Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T04:24:17.920Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20-year neurocognitive development following a schizophrenia spectrum disorder and associations with symptom severity and functional outcomes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 February 2024

Marie Starzer*
Affiliation:
Copenhagen Research Center for Mental Health – CORE, Mental Health Center Copenhagen, Mental Health Services in the Capital Region, Copenhagen, Denmark Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
Helene Gjervig Hansen
Affiliation:
Copenhagen Research Center for Mental Health – CORE, Mental Health Center Copenhagen, Mental Health Services in the Capital Region, Copenhagen, Denmark Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
Carsten Hjorthøj
Affiliation:
Copenhagen Research Center for Mental Health – CORE, Mental Health Center Copenhagen, Mental Health Services in the Capital Region, Copenhagen, Denmark Department of Public Health, University of Copenhagen, Section of Epidemiology, Copenhagen, Denmark
Nikolai Albert
Affiliation:
Copenhagen Research Center for Mental Health – CORE, Mental Health Center Copenhagen, Mental Health Services in the Capital Region, Copenhagen, Denmark Mental Health Centre Amager, Mental Health Services in the Capital Region, Copenhagen, Denmark
Kathryn E. Lewandowski
Affiliation:
Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder Program, McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Louise Birkedal Glenthøj
Affiliation:
Copenhagen Research Center for Mental Health – CORE, Mental Health Center Copenhagen, Mental Health Services in the Capital Region, Copenhagen, Denmark Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
Merete Nordentoft
Affiliation:
Copenhagen Research Center for Mental Health – CORE, Mental Health Center Copenhagen, Mental Health Services in the Capital Region, Copenhagen, Denmark Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
*
Corresponding author: Marie Starzer; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Cognitive deficits are a core feature of schizophrenia and are closely associated with poor functional outcomes. It remains unclear if cognitive deficits progress over time or remain stable. Determining patients at increased risk of progressive worsening might help targeted neurocognitive remediation approaches.

Methods

This 20-year follow-up study examined neurocognitive outcomes of 156 participants from the OPUS I trial. Neurocognition was assessed using the brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia at the 10- and 20-year follow-up, allowing us to examine changes in neurocognition over ten years.

Results

We found that 30.5% of patients had a declining course of neurocognition, 49.2% had a stable course of neurocognition and 20.3% experienced improvements in neurocognition. Good cognitive functioning at the 20-year follow-up was significantly associated with higher levels of social functioning (B 6.86, CI 4.71–9.02, p < 0.001) while increasing experiential negative symptoms were significantly correlated to cognitive worsening (PC-0.231, p = 0.029). Younger age at inclusion (B: 0.23 per 10-years, CI 0.00–0.045, p = 0.047) and low level of education (below ten years) (mean difference: −0.346, CI −0.616 to −0.076, p = 0.012) predicted declining neurocognition.

Conclusion

Our findings support the notion of different schizophrenia subtypes with varying trajectories. Neurocognitive impairment at the 20-year follow-up was associated with other poor outcomes, highlighting the importance of treatments aimed at improving neurocognition in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Schizophrenia is an often long-lasting and debilitating illness with recovery rates ranging from 14 to 32 percent (Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Speyer, Starzer, Albert, Hjorthøj, Eplov and Nordentoft2023a; Jääskeläinen et al., Reference Jääskeläinen, Juola, Hirvonen, McGrath, Saha, Isohanni and Miettunen2013; Lally et al., Reference Lally, Ajnakina, Stubbs, Cullinane, Murphy, Gaughran and Murray2017). Apart from psychotic and negative symptoms, patients often also display significant cognitive impairments, which are associated with poor social and vocational function (Cowman et al., Reference Cowman, Holleran, Lonergan, O'Connor, Birchwood and Donohoe2021). Back in the 1960ties, the introduction of antipsychotic medication revolutionized the treatment of psychotic symptoms. Still, antipsychotics show minimal effects on improving cognitive deficits (Guilera, Pino, Gómez-Benito, & Rojo, Reference Guilera, Pino, Gómez-Benito and Rojo2009). Different psychosocial interventions have been developed to improve symptom levels and functioning (Bighelli et al., Reference Bighelli, Pitschel-Walz, Schneider-Thoma, Siafis, Wu, Wang and Leucht2021). These interventions show great effects whilst ongoing (Bertelsen et al., Reference Bertelsen, Jeppesen, Petersen, Thorup, Øhlenschlæger, Le Quach and Nordentoft2008). Still, long-term cognitive deficits, negative symptoms and low functioning levels remain a debilitating reality for many patients with schizophrenia (Halverson et al., Reference Halverson, Orleans-Pobee, Merritt, Sheeran, Fett and Penn2019; Silberstein & Harvey, Reference Silberstein and Harvey2019). This is well illustrated by the fact that recovery rates today do not differ greatly from those predating the neuroleptic era (Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Speyer, Starzer, Albert, Hjorthøj, Eplov and Nordentoft2023a). Cognitive remediation has emerged as a targeted intervention for cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Still, despite the established efficacy of cognitive remediation in improving cognitive and functional outcomes (Wykes, Huddy, Cellard, McGurk, & Czobor, Reference Wykes, Huddy, Cellard, McGurk and Czobor2011; Lejeune, Northrop, & Kurtz, Reference Lejeune, Northrop and Kurtz2021), its inclusion within established intervention programs for schizophrenia remains limited.

Two models pertaining to the basic nature of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia have been proposed (Bora & Murray, Reference Bora and Murray2014). In the neurodevelopmental model, cognitive impairments arise in the premorbid phase of the illness and around illness onset, after which cognition remains stable through the remaining course of the illness. In the neurodegenerative model, cognition continues to decline after illness onset more rapidly than in healthy individuals. In addition to these models, it has also been suggested that some patients may recover and return to premorbid cognitive functioning after some time (Hedman, van Haren, van Baal, Kahn, & Hulshoff Pol, Reference Hedman, van Haren, van Baal, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2013).

