Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T01:41:13.442Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

John T. Jensen (2022). The Lexical and Metrical Phonology of English: The Legacy of The Sound Pattern of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pp. xv + 379.

Review products

John T. Jensen (2022). The Lexical and Metrical Phonology of English: The Legacy of The Sound Pattern of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pp. xv + 379.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 January 2024

Quentin Dabouis*
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Recherche sur le Langage (UR 999), Université Clermont Auvergne, Clermont-Ferrand, France. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Review
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

1 Overview of the volume

While there have been several books on the topic of English phonology, such as Burzio (Reference Burzio1994), Giegerich (Reference Giegerich1992) or Hammond (Reference Hammond1999), none of them covers all types of phonological processes, segmental and suprasegmental, and tries to unite them within a single theory, as Chomsky & Halle (Reference Chomsky and Halle1968) had done in their foundational The Sound Pattern of English (henceforth SPE). The last one being over 20 years old, it is more than welcome to have a new general book on the phonology of English.

In this review, I will first give a detailed overview of the book in §2 before turning to a more critical discussion in §3, in which I will argue that this book, as rich as it is in details and analyses, is disconnected from the empirical methods and theoretical discussions that have animated the field in the past 20 or 30 years.

2 Summary of the volume

Jensen’s volume gives a rich description of various facts regarding English phonology. The book is well-written and is quite easy to read. Many concepts are introduced using data from other languages, which broadens the scope of the book and shows how theoretical tools that are useful for English may also be useful for other languages.

The book begins with a brief preface giving the rationale for the book and how it relates to existing book-length works on English phonology, along with a word on the transcription system used throughout the book. The goal is ‘to give an internally consistent, coherent and complete analysis of the phonology (and morphology) of English’ (p. xi), and it is a considerably revised and expanded version of Jensen (Reference Jensen1993).

Then, the book carries on to Chapter 1 on ‘Theories of Phonology’, which starts with a discussion of generative phonology and SPE, introducing some of the key concepts of that framework, such as the use of underlying representations, of sequential rules and how those can be useful to deal with cases of phonological opacity. Five ‘principles of generative phonology’ are also presented (morphological uniqueness, predictability, naturalness, simplicity and preference for phonological solutions). The chapter then covers Metrical Phonology and grid theory, and Jensen argues in favour of adopting arboreal representations. The Prosodic Hierarchy is briefly introduced and will be the focus of Chapter 5. Then, Jensen discusses some of the key concepts of Lexical Phonology and Morphology, such as stratification, Structure Preservation and Strict Cyclicity. Those theories will be those on which Jensen bases his analysis. The chapter concludes with a brief presentation of ‘other post-SPE developments’, such as Autosegmental Phonology (tonal phenomena and Feature Geometry are discussed), Underspecification Theory and Optimality Theory.

Chapter 2 introduces the segmental system of English. The chapter begins with a discussion of the levels of representations used in Jensen’s analysis: the underlying, lexical, systematic phonetic and physical levels. Then, he proceeds with a description of English consonants and vowels using distinctive features (the classification of vowels at the underlying and lexical levels is nearly identical to that of Halle & Mohanan Reference Halle and Mohanan1985). The notable aspects of the analysis are that Jensen uses [±ATR] instead of SPE’s [±tense] (slightly differently though, as he assumes that certain [−ATR] vowels may be long), and the assumption that there are three possible unstressed vowels [ɪ], [ʊ] and [ə], but that all three may be stressed. Jensen also uses quite abstract underlying representations that are reminiscent of SPE (e.g., ‘lexical’ [eɪ̯] is assumed to derive from underlying //), although those representations are only posited to be different from surface representations if there are alternations or rules to derive the correct surface representations. For example, reduce is assumed to be /re + dʌk + ɛ/, where /e/ undergoes Vowel Shift to [iɪ̯] and the final /ɛ/ is needed to derive the surface [s] through Velar Softening (cf. redu[k]tion). Thus, underlying representations contain what cannot be predicted through lexical rules. The chapter concludes with a small section on orthography and phonology, in which Jensen seems to embrace SPE’s view that English orthography is close to optimal for the underlying level.

