Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T15:38:28.546Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

High-risk pregnancy and its relationship with the neurodevelopment and behavior of 2-year-old children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 April 2024

Carolina Mariño-Narvaez
Affiliation:
Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Jose A. Puertas-Gonzalez
Affiliation:
Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Borja Romero-Gonzalez*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education, University of Valladolid, Soria, Spain
Milagros Cruz-Martinez
Affiliation:
Obstetrics and Gynaecology Department, University Hospital San Cecilio, Granada, Spain
Raquel Gonzalez-Perez
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, CIBERehd, Faculty of Pharmacy, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Yaima Juncosa-Castro
Affiliation:
Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Maria Isabel Peralta-Ramirez
Affiliation:
Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain Department of Personality, Assessment and Psychological Treatment, Faculty of Psychology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
*
Corresponding author: B. Romero-Gonzalez; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

High-risk pregnancies elevate maternal stress, impacting offspring neurodevelopment and behavior. This study, involving 112 participants, aimed to compare perceived stress, neurodevelopment, and behavior in high-risk and low-risk pregnancies. Two groups, high-risk and low-risk, were assessed during pregnancy for stress using hair cortisol and psychological analysis. At 24 months post-birth, their children’s neurodevelopment and behavior were evaluated. Results revealed higher perceived stress and pregnancy-related concerns in high-risk pregnancies, contrasting with low-risk pregnancies. Offspring from high-risk pregnancies displayed elevated internalizing behavior scores, while low-risk pregnancies showed higher externalizing behavior scores. Additionally, women in low-risk pregnancies exhibited increased cortisol concentrations 24 months post-delivery. These findings underscore the necessity for early stress detection and prevention programs during pregnancy, particularly in high-risk cases, to enhance maternal and infant health.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Pregnancy is a stage in the life of women that implies physical and psychological changes, which may present complications, leading to high-risk pregnancy. Due to these substantial changes, the health and life of both the mother and the infant are at greater risk (NICHD, 2020), thus it is a condition that requires specialized medical care. According to Holness (Reference Holness2018), there are four types of risk factors: psychosocial factors, medical history, bad obstetric history, and typical conditions of pregnancy.

Some of the causes of high-risk pregnancy are maternal age (over 35 years of age or adolescent), obesity or overweight, psychiatric diseases, autoimmune diseases, pre-eclampsia, gestational diabetes, threatened preterm labor, multiple pregnancy, and maternal infectious disease (Holness, Reference Holness2018).

The psychological consequences of this type of pregnancy are related to the fact that women with high-risk pregnancy present a greater prevalence of psychological disorders, especially anxiety, depression and stress, compared to women with low-risk pregnancy (Williamson et al., Reference Williamson, Moffitt, Broadbent, Neumann and Hamblin2023). This could be due to the greater concerns shown by high-risk pregnancy women about their health and that of their children, as well as the complications that may derive from the labor (Mohammadi et al., Reference Mohammadi, Shojaei, Maraghi and Motaghi2022; Ray et al., Reference Ray, Jeffrey, Nair, Vu, King and Schmied2022). Another possible explanation is that they tend to experience greater feelings of guilt and difficulty to manage their emotions, as they feel responsible for what may happen to their babies (McCoyd et al., Reference McCoyd, Curran and Munch2020). They also tend to be afraid throughout the pregnancy, and there may appear feelings of frustration, isolation, loneliness, rage, and depression, which are all related to the hospitalizations and the severity of their diagnosis (Isaacs & Andipatin, Reference Isaacs and Andipatin2020).

In general, these women experience the period of pregnancy with moments of uncertainty and threat, which generate high levels of stress, potentially affecting their offspring. In this line, the fetal programing hypothesis (Barker, Reference Barker2007) states that the biological development of the fetus can be affected by the exposure of the mother to adverse environmental situations, such as bad nutrition, stressful events and infections, in sensitive periods of pregnancy, producing excess maternal glucocorticoids, which have a negative impact on placental function (Chen et al., Reference Chen, He, Chen, Liu and Wang2019), leading to a greater probability of developing chronic diseases, even in adulthood (Shchurevska, Reference Shchurevska2022; Van Den Bergh et al., Reference Van Den Bergh, Van Den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, De Rooij, Entringer, Hoyer, Roseboom, Räikkönen, King and Schwab2020).

Specifically, prenatal stress has different negative effects on the development of the offspring. Different studies have analyzed its influence on the shortening of telomeres, which is associated with shorter life (G FC;m FC;ş, 2022). Moreover, maternal anxiety has been related to other adverse perinatal results, such as premature birth, low birthweight (Sanjuan et al., Reference Sanjuan, Fokas, Tonigan, Henry, Christian, Rodriguez, Larsen, Yonke and Leeman2021), and shorter cephalic perimeter (Grigoriadis et al., Reference Grigoriadis, Graves, Peer, Mamisashvili, Tomlinson, Vigod, Dennis, Steiner, Brown, Cheung, Dawson, Rector, Guenette and Richter2018). Other studies have associated it with a greater probability of developing behavior disorders in the offspring (Madigan et al., Reference Madigan, Oatley, Racine, Fearon, Schumacher, Akbari, Cooke and Tarabulsy2018). In this line, Santelices et al. (Reference Santelices, Irarrázaval, Jervis, Brotfeld, Cisterna and Gallardo2021) found that children of mothers who had suffered from depression during pregnancy were more likely to develop externalizing behaviors.

