Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T17:35:14.917Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stress and diurnal cortisol among Latino/a college students: A multi-risk model approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 February 2023

Jeri Sasser*
Affiliation:
Arizona State University, Department of Psychology, Tempe, AZ, USA
Leah D. Doane
Affiliation:
Arizona State University, Department of Psychology, Tempe, AZ, USA
Jinni Su
Affiliation:
Arizona State University, Department of Psychology, Tempe, AZ, USA
Kevin J. Grimm
Affiliation:
Arizona State University, Department of Psychology, Tempe, AZ, USA
*
Corresponding author: Jeri Sasser, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The transition to college is a time of increased opportunity and stress spanning multiple domains. Adolescents who encounter significant stress during this transition may be vulnerable to adverse outcomes due to a “wear and tear” of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis. Latino/a students may be particularly at-risk for heightened stress exposure due to experiences of both minority-specific and general life stress. Despite this, little is known regarding the cumulative impact of multiple stressors on Latino/a students’ HPA axis functioning. The present study employed a “multi-risk model” approach to examine additive, common, and cumulative effects of multiple stress forms (general, academic, social, financial, bicultural, ethnic/racial discrimination) on diurnal cortisol in a sample of first-year Latino/a college students (N = 196; 64.4% female; Mage = 18.95). Results indicated that no stress forms were additively associated with the cortisol awakening response (CAR), but general stress was associated with a flatter diurnal cortisol slope (DCS) and bicultural stress was linked with a steeper DCS. A college stress latent factor was associated with a lower CAR, whereas a latent factor of discrimination was not associated with diurnal cortisol. Cumulative risk was linked with a lower CAR. Findings highlight the physiological correlates of various stressors experienced by Latino/a college students.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

The transition to college is a time of opportunity and stress, consisting of both developmental (e.g., into emerging adulthood) and ecological change (e.g., into new social contexts; Seidman & French, Reference Seidman and French2004). Adolescents who encounter significant stress during the transition to college may be particularly at-risk for adverse outcomes, due to repeated activation of stress-sensitive physiological systems. Evidence suggests that Latino/aFootnote 1 students may encounter greater stress during the transition to college, as minority youth often experience cumulative minority-specific and general life stress (Phinney & Haas, Reference Phinney and Haas2003; Wei et al., Reference Wei, Ku and Liao2011). Importantly, Hispanic/Latino students represent the largest ethnic/racial minority group in higher education (McFarland et al., Reference McFarland, Hussar, de Brey, Snyder, Wang, Wilkinson-Flicker, Gebrekristos, Zhang, Rathbun, Barmer, Bullock Mann and Hinz2017), but are also the minority group least likely to graduate from a 4-year institution (Snyder et al., Reference Snyder, de Brey and Dillow2019). Alterations in typical stress responsive systems may serve as potential mechanisms underlying the development of mental and physical health problems, including anxiety disorders (Adam et al., Reference Adam, Vrshek-Schallhorn, Kendall, Mineka, Zinbarg and Craske2014), major depressive disorder (Adam et al., Reference Adam, Doane, Zinbarg, Mineka, Craske and Griffith2010; Zajkowska et al., Reference Zajkowska, Gullett, Walsh, Zonca, Pedersen, Souza, Kieling, Fisher, Kohrt and Mondelli2022), inflammation (Adam et al., Reference Adam, Quinn, Tavernier, McQuillan, Dahlke and Gilbert2017), and cardiovascular disease (Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Schwartz, Cohen and Seeman2006). However, there is a lack of research disentangling the effects of different types of stress simultaneously (i.e., alongside each other) on physiological outcomes in Latino/a college students. Thus, the proposed study will investigate multiple forms of stress (e.g., general, college-related, minority-specific) as they relate to Latino/a college students’ diurnal cortisol modeled via three-level growth curve models to best capture the diurnal pattern of cortisol across moments, days, and individuals. Further, in an effort to disentangle the complex associations between stress and cortisol, a multi-risk model approach will be implemented to best characterize perceptions of stressFootnote 2 during the first year of college.