Chapter 3 deals with syllables and moras, starting with a definition of those two units and giving some evidence for the mora based on Japanese orthography. Then, Jensen evokes the absence of the syllable in SPE, which uses strong and weak clusters to capture generalisations about stress placement, and some early approaches to the syllable: the syllable boundary approach, the autosegmental approach, the constituent structure approach and the moraic approach. Jensen adopts the latter, and one originality is that he assumes that onsets are connected to the first mora of the syllable. Moreover, in his approach, a segment that is connected to two moras is strengthened, not lenited, and so he rejects ambisyllabicity. He also introduces several possible constraints on syllable structure taken from the existing literature, such as the Sonority Sequencing Generalization, the Linking Constraint and conditions on codas. The chapter continues with a description of the phonotactic patterns of onsets and codas in English. The last comparable description is Hammond (Reference Hammond1999), and so this is a welcome section, especially considering the distinction of ‘marginal’ clusters (although we are not told how this is determined).

Chapter 4 covers stress placement. Jensen uses a metrical approach using parameters, following Hayes (Reference Hayes1980, Reference Hayes1982, Reference Hayes1995). As opposed to most more recent analyses (see Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero2018a), Jensen does not consider the moraic trochee to be the basic foot structure in English. The stress generalisations that are described are close to identical to those developed in SPE, enriched with later developments such as extrametricality. The chapter carries on with destressing rules, which account for the distribution of reduced vowels and ‘trim back excess stresses’ assigned by the previous rules. Thus, a word like abracadabra is initially footed as (a)(braca)(dabra), and destressing and adjunction rules remove the middle foot and adjoin the two stray syllables to surrounding feet, yielding ((a)bra)(ca(dabra)). The chapter carries on with a discussion of the effects of cyclicity on stress. The absence of destressing in còndènsátion, elàstícity or the relative prominence in the first two syllables of sènsàtionálity are attributed to cyclic preservation from the bases condénse, elástic and sènsátional. Additionally, there is an original analysis assuming that suffixes that may be metrified on their own (e.g., -ate, -ize and -ation) do not necessarily cause remetrification in derived words. Thus, extrafenestral stress in words such as óxygenàte is assumed to be regular cyclic preservation from óxygen, while hỳdrógenàte is assumed to be derived from the root hydrogen-. This is different from approaches that assume a dual-affiliation of certain affixes to Strata 1 and 2 (e.g., Giegerich Reference Giegerich1999; Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero, Hannahs and Bosch2018b), and here no criteria are given to predict when a word will be derived from a stem or from a root. Such an approach is also at odds with traditional approaches to cyclicity, in which stress rules apply at each Stratum 1 cycle, possibly resulting in stress shifts. In those approaches, the preservation of primary stress (which Burzio Reference Burzio1994 calls ‘Strong Preservation’) is not the default case under Stratum 1 affixation, while ‘Weak Preservation’ (i.e., preservation of stress in the form of subsidiary stresses) may occur, but not systematically (see §3.1).

Chapter 5 deals with the Prosodic Hierarchy. Jensen starts by exposing the main assumptions of the theory: non-isomorphism between prosodic and morphosyntactic boundaries, the Strict Layer Hypothesis, the different types of branching (binary vs. n-ary) and the different types of rules (domain span, domain limit and domain juncture). He assumes eight prosodic categories: the mora, the syllable, the foot, the phonological word, the clitic group (although most prosodic phonologists have rejected it; see Scheer Reference Scheer2011: fn. 36), the phonological phrase, the intonation phrase and the phonological utterance. The rest of the chapter is an exemplification of prosodic categories using processes that refer to them, not all from English. A substantial amount of this chapter is taken from Nespor & Vogel (Reference Nespor and Vogel1986).

Chapter 6 deals both with a more detailed presentation of Lexical Phonology and with the ‘cyclic rules’, that is, the rules of Stratum 1. Jensen briefly presents the idea of stratification and begins with arguments taken over from Kiparsky (Reference Kiparsky, Hulst and Smith1982a, Reference Kiparsky1982b) on affixes that are sensitive to stress and zero derivation. However, we are not told why the former are relevant to the model: this was a key argument to show that some phonological computation must have taken place before certain morphological operations, thus showing the necessity of an interleaving of phonology and morphology. Then, Jensen describes the general properties of each stratum (e.g., cyclicity, Structure Preservation, the Strict Cycle Condition and lexical exceptions) and then proceeds with a discussion of several cyclic rules (Trisyllabic Laxing, Degemination and CiV Tensing) and of the Strict Cycle Condition. Recent approaches unify the fact that -ic Laxing is assumed to be an exception to Syllable Extrametricality (see, e.g., Bermúdez-Otero & McMahon Reference Bermúdez-Otero, McMahon and Trommer2006), but this connection is not mentioned here. The chapter concludes with how stress rules interact with cyclic segmental rules.