Deficiencies have also been detected in social communication and language, which are related to autism spectrum disorders (Brachetti et al., Reference Brachetti, Ruperti, Irigoyen and Brito2020), anxiety and depression (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Hankin, Glynn, Head, Kim and Sandman2020; Hentges et al., Reference Hentges, Graham, Plamondon, Tough and Madigan2019; McLean et al., Reference McLean, Cobham and Simcock2018), motor problems, deficient cognitive development, and alterations in brain development (Van Den Bergh et al., Reference Van Den Bergh, Van Den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, De Rooij, Entringer, Hoyer, Roseboom, Räikkönen, King and Schwab2020).

These alterations are associated with structural changes in the limbic and frontotemporal networks (cortical thinning and enlarged amygdala) and in the functional and microstructural connections that link them (Lautarescu et al., Reference Lautarescu, Craig and Glover2020), and they may be related to an over activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in cases of stress, which increases the level of glucocorticoids, including cortisol, eventually affecting the fetus (Monk et al., Reference Monk, Lugo-Candelas and Trumpff2019).

The influence of high-risk pregnancy on the neurodevelopment and behavior of the offspring has been poorly studied. For instance, in contrast with what would be expected, Romero-Gonzalez et al. (Reference Romero-Gonzalez, Caparros-Gonzalez, Cruz-Martinez, Gonzalez-Perez, Gallego-Burgos and Peralta-Ramirez2020) reported that 6-month-old babies born from high-risk pregnancies presented a better cognitive, language and motor development than babies born from low-risk pregnancies.

Other studies have related different prenatal risk factors to the behavior of the offspring. Marceau et al. (Reference Marceau, Hajal, Leve, Reiss, Shaw, Ganiban, Mayes and Neiderhiser2013) found a relationship between perinatal risk factors and the genetic and environmental influences on child behavior. On the other hand, different authors have analyzed, separately, the influence of different prenatal risk factors on the behavior of the infants (Krzeczkowski et al., Reference Krzeczkowski, Lau, Fitzpatrick, Tamana, Smithson, de Souza, Kozyrskyj, Lefebvre, Becker, Subbarao, Turvey, Pei, Schmidt, Sears, Van Lieshout and Mandhane2019; Tien et al., Reference Tien, Lewis and Liu2020). However, these studies do not address, specifically, how the type of pregnancy risk (high or low) influences the behavior of the offspring.

For the above mentioned, and taking into account that high-risk pregnancy entails greater maternal stress levels, which can have a negative effect on the infant, the aim of the present study was to analyze the possible differences in the level of stress between women with high-risk pregnancy and women with low-risk pregnancy, throughout pregnancy and at 24 months after delivery, as well as to explore the possible consequences of high-risk pregnancy on the neurodevelopment and behavior of their offspring at 2 years of age. We hypothesize that women with high-risk pregnancies will have higher levels of stress (psychological and physiological through cortisol) in comparison to those with low-risk pregnancies. In addition, we hypothesize their children will portray higher behavioral issues.

Method

Participants

The sample was constituted by a total of 56 pregnant women (M = 34.43 years; SD = 3.46) and their 56 children (26 boys and 30 girls), who were evaluated at 24 months of age (M = 24.08; SD = 0.82). The women were divided according to the type of pregnancy risk, with 27 women in the high-risk group and 29 women in the low-risk group.

The participants were informed about the study in the first pregnancy visit to the obstetrician, in a health center of Granada (Spain), where, after agreeing to participate voluntarily, they read the information sheet and signed the informed consent form.

The inclusion criteria were: (1) being pregnant, (2) being at least 18 years old, (3) mastering the Spanish language, both orally and in written, and (4) in the case of the high-risk pregnancy group, having a medical diagnosis of this condition. The study excluded women who suffered from psychiatric diseases that required treatment and/or the intake of corticoids.

The study was conducted in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013) and the guidelines of Good Clinical Practice (Directive 2005/28/CE) of the European Union. It was also approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Granada (968/CEIH/2019).

Instruments

The sociodemographic and obstetric data were gathered using a questionnaire that was completed by the participants during their pregnancy. The sociodemographic information was related to age, marital status, place of birth and country of origin, education level, and employment situation, among others. The obstetric data were related to the type of pregnancy (whether it was primiparous), number of previous children, etc.

Psychological evaluation of the mother

Perceived stress scale (PSS)

(Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983; Remor, Reference Remor2006). This scale evaluates the level of stress perceived during the last month. It is a self-report of 14 items with a five-point response scale (0 = never, 1 = almost never, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often, 4 = very often). In the Spanish version, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of reliability was 0.81.

Prenatal distress questionnaire (PDQ)

(Caparros-Gonzalez et al., Reference Caparros-Gonzalez, Perra, Alderdice, Lynn, Lobel, García-García and Peralta-Ramírez2019; Yali & Lobel, Reference Yali and Lobel1999). The PDQ evaluates the concerns related to the specific stress of pregnancy. It addresses variables about the infant and her/his birth, child weight, the body image of the pregnant mother and her emotions and relationship with her child. It consists of 12 items, which are responded to through a 5-point Likert scale, from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). In its Spanish version, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.74.