Theories of stress, neurobiology, and health

Allostatic load theory

A prominent theory in the examination of stress exposure as it relates to biological functioning is allostatic load (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998). Allostasis refers to the body’s adjustment of biological responses to meet the demands of acute stressors (e.g., psychosocial stress; McEwen, Reference McEwen1998). Whereas this process is adaptive under normal levels of stress exposure, individuals who undergo chronic and/or ongoing perceptions of stress often experience the cumulative “wear and tear” of the body’s biological systems, wherein the overactivation of these systems results in diminished biological functioning (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998). The hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis is a biological system that is particularly influenced by psychosocial stress and is hypothesized to be a key physiological mechanism in allostatic load (McEwen & Seeman, Reference McEwen and Seeman1999). Similar to allostasis, immediate activation of the HPA axis in response to acute stress is adaptive in the short-term (i.e., release of cortisol helps the body manage stress), however, chronic activation of the HPA axis can cause a counterregulatory response in which chronically high cortisol levels (e.g., hypercortisolism) begin to drop below normal due to desensitization (e.g., hypocortisolism; Adam, Reference Adam2012; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007). Both hypercortisolism and hypocortisolism have been linked to numerous negative outcomes, including risk for mental and physical health problems (Adam et al., Reference Adam, Quinn, Tavernier, McQuillan, Dahlke and Gilbert2017). Thus, it is paramount that research examines predictors of HPA axis functioning among populations that are known to experience high levels of chronic and/or cumulative stress, such as minority students transitioning to college.

Cultural neurobiology framework

Emerging research has called for the need to examine how various aspects of culture, including culturally related stress, may affect biological systems (Causadias et al., Reference Causadias, Telzer and Lee2017). The cultural neurobiology framework posits that cultural processes can influence ethnic/racial minority youths’ neurobiological stress systems, including the HPA axis, across multiple time frames (e.g., moment, daily, yearly; Doane et al., Reference Doane, Sladek, Adam, Causadias, Telzer and Gonzales2018a). This framework also recognizes the discrete and often ongoing stressors that are experienced by minoritized groups in the United States (e.g., discrimination, acculturative stress), which may underlie ethnic/racial differences in stress biology and health status due to cumulative vulnerability (Myers, Reference Myers2009). Chronic stress, including age and ethnicity-related stressors, have been linked to an increased risk for numerous adverse outcomes in ethnic/racial minority college student and adult samples, including depressive symptom severity (Arbona & Jiménez, Reference Arbona and Jiménez2014; Wei et al., Reference Wei, Liao, Chao, Mallinckrodt, Tsai and Botello-Zamarron2010), poorer perceived physical health (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Wilborn and Mangold2017), and risk for diabetes, hypertension, smoking, and coronary heart disease (Gallo et al., Reference Gallo, Roesch, Fortmann, Carnethon, Penedo, Perreira, Birnbaum-Weitzman, Wassertheil-Smoller, Castañeda, Talavera, Sotres-Alvarez, Daviglus, Schneiderman and Isasi2014). Importantly, individuals who experience the cumulative effects of multiple stressors may be particularly vulnerable to later negative health outcomes. Given that ethnic/racial minority adolescents often experience the chronic burden of minority-specific stressors in addition to the typical stressors associated with college (e.g., Arbona & Jiménez, Reference Arbona and Jiménez2014; Wei et al., Reference Wei, Liao, Chao, Mallinckrodt, Tsai and Botello-Zamarron2010), it is plausible that Latino/a college students may experience diminished HPA axis activity as a result of cumulative stress.

Stress and the HPA axis

The HPA axis is one of the body’s major stress response systems that, once activated, leads to the eventual release of cortisol, a hormone that helps the body manage stress and affects biological processes essential for daily functioning (e.g., heart rate, metabolism; Kirschbaum & Hellhammer, Reference Kirschbaum, Hellhammer and Fink2000). Whereas prior research has frequently focused on the immediate response to psychosocial stress (e.g., cortisol reactivity; Dickerson & Kemeny, Reference Dickerson and Kemeny2004), the chronic effects of stress may be better understood by the diurnal activity of the HPA axis (Adam, Reference Adam2006; Adam, Reference Adam2012). Cortisol follows a typical diurnal rhythm, with high levels upon waking, a 50%–65% increase approximately 30 min after waking (cortisol awakening response (CAR); Stalder et al., Reference Stalder, Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Adam, Pruessner, Wüst, Dockray, Smyth, Evans, Hellhammer, Miller, Wetherell, Lupien and Clow2016), and an overall decrease throughout the day with lowest levels at midnight (Adam & Kumari, Reference Adam and Kumari2009). Two parameters commonly used to characterize the diurnal pattern are the diurnal cortisol slope (DCS; decline in cortisol levels across the day) and the CAR.

