components scores to be correlated. Age, gender and education were used as covariates in the model.

Analyses were performed using the lavaan package of R statistical software version 3.3 with the maximum likelihood estimation method. Linear regression analyses were conducted to evaluate the relationships among the variables and were indexed using standardised path coefficients. Because the FAST total score is usually not normally distributed in euthymic bipolar disorder, Reference Bas, Poyraz, Bas, Poyraz and Tosun34 we used a non-parametric bootstrapping of the standard errors with 2000 iterations for the correlation and path analyses. The fit between the model and the data was assessed using four indices: the chi-square goodness-of-fit statistic (χ2), comparative fit index (CFI), root-mean-squared error of approximation (RMSEA) and standardised root-mean-square residual (SRMR).

Results

Participants

We included 241 patients. Table 1 reports their sociodemographic and clinical characteristics and Table 2 the results of the battery of cognitive tests. No patient had more than 5% missing cognitive data; the missing cognitive data were estimated using a multivariate imputation by chained equations in the mice package of R.

Table 1 Participant sociodemographical and clinical characteristics (n = 241)

Variable
Age, years: mean (s.d.) range 41 (11.3) 19–65
Educational level, years: mean (s.d.) range 14.5 (2.7) 7–22
Age at onset, years: mean (s.d.) range 25 (9.5) 12–60
Number of episodes, mean (s.d.) range
    Mixed   0.2 (0.7) 0–8
    Hypomanic   2.8 (5.3) 0–30
    Manic   1.4 (1.9) 0–10
    Major depressive   5.2 (5.4) 0–30
Scores, mean (s.d.) range
    Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (0–60)   4 (3.4) 0–10
    Young Mania Rating Scale (0–60)   1.1 (2) 0–10
    Functioning Assessment Short Test (0–72) 16.8 (13.2) 0–64
Gender, % male 40.2
Diagnosis, %
    Type I 55.6
    Type II 29.5
    Not otherwise specified 14.9
History of psychosis, % 43.6
Medication, %
    Antidepressant 26
    Lithium carbonate 25
    Anticonvulsant 32.7
    Antipsychotic 28.4

Table 2 Participant neuropsychological performance

Variable Percentile,
mean (s.d.)
Raw, mean
(s.d.)
Motor speed
Digit/symbol coding 43.1 (25.8) 70.2 (15.2)
Symbol search 56.2 (28) 33.7 (7.5)
Attention
Continuous Performance Test
    Omissions a 43.1 (27.9) 2.2 (4.6)
    Commissions a 53.6 (30.1) 30.8 (20.2)
    Reaction time b 52 (32.6) 402.5 (84.2)
Trail Making Test c
    Part A 49.6 (25.7) 35 (13)
    Part B 45.5 (28.3) 104.1 (15.9)
Executive functions
Stroop test
    Word 45.8 (22.8) 104.1 (15.9)
    Colour 37.4 (25.2) 73 (12.9)
    Colour/word 47 (29.8) 43.1 (11.1)
Verbal fluency
    Phonemic 48.2 (31.3) 24.1 (7.3)
    Semantic 39.9 (27.9) 31.7 (8)
Verbal memory
California verbal Learning Test
    Immediate recall 46.8 (32.5) 56.4 (9.9)
    Short delay free recall 46.9 (30) 11.9 (2.9)
    Long delay free recall 44.8 (30.3) 12.2 (2.7)
    Total recognition 52.8 (26.3) 15.1 (1.2)
Working memory
Spatial span
    Forward 46.1 (26.5) 8.3 (1.9)
    Backward 43.9 (26) 7.4 (1.8)
Digit span
    Total 43.6 (25.9)
    Forward 9.4 (2)
    Backward 6.4 (2.3)
Intellectual functioning
Matrix reasoning 45.8 (29) 18.3 (4.5)
Vocabulary 61.9 (28) 42.9 (10.2)

a. Raw scores are percentages.

b. Raw scores are in ms.

c. Raw scores are in s.