Chapter 7 addresses the rules posited to apply on Stratum 2. Those include rules that affect vowels (e.g., Vowel Shift, Diphthongisation and j-insertion) or consonants (e.g., Velar Softening, Palatalization and simplification of stem-final clusters involving a nasal). The chapter concludes with a word on the different types of rule ordering: (counter)bleeding and (counter)feeding.

The final chapter of the book covers ‘further issues in phonological theory’, and it opens with a discussion of umlaut and ablaut, which are eventually analysed as morphological rules. Then, following Kiparsky (Reference Kiparsky and Ingemann1983), Jensen rejects apparent violations of affix ordering and analyses bracketing paradoxes by allowing certain forms to be exceptions to Bracket Erasure. He also discusses the suffix -able, arguing that there are actually two suffixes, one on each lexical stratum, in line with Aronoff (Reference Aronoff1976). The last section is a short discussion of Optimality Theory, which is briefly presented and judged inappropriate to deal with the various generalisations detailed in the book, especially opacity.

3 Critical discussion of the volume

As can be seen from the summary in the previous section, Jensen covers a broad range of processes of English phonology, and some might argue that it is too broad and would assume that certain processes are actually not phonological (see, e.g., Szigetvári Reference Szigetvári2018 on vowel reduction or Kaye Reference Kaye, Durand and Katamba1995 on Velar Softening). The book is thus extremely rich and insightful, even for specialists of English phonology, although I am sceptical about its use with undergraduate students due to the complexities of the proposed analyses.

However, the book is mainly taking over and arranging examples and analyses from the early generative literature, and does not engage with most of the issues that have animated the discussions surrounding English phonology in recent years. This can be seen straightaway from the cited references: only 20/175 (11%) were published after 2000, and those are not discussed in as much detail as the literature of the 1960s–1980s is. In the following two subsections, I first detail the issues that I see with the data used in the book and then those regarding the theoretical model.

3.1 The empirical data

The main issue that I have with the data used in the book is that we are almost never told where the generalisations and the examples that illustrate them come from, carrying on a way of doing phonology by relying on examples taken from previous work and introspection. This was the case in most of the literature on English stress of the second half of the twentieth century, where the same examples are used repeatedly, including non-existing words such as hamamelidanthemum. Jensen continues with this tradition, and thus no quantification of the size of the classes and the efficiency of the rules is given. Exceptions are sometimes discussed, but we are not told whether a rule applies to few or many items (sometimes resulting in ‘rules’ that apply to as few as two words, in the case of the rule of Nasal Deletion given on p. 327). Variation is almost absent from the discussion, as illustrated, for example, by the cases of cyclic stress preservation in words such as orìginálity (with second-syllable stress arguably preserved from oríginal), ìnf[ə]rmátion vs. ìmp[ɔː]rtátion or elàstícity vs. àdaptátion (all derived from a base with second-syllable stress) which are known to be variable (Collie Reference Collie2007, Reference Collie2008; Kraska-Szlenk Reference Kraska-Szlenk2007; Dabouis Reference Dabouis2019; Dabouis & Fournier Reference Dabouis and Fournier2019). Certain points are also asserted without any supporting evidence, such as the assumption that there is no syllabification after Stratum 1, although there is evidence pointing to the contrary (Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero2007).

Moreover, it is unclear which variety of English is described throughout the book, although there are some references to specific varieties (mainly RP). Recent developments in English phonology are not discussed. For example, the increasing rates of antepenultimate stress observed among words with suffixes that are often classified as ‘stress-neutral’ such as -arily, -orily, -atory (British English) or -\=VCable (Trevian Reference Trevian2007; Arndt-Lappe & Hedia Reference Arndt-Lappe and Hedia2019), or the development of palatalisation in foot-initial position (e.g., tune [ˈtʃuːn], dune [ˈdʒuːn] and assume [əˈʃuːm]), which is said to be impossible (p. 295) but has been developing in recent years (Glain Reference Glain2012, Reference Glain and Lee2014), could have been mentioned.