Previous research has illustrated that cognitive decline occurs in the prodromal phase of illness, before actual psychosis onset (Mollon & Reichenberg, Reference Mollon and Reichenberg2018; Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Deste, Smieskova, Barlati, Yung, Howes and Borgwardt2012). There is, however, conflicting evidence as to how further changes in cognition occur over time, and it is uncertain if cognitive function remains stable in the later phase of illness. Most studies conducted on patients with first-episode psychosis have short-term follow-ups ranging from 1–6 years and generally report a stable course of neurocognition (Bozikas & Andreou, Reference Bozikas and Andreou2011; Rajji, Miranda, & Mulsant, Reference Rajji, Miranda and Mulsant2014; Szöke et al., Reference Szöke, Trandafir, Dupont, Méary, Schürhoff and Leboyer2008). However, findings have been mixed with the continued decline (Barder et al., Reference Barder, Sundet, Rund, Evensen, Haahr, ten Velden Hegelstad and Friis2013; Habtewold et al., Reference Habtewold, Rodijk, Liemburg, Sidorenkov, Boezen, Bruggeman and Alizadeh2020; Pukrop et al., Reference Pukrop, Schultze-Lutter, Ruhrmann, Brockhaus-Dumke, Tendolkar, Bechdolf and Klosterkötter2006), and some showing improvement in some areas (Murillo-García et al., Reference Murillo-García, Ortíz-García de la Foz, Miguel-Corredera, Vázquez-Bourgon, Setién-Suero, Neergaard and Ayesa-Arriola2023). Long-term studies with more than ten years of follow-up are limited but do generally report a declining course of neurocognition in schizophrenia (Fett et al., Reference Fett, Velthorst, Reichenberg, Ruggero, Callahan, Fochtmann and Kotov2020; Jonas et al., Reference Jonas, Lian, Callahan, Ruggero, Clouston, Reichenberg and Kotov2022). Often, these studies suffer from small sample sizes (Fett, Reichenberg, & Velthorst, Reference Fett, Reichenberg and Velthorst2022; Tschentscher et al., Reference Tschentscher, Woll, Tafelmaier, Kriesche, Bucher, Engel and Karch2023). While indirect, findings from cross-sectional studies in schizophrenia patients at different phases of illness tend to report significantly greater cognitive impairment in patients who have been ill for many years, (Bilder et al., Reference Bilder, Lipschutz-broch, Reiter, Geisler, Mayerhoff and Lieberman1992; Dickinson, Ragland, Gold, & Gur, Reference Dickinson, Ragland, Gold and Gur2008; Seidman, Buka, Goldstein, & Tsuang, Reference Seidman, Buka, Goldstein and Tsuang2006), compared to those experiencing an initial episode (Bora & Pantelis, Reference Bora and Pantelis2015; Pantelis et al., Reference Pantelis, Wood, Proffitt, Testa, Mahony, Brewer and McGorry2009; Seidman et al., Reference Seidman, Giuliano, Meyer, Addington, Cadenhead, Cannon and Cornblatt2010), these cross-sectional studies might be flawed by selection bias. Some studies have tried to determine predictors of deteriorating or changing neurocognition, but results have been inconsistent (Ayesa-Arriola et al., Reference Ayesa-Arriola, Miguel-Corredera, De La Foz, Neergaard, Correa-Ghisays, Setién-Suero and Crespo-Facorro2023; Fett et al., Reference Fett, Velthorst, Reichenberg, Ruggero, Callahan, Fochtmann and Kotov2020; Hoff, Svetina, Shields, Stewart, & DeLisi, Reference Hoff, Svetina, Shields, Stewart and DeLisi2005; Luther et al., Reference Luther, Suor, Rosen, Jobe, Faull and Harrow2020; Rund et al., Reference Rund, Barder, Evensen, Haahr, Hegelstad, Joa and Friis2016; Zanelli et al., Reference Zanelli, Mollon, Sandin, Morgan, Dazzan, Pilecka and Reichenberg2019).

The current literature on long-term changes in neurocognition and its association with clinical and functional outcomes is scarce and predominantly comprises small study populations. This study aimed to extend the evidence on the long-term development of cognition in schizophrenia.

The aim of our study was to (1) determine changes in neurocognitive function over a ten-year period after initial diagnosis and to examine possible correlations with changes in symptom severity and social functioning, (2) to investigate 20-year cognitive outcomes and to determine associations with other long-term outcomes, and (3) to examine if specific baseline characteristics would predict changes in neurocognition over ten years.

Method

Study design and participants

This study reassessed participants from the OPUS I randomized clinical trial 20 years after the first inclusion. The original trial was conducted from 1998 until 2000 and included 578 participants with an incident schizophrenia spectrum diagnosis (ICD-10 classification: F20-F25, F28-F29). All participants had to be between the ages of 18 and 45 years and could not have received more than 12 weeks of consecutive anti-psychotic treatment before inclusion. Participants were randomized to specialized early intervention treatment (OPUS) comprised of assertive community treatment, family involvement and psychoeducation (n 275), prolonged rehabilitation (n 31), or treatment as usual (n 272) (Bertelsen et al., Reference Bertelsen, Jeppesen, Petersen, Thorup, Øhlenschlæger, Le Quach and Nordentoft2008; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Jeppesen, Thorup, Abel, Øhlenschlaeger, Christensen and Nordentoft2005a). After two years, all patients were reassessed, and participants receiving OPUS treatment were transferred to treatment as usual. All participants were subsequently invited to participate in 5-, 10- and 20-year follow-up reassessments and provided written informed consent before each follow-up. Participants were assessed using semi-structured interviews conducted by independent clinical staff blinded to the original treatment allocation. Regular interrater sessions were conducted to secure high interrater reliability. We included 174 in the 20-year reassessment; 113 were diagnosed with schizophrenia at baseline, and 67% of these patients also qualified for schizophrenia diagnosis assessed using SCAN at the 20-year follow-up. Thirty-one were diagnosed with schizotypal disorder at baseline, and 30 were diagnosed with other psychosis at baseline (F22 – F29); 56% of these patients qualified for a schizophrenia diagnosis at the 20-year follow-up. The main sample included in this paper comprised 156 participants who had completed the full cognitive assessment battery at the 20-year follow-up, while in total, 160 had completed parts of the cognitive assessment. For the analysis of change in cognition over time, we were able to include data on 129 participants who had completed the full cognitive test battery at both the 10- and 20-year follow-up, while a total of 140 participants had repeated measurements for any of the individual cognitive domains at both follow-up points.

Measures

Main outcome

The main outcome of this study was cognitive function. The brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia (BACS) was used to index neurocognitive functioning (Bora & Murray, Reference Bora and Murray2014; Keefe et al., Reference Keefe, Goldberg, Harvey, Gold, Poe and Coughenour2004) at the 10 and 20-year follow-up. BACS-certified clinicians administered the test. The BACS comprises six subtests and generates one global measure of cognitive functioning. Factor analyses have shown that BACS fits a three-factor model (Keefe et al., Reference Keefe, Goldberg, Harvey, Gold, Poe and Coughenour2004) indexing neurocognition in three subdomains: (1) Verbal learning and memory comprised of verbal memory (verbal memory task) and working memory (digit sequencing task). (2) Speed of processing (SP) comprised of verbal fluency (verbal fluency task), attention and information processing speed (symbol coding task), and motor speed (token motor task). (3) Reasoning and problem-solving comprised the executive functions assessment (Tower of London task). The three-factor model is illustrated in Fig. 1.

Figure 1. Neurocognitive subdomains - BACS three factor model (Keefe et al., Reference Keefe, Goldberg, Harvey, Gold, Poe and Coughenour2004).

Secondary outcome measures

We used the Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) and the Scale for Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) (Andreasen, Flaum, Swayze, Tyrrell, & Arndt, Reference Andreasen, Flaum, Swayze, Tyrrell and Arndt1990) to assess positive and negative symptoms. For both dimensions, we calculated composite scores ranging from 0–5. The ‘psychotic dimension’ was the mean score of the global ratings for hallucinations and delusions. The ‘negative dimension’ was the mean score of four global ratings of negative domains in SANS (Arndt, Andreasen, Flaum, Miller, & Nopoulos, Reference Arndt, Andreasen, Flaum, Miller and Nopoulos1995), excluding the attention domain. A symptom score above two on all global ratings of symptoms was to be clinically significant symptoms (Andreasen et al., Reference Andreasen, Carpenter, Kane, Lasser, Marder and Weinberger2005). We also used a two-factor model for negative symptoms, looking at experiential symptoms (comprised of anhedonia and avolition SANS scores) and expressive symptoms (comprised of affective flattening and alogia SANS scores) (Foussias & Remington, Reference Foussias and Remington2010; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Horan, Kirkpatrick, Fischer, Keller, Miski and Carpenter2013).