PCA

The KMO measure for the overall cognitive data-set was 0.85 and Bartlett's test of sphericity was significant (χ2(231) = 2295, P < 0.001), both indicating good factorability of the cognitive data. Horn's parallel analysis showed that five components should be retained, as their adjusted eigenvalue was above 1 (online Table DS1). The five-component structure explained 61% of the variance (online Table DS2). All component loadings were greater than 0.4, and all communalities were higher than 0.3 (Table 3). The first component consisted of all measures of the CVLT and was designated ‘verbal memory’. The second component bundled TMT, CPT omissions, symbol search, vocabulary, symbol coding and semantic verbal fluency and was designated ‘speed of processing and verbal knowledge’. The third component included all measures of spatial and digit spans, with matrix reasoning, and was designated ‘working memory and problem-solving’. The fourth component consisted of all measures of the Stroop test, with phonemic verbal fluency, and was designated ‘verbal fluency and inhibition’. The final component included CPT reaction time and commission and was designated ‘visual sustained attention’.

Table 3 Component loadings and communalities for the cognitive variables

Component loadings
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 Communality
CVLT, long delay free recall   0.94   0.03 −0.06   0.04   0.02 0.88
CVLT, short delay free recall   0.92   0.03 −0.03   0.01   0.01 0.85
CVLT, immediate recall   0.84   0.02   0.05   0.09 −0.05 0.78
CVLT, total recognition   0.73 −0.15   0.06 −0.23   0.07 0.55
TMT, B −0.13 −0.69 −0.16   0.03 −0.08 0.66
CPT, omissions   0.26 −0.65   0.17   0.04   0.02 0.37
Symbol search   0.10   0.65   0.06   0.17 −0.10 0.62
Vocabulary −0.03   0.62   0.04   0   0.25 0.44
Symbol coding   0.17   0.61   0.05   0.21 −0.15 0.65
TMT A −0.07 −0.52 −0.20 −0.03   0.16 0.46
Verbal fluency semantic   0.03   0.46   0.13   0.24   0 0.41
Spatial span backward −0.02   0.16   0.74 −0.09 −0.03 0.61
Spatial span forward   0.01   0.04   0.73 −0.02 −0.06 0.55
Digit span backward   0.08 −0.04   0.69   0.21   0.06 0.62
Digit span forward −0.08 −0.14   0.65   0.35   0.17 0.57
Matrix reasoning   0.18   0.30   0.56 −0.35   0.01 0.62
Stroop, word −0.04   0.08   0.01   0.77   0.02 0.63
Stroop, colour   0.13   0.08   0.10   0.69 −0.06 0.62
Stroop, word/colour   0.11   0.09   0.18   0.56 −0.16 0.50
Verbal fluency phonemic   0.09   0.27 −0.18   0.41   0.34 0.39
CPT, reaction time −0.03 −0.13   0 −0.09   0.86 0.78
CPT, commissions −0.07 −0.14 −0.05 −0.06 −0.83 0.74

CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; TMT, Trail Making Test; CPT, Conners' Continuous Performance Test II.

Path and mediation analyses

Online Table DS3 reports the zero-order correlations between the variables included in the model. The path analysis model allowed correlations between the residual variances of all cognitive components, except ‘visual sustained attention’, which was not correlated with any other cognitive components.

The path analysis model is shown in Fig. 1. We represent neither covariances between cognitive components, nor the regressions on covariates, to enhance readability. Path analysis requires at least 15 participants for each variable. Reference Bentler and Chou35 We included ten variables in the model and therefore required at least 150 participants. There were 0.8% missing data, which were handled using the full information maximum likelihood estimation. The four patterns of missingness are reported in online Table DS4 and the covariance coverage matrix of missing data in online Table DS5.

Fig. 1 Mediation model.

Rectangles represent the observed measured variables. Arrows showing the free regression weight are drawn between variables, values are the standardised path coefficients. The squared multiple correlation (R) value for the dependent variable appears in the upper right corner of each rectangle. Covariates and covariation between the cognitive components are not drawn to increase readability but were included in the model. MADRS, Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale; FAST, Functioning Assessment Short Test. ***P>0.001.

The model provided a good fit for the data, as suggested by the non-significant chi-square goodness-of-fit statistic (χ2(4) = 7.5, P = 0.113), a CFI of 0.989 (thus greater than 0.95), an RMSEA of 0.06 (which was not significantly greater than 0.05, as the P-value of the one-sided test of the null hypothesis that RMSEA was greater than 0.05 was equal to 0.329), and SRMR of 0.017 (thus lower than 0.08).