Finally, in line with my general criticism about the disconnect between this book and the recent literature, many of the empirical facts that have been described and analysed in recent years are absent. The list cannot be exhaustive and so I will only mention four of those. First, Jensen, following SPE, assumes that stress placement in nouns obeys the following rule, sometimes called the Latin Stress Rule (here taken over from Moore-Cantwell Reference Moore-Cantwell2020) in (1).

While (1a) generally holds true (although that depends on the part of the lexicon considered; see Dabouis & Fournier Reference Dabouis, Fournier, Arigne and Rocq-Migette2022), (1b) has been shown to be riddled with exceptions, especially at the periphery of the lexicon (Pater Reference Pater1994). Recent work by Moore-Cantwell (Reference Moore-Cantwell2020) also reports that the nature of the final vowel interacts with the position of stress in nouns. Second, although there are quite extensive discussions of the behaviour of suffixed words, the book hardly mentions the prosodic and segmental characteristics of prefixes, especially productive ones. One footnote on p. 215 mentions that some have ‘phonological word status’, although there is a wide body of research on this (Booij & Rubach Reference Booij and Rubach1984; Wennerstrom Reference Wennerstrom1993; Raffelsiefen Reference Raffelsiefen, Alan Hall and Kleinhenz1999, Reference Raffelsiefen, Booij, Ducceschi, Fradin, Guevara, Ralli and Scalise2007). Related to this is the treatment that Jensen makes of the negative prefix in-. As in most works of early generative phonology, he assumes that it is a Stratum 1 prefix because of its assimilation behaviour, but Raffelsiefen (Reference Raffelsiefen, Alan Hall and Kleinhenz1999) shows that it has the properties of prefixes that form phonological words, which would be Stratum 2 in Jensen’s model. Those properties even include gemination (contrary to what Jensen claims on p. 254), which has been found for in- in all acoustic studies on nasal gemination (Kaye Reference Kaye2005; Oh & Redford Reference Oh and Redford2012; Oh Reference Oh2013; Ben Hedia & Plag Reference Ben Hedia and Plag2017; Yu Reference Yu2022), and even when it is realised as ir- (Dabouis et al. Reference Dabouis, Glain and Navarro2023). Third, there is no discussion of compound stress except through the general principles of Word Tree Construction (p. 172) in the context of the Rhythm Rule (pp. 213–216), even though there is ample recent literature on the issue that seeks to account for its variability (Giegerich Reference Giegerich2004, Reference Giegerich2009; Plag Reference Plag2010; Arndt-Lappe Reference Arndt-Lappe2011; Bell & Plag Reference Bell and Plag2012, Reference Bell and Plag2013). Finally, there is no mention of paradigmatic dependencies between words that do not stand in a relationship of containment. For example, Steriade (Reference Steriade, Bullock, Authier and Reed1999) attributes the stress-shifting behaviour of -able in remédiable (cf. its local base rémedy) to the existence of remédial, a behaviour that is distinct from párodiable (cf. párody), as there is no *paródial. This issue has been central in the debate between Lexical Conservatism (Steriade Reference Steriade, Bullock, Authier and Reed1999; Steriade & Stanton Reference Steriade and Stanton2020; Breiss Reference Breiss2021), which assumes that bases need not be contained within derivatives, and Stratal Phonology (Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero, Hannahs and Bosch2018b), which assumes that they must.

Thus, the book discusses a wealth of existing data on English phonology, and it certainly cannot deal with everything given the richness of the literature, but it would have been welcome for Jensen to engage more with some of these contemporary issues.

3.2 Theoretical issues

On the theoretical side, I will focus on two points that seem problematic to me: the treatment of the morphosyntax–phonology interface and the theory of Lexical Phonology used in the book.

