Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T12:27:23.876Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Author's reply

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Peter Tyrer*
Affiliation:
The Royal College of Psychiatrists, 17 Belgrave Square, London SW1X 8PG, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2008 

I share Dr Gangdev's concerns in part. The impact factor, an invention of Eugene Garfield, Reference Garfield1 is not a necessary part of science. It merely reflects our preoccupation with league tables in every part of life. Any senior professional, whether editor, headmaster, company director or hospital manager, likes to know exactly where their organisation stands with respect to others on at least an annual basis; this seems to be so much more important than non-numerical measures such as letters of appreciation or complaint. It therefore seems to have little relevance to readers of a learned journal, who are not the slightest bit interested in the level of inflation of the Editor's ego, but only in the content of papers published in the journal. There is now evidence that the impact factor does indeed provide a reasonable comparison of the relative quality of a journal; however, what it does not do, despite increasing claims to the contrary, is provide a valid ‘assessment of the quality of individual papers, scientists and departments’. Reference Opthof2 All that can be said about the publication of a paper in a high-quality journal is that the review process is likely to have been carried out with a higher degree of precision and care than that for an equivalent paper in a journal of very low impact factor; therefore, in general, the reader can have more confidence in the presentation of the findings. This is not to say they are necessarily more accurate or of greater scientific significance, although in the broadest terms, they probably are.

But the highly informed reader can select good papers from poor ones without the aid of the impact factor, and the preoccupation of the scientific community with its importance sometimes approaches the ludicrous, such as with the research assessment exercise (RAE) in the UK, which demands articles from high-impact-factor journals, among other measures, in comparing the relative value of scientists. How a nutritionist or a historian can be validly compared with a psychiatrist is, in my view, intrinsically meaningless. I have helped colleagues who have decided to leave academia for a less topsy-turvy land with a set of verses, also derived in part from Robert Burns, and which include the following (sung to the tune of Auld Lang Syne as they make their last journey down the university corridor);

Footnotes

No longer will I troubled be With targets to be won Flush RAE down the lavatory ‘Cos its impact factor's none.

Declaration of interest

P.T. is Editor of The British Journal of Psychiatry.

References

1 Garfield, E. Citation analysis as a tool in journal evaluation. Science 1972; 178: 471–9.Google ScholarPubMed
2 Opthof, T. Sense and nonsense about the impact factor. Cardiovasc Res 1997; 33: 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.