The model explained 30% of the variance in functioning. Altogether, the analysis revealed the following relationships between the variables (Fig. 1): a significant positive association between MADRS and FAST, a significant negative association between ‘verbal memory’ cognitive component and FAST, and a significant negative association between the ‘verbal fluency and inhibition’ cognitive component and FAST. We found no other significant associations, and in particular, MADRS was not significantly associated with any cognitive component, thus showing that cognitive component scores did not mediate the relationship between MADRS and FAST. Estimated standardised path coefficients for all variables included in the path analysis model (with the covariates) and residual correlation coefficients between cognitive components are reported in online Table DS6.

Discussion

Main findings and comparison with other studies

Here, we first used a PCA to identify the underlying architecture of cognitive processing in patients with bipolar disorder who were euthymic. We then used a path analysis to evaluate whether cognition mediated the relationship between residual depressive symptoms and functioning. We found five underlying components involved in cognition in euthymic bipolar disorder. Two components were derived from individual variables that were relatively homogeneous and specific regarding modality: the ‘verbal memory’ component was derived only from CVLT measures and the ‘visual sustained attention’ only from CPT measures. The remaining three components were more heterogeneous. The ‘verbal fluency and inhibition’ component consisted of verbal responses sometimes involving inhibition (phonemic verbal fluency and colour–word condition of the Stroop test) and sometimes not (colour and word condition of the Stroop test). The ‘speed of processing and verbal knowledge’ component contained a combination of visuospatial and verbal variables. The ‘working memory and problem-solving’ component bundled non-verbal reasoning and working memory measures. Greater variability within cognitive components has already been reported in bipolar disorder and was interpreted as a decrease in the differentiation of previously discrete cognitive processes through a decline in neural connectivity. Reference Gallagher, Gray and Watson8 The number of extracted cognitive components is similar to that of previous studies that found five Reference Schretlen, Pena, Aretouli, Orue, Cascella and Pearlson6 or six underlying dimensions in cognition for bipolar disorder. Reference Czobor, Jaeger, Berns, Gonzalez and Loftus7 The labels used in these previous studies to describe the cognitive dimensions were similar to those applied in the current study, consisting of verbal memory, speed of processing, working memory, executive functions, verbal knowledge and attention dimensions. This suggests a relative stability and reliability of the method we used to uncover the underlying cognitive components in bipolar disorder.

There was a negative association between residual depressive symptoms and functioning: individuals with more pronounced depressive symptoms had poorer social functioning, which was not explained by age, gender, lower education or poorer cognition. This finding is in accordance with previous cross-sectional Reference Aparicio, Santos, Jimenez-Lopez, Bagney, Rodriguez-Jimenez and Sanchez-Morla13,Reference del Mar Bonnin, González-Pinto, Solé, Reinares, González-Ortega and Alberich17,Reference Bowie, Depp, McGrath, Wolyniec, Mausbach and Thornquist18,Reference Bas, Poyraz, Bas, Poyraz and Tosun34 and longitudinal studies, Reference Bonnin, Martinez-Aran, Torrent, Pacchiarotti, Rosa and Franco9,Reference Martino, Marengo, Igoa, Scapola, Ais and Perinot14 showing that subclinical depressive symptoms in bipolar disorders are the main predictors of poor functional outcome, particularly work functioning. Reference Burdick, Goldberg and Harrow36 The present study included only patients who were euthymic, based on stringent criteria: the mean score for depression was very low but similar to previous studies exploring social functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder. Reference Bonnin, Martinez-Aran, Torrent, Pacchiarotti, Rosa and Franco9,Reference Aparicio, Santos, Jimenez-Lopez, Bagney, Rodriguez-Jimenez and Sanchez-Morla13,Reference del Mar Bonnin, González-Pinto, Solé, Reinares, González-Ortega and Alberich17,Reference Bas, Poyraz, Bas, Poyraz and Tosun34 However, the data show that functional impairment may be associated with even very low residual depressive symptoms, measured with a scale that was not specifically designed to assess subsyndromal depressive symptoms.