It is well-established that, within modular frameworks, morphosyntactic information may be translated into the phonology in two ways: procedural and representational (Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero, Oostendorp, Ewen, Hume and Rice2011; Scheer Reference Scheer2011). In Jensen’s analysis, procedural morphosyntactic conditioning is achieved through strata and stratum-internal cyclicity, and representational morphosyntactic conditioning is achieved through the Prosodic Hierarchy. As Bermúdez-Otero (Reference Bermúdez-Otero, Oostendorp, Ewen, Hume and Rice2011) observes, ‘the uncertainty whether a particular instance of morphosyntactic conditioning in phonology should be analysed procedurally or representationally is in fact one of the most serious and recurrent obstacles faced by empirical research into the morphosyntax–phonology interface’, and there is disagreement on the criteria to do so (Raffelsiefen Reference Raffelsiefen, Downing, Alan Hall and Raffelsiefen2005; Bermúdez-Otero & Luís Reference Bermúdez-Otero and Luís2009). Here, Jensen does not say how strata and prosodic constituents interact. To take but one example, in Lexical Phonology, certain suffixes are attached on Stratum 2, but Jensen does not discuss how they should be prosodified, although there is debate on the issue (Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero, Oostendorp, Ewen, Hume and Rice2011). For example, should helpless be prosodified as in (2a) or as in (2b) (where ω° represents a minimal phonological word, ω $'$ represents a maximal phonological word and F represents a foot)? In a personal communication, Jensen instead assumes that the suffix would be adjoined to a higher foot projection, following the Strict Layer Hypothesis, as shown in (2c).

Moreover, the author uses two theoretical models which have been designed to make indirect reference to morphosyntactic information, but rules (20), (60), (63), (66) and (75) of Chapter 7 refer directly to word boundaries, which could be considered violations of modularity. Some of these cases could probably have been avoided by referring to the right edge of the phonological word, although some would require an analysis based on cyclic domains (see the discussion of stem-final clusters involving a nasal below).

Then, the version of Lexical Phonology that is used is mostly taken over from Kiparsky (Reference Kiparsky, Hulst and Smith1982a, Reference Kiparsky1982b) but, in 40 years, many limitations of that model have been identified, and there are still models that preserve a stratal architecture but with significant differences. More recent models typically assign names to strata that clearly indicate which morphosyntactic constituents are assigned to each stratum: the stem level, the word level and the phrase level. Here, Jensen does not state explicitly which units are assigned to each level and concepts like stem, root and word are not defined. Moreover, the assignation of an affix to a given stratum seems to be diacritic, and Giegerich’s (Reference Giegerich1999) claim that most affixes are dual-level and so stratal affiliation must be (at least partly) driven by the nature of bases, is rejected. Like most lexical phonologists, Jensen assigns a number of processes to Stratum 2 although their domain excludes Stratum 2 affixes. This is the case for the simplification of stem-final clusters involving a nasal (e.g., damning, signing and singer), which Jensen accounts for by assuming that the underlying clusters /mn/, /gn/ and /ng/ are simplified on Stratum 2 before a morphological bracket, in violation of modularity, as pointed out in the previous paragraph. Because of Bracket Erasure at the end of Stratum 1, damnation is assumed to have no internal bracket on Stratum 2 and does not undergo cluster simplification, while damning still has a bracket and therefore undergoes simplification. To avoid the modularity violation, one option would be to assume that the simplification of such clusters occurs at the end of a prosodic constituent, but, as no resyllabification is assumed to take place on Stratum 2 and as Jensen adopts a strict version of the Strict Layer Hypothesis, the difference would have to do with syllabification, an option that Jensen explicitly rejects. An alternative is then to use cyclic domains, and assume that this simplification occurs at the end of a phonological word at the stem level (Bermúdez-Otero & McMahon Reference Bermúdez-Otero, McMahon and Trommer2006; Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero, Oostendorp, Ewen, Hume and Rice2011). Incidentally, Jensen’s model cannot account for varieties of English in which such cluster simplification occurs word-finally but not stem-finally (e.g., singing [ˈsɪŋgɪŋ]). Finally, as mentioned above, more recent models such as Stratal Phonology have a far broader empirical coverage which includes up-to-date empirical observations such as what Bermúdez-Otero (Reference Bermúdez-Otero2013) calls ‘the stem-level syndrome’, the Russian Doll Theorem (Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero, Oostendorp, Ewen, Hume and Rice2011) or the life cycle of phonological processes (Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero, Honeybone and Salmons2015).

To conclude, Jensen’s book constitutes a rich collection of facts and analyses on English phonology, which makes it a useful read for advanced students of English phonology. It is a welcome contribution by the mere scope of the book, but it does not engage with the issues that have animated the field in recent years. One may get the impression that the author rejects or is unaware of most of the research that has been produced since the 1990s. For those reasons, I believe that this book is very rich, valuable and interesting but cannot be treated as current.