At the 20-year follow-up global level of functioning was assessed using the Global Assessment of Functioning scale (GAF-F) ranging from 0–100. Current psychiatric diagnoses and substance use disorders were assessed using the schedule for clinical assessment in neuropsychiatry (SCAN 2.1) (Schützwohl, Kallert, & Jurjanz, Reference Schützwohl, Kallert and Jurjanz2007; Wing, Sartorius, & Üstun, Reference Wing, Sartorius and Üstun1998). Information on a variety of clinical characteristics such as living conditions and housing situation, partnership situation and children, educational level, employment and workability, current psychiatric treatment, treatment with antipsychotics, suicidal ideations, quality of life, self-perceived symptoms and self-rated health were assessed in semi-structured interviews and using questionnaires (the WHO Quality of Life-BREF (WHOQoL-Bref) (The WHOQOL Group, 1996; von Knorring, Reference von Knorring2001), the EQ-5D (Brooks, Reference Brooks1996), the Danish Sundhedsprofilen).

At baseline, pre-morbid social and academic functioning was assessed using the Pre-morbid Adjustment Scale (Brill, Reichenberg, Weiser, & Rabinowitz, Reference Brill, Reichenberg, Weiser and Rabinowitz2008) and duration of untreated psychosis using the Interview for Retrospective Assessment of Onset of Schizophrenia (IRAOS) (Häfner et al., Reference Häfner, Riecher-Rössler, Hambrecht, Maurer, Meissner, Schmidtke and van der Heiden1992).

At the 10- and 20-year follow-up, social functioning was measured using the Personal and Social Performance Scale (PSP) (Tegeler & Juckel, Reference Tegeler and Juckel2007). This scale measures four domains of social functioning: useful activities, personal relationships, self-care, aggressive and disturbing behavior – combined and assessed on a scale ranging from 0–100.

Predictors of neurocognitive change

To establish stronger evidence for an association between baseline clinical characteristics and changes in cognitive function over time, we wanted to cross-validate predictors of cognitive outcomes already identified in other studies. The choice of baseline predictors of cognitive development was based on a systematic literature search of cross-sectional and longitudinal predictors of cognition in FEP identified in previous studies. The search strategy and selection process are displayed in Appendix A. Variables associated with long-term neurocognition were: (1) sex, (2) age, (3) age at illness onset, (4) employment status, (4) level of education (above/below ten years) (5) premorbid academic functioning and (6) schizophrenia diagnosis, (7) early specialized intervention treatment (8) cannabis cessation after the 10-year assessment, and (9) stable symptom remission within the first year after diagnosis.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using SPSS version 25.

The raw scores of all neurocognitive subdomains were normally distributed, except for the Tower of London score assessing reasoning and problem-solving. Because of the skewness in this domain, z-scores lower than three standard deviations below the mean were recoded into −3. For cross-sectional analysis at the 20-year follow-up, we calculated Z-scores for all neurocognitive domains using the mean and s.d. of the sample (N = 156) at 20 years. We calculated 10- and 20-year Z-scores for longitudinal analysis using the mean and s.d. from the 10-year follow-up. Z-scores for all functions were combined into one composite z-score for BACS, termed the global neurocognitive score. Z-scores from the verbal fluency task, the symbol coding task and the token motor task were combined into a subdomain Z-score for speed of processing and Z-scores from the verbal memory task, and the digit sequencing task was combined into a Z-score for verbal learning and memory. To determine if 20-year-neurocognitive scores were associated with other 20-year outcomes, we used linear regression analysis for numerical outcomes and binary logistic regression for categorical variables.

Longitudinal analyses

We used paired t tests to assess significant differences in participants levels of neurocognition and symptoms from 10 to 20 years. Change scores were calculated for all neurocognitive domains by subtracting the 10-year from the 20-year Z-scores. Pearson correlation analyses were used to examine correlations between neurocognitive change-scores and change-scores of symptom dimensions and social functioning from the 10- to 20-year follow-up.

To determine change-scores we subtracted the 10-year Z-scores from the 20-year Z-scores. We used univariable and multivariable ANCOVA analysis to assess statistically significant predictors of neurocognitive change scores. All tests were two-tailed. Significant variables from the univariable analysis (significance set at p = 0.1) were included in a multivariable model using a backward elimination approach and set significance level at p = 0.05. We also divided participants into three groups based on these change scores, one group representing patients with improving neurocognition with change scores >0.5 s.d., one group representing participants with stable neurocognition with changes scores between 0.5; and −0.5 s.d., and one group representing patients with declining neurocognition with change scores <-0.5 s.d.

Results

Drop-out analysis

At the 20-year follow-up, we reassessed 174 participants, and 156 completed the full neurocognitive test battery. Due to the notable attrition rate in our sample, a drop-out analysis was performed to investigate significant differences between participants and non-participants at baseline. The results, presented in online Supplementary eTable 2 in Appendix B, revealed that participants in this study were younger (25.7 (s.d. 5.7) v. 26.9 (s.d. 6.5) p = 0.021), and there was a lower proportion of men (48.7% v. 63,3% p = 0.002) among the participants, compared to non-participants. Additionally, participants had higher levels of education (43.6% v. 29.7% p = 0.002), a higher prevalence of schizotypal disorder (19.2% v. 12.3% p = 0.043), a significantly higher level of functioning (GAF 44.2 [s.d. 13.2] v. 39.7 [s.d. 13.1] p < 0.001) and lower levels of negative symptoms (1.9 [s.d. 1.1] v. 2.3 [s.d. 1.2], p = 0.001) compared to non-participants at baseline. An additional analysis of significant differences in 10-year cognitive function between participants and non-participants in the 20-year follow-up also revealed that participants in the 20-year reassessment had better 10-year cognition than non-participants (mean 0.24 s.d. 1.33 v. mean −0.17 s.d. 0.63, p < 0.001), results are presented in online Supplementary eTable 3.

Long-term neurocognitive and sociodemographic outcomes

Cognitive test outcomes, symptom levels, and social functioning between the 10- and 20-year follow-up are illustrated in Fig. 2, sociodemographic data and psychopathological raw-scores are also presented in online Supplementary eTable 2 in Appendix B. With exception of the symbol coding task (mean 47.7 [s.d. 13.8] at 10-year v. 50.8 [s.d. 14.7] at 20-year p < 0.001), we found no significant differences between the two time points. Psychotic symptom scores and level of social functioning also did not differ significantly, but overall negative symptoms (1.6 [s.d. 1.1] v. 1.2 [s.d. 1] p < 0.001) and the negative symptom subdomain experiential symptoms (1.4 [s.d. 1] v. 1 [s.d. 0.9] p < 0.001) were significantly higher at the 20-year follow-up.