Among the five cognitive components found in this study, only two were positively associated with functioning: ‘verbal memory’ and ‘verbal fluency and inhibition’. Patients with better verbal memory, verbal fluency and inhibition also had a better social functioning. These results are consistent with several prior reports indicating that verbal memory Reference Martino, Marengo, Igoa, Scapola, Ais and Perinot14 and inhibitory control (Stroop colour–word test) Reference Reinares, Papachristou, Harvey, Mar Bonnin, Sanchez-Moreno and Torrent37 were more highly associated with functioning than other cognitive functions. That functioning was more highly related to residual depressive symptoms than cognition could be explained by the auto-evaluation method we used to measure everyday functioning. Self-reported measures of social functioning may be influenced less by objective cognitive performance and more by depressive symptomatology, because of pessimistic subjective appraisal of oneself and one's environment. This influence might be particularly important for low FAST scores (better functioning), like in our sample, where the distribution of FAST scores was skewed on the right. In contrast, the reverse may be true for performance-based measures of functioning and real-world functional milestones in bipolar disorder, which may be influenced more by objective cognitive performance and less by depression. Reference Depp, Mausbach, Harmell, savla, Bowie and Harvey5,Reference Allen, Bello and Thaler12

No associations between residual depressive symptoms and cognitive components were significant, showing that cognition does not mediate the relationship between subclinical residual depressive symptoms and functioning. This result is in accordance with a previous study reporting that perceived cognitive impairment and subclinical residual depressive symptoms are two independent sources of variation in the functioning of individuals with bipolar disorder. Reference Samalin, Boyer, Murru, Pacchiarotti, Reinares and Bonnin38 However, this mediation might occur for higher levels of depressive symptoms, as the impact of depressive symptoms on cognition varied according to the clinical response after treatment. Reference Gorwood, Corruble, Falissard and Goodwin39

Our model explained only 30% of the variance in functioning, supporting a role for other factors that were not measured here. Previous studies have suggested that functioning may be impaired when sleep is persistently disrupted, Reference Boland, Stange, Molz Adams, LaBelle, Ong and Hamilton40 when social cognition is impaired, Reference Aparicio, Santos, Jimenez-Lopez, Bagney, Rodriguez-Jimenez and Sanchez-Morla13 and when episode density is high. Reference Reinares, Papachristou, Harvey, Mar Bonnin, Sanchez-Moreno and Torrent37 The mean level of functioning in participants recruited in this study corresponds to moderate functional difficulties. Reference Sanchez-Moreno, Martinez-Aran and Vieta3 It is in the range (from 6 to 29) of those found in studies exploring the relationship between cognition and functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder. Reference Aparicio, Santos, Jimenez-Lopez, Bagney, Rodriguez-Jimenez and Sanchez-Morla13,Reference Bas, Poyraz, Bas, Poyraz and Tosun34,Reference Samalin, Boyer, Murru, Pacchiarotti, Reinares and Bonnin38 This consistency supports the general applicability of our findings to patients with euthymic bipolar disorder, provided the same assessment tools are used.

Limitations

Limitations of our study include the cross-sectional design, which precludes the assessment of causality and the direction of potential causal links. Another limitation is the lack of inclusion of social cognitive tasks in the assessment. We did not compare the cognitive architecture found in participants with bipolar disorder with a control group. Finally, the sample size was not large enough to test a more complex model that includes other variables of the illness (type of bipolar disorder, number of previous episodes, age at onset and history of psychosis) and medication. The time since the last mood episode might also be an important factor lacking in the present study. These variables might have also had an effect on functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder.

Implications

Our findings have important implications for future clinical studies. Individuals with bipolar disorder who respond to treatment may nevertheless continue to experience residual depressive and cognitive symptoms, leading to difficulties in functioning. First, it seems crucial to improving the assessment and the characterisation of residual depressive symptoms, with, for example, specific scales. Treatment approaches should possibly include cognitive performance improvement and full residual depressive symptom remission as important targets to obtain functional recovery in bipolar disorder. The optimal method to treat residual depressive symptoms is not clear, but they may be targeted by the use of mood stabilisers effective in the treatment of depressive polarity. Evidence for the efficacy of pharmacological and psychological interventions that target cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder is still preliminary, despite promising avenues such as functional remediation. Reference Bonnin, Reinares, Martinez-Aran, Balanza-Martinez, Sole and Torrent41 Among cognitive dimensions for cognitive remediation in euthymic bipolar disorder, our data suggest that verbal memory and fluency, and inhibition may be choice targets.

In summary, this study provides further evidence that cognitive impairments in specific dimensions are a core feature of bipolar disorder. This study also suggests that cognition is a separable dimension from depressive symptoms that persist during the inter-episodic period of bipolar disorder. Verbal memory and fluency and Stroop test performance were particularly associated with functioning in our participants with euthymic bipolar disorder and should be assessed in future studies focusing on functional outcome in bipolar disorder.

Funding

This work was supported by the Centre Hospitalier de Versailles, Foundation FondaMental, Créteil, France, and by the Investissements d'Avenir Programs managed by the ANR under references and .

Acknowledgements

We thank the Centre Hospitalier de Versailles and William Hempel of Alex Edelman 8t Associates for editorial assistance.

Appendix

List of FACE-BD collaborators

FACE-BD Clinical Coordinating Center (FondaMental Foundation): B. Etain, C. Henry and M. Leboyer.

FACE-BD Data Coordinating Center (FondaMental Foundation): O. Godin, H. Laouamri and N. Ngo-Nguyen.

FACE-BD clinical sites and principal collaborators in France: AP-HP, Hôpital H. Mondor – A. Chenevier, Pôle de Psychiatrie, Créteil – C. Daban, S. Hotier, S. Lauer, A. Leduc, A. Pelletier and A. Raust; AP-HP, GH Saint-Louis–Lariboisière–Fernand Widal, Pôle Neurosciences, Paris – F. Bellivier, M. Carminati, B. Etain, J. P. Lépine, I Nieto, P. Seguin and S. Sportiche; Hôpital C. Perrens, Centre Expert Trouble Bipolaire, Service de Psychiatrie Adulte, Pôle 3-4-7, Bordeaux – B. Antoniol, A. Desage, S. Gard, A. Jutant, K. Mbailara, I. Minois and L Zanouy; Département d'Urgence et Post Urgence Psychiatrique, CHRU Montpellier, Montpellier – S. Bonnet, F. Coppola, P. Courtet, D. Ducasse, M. Gachet, L. Matos, F. Molière, B. Noisette, E. Olié and G. Tarquini; Département de Psychiatrie, Hôpital Sainte Marguerite, Marseille – J. M. Azorin, R. Belzeaux and N. Corréard; Service de Psychiatrie et Psychologie Clinique, CHU de Nancy, Hôpitaux de Brabois, Vandoeuvre Les Nancy – R. Cohen, J.P. Kahn, P. Kieffer and O. Wajsbrot-Elgrabli; Clinique Universitaire de Psychiatrie, CHU de Grenoble, Grenoble – T. Bougerol, M. A. De Pourtales, B. Fredembach, S. Garçon, Alexandre Perrin and M. Polosan; Centre Hospitalier de Versailles, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie d'adultes, Le Chesnay – A.M. Galliot, I. Grévin, M. C. Hardy-Bayle, A.S. Cannavo, N. Kayser, C. Passerieux and P. Roux; Service de Psychiatrie, Centre Hospitalier Princesse Grace, Monaco – L. Albertini, V. Aubin, E. Beetz, I. Cussac, J. Loftus and I. Medecin.

Footnotes

*

See the Appendix for the list of collaborators.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Rosa, AR, Franco, C, Martinez-Aran, A, Sanchez-Moreno, J, Reinares, M, Salamero, M, et al. Functional impairment in patients with remitted bipolar disorder. Psychother Psychosom 2008; 77: 390–2.Google Scholar
2 Merikangas, KR, Jin, R, He, JP, Kessler, RC, Lee, S, Sampson, NA, et al. Prevalence and correlates of bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental health survey initiative. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68: 241–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Sanchez-Moreno, J, Martinez-Aran, A, Vieta, E. Treatment of functional impairment in patients with bipolar disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2017; 19:3.Google Scholar
4 Martinez-Aran, A, Vieta, E, Torrent, C, Sanchez-Moreno, J, Goikolea, JM, Salamero, M, et al. Functional outcome in bipolar disorder: the role of clinical and cognitive factors. Bipolar Disord 2007; 9: 103–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Depp, CA, Mausbach, BT, Harmell, AL, savla, GN, Bowie, CR, Harvey, PD, et al. Meta-analysis of the association between cognitive abilities and everyday functioning in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord 2012; 14: 217–26.Google Scholar
6 Schretlen, DJ, Pena, J, Aretouli, E, Orue, I, Cascella, NG, Pearlson, GD, et al. Confirmatory factor analysis reveals a latent cognitive structure common to bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and normal controls. Bipolar Disord 2013; 15: 422–33.Google Scholar
7 Czobor, P, Jaeger, J, Berns, SM, Gonzalez, C, Loftus, S. Neuropsychological symptom dimensions in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Bipolar Disord 2007; 9: 7192.Google Scholar
8 Gallagher, P, Gray, JM, Watson, S. Young AH, Ferrier IN. Neurocognitive functioning in bipolar depression: a component structure analysis. Psychol Med 2014; 44: 961–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Bonnin, CM, Martinez-Aran, A, Torrent, C, Pacchiarotti, I, Rosa, AR, Franco, C, et al. Clinical and neurocognitive predictors of functional outcome in bipolar euthymic patients: a long-term, follow-up study. J Affect Disord 2010; 121: 156–60.Google Scholar
10 Dias, W, Brissos, S, Frey, BN, Kapczinski, F. Insight, quality of life and cognitive functioning in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 2008; 110: 7583.Google Scholar
11 Belzeaux, R, Correard, N, Boyer, L, Etain, B, Loftus, J, Bellivier, F, et al. Depressive residual symptoms are associated with lower adherence to medication in bipolar patients without substance use disorder: results from the FACE-BD cohort. J Affect Disord 2013; 151: 1009–15.Google Scholar
12 Allen, DN, Bello, DT, Thaler, NS. Neurocognitive predictors of performance-based functional capacity in bipolar disorder. J Neuropsychol 2015; 9: 159–71.Google Scholar
13 Aparicio, A, Santos, JL, Jimenez-Lopez, E, Bagney, A, Rodriguez-Jimenez, R, Sanchez-Morla, EM. Emotion processing and psychosocial functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2017; 135: 339–50.Google Scholar
14 Martino, DJ, Marengo, E, Igoa, A, Scapola, M, Ais, ED, Perinot, L, et al. Neurocognitive and symptomatic predictors of functional outcome in bipolar disorders: a prospective 1 year follow-up study. J Affect Disord 2009; 116: 3742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Bourne, C, Bilderbeck, A, Drennan, R, Atkinson, L, Price, J, Geddes, JR, et al. Verbal learning impairment in euthymic bipolar disorder: BDI v BDII. J Affect Disord 2015; 182: 95100.Google Scholar
16 Bourne, C, Aydemir, O, Balanza-Martinez, V, Bora, E, Brissos, S, Cavanagh, JT, et al. Neuropsychological testing of cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar disorder: an individual patient data meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr scand 2013; 128: 149–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17 del Mar Bonnin, C, González-Pinto, A, Solé, B, Reinares, M, González-Ortega, I, Alberich, S, et al. Verbal memory as a mediator in the relationship between subthreshold depressive symptoms and functional outcome in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 2014; 160: 50–4.Google Scholar
18 Bowie, CR, Depp, C, McGrath, JA, Wolyniec, P, Mausbach, BT, Thornquist, MH, et al. Prediction of real-world functional disability in chronic mental disorders: a comparison of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167: 1116–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn, revised) (DSM-IV-TR). APA, 2000.Google Scholar
20 Montgomery, SA, Asberg, M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry 1979; 134: 382–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21 Young, RC, Biggs, JT, Ziegler, VE, Meyer, DA. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry 1978; 133: 429–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22 Rosa, AR, Sanchez-Moreno, J, Martinez-Aran, A, Salamero, M, Torrent, C, Reinares, M, et al. Validity and reliability of the Functioning Assessment Short Test (FAST) in bipolar disorder. Clin Pract Epidemiol Merit Health 2007; 3: 5.Google Scholar
23 Yatham, LN, Torres, U, Malhi, GS, Frangou, S, Glahn, DC, Bearden, CE, et al. The Iternational Society for Bipolar Disorders-Battery for Assessment of Neurocognition (ISBD-BANC). Bipolar Disord 2010; 12: 351–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24 Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (3rd edn) (WAIS-III). Psychological Corporation, 1997.Google Scholar
25 Conners, CK, Staff, M. Conners' Continuous Performance Test II. Multi-Health Systems Inc. 2000.Google Scholar
26 Reitan, RM. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills 1958; 8: 271–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
27 Lezak, MD. Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford University Press, 2004.Google Scholar
28 Delis, D, Kramer, J, Kaplan, E, Ober, B. CVLT-II California Verbal Learning Test Manual Adult Version. The Psychological Corporation, 2000.Google Scholar
29 Roux, P, Raust, A, Cannavo, AS, Aubin, V, Aouizerate, B, Azorin, JM, et al. Cognitive profiles in euthymic patients with bipolar disorders: results from the FACE-BD cohort. Bipolar Disord 2017; 19: 146–53.Google Scholar
30 Dziuban, CD, Shirkey, EC. When is a correlation matrix appropriate for factor analysis? Some decision rules. Psychol Bull 1974; 81: 358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
31 Bartlett, MS. The effect of standardization on a 2 approximation in factor analysis. Biometrika 1951; 38: 337–44.Google Scholar
32 Horn, JL. A Rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika 1965; 30: 179–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33 Bryant, FB, Yarnold, PR. Principal-components analysis and exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. In Reading and Understanding Multivariate Statistics (eds In Grimm, LG and Yarnold, PR): 99136). American Psychological Association, 1995.Google Scholar
34 Bas, TO, Poyraz, CA, Bas, A, Poyraz, BC, Tosun, M. The impact of cognitive impairment, neurological soft signs and subdepressive symptoms on functional outcome in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 2015; 174: 336–41.Google Scholar
35 Bentler, PM, Chou, C-P. Practical issues in structural modeling, Sociol Methods Res 1987; 16: 78117.Google Scholar
36 Burdick, KE, Goldberg, JF, Harrow, M. Neurocognitive dysfunction and psychosocial outcome in patients with bipolar I disorder at 15-year follow-up. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2010; 122: 499506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37 Reinares, M, Papachristou, E, Harvey, P, Mar Bonnin, C, Sanchez-Moreno, J, Torrent, C, et al. Towards a clinical staging for bipolar disorder: defining patient subtypes based on functional outcome. J Affect Disord 2013; 144: 6571.Google Scholar
38 Samalin, L, Boyer, L, Murru, A, Pacchiarotti, I, Reinares, M, Bonnin, CM, et al. Residual depressive symptoms, sleep disturbance and perceived cognitive impairment as determinants of functioning in patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 2017; 210: 280–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39 Gorwood, P, Corruble, E, Falissard, B, Goodwin, GM. Toxic effects of depression on brain function: impairment of delayed recall and the cumulative length of depressive disorder in a large sample of depressed outpatients. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165: 731–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
40 Boland, EM, Stange, JP, Molz Adams, A, LaBelle, DR, Ong, ML, Hamilton, JL, et al. Associations between sleep disturbance, cognitive functioning and work disability in bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res 2015; 230: 567–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41 Bonnin, CM, Reinares, M, Martinez-Aran, A, Balanza-Martinez, V, Sole, B, Torrent, C, et al. Effects of functional remediation on neurocognitively impaired bipolar patients: enhancement of verbal memory. Psychol Med 2016; 46: 291301.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Participant sociodemographical and clinical characteristics (n = 241)

Figure 1

Table 2 Participant neuropsychological performance

Figure 2

Table 3 Component loadings and communalities for the cognitive variables

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Mediation model.Rectangles represent the observed measured variables. Arrows showing the free regression weight are drawn between variables, values are the standardised path coefficients. The squared multiple correlation (R) value for the dependent variable appears in the upper right corner of each rectangle. Covariates and covariation between the cognitive components are not drawn to increase readability but were included in the model. MADRS, Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale; FAST, Functioning Assessment Short Test. ***P>0.001.

Supplementary material: PDF

Roux et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Roux et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.6 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.