References

Arndt-Lappe, Sabine (2011). Towards an exemplar-based model of stress in English noun–noun compounds. JL 47. 549585.10.1017/S0022226711000028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arndt-Lappe, Sabine & Hedia, Sonia Ben (2019). Stress in the family: reconsidering stress preservation in English -ory adjectives. Poster presented at the 12th Mediterranean Morphology Meeting, Ljubljana, June 2019.Google Scholar
Aronoff, Mark (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bell, Melanie J. & Plag, Ingo (2012). Informativeness is a determinant of compound stress in English. JL 48. 485520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, Melanie J. & Plag, Ingo (2013). Informativity and analogy in English compound stress. Word Structure 6. 129155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben Hedia, Sonia & Plag, Ingo (2017). Gemination and degemination in English prefixation: phonetic evidence for morphological organization. JPh 62. 3449.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2007). Word-final prevocalic consonants in English: representation vs. derivation. Paper presented at the 4th Old World Conference in Phonology, University of the Aegean, Rhodes, January 2007.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2011). Cyclicity. In van Oostendorp, Marc, Ewen, Colin J., Hume, Elizabeth & Rice, Keren D. (eds.) The Blackwell companion to phonology, volume 4. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 20192048.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2013). The stem-level syndrome. Paper presented at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, April 2013. Handout available online at http://bermudez-otero.com/stemlevel.pdf.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2015). Amphichronic explanation and the life cycle of phonological processes. In Honeybone, Patrick & Salmons, Joseph (eds.) The Oxford handbook of historical phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 374399.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2018a). Diagnostics of the moraic trochee from Proto-Germanic to Present-Day English. Paper presented at the Workshop on the Foot in the Phonological History of English, ICEHL XX, Edinburgh, August 2018.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2018b). Stratal phonology. In Hannahs, S. J. & Bosch, Anna R. K. (eds.) The Routledge handbook of phonological theory. Abingdon: Routledge, 100134.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo & Luís, Ana R. (2009). Cyclic domains and prosodic spans in the phonology of European Portuguese functional morphs. Paper presented at the 6th Old World Conference in Phonology, University of Edinburgh, January 2009.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo & McMahon, April (2006). English phonology and morphology. In Trommer, Jochen (ed.) The handbook of English linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 382410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booij, Geert & Rubach, Jerzy (1984). Morphological and prosodic domains in lexical phonology. Phonology 1. 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breiss, Canaan (2021). Lexical Conservatism in phonology: theory, experiments, and computational modeling. PhD dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Burzio, Luigi (1994). Principles of English stress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam & Halle, Morris (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Collie, Sarah (2007). English stress preservation: a case for Stratal Optimality Theory. PhD dissertation, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Collie, Sarah (2008). English stress preservation: the case for ‘fake cyclicity’. English Language and Linguistics 12. 505532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dabouis, Quentin (2019). When accent preservation leads to clash. English Language and Linguistics 32. 363404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dabouis, Quentin & Fournier, Jean-Michel (2019). On the role of morphology, syllable structure, frequency and spelling in English vowel reduction. Paper presented at the 27th Manchester Phonology Meeting, June 2019.Google Scholar
Dabouis, Quentin & Fournier, Pierre (2022). English phonologies? In Arigne, Viviane & Rocq-Migette, Christiane (eds.) Modèles et modélisation en linguistique/Models and modelisation in linguistics. Brussels: Peter Lang, 215258.Google Scholar
Dabouis, Quentin, Glain, Olivier & Navarro, Sylvain (2023). Patterns of /r/ gemination in British and American English: a comparative study. Paper presented at the 16th PAC Conference, Université Paris Nanterre, April 2023.Google Scholar
Giegerich, Heinz J. (1992). English phonology: an introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giegerich, Heinz J. (1999). Lexical strata in English: morphological causes, phonological effects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511486470CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giegerich, Heinz J. (2004). Compound or phrase? English noun-plus-noun constructions and the stress criterion. English Language and Linguistics 8. 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giegerich, Heinz J. (2009). The English compound stress myth. Word Structure 2. 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glain, Olivier (2012). The yod /j/: palatalise it or drop it – how traditional yod forms are disappearing from Contemporary English. Cercles 22. 424.Google Scholar