Evaluation of the children

Achenbach’s child behavior checklist (CBCL)

(Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2000). The CBCL was used in its version for children aged between 1 ½ and 5 years to evaluate the behavioral problems of the participants’ children. This questionnaire asks the parents or guardians to mark 99 specific behaviors of the child and, in addition, they are allowed to include 3 more behaviors that they consider problematic and which are not in the list. The items are responded to in a Likert scale of 0 to 2 points (0 = not true; 1 = somewhat or sometimes true; 2 = very true or often true). It consists of scales for internalizing behaviors (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.89), externalizing behaviors (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92) and total behaviors. It also contains other scales that contribute to the internalizing and externalizing scores and an additional stress scale.

Bayley’s child development test-III (Bayley-III)

(Bayley, Reference Bayley2006). This test evaluates the cognitive, language and motor development of children aged 1–42 months. The cognitive scale evaluates aspects such as visual preference, attention, memory, sensory-motor processing, exploration and manipulation, and concept formation. The language scale analyses the receptive and expressive language, and the motor scale assesses the fine and coarse motor skills. This test presents adequate internal validity, with a reliability coefficient ranging from 0.86 (in the fine motor subscale) to 0.91 (for coarse motor skills, cognition, and expressive communication); the remaining subscale of receptive language has a reliability coefficient of 0.87.

Evaluation of maternal cortisol

Hair cortisol concentrations

Chronic stress was evaluated through the analysis of cortisol in hair. This technique is noninvasive and enables a retrospective measurement of cortisol, from weeks to months. Moreover, it is not affected by the circadian fluctuation of cortisol, and the sample is kept at room temperature, with a long useful life (Greff et al., Reference Greff, Levine, Abuzgaia, Elzagallaai, Rieder and Van Uum2019). A lock of hair with approximately 150 hairs was collected from the participants, as close to the cranium as possible and from the back of the head (Sauvé et al., Reference Sauvé, Koren, Walsh, Tokmakejian and Van Uum2007). In order to determine the cortisol levels in the previous 3 months, coinciding with each trimester of pregnancy, 3 cm of each sample were analyzed (Stalder & Kirschbaum, Reference Stalder and Kirschbaum2012). All samples were kept at room temperature, wrapped in tin foil, to protect them from direct light and humidity, and they were analyzed by the Department of Pharmacology at the University of Granada. The analysis of the protocol is described in Romero-Gonzalez et al. (Reference Romero-Gonzalez, Caparros-Gonzalez, Gonzalez-Perez, Delgado-Puertas and Peralta-Ramirez2018).

Procedure

Information was given to the participants about the study, during their first visit to the midwife, when they were briefed on the importance of this investigation and how it was going to be carried out. Those who agreed to participate read the information sheet and signed the informed consent form.

Next, a hair sample was collected for each participant, in order to analyze the cortisol concentration, and a new sample was collected in each trimester. Upon collection, the samples were immediately kept and marked with a code that was associated with the corresponding participant, which guaranteed their anonymity.

Furthermore, the participants completed the questionnaires online in each trimester of pregnancy through the Google Forms platform. These questionnaires included sociodemographic data (which were only gathered in the first contact), the PSS and the PDQ.

In the days prior to the child being 2 years old, the participants were contacted again, and a new hair sample was collected from them, following the previously explained protocol. In that moment, their children were also evaluated, with the CBCL, through an online form, and Bayley-III, by the same evaluator, at the Mind, Brain and Behaviour Research Centre of the University of Granada.

Data analysis

Firstly, we verified the assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance through Shapiro-Wilk and Levene tests. However, the measurements of cortisol did not show a normal distribution, thus a logarithmic transformation was performed to adjust them to a normal distribution, following the analysis recommendations for this type of sample (Stalder & Kirschbaum, Reference Stalder and Kirschbaum2012).

Similarly, with the aim of analyzing the influence of stress throughout pregnancy on the neurodevelopment and behavior of the children, we calculated the mean scores in PSS, PDQ, and hair cortisol of the three measurements recorded in each trimester.

To verify the existence of differences between the main sociodemographic and obstetric variables of the mothers of both groups, Student’s t-tests and Chi-squared tests were performed for the continuous and categorical variables, respectively. On the other hand, to explore the differences in maternal stress and cortisol between groups, as well as in the neurodevelopment and behavior of their children at 24 months, new Student’s t-tests were carried out.

Lastly, the effect size of the differences in neurodevelopment and cortisol was determined through Cohen’s d, with the following values: d ≤ 0.20 indicates a small effect size, d ≤ 0.50 indicates a moderate effect size, and d ≤ 0.80 indicates a large effect size (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988).

The analyses were conducted using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences 20.0 Macintosh v26 (SPSS, Armonk, New York, USA).

Results

Sample description

The 56 participants were divided into two groups according to the type of pregnancy: 27 women in the high-risk group (M = 35.4 years; SD = 3.6) and 29 in the low-risk group (M = 33.7 years; SD = 3.2).

Table 1 shows the main sociodemographic and obstetric variables of both groups. The results indicate that both groups had similar data in the sociodemographic and obstetric variables, since there were no differences between the groups in the analyzed variables.

Table 1. Differences in sociodemographic variables and obstetric information between high-risk and low-risk pregnant women

Note: Student t-tests were used to compare quantitative variables whereas chi-square tests for qualitative information.

Differences in the stress level, measured through questionnaires and hair cortisol, between high-risk and low-risk pregnant women

The comparison of the two groups shows that there were significant differences in the scores of perceived stress (t = 2.17; p < .05) and prenatal distress (t = 2.15; p < .05), with the high-risk women presenting higher scores in both scales with respect to the low-risk women. The effect size of the two scales was moderate (d = 0.62). No significant differences were found in the concentrations of cortisol in hair during pregnancy between the two groups (Table 2).

Table 2. Differences in maternal hair cortisol levels and psychological variables associated with pregnancy between high-risk and low-risk pregnancy

Note: HCC = Hair cortisol concentrations; PSS-14 = Perceived Stress Scale; PDQ = Prenatal Distress Questionnaire EEP.

Table 3 shows the results of the analysis of maternal hair cortisol at 24 months, along with the evaluation of their children with Bayley-III and CBCL. The data show significant differences of maternal cortisol at 24 months after delivery (t = −2.65; p < .05), with the high-risk pregnancy group presenting lower maternal hair cortisol concentrations compared to the low-risk group, with a near-large effect size (d = −0.73).

Table 3. Comparison between groups of maternal hair cortisol levels and scores on behavior and neurodevelopment

Note: HCC = Hair cortisol concentrations.

Differences in neurodevelopment and behavior in offspring of high vs low-risk pregnancies

Regarding the children’s scores, no differences were found between their ages. On the other hand, concerning the behavioral assessment, significant differences were found between the two groups in both externalizing and internalizing behaviors (t = 2.06; p < .05 and t = −2.12; p < .05, respectively). The children of the high-risk pregnant women presented a greater t score of internalizing behaviors, whereas the low-risk group obtained higher scores in externalizing behaviors. The effect size of both behaviors was moderate (d = 0.55 and d = 0.57 for internalizing and externalizing behaviors, respectively).

In regard with the results of neurodevelopment assessed with Bayley-III, no significant differences were found in any of the subscales of this instrument.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to verify the existence of differences in the stress level, measured as perceived stress, specific pregnancy stress and hair cortisol concentration, between women with high-risk pregnancy and women with low-risk pregnancy, as well as to explore the possible differences in the neurodevelopment and behavior of their offspring at 24 months after delivery. The results showed that the levels of psychological stress were higher in the high-risk group, specifically perceived stress and distress related to specific pregnancy stress, detecting significant differences with respect to the low-risk group. These results are related to the characteristics of high-risk pregnancies, where mothers have multiple concerns related to the present and future health of themselves and that of their children, and they also usually experience contradicting emotions that alternate between happiness for the arrival of the baby and frustration and uncertainty (Holness, Reference Holness2018).

Our results are in line with those reported by Rodrigues et al., (Rodrigues et al., Reference Rodrigues, Zambaldi, Cantilino and Sougey2016), who found that women with high-risk pregnancy had significantly higher stress levels and a worse emotional state than women with low-risk pregnancy. Moreover, it has been demonstrated that perceived stress in pregnant women predicts the obstetric risk and, therefore, is associated with greater complications in their health (Roy-Matton et al., Reference Roy-Matton, Moutquin, Brown, Carrier and Bell2011; Stark & Brinkley, Reference Stark and Brinkley2007), which would have a greater negative impact on high-risk pregnant women, since they already present severe pathologies. In this sense, it is very important to act jointly with health professionals and psychologists in the prevention and treatment of stress during pregnancy.

Surprisingly, psychological stress and cortisol showed different results. Despite the differences observed between the two groups in the levels of perceived stress, there were no differences in the concentration of maternal cortisol during pregnancy, which is in line with the results of a previous study, which found that the dysregulation of HPA is not related to the obstetric risk pregnancy (Romero-Gonzalez et al., Reference Romero-Gonzalez, Caparros-Gonzalez, Cruz-Martinez, Gonzalez-Perez, Gallego-Burgos and Peralta-Ramirez2020). In this line, different studies report that there is no consistent correlation between the concentration of cortisol in maternal hair and the self-reports that evaluate psychological stress, due to different methodological and pathophysiological factors (Musana et al., Reference Musana, Cohen, Kuppermann, Gerona, Wanyoro, Aguilar, Santos, Temmerman and Weiss2020; Mustonen et al., Reference Mustonen, Karlsson, Scheinin, Kortesluoma, Coimbra, Rodrigues and Karlsson2018; Stalder et al., Reference Stalder, Steudte-Schmiedgen, Alexander, Klucken, Vater, Wichmann, Kirschbaum and Miller2017). However, the importance of analyzing hair cortisol concentration, as a biomarker of chronic stress, lies in the possibility of finding differences at the physiological level that can affect fetal programing in different ways (Mustonen et al., Reference Mustonen, Karlsson, Scheinin, Kortesluoma, Coimbra, Rodrigues and Karlsson2018), although some studies state that the impact of prenatal stress on the offspring is not only influenced by maternal cortisol during pregnancy (Zijlmans et al., Reference Zijlmans, Korpela, Riksen-Walraven, De Vos and De Weerth2015).

Regarding maternal stress and the behavioral problems of the children at 24 months after delivery, no significant differences were found between the two groups in terms of total behaviors, although there were significant differences in internalizing and externalizing behaviors, with the high-risk group obtaining higher scores in child internalizing behaviors, whereas the low-risk group presented higher scores in child externalizing behaviors.

These findings are very interesting, as few studies have analyzed the effects of high-risk pregnancy on the behavior of preschool children. Some studies have identified that different maternal risk factors, including smoking during pregnancy and having gestational hypertension, which are associated with stress during pregnancy, are more strongly related to behavioral problems in early childhood than to obstetric results (Tearne et al., Reference Tearne, Allen, Herbison, Lawrence, Whitehouse, Sawyer and Robinson2015). These data indicate that it is very important to research the influence of the prenatal environment on the offspring.

A previous study detected a direct relationship between prenatal maternal stress and internalizing symptoms of 5-year-old children in a low-risk sample (Hentges et al., Reference Hentges, Graham, Plamondon, Tough and Madigan2019). These data are consistent with the findings of our study and show that women with high-risk pregnancy need specialized care, as they have higher levels of perceived stress and distress related to the pregnancy compared to women with low-risk pregnancy, which not only affects them but also their offspring.

Internalizing behaviors are characterized by presenting symptoms that develop toward the interior of the individual. In this sense, there are different biological factors (regulation of the HPA axis, thyroid function, and levels of oxytocin, prolactin, and progesterone) and socio-psychological factors (insufficient social support, unplanned pregnancy, lack of physical activity, and high stress level) that allow explaining the negative impact of pregnancy-related anxiety on the mother and her child (Mikolajkow & Małyszczak, Reference Mikolajkow and Małyszczak2022); moreover, this type of stress is associated with rumination behavior, particularly negative repetitive thoughts, which increase the anxiety (Hirsch et al., Reference Hirsch, Meeten, Gordon, Newby, Bick and Moulds2020).

Despite the above mentioned, these are controversial results, since a previous study reports that greater levels of perceived stress during pregnancy are related to greater externalizing behaviors in 27-month-old children (Gutteling et al., Reference Gutteling, De Weerth, Willemsen-Swinkels, Huizink, Mulder, Visser and Buitelaar2005), although the authors assert that this relationship between stress and behavioral problems was not very strong in the analyzed sample.

Regarding the analysis of maternal stress at 24 months after delivery, measured through the concentration of hair cortisol, it was found that this parameter was higher in the low-risk group than in the high-risk group, which in turn showed greater externalizing behaviors. Given the temporality of the samples, we could assert that the relationship between maternal stress and the behavior of the children is bidirectional, since the stress of the mothers may cause behavioral problems in their children and, in turn, this behavior may cause maternal stress (Amici et al., Reference Amici, Röder, Kiess, Borte, Zenclussen, Widdig and Herberth2022). Thus, the externalizing symptoms could explain the increase of maternal hair cortisol at 24 months after delivery. The externalizing behaviors of the children have a great impact on the environment, which can be highly stressful for the parents and guardians.

The dysregulation of the HPA axis and the increase of hair cortisol concentration can be due to the chronic exposure to stress (Stalder et al., Reference Stalder, Steudte-Schmiedgen, Alexander, Klucken, Vater, Wichmann, Kirschbaum and Miller2017). In this line, a study showed a relationship between the increase of maternal hair cortisol and how they interact with their children (Tarullo et al., Reference Tarullo, St. John and Meyer2017). Therefore, other variables could be influencing the behavior of the children, such as upbringing styles and marital stress, which have been associated with externalizing behaviors (Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Molnar, Eiden, Shisler, Zhao, Colder and Huestis2020; Steenhoff et al., Reference Steenhoff, Tharner and Væver2021; Van Eldik et al., Reference Van Eldik, Prinzie, Deković and De Haan2017).

Lastly, in regard with the neurodevelopment of the children, no significant differences were found at 24 months after delivery between the high-risk and low-risk groups. These findings differ from those reported by Romero-Gonzalez et al. (Reference Romero-Gonzalez, Caparros-Gonzalez, Cruz-Martinez, Gonzalez-Perez, Gallego-Burgos and Peralta-Ramirez2020), who obtained higher scores in motor, language, and cognitive development in the children of the high-risk group. This could be explained by the child age difference, as the mentioned authors analyzed a sample of 6-month-old babies, whose development may be more immediately and exclusively affected by the risk condition of pregnancy and dissipate in the long term, as was observed in our sample.

Other studies that explored the neurocognitive consequences of high-risk pregnancy due to chemotherapy treatment during gestation found no negative effects on the offspring, which is in line with the results of our study and demonstrates that, even in high-risk pregnancy, there are cases in which no maternal or fetal complications occur (Korakiti et al., Reference Korakiti, Zografos, Van Gerwen, Amant, Dimopoulos and Zagouri2020). Moreover, it is worth mentioning that high-risk pregnancy involves specialized medical care, which contributes to improving the health of these pregnant women to prevent complications, and this could favor the neurodevelopment of their children (Ringholm et al., Reference Ringholm, Damm and Mathiesen2019).

The present study provides data about the influence of high-risk pregnancy on the neurodevelopmental and behavioral problems of the offspring, compared with low-risk pregnancy. Since research in this topic is scarce, this study shows novel results. However, the main limitations of this work are related to the sample size, which was small and prevented us from delving into the types of pathologies that could explain our findings.

Future studies should expand the sample size and follow up the results in different ages, analyzing, in addition, the potential mediator role of different upbringing styles, the duration of breastfeeding and the attachment style in neurodevelopmental and behavioral problems of the offspring, as well as the different factors that may act as buffers of prenatal stress. Moreover, it would be even more interesting to include hospitalized women with high-risk pregnancy, since, as was observed, this condition is an additional source of prenatal stress. Considering the relation found between maternal hair cortisol at 24 months postpartum and child externalizing behavior, it would be interesting to assess this type of measure on the other parent, if any, to study the impact of both parents on the child’s behavior. Finally, another line of study could be to analyze the different symptoms of internalizing behavior present in children born from high-risk pregnancies and explore if these are related to other variables of interest mentioned above.

In conclusion, this study has important clinical implications, as it provides data that demonstrate the importance of taking care of pregnant women’s health, not only from the physical point of view, but also from the psychological perspective. It was found that the women with high-risk pregnancy had higher perceived stress and greater pregnancy-related distress than the women with low-risk pregnancy. Furthermore, the children of the high-risk women obtained higher scores in internalizing behaviors compared to those of the low-risk women, who had higher scores in externalizing behaviors. In addition, greater maternal hair cortisol concentrations at 24 months after delivery were found in the low-risk group.

These findings highlight the need for designing stress detection and prevention programs, from early stages of pregnancy, in order to improve the health of the mother and that of the future child, especially in high-risk pregnancy. It is also important for healthcare professionals to be sensitized with the psychological characteristics of the diagnosis of high-risk pregnancy and their impact on the mothers, which can reduce their stress levels, thereby benefiting the mothers and their offspring.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all of the women that participated in this study as well as the master interns that collaborated with its elaboration. This study is part of the doctoral thesis of Carolina Mariño-Narvaez.

Funding statement

This work makes part of the project I+D+i Ref. PID2019-110115 GB-I00 financed by the Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities and the State Research Agency 10.13039/501100011033.

Competing interests

None.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Amici, F., Röder, S., Kiess, W., Borte, M., Zenclussen, A. C., Widdig, A., & Herberth, G. (2022). Maternal stress, child behavior and the promotive role of older siblings. BMC Public Health, 22(1), 863. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-13261-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, D. (2007). The developmental origins of chronic adult disease. Acta Paediatrica, 93(s446), 2633. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2004.tb00236.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development: Administration manual (3rd ed.). Harcourt.Google Scholar
Brachetti, E., Ruperti, E., Irigoyen, S., & Brito, F. (2020). Efectos del estrés materno intenso y prolongado durante el embarazo y su repercusión sobre el neurodesarrollo del feto. Revista Ecuatoriana De Neurologia, 29(2), 2328. https://doi.org/10.46997/revecuatneurol29200023 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caparros-Gonzalez, R. A., Perra, O., Alderdice, F., Lynn, F., Lobel, M., García-García, I., & Peralta-Ramírez, M. I. (2019). Psychometric validation of the prenatal distress questionnaire (PDQ) in pregnant women in Spain. Women & Health, 59(8), 937952. https://doi.org/10.1080/03630242.2019.1584143 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y., He, Z., Chen, G., Liu, M., & Wang, H. (2019). Prenatal glucocorticoids exposure and fetal adrenal developmental programming. Toxicology, 428, 152308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tox.2019.152308 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404 JSTOR.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., Hankin, B. L., Glynn, L. M., Head, K., Kim, D. J., & Sandman, C. A. (2020). Prenatal maternal stress, child cortical thickness, and adolescent depressive symptoms. Child Development, 91(2), e432e450. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13252 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greff, M. J. E., Levine, J. M., Abuzgaia, A. M., Elzagallaai, A. A., Rieder, M. J., & Van Uum, S. H. M. (2019). Hair cortisol analysis: An update on methodological considerations and clinical applications. Clinical Biochemistry, 63, 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinbiochem.2018.09.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigoriadis, S., Graves, L., Peer, M., Mamisashvili, L., Tomlinson, G., Vigod, S. N., Dennis, C.-L., Steiner, M., Brown, C., Cheung, A., Dawson, H., Rector, N. A., Guenette, M., & Richter, M. (2018). Maternal anxiety during pregnancy and the association with adverse perinatal outcomes. Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 79(5), 17r12011. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.17r12011 Google ScholarPubMed
Gutteling, B. M., De Weerth, C., Willemsen-Swinkels, S. H. N., Huizink, A. C., Mulder, E. J. H., Visser, G. H. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2005). The effects of prenatal stress on temperament and problem behavior of 27-month-old toddlers. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 14(1), 4151. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-005-0435-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hentges, R. F., Graham, S. A., Plamondon, A., Tough, S., & Madigan, S. (2019). A developmental cascade from prenatal stress to child internalizing and externalizing problems. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 44(9), 10571067. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsch, C. R., Meeten, F., Gordon, C., Newby, J. M., Bick, D., & Moulds, M. L. (2020). Repetitive negative thinking and interpretation bias in pregnancy. Clinical Psychology in Europe, 2(4), e3615. https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.v2i4.3615 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holness, N. (2018). High-risk pregnancy. Nursing Clinics of North America, 53(2), 241251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cnur.2018.01.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isaacs, N. Z., & Andipatin, M. G. (2020). A systematic review regarding women’s emotional and psychological experiences of high-risk pregnancies. BMC Psychology, 8(1), 45. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-020-00410-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korakiti, A.-M., Zografos, E., Van Gerwen, M., Amant, F., Dimopoulos, M.-A., & Zagouri, F. (2020). Long-term neurodevelopmental outcome of children after in utero exposure to chemotherapy. Cancers, 12(12), 3623. https://doi.org/10.3390/cancers12123623 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krzeczkowski, J. E., Lau, A., Fitzpatrick, J., Tamana, S., Smithson, L., de Souza, R., Kozyrskyj, A. L., Lefebvre, D., Becker, A. B., Subbarao, P., Turvey, S. E., Pei, J., Schmidt, L. A., Sears, M. R., Van Lieshout, R. J., Mandhane, P. J., & the CHILD Study Investigators (2019). Maternal metabolic complications in pregnancy and offspring behavior problems at 2 Years of age. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 23(6), 746755. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-018-2691-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lautarescu, A., Craig, M. C., & Glover, V. (2020). Prenatal stress: Effects on fetal and child brain development. In International review of neurobiology. (vol. 150, pp. 1740). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.irn.2019.11.002 Google Scholar
Madigan, S., Oatley, H., Racine, N., Fearon, R. M. P., Schumacher, L., Akbari, E., Cooke, J. E., & Tarabulsy, G. M. (2018). A meta-analysis of maternal prenatal depression and anxiety on child socioemotional development. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(9), 645657.e8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marceau, K., Hajal, N., Leve, L. D., Reiss, D., Shaw, D. S., Ganiban, J. M., Mayes, L. C., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2013). Measurement and associations of pregnancy risk factors with genetic influences, postnatal environmental influences, and toddler behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37(4), 366375. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413489378 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCoyd, J. L. M., Curran, L., & Munch, S. (2020). They say, “If you don’t relax…You’re going to make something bad happen”: Women’s emotion management during medically high-risk pregnancy. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 44(1), 117129. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684319883199 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLean, M. A., Cobham, V. E., & Simcock, G. (2018). Prenatal maternal distress: A risk factor for child anxiety? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 21(2), 203223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-017-0251-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikolajkow, A., & Małyszczak, K. (2022). Biological factors and consequences of pregnancy-related anxiety – what do we know so far? Psychiatria Polska, 56(6), 12891314. https://doi.org/10.12740/PP/144138 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohammadi, S., Shojaei, K., Maraghi, E., & Motaghi, Z. (2022). Explaining the psychological distress of women with high-risk pregnancies in Iran: A qualitative study. Iranian Journal of Nursing and Midwifery Research, 27(6), 560566. https://doi.org/10.4103/ijnmr.ijnmr_321_21 Google ScholarPubMed
Monk, C., Lugo-Candelas, C., & Trumpff, C. (2019). Prenatal developmental origins of future psychopathology: Mechanisms and pathways. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15(1), 317344. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095539 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musana, J. W., Cohen, C. R., Kuppermann, M., Gerona, R., Wanyoro, A., Aguilar, D., Santos, N., Temmerman, M., & Weiss, S. J. (2020). Association of differential symptoms of stress to hair cortisol and cortisone concentrations among pregnant women in Kenya. Stress-the International Journal On The Biology of Stress, 23(5), 556566. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2019.1696305 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mustonen, P., Karlsson, L., Scheinin, N. M., Kortesluoma, S., Coimbra, B., Rodrigues, A. J., & Karlsson, H. (2018). Hair cortisol concentration (HCC) as a measure for prenatal psychological distress—A systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 92, 2128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.03.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NICHD (2020). ¿Qué es un embarazo de alto riesgo?. NICHD Español. https://espanol.nichd.nih.gov/salud/temas/pregnancy/informacion/riesgo Google Scholar
Ray, A. E., Jeffrey, K. N., Nair, P. H., Vu, Q. D., King, F., & Schmied, V. (2022). You’re a high-risk customer”: A qualitative study of women’s experiences of receiving information from health professionals regarding health problems or complications in pregnancy. Women and Birth, 35(5), e477e486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wombi.2021.12.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Remor, E. (2006). Psychometric properties of a european spanish version of the perceived stress scale PSS. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 9(1), 8693. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1138741600006004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ringholm, L., Damm, P., & Mathiesen, E. R. (2019). Improving pregnancy outcomes in women with diabetes mellitus: Modern management. Nature Reviews Endocrinology, 15(7), 406416. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41574-019-0197-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodrigues, P. B., Zambaldi, C. F., Cantilino, A., & Sougey, E. B. (2016). Special features of high-risk pregnancies as factors in development of mental distress: A review. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 38(3), 136140. https://doi.org/10.1590/2237-6089-2015-0067 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romero-Gonzalez, B., Caparros-Gonzalez, R. A., Cruz-Martinez, M., Gonzalez-Perez, R., Gallego-Burgos, J. C., & Peralta-Ramirez, M. I. (2020). Neurodevelopment of high and low-risk pregnancy babies at 6 months of age. Midwifery, 89, 102791. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.midw.2020.102791 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romero-Gonzalez, B., Caparros-Gonzalez, R. A., Gonzalez-Perez, R., Delgado-Puertas, P., & Peralta-Ramirez, M. I. (2018). Newborn infants’ hair cortisol levels reflect chronic maternal stress during pregnancy. PloS One, 13(7), e0200279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy-Matton, N., Moutquin, J. M., Brown, C., Carrier, N., & Bell, L. (2011). The impact of perceived maternal stress and other psychosocial risk factors on pregnancy complications. Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology Canada, 33(4), 344352. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1701-2163(16)34852-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanjuan, P. M., Fokas, K., Tonigan, J. S., Henry, M. C., Christian, K., Rodriguez, A., Larsen, J., Yonke, N., & Leeman, L. (2021). Prenatal maternal posttraumatic stress disorder as a risk factor for adverse birth weight and gestational age outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 295, 530540. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.08.079 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santelices, M. P., Irarrázaval, M., Jervis, P., Brotfeld, C., Cisterna, C., & Gallardo, A. M. (2021). Does maternal mental health and maternal stress affect preschoolers. Behavioral Symptoms? Children, 8(9), 816. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8090816 Google ScholarPubMed
Sauvé, B., Koren, G., Walsh, G., Tokmakejian, S., & Van Uum, S. H. (2007). Measurement of cortisol in human hair as a biomarker of systemic exposure. Clinical & Investigative Medicine, 30(5), 183. https://doi.org/10.25011/cim.v30i5.2894 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuetze, P., Molnar, D., Eiden, R. D., Shisler, S., Zhao, J., Colder, C. R., & Huestis, M. A. (2020). The effect of prenatal adversity on externalizing behaviors at 24 months of age in a high-risk sample: Maternal sensitivity as a moderator. Infant Mental Health Journal, 41(4), 530542. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21863 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shchurevska, O. D. (2022). Small baby syndrome, as a pregnancy-associted general adaptation syndrome (review). Wiadomości Lekarskie, 75(9), 21462151. https://doi.org/10.36740/WLek202209118 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stalder, T., & Kirschbaum, C. (2012). Analysis of cortisol in hair – state of the art and future directions. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 26(7), 10191029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2012.02.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stalder, T., Steudte-Schmiedgen, S., Alexander, N., Klucken, T., Vater, A., Wichmann, S., Kirschbaum, C., & Miller, R. (2017). Stress-related and basic determinants of hair cortisol in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 77, 261274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.12.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, M. A., & Brinkley, R. L. (2007). The relationship between perceived stress and health-promoting behaviors in high-risk pregnancy. The Journal of Perinatal & Neonatal Nursing, 21(4), 307314. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.JPN.0000299788.01420.6e CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steenhoff, T., Tharner, A., & Væver, M. S. (2021). Internalizing and externalizing problems in preschool children: The role of mothers’ and fathers’ observed parenting behavior in a well-resourced sample. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 62(3), 374385. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12724 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tarullo, A. R., St. John, A. M., & Meyer, J. S. (2017). Chronic stress in the mother-infant dyad: Maternal hair cortisol, infant salivary cortisol and interactional synchrony. Infant Behavior and Development, 47, 92102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.03.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tearne, J. E., Allen, K. L., Herbison, C. E., Lawrence, D., Whitehouse, A. J. O., Sawyer, M. G., & Robinson, M. (2015). The association between prenatal environment and children’s mental health trajectories from 2 to 14 years. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 24(9), 10151024. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-014-0651-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tien, J., Lewis, G. D., & Liu, J. (2020). Prenatal risk factors for internalizing and externalizing problems in childhood. World Journal of Pediatrics, 16(4), 341355. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12519-019-00319-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Den Bergh, B. R. H., Van Den Heuvel, M. I., Lahti, M., Braeken, M., De Rooij, S. R., Entringer, S., Hoyer, D., Roseboom, T., Räikkönen, K., King, S., & Schwab, M. (2020). Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: The influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 117, 2664. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Eldik, W. M., Prinzie, P., Deković, M., & De Haan, A. D. (2017). Longitudinal associations between marital stress and externalizing behavior: Does parental sense of competence mediate processes? Journal of Family Psychology, 31(4), 420430. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000282 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williamson, S. P., Moffitt, R. L., Broadbent, J., Neumann, D. L., & Hamblin, P. S. (2023). Coping, wellbeing, and psychopathology during high-risk pregnancy: A systematic review. Midwifery, 116, 103556. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.midw.2022.103556 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Medical Association (2013). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Jama, 310(20), 21912194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yali, A. M., & Lobel, M. (1999). Coping and distress in pregnancy: An investigation of medically high risk women. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & Gynecology, 20(1), 3952. https://doi.org/10.3109/01674829909075575 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zijlmans, M. A. C., Korpela, K., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., De Vos, W. M., & De Weerth, C. (2015). Maternal prenatal stress is associated with the infant intestinal microbiota. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 53, 233245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.01.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Differences in sociodemographic variables and obstetric information between high-risk and low-risk pregnant women

Figure 1

Table 2. Differences in maternal hair cortisol levels and psychological variables associated with pregnancy between high-risk and low-risk pregnancy

Figure 2

Table 3. Comparison between groups of maternal hair cortisol levels and scores on behavior and neurodevelopment