Figure 2. Significant difference in Cognitive BACS test raw-scores, symptom levels, and social functioning from the 10- and 20-year follow-up reassessments following a first incident schizophrenia spectrum diagnosis.

Sociodemographic 20-year outcomes associated with the global level of neurocognition at the 20-year follow-up, are shown in Table 1, subdomains are presented in online Supplementary eTable 4. Participants with higher neurocognitive levels were significantly more likely to be in a relationship (OR: 1.56, CI 1.1–2.2, p = 0.013) and be employed (OR:2.07, CI 1.35–3.17, p < 0.001). They were less likely to have a schizophrenia diagnosis (OR:0.59, CI 0.42–0.84, p = 0.003) or receive treatment with antipsychotic medication (OR:0.39, CI 0.26–0.58, p < 0.001). Higher global neurocognitive scores at the 20-year follow-up were also significantly associated with higher levels of social functioning (B 6.86, CI 4.71–9.02, p < 0.001), explaining 21% of the variance (R2 0.21), lower levels of psychotic symptoms (B:-0.24, CI −0.46 to −0.03, p = 0.027) explaining 3% of the variance (R2 0.03) and lower levels of negative symptoms (B:-0.476, CI −0.64 to −0.32, p < 0.001) explaining 18% of the variance (R2 0.18).

Table 1. Analysis of 20-year clinical outcomes associated with 20-year neurocognition z-scores

Changes in neurocognition

Participants were divided into three groups based on their change scores. Half of the participants (49.2%) had stable global neurocognitive scores (change scores between 0.5 s.d. and −0.5 s.d. of the mean), 20.3% had improving neurocognitive scores (change scores above 0.5 s.d. of the mean), and 30.5% had deteriorating neurocognitive scores (change scores below 0.5 s.d. of the mean). Similar participant distributions were seen in all cognitive subdomains, with 25.5% showing deterioration of executive functioning, 34.6% showing deterioration of speed of processing and 34.1% showing deterioration of verbal learning and memory. Results are shown in Table 2, and Fig. 3 illustrates that participants in the cognitive decline group had higher cognitive scores at both assessments and participants in the improved cognition group had the lowest cognitive function at both time points. Age and sex distribution within the groups is shown in Table 3; the mean age in the decline group was 44.4 (s.d. 4.3); in the stable group, it was 46.5 (s.d. 6); and in the improving group, it was 47.3 (s.d. 6.1) but the difference was not significant (p = 0.086).

Table 2. Subdivision of participants depending on cognitive change-scores

Figure 3. Couse of cognitive change based on subdivision of participants depending on cognitive change-scores.

Table 3. Age and sex differences depending on cognitive change subgroups

Correlations between changes in cognitive functioning, symptom severity, and social functioning

Using Pearson Correlation analyses, we identified that increasing experiential negative symptom scores were significantly correlated to decreasing global neurocognitive scores (PC-0.231, p = 0.029). Results are shown in Table 4. In the subgroup analysis shown in online Supplementary eTable 5 increasing negative symptom scores were significantly associated with decreasing verbal learning and memory and speed of processing scores (R0.177, p = 0.038 and PC −0.192, p = 0.026).

Table 4. Correlations between change in global cognition and change in symptom severity and social functioning, Pearson correlation analysis

Predictors of change in neurocognition

One-way ANCOVA analysis was performed to identify baseline clinical and demographic characteristics associated with long-term changes in neurocognition. In the univariate analyses, we found younger age, unemployment, lower level of education and lower premorbid academic functioning at baseline were all associated with a decline in global neurocognition. The neurocognitive subgroup analysis results are shown in eTable 4 in supplementary material Appendix B. We then conducted a multivariate ANCOVA with the significant variables identified in the univariable analyses. Only younger age at baseline (B: 0.027 per 10 years, CI 0.05–0.049, p = 0.016) and low level of education (below ten years) (mean difference-0.438, CI −0.694 to −0.182, p < 0.001) remained significantly associated with a decline in global neurocognition, results are shown in Table 5. Results of the neurocognitive subgroup analysis are shown in eTable 4 in supplementary materials.

Table 5. Baseline predictors of changes in global neurocognition from the 10-to 20-year follow-up

In univariate analysis we found no variables associated with verbal learning and memory, speed of processing and executive functions, so we did not conduct multivariable analysis for these domains.

Discussion

We found no significant differences between the six BACS domain scores at the 10- and the 20-year follow-up except for the symbol coding task that showed higher test scores at the 20-year follow-up. Social functioning and psychotic symptoms also remained stable, but the global level of negative symptoms, and in particular experiential negative symptoms, was higher at the 20-year follow-up. This contrasts with previous long-term follow-up studies (Fett et al., Reference Fett, Velthorst, Reichenberg, Ruggero, Callahan, Fochtmann and Kotov2020; Jonas et al., Reference Jonas, Lian, Callahan, Ruggero, Clouston, Reichenberg and Kotov2022), generally reporting a cognitive decline. Additionally, the findings highlight expressive negative symptoms as a specific symptom aspect that may be further impaired in the long-term course of schizophrenia and may be an essential focus for targeted interventions in FEP.

Changes in neurocognition

Although we could not identify significant differences in neurocognitive test-scores from the 10-to the 20-year follow-up across the total sample, we determined subgroups according to BACS change-scores. We found that 49.2% of participants had a stable course of global neurocognition, while 30.5% had declining neurocognitive scores, and 20.3% showed improvement. These findings disprove neither the neurodevelopmental theory, in which cognition remains stable after the initial onset of psychosis, nor the neurodegenerative model, in which cognition declines further following the onset of psychosis, as both scenarios occur in different patients. Some patients even seem to recover cognitive functions. This emphasizes the possibility of very different outcomes in schizophrenia. The distribution of patients in these groups was similar to that reported by other long-term follow-up studies (Rodríguez-Sánchez et al., Reference Rodríguez-Sánchez, Setién-Suero, Suárez-Pinilla, Mayoral Van Son, Vázquez-Bourgon, Gil López and Ayesa-Arriola2022; Zanelli et al., Reference Zanelli, Mollon, Sandin, Morgan, Dazzan, Pilecka and Reichenberg2019), albeit some studies have reported no patients with an ongoing decline of neurocognition (Murillo-García et al., Reference Murillo-García, Ortíz-García de la Foz, Miguel-Corredera, Vázquez-Bourgon, Setién-Suero, Neergaard and Ayesa-Arriola2023; van Winkel et al., Reference van Winkel, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul, Peuskens, De Hert and van Os2006). In general, the research field is dominated by conflicting evidence on the stability of cognitive function after illness onset. This may be because follow-up studies examining the period after a first-episode psychosis include mainly young patients with subtle cognitive changes hard to detect. As patients age, cognition may start to decline more rapidly and, therefore, is more easily detected. Perhaps more broad-based cognitive testing at illness onset could increase the possibility of detecting subtle deficit areas.

We found no significant differences between the groups with declining, stable or improving cognition regarding age and sex, but there was a tendency towards participants with decline to be younger. This is in contrast to a study of older chronically ill patients with schizophrenia reporting that a larger portion of patients started to display a declining course of neurocognition as age increased (Kida et al., Reference Kida, Niimura, Nemoto, Ryu, Sakuma, Mimura and Mizuno2020). Our study detected higher attrition at the 20-year follow-up of patients with poor cognitive function at the 10-year follow-up. Patients seemed likelier to participate in the 20-year reassessment if they had good cognitive function, especially in older patients, as they mainly belong to the continued stable or improving cognition groups. This indicates that cognitive decline might prevent participation in our follow-up because only those with a decline from high cognitive function are represented in the sample. It is possible and very worrisome that cognitive decline later during illness has been overlooked because of a systematic failure to include the most affected patients in follow-up assessments. The Suffolk County 20-year follow-up had a remarkably low drop-out rate, and they are the only large longitudinal study to show consistent and significant cognitive decline in their patient population. (Jonas et al., Reference Jonas, Lian, Callahan, Ruggero, Clouston, Reichenberg and Kotov2022). We found that 30% of patients experienced cognitive decline, and our findings may underestimate the rates of patients with cognitive decline in a real-world schizophrenia population. Pro-cognitive interventions in schizophrenia should not only be offered as part of early intervention services; they might also really benefit some patients in more chronic illness stages. Further research is needed to clarify this issue.

Predictors of change in neurocognition

Our study only identified younger age at baseline and lower level of education as predictors of declining global neurocognition. That younger age at baseline predicted declining cognition in our study contrasts findings from other long-term follow-up studies (Fett et al., Reference Fett, Velthorst, Reichenberg, Ruggero, Callahan, Fochtmann and Kotov2020; Meier et al., Reference Meier, Caspi, Reichenberg, Keefe, Fisher, Harrington and Moffitt2014). These studies indicated that the influence of increasing age on cognition became significant after a certain age had been reached, and this is further supported by the Suffolk County study reporting declining neurocognitive function in both patients and controls but a more rapid decline in patients with schizophrenia compared to individuals without schizophrenia (Jonas et al., Reference Jonas, Lian, Callahan, Ruggero, Clouston, Reichenberg and Kotov2022).

The finding that a lower level of education was associated with cognitive decline corroborates findings from the Spanish PAFIP 10-year follow-up (Ayesa-Arriola et al., Reference Ayesa-Arriola, Miguel-Corredera, De La Foz, Neergaard, Correa-Ghisays, Setién-Suero and Crespo-Facorro2023; Murillo-García et al., Reference Murillo-García, Ortíz-García de la Foz, Miguel-Corredera, Vázquez-Bourgon, Setién-Suero, Neergaard and Ayesa-Arriola2023). We could not cross-validate other predictors from previous long-term follow-up studies, perhaps because our longitudinal data lacked cognitive assessments at baseline, which limited our ability to conclude predictors of the course of cognition from illness onset. All predictors of poor outcomes in the 10-year OPUS follow-up study were included in the prediction analysis of this paper, see Appendix A. Only baseline lower level of education predicted poor cognitive function in the OPUS 10-year follow-up and cognitive decline between the 10-and 20-year follow-up. Because the sample in the 10-year follow-up was twice the size of the 20-year follow-up, we may have lacked the power to determine the same predictors. However, drop-out analysis revealed a tendency for participants with poor cognition to drop out, and we found that those with the lowest cognitive function did not decline. Rather, those with the highest cognitive function seemed to decline between the 10- and 20-year follow-up; this could also explain why predictors of poor cognitive function at the 10-year follow-up would not predict further decline, as those with poor cognition did not experience the worst decline.

It may seem especially surprising that we did not find any associations between cessation of substance use and changes in cognition. Because we used SCAN-verified diagnosis of substance use as the only measure of substance use, this very likely led to the exclusion of participants with recreational use from the analysis, and perhaps we did not find an association because our definition was too narrow and the sample too small. Alcohol use (Bruijnen et al., Reference Bruijnen, Dijkstra, Walvoort, Markus, VanDerNagel, Kessels and DE Jong2019), substance use (Ramey & Regier, Reference Ramey and Regier2019) and cannabis use (Meier et al., Reference Meier, Caspi, Knodt, Hall, Ambler, Harrington and Moffitt2022) have been strongly associated with cognitive dysfunction. At the same time, our findings suggest that the course of neurocognition in schizophrenia was explicitly linked to the illness and not to comorbid substance use or other risk factors; the association between comorbid substance use and cognitive decline in patients with schizophrenia warrants further research.

Long-term neurocognition and correlations of change

We found that higher levels of neurocognition at the 20-year follow-up were significantly associated with better functional outcomes. This is in line with consistent evidence of poor neurocognition being associated with poor outcomes in schizophrenia (Fett et al., Reference Fett, Viechtbauer, Dominguez, Penn, van Os and Krabbendam2011; Halverson et al., Reference Halverson, Orleans-Pobee, Merritt, Sheeran, Fett and Penn2019). However, neurocognition only explained small proportions of the variance (5–21%). Review findings indicate that social cognition may be more strongly associated with poor outcomes, possibly mediating some of the effects of neurocognition on poor outcomes. Furthermore, evidence suggests that negative symptoms mediate the effect of neurocognition on functional outcomes (Glenthøj, Kristensen, Wenneberg, Hjorthøj, & Nordentoft, Reference Glenthøj, Kristensen, Wenneberg, Hjorthøj and Nordentoft2020; Ventura, Hellemann, Thames, Koellner, & Nuechterlein, Reference Ventura, Hellemann, Thames, Koellner and Nuechterlein2009). Our study found neurocognition to be more strongly associated with negative symptoms than positive psychotic symptoms, and specifically, an increase in experiential negative symptoms significantly correlated with cognitive decline. These findings are in keeping with emerging evidence on the key role of experiential negative symptoms in the cognitive and functional outcome of psychotic disorders (Glenthøj et al., Reference Glenthøj, Kristensen, Wenneberg, Hjorthøj and Nordentoft2020; Glenthøj et al., Reference Glenthøj, Kristensen, Wenneberg, Hjorthøj and Nordentoft2020; Llerena, Reddy, & Kern, Reference Llerena, Reddy and Kern2018). Additionally, it supports including a two-factor model of negative symptoms (expressive and experiential) in clinical studies, as these two aspects may have a differential impact on clinical variables. Speculating, it could be that experiential negative symptoms mediated the effect of cognition on functional outcomes, which would indicate the importance of addressing this negative symptom domain when aiming at improving the cognitive and functional outcome of schizophrenia. Future studies could help clarify this issue.

Strengths and limitations

The strength of this study was its prospective design and long-term follow-up, along with the large sample size and the representativeness of the OPUS cohort (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Nordentoft, Jeppesen, Øhlenschlæger, Thorup, Christensen and Jørgensen2005b). The main limitation of our study was the significant drop-out rate from the 10 to the 20-year follow-up. Our drop-out analysis indicated that those lost to follow-up may have suffered more severe psychopathology at baseline, meaning there is a risk of an attrition bias in our sample, and in a previous paper, we found that this was true regardless of treatment allocation in the randomized OPUS trial (Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Starzer, Nilsson, Hjorthøj, Albert and Nordentoft2023b). In addition, drop-out analysis of cognitive score between the 10- and 20-year follow-up revealed that non-participants in the 20-year follow-up had worse cognitive function at the 10-year follow-up. Consequently, our results reflect patients with a better outcome. Those with poor or declining neurocognition were less likely to participate in our follow-up assessments, and it seems possible that poor neurocognition may have been a direct factor hindering follow-up participation. Therefore, our findings may not be representative of all patients with schizophrenia, and we cannot rule out that real-world outcomes are worse than what our findings show. The findings can, however, still be used as evidence of different patterns of neurocognitive trajectories in subgroups of patients with schizophrenia.

Another important limitation was the need for a healthy control group and the lack of BACS normative data for the healthy Danish background population. This limitation means our findings of subgroups with declining, stable, or improving neurocognition cannot be used as firm evidence of different neurocognitive trajectories in schizophrenia compared to healthy controls.

Other limitations include the BACS test, which does not constitute a full cognitive assessment; therefore, we may not have been able to detect a broader range of cognitive deficits corresponding to the MATRICS-defined subdomains. Additionally, no cognitive assessments were conducted at baseline, precluding investigating cognitive changes from illness onset and onwards, and this may also have hindered our ability to identify significant predictors of change.

Conclusion

Mean neurocognition remained stable from the 10- to the 20-year follow-up. Still, we found that 30.5% of patients had a declining course of neurocognition, 49.2% had a stable course, and 20.3% experienced improvements in neurocognition. These findings support the two existing models of neurocognitive development in schizophrenia and support the growing body of evidence suggesting different subtypes of schizophrenia characterized by different illness phenotypes exist. Unfortunately, because of attrition bias, patients with poorer cognitive function seem underrepresented in our study, and real-life long-term cognitive development might be worse than we could describe. In our patient population, poor long-term cognition was associated with other poor outcomes at the 20-year follow-up, highlighting the importance of treatments aimed at improving neurocognition in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Lejeune et al., Reference Lejeune, Northrop and Kurtz2021; Wykes et al., Reference Wykes, Huddy, Cellard, McGurk and Czobor2011).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724000096.

Supplementary material associated with this article is Appendix A reporting the systematic search strategy and Appendix B containing supplementary tables.

Acknowledgements

A special thank you to our coworkers who were part of the clinical assessment team and conducted many clinical interviews in the 20-year follow-up. Most importantly, we are very grateful to the trial participants, who took the time to share their stories and provided the clinical data for this study again 20 years after they first fell ill.

Funding statement

The project was funded by unrestricted grants from the Tryg Foundation, Lundbeck Foundation and Helsefonden. The sponsors had no role in the acquisition of the data, interpretation of the results or the decision to publish the findings.

Declaration of interests

We declare no competing interests. The project was funded by unrestricted grants from the Tryg Foundation, Lundbeck Foundation and Helsefonden. The sponsors had no role in the acquisition of the data, interpretation of the results or the decision to publish the findings.

Ethical standards

The OPUS trial has been approved by the Regional Ethical Scientific Committee (Protocol nr.: 17023873), by the Danish data protection agency (RHP-2017-047, I-Suite nr: 05855) and registered at ClinicalTrials.gov NCT00157313.

References

Andreasen, N. C., Carpenter, W. T. Jr., Kane, J. M., Lasser, R. A., Marder, S. R., & Weinberger, D. R. (2005). Remission in schizophrenia: Proposed criteria and rationale for consensus. The American Journal of Psychiatry 162(3), 441449. https://doi.org/10.1176/APPI.AJP.162.3.441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, N. C., Flaum, M., Swayze, V. W., Tyrrell, G., & Arndt, S. (1990). Positive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia: A critical reappraisal. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47(7), 615621. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHPSYC.1990.01810190015002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arndt, S., Andreasen, N. C., Flaum, M., Miller, D., & Nopoulos, P. (1995). A longitudinal study of symptom dimensions in schizophrenia. Prediction and patterns of change. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52(5), 352360. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHPSYC.1995.03950170026004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayesa-Arriola, R., Miguel-Corredera, M., De La Foz, V. O. G., Neergaard, K. D., Correa-Ghisays, P., Setién-Suero, E., & Crespo-Facorro, B. (2023). Education and long-term outcomes in first episode psychosis: 10-year follow-up study of the PAFIP cohort. Psychological Medicine, 53(1), 6677. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721001112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barder, H. E., Sundet, K., Rund, B. R., Evensen, J., Haahr, U., ten Velden Hegelstad, W., … Friis, S. (2013). Neurocognitive development in first episode psychosis 5 years follow-up: Associations between illness severity and cognitive course. Schizophrenia Research, 149(1–3), 6369. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2013.06.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertelsen, M., Jeppesen, P., Petersen, L., Thorup, A., Øhlenschlæger, J., Le Quach, P., … Nordentoft, M. (2008). Five-year follow-up of a randomized multicenter trial of intensive early intervention vs standard treatment for patients with a first episode of psychotic illness: The OPUS trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(7), 762771. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHPSYC.65.7.762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bighelli, I., Pitschel-Walz, G., Schneider-Thoma, J., Siafis, S., Wu, H., Wang, D., … Leucht, S. (2021). Psychosocial and psychological interventions for relapse prevention in schizophrenia: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00243-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilder, R. M., Lipschutz-broch, L., Reiter, G., Geisler, S. H., Mayerhoff, D. I., & Lieberman, J. A. (1992). Intellectual deficits in first-episode schizophrenia: Evidence for progressive deterioration. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 18(3), 437448. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCHBUL/18.3.437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E., & Murray, R. M. (2014). Meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in ultra-high risk to psychosis and first-episode psychosis: Do the cognitive deficits progress over, or after, the onset of psychosis? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(4), 744755. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCHBUL/SBT085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E., & Pantelis, C. (2015). Meta-analysis of cognitive impairment in first-episode bipolar disorder: Comparison with first-episode schizophrenia and healthy controls. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(5), 10951104. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCHBUL/SBU198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozikas, V. P., & Andreou, C. (2011). Longitudinal studies of cognition in first episode psychosis: A systematic review of the literature. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 45(2), 93108. https://doi.org/10.3109/00048674.2010.541418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brill, N., Reichenberg, A., Weiser, M., & Rabinowitz, J. (2008). Validity of the premorbid adjustment scale. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(5), 981983. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCHBUL/SBM128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, R. (1996). EuroQol: The current state of play. Health Policy, 37(1), 5372. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-8510(96)00822-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruijnen, C. J. W. H., Dijkstra, B. A. G., Walvoort, S. J. W., Markus, W., VanDerNagel, J. E. L., Kessels, R. P. C., & DE Jong, C. A. J. (2019). Prevalence of cognitive impairment in patients with substance use disorder. Drug and Alcohol Review, 38(4), 435442. https://doi.org/10.1111/DAR.12922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowman, M., Holleran, L., Lonergan, E., O'Connor, K., Birchwood, M., & Donohoe, G. (2021). Cognitive predictors of social and occupational functioning in early psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis of cross-sectional and longitudinal data. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(5), 1243. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCHBUL/SBAB033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, D., Ragland, J. D., Gold, J. M., & Gur, R. C. (2008). General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: Goliath defeats David? Biological Psychiatry, 64(9), 823. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2008.04.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fett, A. K. J., Reichenberg, A., & Velthorst, E. (2022). Lifespan evolution of neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia - A narrative review. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 28, 100237. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCOG.2022.100237.Google ScholarPubMed
Fett, A. K. J., Velthorst, E., Reichenberg, A., Ruggero, C. J., Callahan, J. L., Fochtmann, L. J., … Kotov, R. (2020). Long-term changes in cognitive functioning in individuals with psychotic disorders: Findings from the Suffolk county mental health project. JAMA Psychiatry, 77(4), 387396. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.3993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fett, A. K. J., Viechtbauer, W., Dominguez, M. de G., Penn, D. L., van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2011). The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 573588. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2010.07.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foussias, G., & Remington, G. (2010). Negative symptoms in schizophrenia: Avolition and Occam's razor. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(2), 359. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCHBUL/SBN094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Deste, G., Smieskova, R., Barlati, S., Yung, A. R., Howes, O., … Borgwardt, S. (2012). Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: A meta-analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(6), 562571. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenthøj, L. B., Kristensen, T. D., Wenneberg, C., Hjorthøj, C., & Nordentoft, M. (2020). Experiential negative symptoms are more predictive of real-life functional outcome than expressive negative symptoms in clinical high-risk states. Schizophrenia Research, 218, 151156. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2020.01.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guilera, G., Pino, O., Gómez-Benito, J., & Rojo, J. E. (2009). Antipsychotic effects on cognition in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. The European Journal of Psychiatry, 23(2), 7789. Retrieved from https://scielo.isciii.es/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0213-61632009000200002&lng=es&nrm=iso&tlng=en.10.4321/S0213-61632009000200002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habtewold, T. D., Rodijk, L. H., Liemburg, E. J., Sidorenkov, G., Boezen, H. M., Bruggeman, R., & Alizadeh, B. Z. (2020). A systematic review and narrative synthesis of data-driven studies in schizophrenia symptoms and cognitive deficits. Translational Psychiatry, 10(1), 244. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-00919-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Häfner, H., Riecher-Rössler, A., Hambrecht, M., Maurer, K., Meissner, S., Schmidtke, A., … van der Heiden, W. (1992). IRAOS: An instrument for the assessment of onset and early course of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 6(3), 209223. https://doi.org/10.1016/0920-9964(92)90004-O.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halverson, T. F., Orleans-Pobee, M., Merritt, C., Sheeran, P., Fett, A. K., & Penn, D. L. (2019). Pathways to functional outcomes in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Meta-analysis of social cognitive and neurocognitive predictors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 105(August), 212219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.07.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, H. G., Speyer, H., Starzer, M., Albert, N., Hjorthøj, C., Eplov, L. F., & Nordentoft, M. (2023a). Clinical recovery among individuals with a first-episode schizophrenia an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 49(2), 297308. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCHBUL/SBAC103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, H. G., Starzer, M., Nilsson, S. F., Hjorthøj, C., Albert, N., & Nordentoft, M. (2023b). Clinical recovery and long-term association of specialized early intervention services vs treatment as usual among individuals with first-episode schizophrenia spectrum disorder: 20-year follow-up of the OPUS trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 80(4), 371379. https://doi.org/10.1001/JAMAPSYCHIATRY.2022.5164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedman, A. M., van Haren, N. E. M., van Baal, C. G. M., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2013). IQ change over time in schizophrenia and healthy individuals: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 146(1–3), 201208. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2013.01.027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, A. L., Svetina, C., Shields, G., Stewart, J., & DeLisi, L. E. (2005). Ten year longitudinal study of neuropsychological functioning subsequent to a first episode of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 78(1), 2734. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2005.05.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jääskeläinen, E., Juola, P., Hirvonen, N., McGrath, J. J., Saha, S., Isohanni, M., … Miettunen, J. (2013). A systematic review and meta-analysis of recovery in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(6), 12961306. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonas, K., Lian, W., Callahan, J., Ruggero, C. J., Clouston, S., Reichenberg, A., … Kotov, R. (2022). The course of general cognitive ability in individuals with psychotic disorders. JAMA Psychiatry, 79(7), 659666. https://doi.org/10.1001/JAMAPSYCHIATRY.2022.1142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keefe, R. S. E., Goldberg, T. E., Harvey, P. D., Gold, J. M., Poe, M. P., & Coughenour, L. (2004). The brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: Reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophrenia Research, 68(2–3), 283297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kida, H., Niimura, H., Nemoto, T., Ryu, Y., Sakuma, K., Mimura, M., & Mizuno, M. (2020). Community transition at younger ages contributes to good cognitive function outcomes in long-term hospitalized patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorder: A 15-year follow-up study with group-based trajectory modeling. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 74(2), 105111. https://doi.org/10.1111/PCN.12941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lally, J., Ajnakina, O., Stubbs, B., Cullinane, M., Murphy, K. C., Gaughran, F., & Murray, R. M. (2017). Remission and recovery from first-episode psychosis in adults: Systematic review and meta-analysis of long-term outcome studies. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 211(6), 350358. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.117.201475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lejeune, J. A., Northrop, A., & Kurtz, M. M. (2021). A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: Efficacy and the role of participant and treatment factors. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(4), 9971006. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCHBUL/SBAB022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Llerena, K., Reddy, L. F., & Kern, R. S. (2018). The role of experiential and expressive negative symptoms on job obtainment and work outcome in individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 192, 148153. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2017.06.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luther, L., Suor, J. H., Rosen, C., Jobe, T. H., Faull, R. N., & Harrow, M. (2020). Clarifying the direction of impact of negative symptoms and neurocognition on prospective work functioning in psychosis: A 20-year longitudinal study. Schizophrenia Research, 220, 232239. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2020.03.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, M. H., Caspi, A., Knodt, A. R., Hall, W., Ambler, A., Harrington, H. L., … Moffitt, T. E. (2022). Long-term cannabis use and cognitive reserves and hippocampal volume in midlife. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 179(5), 362374. https://doi.org/10.1176/APPI.AJP.2021.21060664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, M. H., Caspi, A., Reichenberg, A., Keefe, R. S. E., Fisher, H. L., Harrington, H., … Moffitt, T. E. (2014). Neuropsychological decline in schizophrenia from the premorbid to the postonset period: Evidence from a population-representative longitudinal study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(1), 91101. https://doi.org/10.1176/APPI.AJP.2013.12111438/ASSET/IMAGES/LARGE/91F2.JPEG.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mollon, J., & Reichenberg, A. (2018, February 1). Cognitive development prior to onset of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 48, 392403. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murillo-García, N., Ortíz-García de la Foz, V., Miguel-Corredera, M., Vázquez-Bourgon, J., Setién-Suero, E., Neergaard, K., … Ayesa-Arriola, R. (2023). Intelligence quotient changes over 10 years: Diversity of cognitive profiles in first episode of psychosis and healthy controls. Schizophrenia Research, 254, 163172. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2023.02.025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pantelis, C., Wood, S. J., Proffitt, T. M., Testa, R., Mahony, K., Brewer, W. J., … McGorry, P. D. (2009). Attentional set-shifting ability in first-episode and established schizophrenia: Relationship to working memory. Schizophrenia Research, 112(1–3), 104113. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2009.03.039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, L., Jeppesen, P., Thorup, A., Abel, M. B., Øhlenschlaeger, J., Christensen, T. Ø., … Nordentoft, M. (2005a). A randomised multicentre trial of integrated versus standard treatment for patients with a first episode of psychotic illness. British Medical Journal, 331(7517), 602605. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.38565.415000.E01.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, L., Nordentoft, M., Jeppesen, P., Øhlenschlæger, J., Thorup, A., Christensen, T. Ø., … Jørgensen, P. (2005b). Improving 1-year outcome in first-episode psychosis: OPUS trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry. Supplement, 48(SUPPL. 48), s98s103. https://doi.org/10.1192/BJP.187.48.S98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pukrop, R., Schultze-Lutter, F., Ruhrmann, S., Brockhaus-Dumke, A., Tendolkar, I., Bechdolf, A., … Klosterkötter, J. (2006). Neurocognitive functioning in subjects at risk for a first episode of psychosis compared with first- and multiple-episode schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(8), 13881407. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390500434425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rajji, T. K., Miranda, D., & Mulsant, B. H. (2014). Cognition, function, and disability in patients with schizophrenia: A review of longitudinal studies. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 59(1), 1317. https://doi.org/10.1177/070674371405900104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramey, T., & Regier, P. S. (2019). Cognitive impairment in substance use disorders. CNS Spectrums, 24(1), 102. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852918001426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez-Sánchez, J. M., Setién-Suero, E., Suárez-Pinilla, P., Mayoral Van Son, J., Vázquez-Bourgon, J., Gil López, P., … Ayesa-Arriola, R. (2022). Ten-year course of cognition in first-episode non-affective psychosis patients: PAFIP cohort. Psychological Medicine, 52(4), 770779. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720002408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rund, B. R., Barder, H. E., Evensen, J., Haahr, U., Hegelstad, W. T. V., Joa, I., … Friis, S. (2016). Neurocognition and duration of psychosis: A 10-year follow-up of first-episode patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(1), 8795. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCHBUL/SBV083.Google ScholarPubMed
Schützwohl, M., Kallert, T., & Jurjanz, L. (2007). Using the schedules for clinical assessment in neuropsychiatry (SCAN 2.1) as a diagnostic interview providing dimensional measures: Cross-national findings on the psychometric properties of psychopathology scales. European Psychiatry, 22(4), 229238. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EURPSY.2006.10.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidman, L. J., Buka, S. L., Goldstein, J. M., & Tsuang, M. T. (2006). Intellectual decline in schizophrenia: Evidence from a prospective birth cohort 28 year follow-up study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 225242. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390500360471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidman, L. J., Giuliano, A. J., Meyer, E. C., Addington, J., Cadenhead, K. S., Cannon, T. D., … Cornblatt, B. A. (2010). Neuropsychology of the prodrome to psychosis in the NAPLS consortium: Relationship to family history and conversion to psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(6), 578588. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHGENPSYCHIATRY.2010.66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silberstein, J., & Harvey, P. D. (2019, February 1). Cognition, social cognition, and self-assessment in schizophrenia: Prediction of different elements of everyday functional outcomes. CNS Spectrums, 24, 8893. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852918001414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., Horan, W. P., Kirkpatrick, B., Fischer, B. A., Keller, W. R., Miski, P., … Carpenter, W. T. (2013). Deconstructing negative symptoms of schizophrenia: Avolition–apathy and diminished expression clusters predict clinical presentation and functional outcome. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(6), 783790. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JPSYCHIRES.2013.01.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szöke, A., Trandafir, A., Dupont, M. E., Méary, A., Schürhoff, F., & Leboyer, M. (2008). Longitudinal studies of cognition in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 192(4), 248257. https://doi.org/10.1192/BJP.BP.106.029009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tegeler, D., & Juckel, G. (2007). Validation of Personal and Social Performance Scale (PSP) on a random test of acute schizophrenic patients. Nervenarzt, 78, 74.Google Scholar
The WHOQOL Group. (1996). Whoqol-Bref: Introduction, Administration, Scoring and Generic Version of the Assessment. Programme on Mental Health, (December), 16. https://doi.org/10.1037/t01408-000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tschentscher, N., Woll, C. F. J., Tafelmaier, J. C., Kriesche, D., Bucher, J. C., Engel, R. R., & Karch, S. (2023). Neurocognitive deficits in first-episode and chronic psychotic disorders: A systematic review from 2009 to 2022. Brain Sciences, 13(2), 299. https://doi.org/10.3390/BRAINSCI13020299/S1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Winkel, R., Myin-Germeys, I., Delespaul, P., Peuskens, J., De Hert, M., & van Os, J. (2006). Premorbid IQ as a predictor for the course of IQ in first onset patients with schizophrenia: A 10-year follow-up study. Schizophrenia Research, 88(1–3), 4754. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2006.06.033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ventura, J., Hellemann, G. S., Thames, A. D., Koellner, V., & Nuechterlein, K. H. (2009). Symptoms as mediators of the relationship between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 113(2–3), 189199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2009.03.035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Knorring, L. (2001). Danish validation of WHO quality of life questionnaire. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 55(4), 227.Google ScholarPubMed
Wing, J. K., Sartorius, N., & Üstun, T. B. (1998). WHO diagnosis and clinical measurement in psychiatry. A reference manual for SCAN. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511666445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wykes, T., Huddy, V., Cellard, C., McGurk, S. R., & Czobor, P. (2011). A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: Methodology and effect sizes. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(5), 472485. https://doi.org/10.1176/APPI.AJP.2010.10060855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zanelli, J., Mollon, J., Sandin, S., Morgan, C., Dazzan, P., Pilecka, I., … Reichenberg, A. (2019). Cognitive change in schizophrenia and other psychoses in the decade following the first episode. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 176(10), 811819. https://doi.org/10.1176/APPI.AJP.2019.18091088.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Neurocognitive subdomains - BACS three factor model (Keefe et al., 2004).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Significant difference in Cognitive BACS test raw-scores, symptom levels, and social functioning from the 10- and 20-year follow-up reassessments following a first incident schizophrenia spectrum diagnosis.

Figure 2

Table 1. Analysis of 20-year clinical outcomes associated with 20-year neurocognition z-scores

Figure 3

Table 2. Subdivision of participants depending on cognitive change-scores

Figure 4

Figure 3. Couse of cognitive change based on subdivision of participants depending on cognitive change-scores.

Figure 5

Table 3. Age and sex differences depending on cognitive change subgroups

Figure 6

Table 4. Correlations between change in global cognition and change in symptom severity and social functioning, Pearson correlation analysis

Figure 7

Table 5. Baseline predictors of changes in global neurocognition from the 10-to 20-year follow-up

Supplementary material: File

Starzer et al. supplementary material 1

Starzer et al. supplementary material
Download Starzer et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 54.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Starzer et al. supplementary material 2

Starzer et al. supplementary material
Download Starzer et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 30.6 KB