Glain, Olivier (2014). Introducing contemporary palatalisation. In Lee, Theodora (ed.) Proceedings of the first Postgraduate and Academic Researchers in Linguistics at York (PARLAY 2013) conference. York: York Papers in Linguistics, 1629.Google Scholar
Halle, Morris & Mohanan, Karuvannur Puthanveettil (1985). Segmental phonology of Modern English. LI 16. 57116.Google Scholar
Hammond, Michael (1999). The phonology of English: a prosodic Optimality-Theoretic approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, Bruce (1980). A metrical theory of stress rules. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Hayes, Bruce (1982). Extrametricality and English stress. LI 13. 227276.Google Scholar
Hayes, Bruce (1995). Metrical stress theory: principles and case studies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Jensen, John T. (1993). English phonology. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, Alan S. (2005). Gemination in English. English Today 21. 4355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, Jonathan (1995). Derivations and interfaces. In Durand, Jacques & Katamba, Francis (eds.) Frontiers of phonology. London: Longman, 289332.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (1982a). From cyclic to Lexical Phonology. In van der Hulst, Harry & Smith, Norval (eds.) The structure of phonological representations. Dordrecht: Foris, 131176.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (1982b). Lexical morphology and phonology. In The Linguistic Society of Korea (ed.) Linguistics in the morning calm: selected papers from SICOL-1981. Seoul: Hanshin, 391.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (1983). Word-formation and the lexicon. In Ingemann, Frances A. (ed.) Proceedings of the 1982 Mid-America Linguistics Conference. Lawrence, KS: University of Kansas, 329.Google Scholar
Kraska-Szlenk, Iwona (2007). Analogy: the relation between lexicon and grammar. Munich: LINCOM Europa.Google Scholar
Moore-Cantwell, Claire (2020). Weight and final vowels in the English stress system. Phonology 37. 657695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nespor, Marina & Vogel, Irene (1986). Prosodic phonology. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Oh, Eunhae (2013). Effects of boundary strength on geminate duration in English. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology and Morphology 19. 457478.Google Scholar
Oh, Grace E. & Redford, Melissa A. (2012). The production and phonetic representation of fake geminates in English. JPh 40. 8291.Google ScholarPubMed
Pater, Joe (1994). Against the underlying specification of an exceptional English stress pattern. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 13. 95121.Google Scholar
Plag, Ingo (2010). Compound stress assignment by analogy: the constituent family bias. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 29. 243282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raffelsiefen, Renate (1999). Diagnostics for prosodic words revisited: the case of historically prefixed words in English. In Alan Hall, T. & Kleinhenz, Ursula (eds.) Studies on the phonological word. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 133201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raffelsiefen, Renate (2005). Paradigm uniformity effects versus boundary effects. In Downing, Laura J., Alan Hall, T. & Raffelsiefen, Renate (eds.) Paradigms in phonological theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 211262.Google Scholar
Raffelsiefen, Renate (2007). Morphological word structure in English and Swedish: the evidence from prosody. In Booij, Geert, Ducceschi, Luca, Fradin, Bernard, Guevara, Emiliano, Ralli, Angela & Scalise, Sergio (eds.) Proceedings of the fifth Mediterranean Morphology Meeting. Bologna: Università degli Studi di Bologna, 209268.Google Scholar
Scheer, Tobias (2011). A guide to morphosyntax–phonology interface theories: how extra-phonological information is treated in phonology since Trubetzkoy’s Grenzsignale. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Steriade, Donca (1999). Lexical conservatism in French adjectival liaison. In Bullock, Barbara E., Authier, Jean-Marc & Reed, Lisa A. (eds.) Formal perspectives in Romance linguistics. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 243270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steriade, Donca & Stanton, Juliet (2020). Productive pseudo-cyclicity and its significance. Paper presented at LabPhon 17, Vancouver, July 2020.Google Scholar
Szigetvári, Péter (2018). Stressed schwa in English. The Even Yearbook 13. 8195.Google Scholar
Trevian, Ives (2007). Stress-neutral endings in contemporary British English: an updated overview. Language Sciences 29. 426450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wennerstrom, Ann (1993). Focus on the prefix: evidence for word-internal prosodic words. Phonology 10. 309324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, Hye Jeong (2022). Geminate and singleton contrast in English affixed words. Phonetics and Speech Sciences 14. 6776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar