Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T16:48:10.900Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Contribution of birth weight to mental health, cognitive and socioeconomic outcomes: two-sample Mendelian randomisation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2021

Massimiliano Orri*
Affiliation:
McGill Group for Suicide Studies, Douglas Mental Health Research Institute, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, Canada; and Bordeaux Population Health Research Centre, Inserm U1219, University of Bordeaux, France
Jean-Baptiste Pingault
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London; and Social Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, King's College London, UK
Gustavo Turecki
Affiliation:
McGill Group for Suicide Studies, Douglas Mental Health Research Institute, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
Anne-Monique Nuyt
Affiliation:
Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Sainte-Justine Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, University of Montreal, Canada
Richard E. Tremblay
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics and Psychology, University of Montreal, Canada; and School of Public Health, University College Dublin, Ireland
Sylvana M. Côté
Affiliation:
Bordeaux Population Health Research Centre, Inserm U1219, University of Bordeaux, France; and Department of Social and Preventive Medicine, School of Public Health, University of Montreal, Canada
Marie-Claude Geoffroy
Affiliation:
McGill Group for Suicide Studies, Douglas Mental Health Research Institute, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal; and Department of Education and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
*
Correspondence: Massimiliano Orri. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Low birth weight is associated with adult mental health, cognitive and socioeconomic problems. However, the causal nature of these associations remains difficult to establish owing to confounding.

Aims

To estimate the contribution of birth weight to adult mental health, cognitive and socioeconomic outcomes using two-sample Mendelian randomisation, an instrumental variable approach strengthening causal inference.

Method

We used 48 independent single-nucleotide polymorphisms as genetic instruments for birth weight (genome-wide association studies’ total sample: n = 264 498) and considered mental health (attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder, bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), schizophrenia, suicide attempt), cognitive (intelligence) and socioeconomic (educational attainment, income, social deprivation) outcomes.

Results

We found evidence for a contribution of birth weight to ADHD (OR for 1 s.d. unit decrease (~464 g) in birth weight, 1.29; 95% CI 1.03–1.62), PTSD (OR = 1.69; 95% CI 1.06–2.71) and suicide attempt (OR = 1.39; 95% CI 1.05–1.84), as well as for intelligence (β = −0.07; 95% CI −0.13 to −0.02) and socioeconomic outcomes, i.e. educational attainment (β = −0.05; 95% CI −0.09 to −0.01), income (β = −0.08; 95% CI −0.15 to −0.02) and social deprivation (β = 0.08; 95% CI 0.03–0.13). However, no evidence was found for a contribution of birth weight to the other examined mental health outcomes. Results were consistent across a wide range of sensitivity analyses.

Conclusions

These findings support the hypothesis that birth weight could be an important element on the causal pathway to mental health, cognitive and socioeconomic outcomes.

Type
Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

Low birth weight (a global index of poor fetal development) has been associated with a range of mental health problems (including attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism, bipolar disorder, depression, schizophrenia and suicide),Reference Abel, Wicks, Susser, Dalman, Pedersen and Mortensen1Reference Orri, Gunnell, Richard-Devantoy, Bolanis, Boruff and Turecki8 as well lower intelligence and socioeconomic statusReference Strauss9Reference Flensborg-Madsen and Mortensen11 (see also the Introduction in the supplementary material available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2021.15).These findings are consistent with the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) hypothesis,Reference O'Donnell and Meaney12,Reference Schlotz and Phillips13 which states that adverse in utero and perinatal experiences may have long-lasting effects on adult health. Yet, the causal nature of these associations remains unclear. Birth weight is influenced by a range of intrauterine exposures and maternal conditions and behaviours, such as mental health and diet, exposure to tobacco and alcohol, toxins, pollution and socioeconomic adversity.Reference Thompson, Levitt and Stanwood14Reference Orri, Russell, Mars, Turecki, Gunnell and Heron20 Those factors are likely to confound the association between birth weight and mental health and socioeconomic outcomes, because such confounding factors may cause a change in both birth weight and outcomes. Clarifying whether birth weight is a causal risk factor for mental, cognitive and socioeconomic problems is important for understanding their aetiology. Given that it is not possible to directly randomise birth weight to probe its causal role on later outcomes, the most robust evidence would come from quasi-experimental designs. Mendelian randomisation is a methodology that strengthens causal inference on the association between an exposure and an outcome using genetic variants as instruments.Reference Burgess, Butterworth and Thompson21Reference Pingault, O'Reilly, Schoeler, Ploubidis, Rijsdijk and Dudbridge23 Genetic variants are randomly allocated at conception and are relatively independent of environmental confounding factors; therefore this design mimics that of a randomised trial in which treatment is randomly allocated and confounding factors do not depend on treatment allocation (Fig. 1; see supplementary material Methods for details on Mendelian randomisation assumptions).Reference Burgess, Butterworth and Thompson21,Reference Davies, Holmes and Davey Smith22 A previous study that used Mendelian randomisation to investigate the role of birth weight in ADHD, major depressive disorder and schizophrenia found no evidence for a causal role of birth weight.Reference Arafat and Minică24 However, a major limitation of that study was the inability to account for the confounding effect of maternal genotype, which can lead to incorrect Mendelian randomisation estimates.Reference Warrington, Freathy, Neale and Evans25,Reference Warrington, Beaumont, Horikoshi, Day, Helgeland and Laurin26 Maternal and individual (i.e. offspring) genotypes are correlated and any effect of intrauterine exposures or maternal behaviour influenced by the mother's genetic make up may also result in an association between the offspring's genotype and mental health outcomes (Fig. 1). However, new data from a recently published genome-wide association study (GWAS) of birth weightReference Warrington, Beaumont, Horikoshi, Day, Helgeland and Laurin26 providing estimates of the association of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) with birth weight after adjustment for the correlation between maternal and individual genotypes enable us, for the first time, to overcome this limitation. The present Mendelian randomisation study relies on summary statistics from the largest available GWASs to estimate the contribution of birth weight to mental health (including common psychiatric disorders and suicide attempt), cognitive (i.e. intelligence) and socioeconomic outcomes (including educational attainment, income and social deprivation).

Fig. 1 Confounding effect of maternal genotype on the association between an individual's genotype and birth weight.

Using the Mendelian randomisation design, it is possible to estimate the association between an individual's birth weight and an outcome (path d in the figure) using the individual's genotype associated with birth weight as the instrumental variable (path b), instead of the observational assessment of birth weight. The association estimated in this way is not confounded by factors (such as maternal substance use) that may confound the association between birth weight and outcome in observational studies. However, this design alone does not take into account the confounding effect of maternal genotype. Indeed, both the individual's genotype (path a) and maternal genotype (path b) have influences on birth weight, the former directly, the latter through the intrauterine environment. Because of the correlation between the individual's genotype and their mother's genotype (r ~ 0.5; path c), the effect of the individual's phenotype on their birth weight may be confounded. To avoid this bias, we used estimates of the association between individuals’ genetic variants adjusted for the correlated maternal effect as instruments (published in the most recent birth-weight GWAS).Reference Warrington, Beaumont, Horikoshi, Day, Helgeland and Laurin26

Method

Data sources

This study relied on summary statistics from GWASs performed by international consortia (Table 1). Only GWASs of individuals of European ancestry were used, as genetic variants can be differently associated with a trait in different ancestry groups owing to specific linkage disequilibrium structures.Reference Gage, Munafò and Davey Smith27 All the GWASs had been adjusted for population stratification using ancestry-informed principal components, as well as for other main covariates (e.g. age and gender; see details in cited publications). All phenotypes were primarily measured among adult individuals and summary statistics were available only for both genders combined. We used the largest available non-overlapping exposure and outcome GWASs whenever possible, i.e. for all outcomes except for ADHD, intelligence and socioeconomic outcomes. However, this overlap is unlikely to bias the results (supplementary Methods). Power analysis is presented in the online material (supplementary Methods).

Table 1 Summary of genome-wide association studies used in the analyses

GWAS, genome-wide association study; SNP, single-nucleotide polymorphism; ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; EGG, Early Growth Genetics consortium; UKB, UK Biobank; PGC, Psychiatric Genomics Consortium; iPSYCH, Lundbeck Foundation Initiative for Integrative Psychiatric Research; EAGLE, Early Genetics and Lifecourse Epidemiology Consortium; IOCDF-GC, International Obsessive Compulsive Disorder Foundation Genetics Collaborative; OCG-AS, OCD Collaborative Genetics Association Studies; SSGAC, Social Science Genetic Association Consortium.

a. The Townsend Deprivation Index is a measure of material deprivation incorporating information on unemployment, non-car ownership, non-home ownership and household overcrowding (higher values indicate higher social deprivation).

Birth weight

In total, n = 209 independent genome-wide significant SNPs associated with birth weight were identified by the largest GWAS meta-analysis conducted by the Early Growth Genetics (EGG) consortium and including the UK Biobank sample (n = 264 498).Reference Warrington, Beaumont, Horikoshi, Day, Helgeland and Laurin26 Among these GWAS significant variants, we selected 48 SNPs identified as having an effect on birth weight after adjusting for the correlated maternal effect on birth weight,Reference Warrington, Freathy, Neale and Evans25 and maintaining statistical significance at P < 1 × 10−5. The mean F-statistic for these SNPs was 36 (median, 28; range, 19–182; supplementary Methods), suggesting that all SNPs were strong instruments according to the suggested threshold of F > 10.Reference Burgess and Thompson28 Birth weight (which had a mean of ~3407 g and standard deviation of ~464 g) was z-score transformed separately for males and females in the studies participating in the GWAS meta-analysis and adjusted for study-specific covariates, including gestational duration (where available).

Outcomes

We obtained the estimates of associations between the birth weight instrument SNPs and our outcomes from the GWAS summary statistics. Whenever possible, instrument SNPs that were unavailable in the GWAS summary statistics of the outcome phenotype were replaced with overlapping proxy SNPs in linkage disequilibrium (r 2 > 0.80) identified using the LDproxy online tool (https://ldlink.nci.nih.gov/). The following outcomes were considered: (a) mental health outcomes (all binary variables): ADHD,Reference Demontis, Walters, Martin, Mattheisen, Als and Agerbo29 autism spectrum disorder,Reference Grove, Ripke, Als, Mattheisen, Walters and Won30 bipolar disorder,Reference Stahl, Breen, Forstner, McQuillin, Ripke and Trubetskoy31 major depressive disorder,Reference Wray, Ripke, Mattheisen, Trzaskowski, Byrne and Abdellaoui32 obsessive–compulsive disorder,33 post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD),Reference Duncan, Ratanatharathorn, Aiello, Almli, Amstadter and Ashley-Koch34 schizophreniaReference Pardiñas, Holmans, Pocklington, Escott-Price, Ripke and Carrera35 and suicide attempt (i.e. hospital admission for a suicide attempt);Reference Erlangsen, Appadurai, Wang, Turecki, Mors and Werge36 (b) cognitive outcome: intelligence (measured as the general factor of intelligence (g) and primarily evaluating fluid domains of cognitive functioning);Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois and de Leeuw37 (c) socioeconomic outcomes: educational attainment (measured as years of education),Reference Lee, Wedow, Okbay, Kong, Maghzian and Zacher38 household income (measured as total income before taxes using five income categories)Reference Hill, Hagenaars, Marioni, Harris, Liewald and Davies39 and social deprivation (measured using the Townsend Social Deprivation Index).Reference Hill, Hagenaars, Marioni, Harris, Liewald and Davies39 Details on phenotype assessment can be found in the individual publications.

Data analysis

We conducted a two-sample Mendelian randomisation analysis in R version 3.6 for Mac40 using the TwoSampleMR,Reference Hemani, Zheng, Elsworth, Wade, Haberland and Baird41 MendelianRandomizationReference Yavorska and Burgess42 and MRPRESSO packages. In two-sample Mendelian randomisation, causal estimates can be obtained using summary statistics from different samples (i.e. GWASs), one for the instrument/SNP-exposure association, another for the instrument/SNP-outcome association. The two data-sets were harmonised and the positive strand alleles were inferred using allele frequencies for palindromes (minor allele frequency up to 0.4) whenever possible. Analyses including and excluding the remaining palindromic SNPs were conducted, yielding consistent results. Therefore, we reported results using the full set of SNPs. For each SNP, the ratio between the SNP-exposure and the SNP-outcome association (Wald test) was calculated. Then, Wald estimates for single SNPs were combined using random-effect inverse-variance weighting (IVW) meta-analysis as the primary analysis. This method corresponds to a weighted regression of SNP-outcome effects on SNP-exposure effects, in which weights were based on a multiplicative random-effects model. Heterogeneity across the meta-analysed estimates, which may be indicative of horizontal pleiotropy (i.e. the fact that the same SNPs influence multiple traits, so the association between instrument SNPs and outcome could not be entirely explained by the exposure, but act through alternative pathways, violating instrumental variable assumptions)Reference Davies, Holmes and Davey Smith22 was quantified using the Q-statistic (a significant test suggests pleiotropy).

A range of analyses were used to test the sensitivity of the IVW estimation. First, Mendelian randomisation-Egger (MR-Egger) regression,Reference Bowden, Davey Smith and Burgess43 which relaxes the assumptions of Mendelian randomisation allowing for unbalanced pleiotropic effects. A major drawback of MR-Egger is the low power of this test; however, consistency in the direction and the size of the effect between the MR-Egger estimate and the IVW estimate can support the validity of the IVW analysis. We also used the intercept of the MR-Egger regression to test for the presence of unbalanced pleiotropy (a significant test suggests unbalanced pleiotropy). Second, we used the weighted median, which assumes that at least 50% of the total weight of the instrument comes from valid variants. It is more likely to give a valid causal estimate than the MR-Egger or the IVW method because it is more consistent with the true causal effect in the presence of up to 50% invalid variants. Third, we used the robust adjusted profile score (RAPS),Reference Zhao, Wang, Hemani, Bowden and Small44 which is an estimator that deals with weak instruments and is robust to pleiotropic effects.

We then performed four further analyses. First, MR-PRESSO (Mendelian Randomisation Pleiotropy Residual Sum and Outlier)Reference Verbanck, Chen, Neale and Do45 was used to detect and correct for outliers that may reflect bias due to pleiotropy. Second, leave-one-out analyses, in which the analyses were repeated by excluding one SNP instrument at a time, were performed to identify whether a single SNP was driving the association. Outlier SNPs were excluded from the analysis. Third, we searched the PhenoScanner database (a curated database of publicly available results from large-scale genetic association studies) for each SNP instrument (and those in linkage disequilibrium within r 2 ≥ 0.80) to see whether they have been associated (P < 1 × 10−5) with traits likely to bias our analysis because of horizontal pleiotropy or because of their association with confounders of the exposure-outcome association. In that case, these SNPs would be excluded in sensitivity analyses. Fourth, we conducted a Steiger filtering analysis to investigate whether the specified direction of the association (birth weight predicting mental health, cognitive and social outcomes) is further supported.

Associations were considered statistically significant at P < 0.05. Additionally, to account for the possibility of false-positive findings, we used the false discovery rate with a q-value <0.05.

Ethical approval

This study is based on publicly available summary statistics from studies that had already obtained ethical approval; therefore, a separate ethical approval was not required.

Results

Contribution of birth weight to mental health outcomes

We found evidence for a contribution of birth weight to ADHD, with an OR of 1.29 (95% CI 1.03–1.62; P = 0.027; q < 0.05) per 1 s.d. unit decrease in birth weight (Fig. 2). No evidence of horizontal pleiotropy was detected (MR-Egger intercept, P = 0.653; supplementary Table 4), but the Q-statistic indicated the presence of significant heterogeneity (P = 0.002). However, the association was consistent across the Mendelian randomisation methods used as sensitivity analyses (MR-RAPS OR = 1.27; 95% CI 1.01–1.61; P = 0.045; weighted median OR = 1.34; 95% CI 1.00–1.81; P = 0.054; MR-Egger OR = 2.11; 95% CI 1.31–3.34; P = 0.001) and the MR-PRESSO and leave-one-out procedures did not detect any outlier. Similarly, we found evidence for a contribution of birth weight to PTSD (OR = 1.69; 95% CI 1.06–2.71; P = 0.029; q < 0.05), with consistent estimates across sensitivity analysis methods (MR-RAPS OR = 1.71; 95% CI 1.02–2.88; P = 0.044; weighted median OR = 2.09; 95% CI 0.98–4.44; P = 0.056; MR-Egger OR = 3.00; 95% CI 0.96–9.38; P = 0.050) and no evidence for heterogeneity (Q-statistic, P = 0.481), unbalanced horizontal pleiotropy (MR-Egger intercept, P = 0.957) and outliers influencing the results. We found no evidence supporting a contribution of birth weight to other psychiatric disorders, including autism spectrum disorder (OR = 1.03; 95% CI 0.85–1.24; P = 0.792), bipolar disorder (OR = 0.93; 95% CI 0.77–1.13; P = 0.476), major depressive disorder (OR = 1.00; 95% CI 0.90–1.12; P = 0.988), obsessive–compulsive disorder (OR = 0.72; 95% CI 0.45–1.16; P = 0.175) and schizophrenia (OR = 1.08; 95% CI 0.91–1.28; P = 0.386). No unbalanced horizontal pleiotropy was detected for these outcomes; correcting for outlier SNPs detected for schizophrenia (rs1547669 and rs222857) did not alter the results. Furthermore, we found evidence supporting a contribution of birth weight to suicide attempt (OR = 1.39; 95% CI 1.05–1.84; P = 0.023; q < 0.05). Consistent results were found in sensitivity analyses (MR-RAPS OR = 1.50; 95% CI 1.11–2.02; P = 0.008; weighted median OR = 1.82; 95% CI 1.21–2.76; P = 0.004; MR-Egger OR = 1.34; 95% CI 0.56–3.23; P = 0.247) and we did not find evidence for heterogeneity (Q-statistic, P = 0.590), unbalanced horizontal pleiotropy (MR-Egger intercept, P = 0.172) and outliers.

Fig. 2 Mendelian randomisation estimates for the association of birth weight with mental health. ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; MR-Egger, Mendelian randomisation-Egger regression; RAPS, robust adjusted profile score; q, q-value from the false discovery rate.

Contribution of birth weight to intelligence

We found evidence for a contribution of birth weight to intelligence (β = −0.07; 95% CI −0.13 to −0.02; P = 0.010; q = 0.001; Fig. 3) after exclusion of one outlier SNP (rs1482852; supplementary Results). This result remained after correction for an additional outlier SNP detected by the MR-PRESSO procedure (rs4144829; β = −0.05; 95% CI −0.11 to −0.01; P = 0.036). We did not find evidence for unbalanced horizontal pleiotropy (MR-Egger intercept, P = 0.123), although there was significant heterogeneity according to the Q-statistic (P < 0.001).

Fig. 3 Mendelian randomisation estimates for the association of birth weight with intelligence and socioeconomic outcomes. MR-Egger, Mendelian randomisation-Egger regression; RAPS, robust adjusted profile score; q, q-value from the false discovery rate.

Contribution of birth weight to socioeconomic outcomes

We found evidence for a contribution of birth weight to educational attainment (β = −0.05; 95% CI −0.09 to −0.01; P = 0.011; q = 0.039), income (β = −0.08; 95% CI −0.15 to −0.02; P = 0.013; q = 0.039) and social deprivation (β = 0.08; 95% CI 0.03–0.13; P = 0.001; q = 0.006) (Fig. 3). MR-PRESSO detected outlier SNPs only for educational attainment (rs112139215, rs1129156, rs11698914, rs222857, rs4144829, rs7402983, rs7968682, rs8756), but outlier correction did not alter the results (β = −0.08; 95% CI −0.08 to −0.02; P = 0.005). Educational attainment showed significant heterogeneity (Q-statistic, P < 0.001). For income, we found evidence of both significant heterogeneity (Q-statistic, P = 0.011) and unbalanced pleiotropy (MR-Egger intercept, P = 0.024), but all sensitivity analyses yielded consistent results (weighted median: β = −0.09, 95% CI −0.17 to −0.00; P = 0.041; MR-Egger: β = −0.11; 95% CI −0.25 to 0.04; P = 0.139; MR-RAPS, β = −0.08; 95% CI −0.15 to −0.02; P = 0.015).

Additional sensitivity analyses

Searching the PhenoScanner database for each SNP instrument revealed associations between these SNPs and other anthropometric (e.g. height), metabolic (e.g. basal metabolism), hypertensive (e.g. blood pressure) and lipoprotein (e.g. high-density lipoproteins) traits. It is unlikely that those traits could generate bias by violating instrumental variable assumptions. Steiger filtering analyses suggested that the genetic variants used were indeed instruments for the exposure rather than for the outcomes (supplementary Results).

Discussion

Using a genetically informed instrumental variable approach to strengthen causal inference, this study investigated the contribution of birth weight to common psychiatric disorders, suicide attempt, and cognitive and socioeconomic outcomes. We found evidence supporting a role of birth weight in the pathway leading to ADHD, PTSD, suicide attempt, intelligence and socioeconomic outcomes (i.e. educational attainment, income and social deprivation), but not to the other examined mental health outcomes.

This study relied on a robust two-sample Mendelian randomisation design, the largest available GWAS summary statistics and multiple genetic instruments indexing birth weight. These features allowed our analyses to be well powered and to limit weak instrument bias.Reference Burgess and Thompson28 Furthermore, an innovative methodological feature is the use of genetic instruments adjusted for the correlated effect of maternal genotype. This approach has been previously applied to cardiometabolic outcomesReference Warrington, Beaumont, Horikoshi, Day, Helgeland and Laurin26 but, to our knowledge, this is the first study relying on adjusted estimates to investigate the association of birth weight with mental health, cognitive and socioeconomic outcomes. As recently shown,Reference Warrington, Freathy, Neale and Evans25,Reference Warrington, Beaumont, Horikoshi, Day, Helgeland and Laurin26 failure to account for this confounding effect may create bias in the causal estimates.

Previous observational,Reference Breslau46,Reference Galéra, Côté, Bouvard, Pingault, Melchior and Michel47 within-siblingReference Pettersson, Larsson, D'Onofrio, Almqvist and Lichtenstein7 and twinReference Lim, Liu, Schoeler, Cecil, Barker and Viding48 studies suggested an association between low birth weight and ADHD. Consistently, our results also suggest a potential causal role of birth weight in the aetiology of ADHD.Reference Pettersson, Larsson, D'Onofrio, Almqvist and Lichtenstein7,Reference Lim, Liu, Schoeler, Cecil, Barker and Viding48 Both ADHD and autism spectrum disorder are neurodevelopmental disorders with childhood onset and both had been associated with low birth weight.Reference Pettersson, Larsson, D'Onofrio, Almqvist and Lichtenstein7 However, our study found evidence for potentially causal contribution of birth weight only to ADHD, suggesting that the contribution of birth weight might be specific to ADHD rather than common to neurodevelopmental disorders. This suggestion deserves further investigations, especially in light of a recent genetically informed (within-sibling) study showing associations with both ADHD and autism, as well as with a common neurodevelopmental latent factor.Reference Pettersson, Larsson, D'Onofrio, Almqvist and Lichtenstein7 Future GWASs of autism, with larger sample size, will also provide the opportunity to re-test the association between birth weight and autism with a more powered analysis.

We found evidence supporting a potential causal role of birth weight on suicide attempt, consistent with a recent meta-analysisReference Orri, Gunnell, Richard-Devantoy, Bolanis, Boruff and Turecki8 but not with a within-sibling Swedish study,Reference Class, Rickert, Larsson, Lichtenstein and D'Onofrio49 which failed to find an association of birth weight with suicide attempt in early adulthood. Differences between the studies’ populations (including age at suicide attempt assessment) and statistical power may explain these divergences. It is worth noting that we did not find evidence for a contribution of birth weight to depression, the psychiatric disorder that most strongly relates to suicide.Reference Turecki and Brent50 As suicide risk is the result of both specific factors and factors shared with major psychiatric disorders comorbid with suicide,Reference Gunnell51 our finding points to birth weight as a factor causally contributing to suicide risk beyond factors also associated with depression. To further probe the role of birth weight in the aetiology of suicide, our finding needs to be replicated using suicide mortality, rather than suicide attempt, as an outcome. This will be possible when large-scale GWASs for suicide mortality become available.

Similarly, the documented association between birth weight and PTSD was in line with observational evidence on stress-related disorders,Reference Kajantie52 but not with a within-sibling study.Reference Pettersson, Larsson, D'Onofrio, Almqvist and Lichtenstein7 However, the literature on this association is scarce and additional studies are needed.

Our study could not support the contribution of birth weight to other psychiatric disorders, including depression, bipolar disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder and schizophrenia. These findings, in line with those of other quasi-experimental studies,Reference Pettersson, Larsson, D'Onofrio, Almqvist and Lichtenstein7 are important, especially considering that available observational evidence was either contradictory (e.g. for depression)Reference Loret de Mola, de França, de Quevedo and Horta5,Reference Wojcik, Lee, Colman, Hardy and Hotopf53 or suggested associations (e.g. for schizophrenia).Reference Abel, Wicks, Susser, Dalman, Pedersen and Mortensen1

It is important to note that our study does not support a widespread contribution of birth weight to the general risk of psychopathology (i.e. the P-factor), but rather specific contributions to ADHD, PTSD and suicide attempt risk. However, future Mendelian randomisation investigations designed to specifically address this hypothesis may be informative to clarify the potential contribution of birth weight to common versus specific psychopathology factors. This effort may be facilitated by reliance on continuously measured outcomes (i.e. considering liability to psychopathology as a continuum) rather than on dichotomous outcomes as in the present study.

Inconsistent observational evidence was also available for the association of birth weight with socioeconomic outcomes, with some studies showing adult negative outcomes for low birth-weight children compared with normal birth-weight children but others showing no differences.Reference Strauss9,Reference Jelenkovic, Mikkonen, Martikainen, Latvala, Yokoyama and Sund10 Our findings across various socioeconomic indices are consistent with a causal role of birth weight.

Finally, in line with observational studies showing lifelong negative cognitive consequences for children born with very low birth weight,Reference Flensborg-Madsen and Mortensen11 this study found evidence supporting the hypothesis that the contribution of birth weight to intelligence may be causal. Additionally, as previous studies mainly focused on children with very low birth weight, our findings add to the literature by replicating these results in a sample of children with birth weight mostly within the normal range. Taken together, available evidence on the association between birth weight and cognitive outcomes suggests that compensation effects of cognitive abilities for children born with low birth weight would not be able to fully counteract the negative effects of low birth weight.Reference Bergman and Siegal54

Implications

Future studies should attempt to clarify the putative causal mechanisms explaining the associations that we found. It has been suggested that restricted fetal growth has a negative impact on brain developmentReference Walhovd, Fjell, Brown, Kuperman, Chung and Hagler55 and that this might be a mechanism explaining part of the association between birth weight and mental health and socioeconomic outcomes. For example, a study found alterations in the brain's reactive system and white matter in very low birth-weight children, which was associated with lowered fluid intelligence and heightened anxiety.Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Brown, Kuperman, Chung and Hagler55 Future studies using quasi-experimental designs should be conducted to establish whether brain development lays on the causal path between birth weight and psychosocial outcomes, as well as to identify the brain regions implicated, which may differ across outcomes. Similarly, environmental mechanisms should be identified, as they might be potential targets for interventions aiming to promote mental and socioeconomic well-being among low birth-weight children.

Limitations

First, the phenotypes considered in this study rely on the definitions and samples used in the original GWASs, which are often highly heterogeneous regarding the recruited population, the definition of the phenotype and the assessment. Although this heterogeneity results from the need to use very large samples to identify small genetic effects, it may also influence our findings. However, studies such as those conducted in independent samples using polygenic scores derived from these GWASs seem to corroborate the validly of their phenotypes. Second, owing to data availability, this study is limited to individuals of European ancestry. Third, because a large proportion of individuals included in the birth-weight GWASs had a birth weight within the normal range, the results of our analyses might not reflect the effect of extremely low/high birth weight on mental health, cognitive and social outcomes. Additionally, our analyses assume a linear relation between birth weight and outcomes.Reference Warrington, Beaumont, Horikoshi, Day, Helgeland and Laurin26,Reference Class, Rickert, Larsson, Lichtenstein and D'Onofrio49 Fourth, we could not explore potential gender differences in the association between birth weight and mental health, as gender-specific GWAS summary statistics were not available. Fifth, although we conducted a large array of sensitivity analyses showing the robustness of our findings, horizontal pleiotropy cannot be completely ruled out, as the biological action of most included SNPs is not fully understood yet. Sixth, most of the reported associations only concerned adults and they may differ during other developmental periods. Seventh, although our analyses took into account the correlated role of maternal genotype, residual confounding dynastic effects cannot be excluded, including those related to paternal effects.Reference Pingault, O'Reilly, Schoeler, Ploubidis, Rijsdijk and Dudbridge23 Future studies including both maternal and paternal genotype, as well as studies based on within-family GWASs (currently not largely available but necessary to go beyond the assumptions of between-family Mendelian randomisation designs)Reference Davey Smith, Holmes, Davies and Ebrahim56 are needed to corroborate our results.Reference Davies, Howe, Brumpton, Havdahl, Evans and Davey Smith57

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2021.15.

Data availability

This study is based on publicly available summary statistics.

Author contributions

M.O. designed the study, performed data analyses, interpreted the data and drafted the manuscript. J.-B.P. contributed to the study design, data analysis, data interpretation and writing of the final manuscript. All authors contributed to data interpretation and writing of the manuscript.

Funding

This project has received funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement no. 793396.

Declaration of interest

J.-B.P. is a fellow of MQ: Transforming Mental Health (MQ16IP16). G.T. holds a Canada Research Chair (Tier 1) and a NARSAD Distinguished Investigator Award and is supported by grants from the Canadian Institute of Health Research (CIHR) (FDN148374 and EGM141899). M.-C.G. and G.T. are supported by the Fonds de recherche du Québec – Santé (FRQS) through the Quebec Network on Suicide, Mood Disorders and Related Disorders. M.-C.G. holds a Canada Research Chair (Tier 2), receives funding from the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention and is a fellow of the Fonds de Recherche du Québec. Funders have no role in the study design, data analysis, interpretation of the data or writing of the manuscript.

References

Abel, KM, Wicks, S, Susser, ES, Dalman, C, Pedersen, MG, Mortensen, PB, et al. Birth weight, schizophrenia and adult mental disorder: is risk confined to the smallest babies? Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67: 923–30.10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franz, AP, Bolat, GU, Bolat, H, Matijasevich, A, Santos, IS, Silveira, RC, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and very preterm/very low birth weight: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2018; 141: e20171645.10.1542/peds.2017-1645CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnell, D, Rasmussen, F, Fouskakis, D, Tynelius, P, Harrison, G. Patterns of fetal and childhood growth and the development of psychosis in young males: a cohort study. Am J Epidemiol 2003; 158: 291300.10.1093/aje/kwg118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lampi, KM, Lehtonen, L, Tran, PL, Suominen, A, Lehti, V, Banerjee, PN, et al. Risk of autism spectrum disorders in low birth weight and small for gestational age infants. J Pediatr 2012; 161: 830–6.10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.04.058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loret de Mola, C, de França, GVA, de Quevedo, LA, Horta, BL. Low birth weight, preterm birth and small for gestational age association with adult depression: systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2014; 205: 340–7.10.1192/bjp.bp.113.139014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosarti, C, Reichenberg, A, Murray, RM, Cnattingius, S, Lambe, MP, Yin, L, et al. Preterm birth and psychiatric disorders in young adult life. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69: 610–7.10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1374CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettersson, E, Larsson, H, D'Onofrio, B, Almqvist, C, Lichtenstein, P. Association of fetal growth with general and specific mental health conditions. JAMA Psychiatry 2019; 76: 536–43.10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.4342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orri, M, Gunnell, D, Richard-Devantoy, S, Bolanis, D, Boruff, J, Turecki, G, et al. In-utero and perinatal influences on suicide risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2019; 6: 477–92.10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30077-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, RS. Adult functional outcome of those born small for gestational age: twenty-six-year follow-up of the 1970 British Birth Cohort. JAMA 2000; 283: 625–32.10.1001/jama.283.5.625CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jelenkovic, A, Mikkonen, J, Martikainen, P, Latvala, A, Yokoyama, Y, Sund, R, et al. Association between birth weight and educational attainment: an individual-based pooled analysis of nine twin cohorts. J Epidemiol Community Health 2018; 72: 832–7.10.1136/jech-2017-210403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flensborg-Madsen, T, Mortensen, EL. Birth weight and intelligence in young adulthood and midlife. Pediatrics 2017; 139: e20163161.10.1542/peds.2016-3161CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Donnell, KJ, Meaney, MJ. Fetal origins of mental health: the developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry 2017; 174: 319–28.10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16020138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlotz, W, Phillips, DIW. Fetal origins of mental health: evidence and mechanisms. Brain Behav Immun 2009; 23: 905–16.10.1016/j.bbi.2009.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, BL, Levitt, P, Stanwood, GD. Prenatal exposure to drugs: effects on brain development and implications for policy and education. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10: 303–12.10.1038/nrn2598CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, AJ, Austin, E, Galbally, M. Prenatal maternal mental health and fetal growth restriction: a systematic review. J Dev Orig Health Dis 2016; 7: 416–28.10.1017/S2040174416000076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aizer, A, Currie, J. The intergenerational transmission of inequality: maternal disadvantage and health at birth. Science 2014; 344: 856–61.10.1126/science.1251872CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dessì, A, Corona, L, Pintus, R, Fanos, V. Exposure to tobacco smoke and low birth weight: from epidemiology to metabolomics. Expert Rev Proteomics 2018; 15: 647–56.10.1080/14789450.2018.1505508CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanphear, BP, Vorhees, CV, Bellinger, DC. Protecting children from environmental toxins. PLoS Med 2005; 2: e61.10.1371/journal.pmed.0020061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Englund-Ögge, L, Brantsæter, AL, Juodakis, J, Haugen, M, Meltzer, HM, Jacobsson, B, et al. Associations between maternal dietary patterns and infant birth weight, small and large for gestational age in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. Eur J Clin Nutr 2019; 73: 1270–82.10.1038/s41430-018-0356-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orri, M, Russell, A, Mars, B, Turecki, G, Gunnell, D, Heron, J, et al. Perinatal adversity profiles and suicide attempt in adolescence and young adulthood: longitudinal analyses from two 20-year birth cohorts. Psychol Med [Epub ahead of print] 6 Oct 2020. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720002974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, S, Butterworth, A, Thompson, SG. Mendelian randomization analysis with multiple genetic variants using summarized data. Genet Epidemiol 2013; 37: 658–65.10.1002/gepi.21758CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, NM, Holmes, MV, Davey Smith, G. Reading Mendelian randomisation studies: a guide, glossary and checklist for clinicians. BMJ 2018; 362: k601.10.1136/bmj.k601CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pingault, J-B, O'Reilly, PF, Schoeler, T, Ploubidis, GB, Rijsdijk, F, Dudbridge, F. Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nat Rev Genet 2018; 19: 566–80.10.1038/s41576-018-0020-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arafat, S, Minică, CC. Fetal origins of mental disorders? An answer based on Mendelian randomization. Twin Res Hum Genet 2018; 21: 485–94.10.1017/thg.2018.65CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, NM, Freathy, RM, Neale, MC, Evans, DM. Using structural equation modelling to jointly estimate maternal and fetal effects on birthweight in the UK Biobank. Int J Epidemiol 2018; 47: 1229–41.10.1093/ije/dyy015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, NM, Beaumont, RN, Horikoshi, M, Day, FR, Helgeland, Ø, Laurin, C, et al. Maternal and fetal genetic effects on birth weight and their relevance to cardio-metabolic risk factors. Nat Genet 2019; 51: 804.10.1038/s41588-019-0403-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gage, SH, Munafò, MR, Davey Smith, G. Causal inference in developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) Research. Annu Rev Psychol 2016; 67: 567–85.10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, S, Thompson, SG. Avoiding bias from weak instruments in Mendelian randomization studies. Int J Epidemiol 2011; 40: 755–64.10.1093/ije/dyr036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demontis, D, Walters, RK, Martin, J, Mattheisen, M, Als, TD, Agerbo, E, et al. Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat Genet 2019; 51: 6375.10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grove, J, Ripke, S, Als, TD, Mattheisen, M, Walters, RK, Won, H, et al. Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat Genet 2019; 51: 431–44.10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stahl, EA, Breen, G, Forstner, AJ, McQuillin, A, Ripke, S, Trubetskoy, V, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 30 loci associated with bipolar disorder. Nat Genet 2019; 51: 793803.10.1038/s41588-019-0397-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, NR, Ripke, S, Mattheisen, M, Trzaskowski, M, Byrne, EM, Abdellaoui, A, et al. Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nat Genet 2018; 50: 668–81.10.1038/s41588-018-0090-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
International Obsessive Compulsive Disorder Foundation Genetics Collaborative (IOCDF-GC), OCD Collaborative Genetics Association Studies (OCGAS). Revealing the complex genetic architecture of obsessive-compulsive disorder using meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 2018; 23: 1181–8.10.1038/mp.2017.154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, LE, Ratanatharathorn, A, Aiello, AE, Almli, LM, Amstadter, AB, Ashley-Koch, AE, et al. Largest GWAS of PTSD (N =20 070) yields genetic overlap with schizophrenia and sex differences in heritability. Mol Psychiatry 2018; 23: 666–73.10.1038/mp.2017.77CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pardiñas, AF, Holmans, P, Pocklington, AJ, Escott-Price, V, Ripke, S, Carrera, N, et al. Common schizophrenia alleles are enriched in mutation-intolerant genes and in regions under strong background selection. Nat Genet 2018; 50: 381–9.10.1038/s41588-018-0059-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erlangsen, A, Appadurai, V, Wang, Y, Turecki, G, Mors, O, Werge, T, et al. Genetics of suicide attempts in individuals with and without mental disorders: a population-based genome-wide association study. Mol Psychiatry 2020; 25: 2410–21.10.1038/s41380-018-0218-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, JE, Jansen, PR, Stringer, S, Watanabe, K, Bryois, J, de Leeuw, CA, et al. Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nat Genet 2018; 50: 912–9.10.1038/s41588-018-0152-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, JJ, Wedow, R, Okbay, A, Kong, E, Maghzian, O, Zacher, M, et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat Genet 2018; 50: 1112–21.10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, WD, Hagenaars, SP, Marioni, RE, Harris, SE, Liewald, DCM, Davies, G, et al. Molecular genetic contributions to social deprivation and household income in UK Biobank. Curr Biol 2016; 26: 3083–9.10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2016 (https://www.R-project.org/).Google Scholar
Hemani, G, Zheng, J, Elsworth, B, Wade, KH, Haberland, V, Baird, D, et al. The MR-Base platform supports systematic causal inference across the human phenome. eLife 2018; 7: e34408.10.7554/eLife.34408CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yavorska, OO, Burgess, S. MendelianRandomization: an R package for performing Mendelian randomization analyses using summarized data. Int J Epidemiol 2017; 46: 1734–9.10.1093/ije/dyx034CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowden, J, Davey Smith, G, Burgess, S. Mendelian randomization with invalid instruments: effect estimation and bias detection through Egger regression. Int J Epidemiol 2015; 44: 512–25.10.1093/ije/dyv080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, Q, Wang, J, Hemani, G, Bowden, J, Small, DS. Statistical inference in two-sample summary-data Mendelian randomization using robust adjusted profile score. Ann Statist 2020; 48: 1742–69.10.1214/19-AOS1866CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verbanck, M, Chen, C-Y, Neale, B, Do, R. Detection of widespread horizontal pleiotropy in causal relationships inferred from Mendelian randomization between complex traits and diseases. Nat Genet 2018; 50: 693–8.10.1038/s41588-018-0099-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N. Psychiatric sequelae of low birth weight. Epidemiol Rev 1995; 17: 96106.10.1093/oxfordjournals.epirev.a036191CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galéra, C, Côté, SM, Bouvard, MP, Pingault, J-B, Melchior, M, Michel, G, et al. Early risk factors for hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories from age 17 months to 8 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68: 1267–75.10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, KX, Liu, C-Y, Schoeler, T, Cecil, CAM, Barker, ED, Viding, E, et al. The role of birth weight on the causal pathway to child and adolescent ADHD symptomatology: a population-based twin differences longitudinal design. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2018; 59: 1036–43.10.1111/jcpp.12949CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Class, QA, Rickert, ME, Larsson, H, Lichtenstein, P, D'Onofrio, BM. Fetal growth and psychiatric and socioeconomic problems: population-based sibling comparison. Br J Psychiatry 2014; 205: 355–61.10.1192/bjp.bp.113.143693CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turecki, G, Brent, DA. Suicide and suicidal behaviour. Lancet 2016; 387: 1227–39.10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00234-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnell, D. A population health perspective on suicide research and prevention. Crisis 2015; 36: 155–60.10.1027/0227-5910/a000317CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kajantie, E. Fetal origins of stress-related adult disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2006; 1083: 1127.10.1196/annals.1367.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wojcik, W, Lee, W, Colman, I, Hardy, R, Hotopf, M. Foetal origins of depression? A systematic review and meta-analysis of low birth weight and later depression. Psychol Med 2013; 43: 112.10.1017/S0033291712000682CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergman, A, Siegal, ML. Evolutionary capacitance as a general feature of complex gene networks. Nature 2003; 424: 549–52.10.1038/nature01765CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walhovd, KB, Fjell, AM, Brown, TT, Kuperman, JM, Chung, Y, Hagler, DJ, et al. Long-term influence of normal variation in neonatal characteristics on human brain development. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2012; 109: 20089–94.10.1073/pnas.1208180109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davey Smith, G, Holmes, MV, Davies, NM, Ebrahim, S. Mendel's laws, Mendelian randomization and causal inference in observational data: substantive and nomenclatural issues. Eur J Epidemiol 2020; 35: 99111.10.1007/s10654-020-00622-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, NM, Howe, LJ, Brumpton, B, Havdahl, A, Evans, DM, Davey Smith, G. Within family Mendelian randomization studies. Hum Mol Genet 2019; 28: R170–9.10.1093/hmg/ddz204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Confounding effect of maternal genotype on the association between an individual's genotype and birth weight.Using the Mendelian randomisation design, it is possible to estimate the association between an individual's birth weight and an outcome (path d in the figure) using the individual's genotype associated with birth weight as the instrumental variable (path b), instead of the observational assessment of birth weight. The association estimated in this way is not confounded by factors (such as maternal substance use) that may confound the association between birth weight and outcome in observational studies. However, this design alone does not take into account the confounding effect of maternal genotype. Indeed, both the individual's genotype (path a) and maternal genotype (path b) have influences on birth weight, the former directly, the latter through the intrauterine environment. Because of the correlation between the individual's genotype and their mother's genotype (r ~ 0.5; path c), the effect of the individual's phenotype on their birth weight may be confounded. To avoid this bias, we used estimates of the association between individuals’ genetic variants adjusted for the correlated maternal effect as instruments (published in the most recent birth-weight GWAS).26

Figure 1

Table 1 Summary of genome-wide association studies used in the analyses

Figure 2

Fig. 2 Mendelian randomisation estimates for the association of birth weight with mental health. ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; MR-Egger, Mendelian randomisation-Egger regression; RAPS, robust adjusted profile score; q, q-value from the false discovery rate.

Figure 3

Fig. 3 Mendelian randomisation estimates for the association of birth weight with intelligence and socioeconomic outcomes. MR-Egger, Mendelian randomisation-Egger regression; RAPS, robust adjusted profile score; q, q-value from the false discovery rate.

Supplementary material: PDF

Orri et al. supplementary material

Orri et al. supplementary material

Download Orri et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 2.8 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.