There is robust evidence that chronic stress is associated with a flatter DCS (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007), a pattern that results from deviations below or above the typical diurnal rhythm (e.g., low cortisol in the morning and/or high cortisol in the morning and evening) and that is linked with numerous adverse health outcomes in adolescents and adults (for review see, Adam et al., Reference Adam, Quinn, Tavernier, McQuillan, Dahlke and Gilbert2017). In contrast, findings regarding the CAR have been more inconsistent (Clow et al., Reference Clow, Thorn, Evans and Hucklebridge2004), with chronic stress linked to both an increase and decline in cortisol output (e.g., Chida & Steptoe, Reference Chida and Steptoe2009). It has been hypothesized that a higher than normal CAR can be adaptive in the short-term by helping individuals prepare to meet the demands of the day (e.g., “boost hypothesis”; Adam et al., Reference Adam2006); however, when these short-term elevations are chronically experienced, a heightened CAR can confer risk for negative outcomes (e.g., major depressive disorder; Adam et al., Reference Adam, Doane, Zinbarg, Mineka, Craske and Griffith2010). Conversely, a lower CAR may reflect HPA axis dysregulation resulting from prior overactivation (i.e., due to exhaustion of physiological systems) and has been associated with conditions such as fatigue and burnout (Chida & Steptoe, Reference Chida and Steptoe2009; Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, Hellhammer and Kirschbaum1999).

Type of stressor

Specificity hypotheses suggest that different types of stress may play a large role in determining how HPA axis activity is impacted (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007). Specifically, different forms of stress often require discrete adaptational demands that are differentially regulated via the HPA axis, pointing to a need to examine multiple types of stress. For example, general life stress has been frequently associated with a heightened CAR (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Margolin, Shapiro and Timmons2017; Morin-Major et al., Reference Morin-Major, Marin, Durand, Wan, Juster and Lupien2016), which may be indicative of metabolic support that prepares adolescents to cope with these daily stressors. In addition, a large body of evidence suggests that social stressors (i.e., that pose social threat to self) are particularly influential on HPA axis reactivity (Dickerson & Kemeny, Reference Dickerson and Kemeny2004), and may also have implications for diurnal patterns. Indeed, prior work found that positive perceptions of family relationships in adolescence were associated with higher waking cortisol and a steeper DCS in young adulthood (Shirtcliff et al., Reference Shirtcliff, Skinner, Obasi and Haggerty2017). Among early adolescents, greater average peer problems were linked to a flatter DCS, whereas day-to-day increases in peer or academic problems were linked with greater morning cortisol (Bai et al., Reference Bai, Robles, Reynolds and Repetti2017). Notably, one study observed that first-year graduate students’ CAR was flatter in the Spring, as opposed to the beginning of classes in Fall, whereas CAR levels remained stable for community comparisons (McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Murphy, Albano and Ceballos2016). This may suggest that first-year graduate students faced stressors unique to that context that were linked with changes in diurnal cortisol patterns, which may also occur for undergraduate students.

Importantly, a growing body of evidence suggests that minority-specific stressors, or additional forms of stress that are unique to members of marginalized communities, are associated with biological functioning (Doane et al., Reference Doane, Sladek, Adam, Causadias, Telzer and Gonzales2018a; Flentje et al., Reference Flentje, Heck, Brennan and Meyer2019; Myers, Reference Myers2009). Currently, the most explored stressor to explain ethnic/racial disparities in physiological stress systems are experiences of discrimination or mistreatment (Huynh et al., Reference Huynh, Guan, Almeida, McCreath and Fuligni2016; Skinner et al., Reference Skinner, Shirtcliff, Haggerty, Coe and Catalano2011; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Hoyt and Adam2014; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Landor, Flores and Brown2018). Indeed, numerous studies have examined the physiological correlates of discrimination experiences in adolescence and young adulthood, among them being greater overall cortisol output, lower waking cortisol, and a flatter DCS (Huynh et al., Reference Huynh, Guan, Almeida, McCreath and Fuligni2016; Skinner et al., Reference Skinner, Shirtcliff, Haggerty, Coe and Catalano2011; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Doane and Roosa2012; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Hoyt and Adam2014). In addition, more subtle forms of discrimination (e.g., microaggressions) have been linked with increased overall cortisol output (e.g., AUCg) among Latino and African American young adults (Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Landor, Flores and Brown2018).

Notably, less work has been done examining stressors that relate to cultural adaptation and diurnal cortisol among Latino/a adolescents. A recent study by Gonzales et al. (Reference Gonzales, Johnson, Shirtcliff, Tein, Eskenazi and Deardorff2018) found that youth who reported higher biculturalism (i.e., high on both Anglo and Mexican orientations) exhibited greater cortisol reactivity. Similarly, higher levels of acculturation (e.g., greater Anglo orientation) were linked with a smaller CAR in a sample of Mexican-American adults (Mangold et al., Reference Mangold, Wand, Javors and Mintz2010). Further, in a sample of Latino late adolescents, Doane et al. (Reference Doane, Sladek, Breitenstein, Park, Castro and Kennedy2018b) found that daily family assistance behaviors (e.g., helping sibling/family, translating for family member) were associated with lower waking cortisol and a flatter DCS the next day. Collectively, these studies suggest that there may be evidence for links between other culturally specific stressors (e.g., bicultural stress) and diurnal cortisol. However, to date, no known study has examined the impact of cultural stressors on diurnal cortisol patterns alongside general and college-specific stressors. This is an important gap in the literature, as emerging work suggests that a better understanding of youths’ physiological stress processes may help address educational inequalities (Obradović & Armstrong-Carter, Reference Obradović and Armstrong-Carter2021), which are particularly evident in higher education (Snyder et al., Reference Snyder, de Brey and Dillow2019). For example, the stress disparity model contends that chronic stress can result in deviations in HPA axis activity, including a lower CAR and a flatter DCS, which in turn negatively affects students’ ability to learn (e.g., trouble focusing, worse cognitive performance; Heissel et al., Reference Heissel, Levy and Adam2017). Thus, the present study also examined the combined impact of multiple stress types in an effort to better capture chronic stress experiences during this time.

Timing of stressor

The time frame, or recency, in which stress is measured (e.g., past year, past month, past week) is also critical to the examination of diurnal cortisol, given that a chronic activation of the HPA axis, often characterized by hypercortisolism, can eventually result in diminished HPA axis activity (i.e., hypocortisolism; Adam, Reference Adam2012; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007). In their seminal meta-analysis of chronic stress and the HPA axis, Miller et al. (Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007) found that time was negatively associated with HPA axis activity (i.e., the longer it had been since the stressor emerged, the lower an individuals’ morning and daily cortisol volume), whereas current experiences of stress were associated with greater morning and daily cortisol output. In the context of college life, it may be expected that recent, developmentally salient stressors (e.g., academic, financial stress) would be positively linked with HPA axis activity, whereas stressors that are often present in ethnic/racial minority youths’ lives prior to college (e.g., discrimination) may be negatively associated with diurnal cortisol. Thus, future research is needed that examines multiple forms of stress that differ in both type (e.g., nature, controllability) and timing (e.g., past semester vs. past week).

A multi-risk model approach

There is prior evidence in adolescent and young adult populations supporting that general life stress (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Margolin, Shapiro and Timmons2017; Morin-Major et al., Reference Morin-Major, Marin, Durand, Wan, Juster and Lupien2016), academic-related stress (Bai et al., Reference Bai, Robles, Reynolds and Repetti2017; McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Murphy, Albano and Ceballos2016), and minority-specific stressors (Skinner et al., Reference Skinner, Shirtcliff, Haggerty, Coe and Catalano2011; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Hoyt and Adam2014) are each implicated in physiological stress activity. However, less is known about the additive influence of these stressors; that is, the impact of specific forms of stress when accounting for other forms. Previous work provides support for the unique effects of minority- and college-related stressors on Latino/a students’ mental health. Specifically, studies found that minority-specific stressors (e.g., college climate, discrimination, intra-ethnic pressures) and general college stress were uniquely associated with depressive symptoms in minority (Wei et al., Reference Wei, Liao, Chao, Mallinckrodt, Tsai and Botello-Zamarron2010) and Latino/a college students (Arbona & Jiménez, Reference Arbona and Jiménez2014; Arbona et al., Reference Arbona, Fan and Olvera2018). Yet, no known study has examined the unique effects of minority-specific stressors alongside other forms of life stress (e.g., general, academic, social) on diurnal cortisol patterns. This is a critical gap in the literature, as these findings may help identify specific forms of stress that are particularly influential on Latino/a college students’ physiological functioning, which may explain, or mediate, associations between stress and mental health.

In addition to the additive effects of stress, it is also meaningful to consider the potential common (i.e., shared) contributions of multiple stress forms. To date, no known studies have modeled different types of stress as an unobserved latent variable predicting physiological functioning. However, a recent study compared alternative methods to assess cumulative risk, among them being latent factor analysis, and found important similarities and differences in predicting outcomes (i.e., children’s externalizing problems; Ettekal et al., Reference Ettekal, Eiden, Nickerson and Schuetze2019), highlighting the importance of testing competing models. Characterizing stress as a latent variable provides the utility of examining what is in common among discrete forms of stress, while also potentially providing an indirect measure of the individuals’ perceptions of stress (i.e., individual differences in stress perceptions). Thus, comparing results of the common (latent) model to the additive effects may provide meaningful information regarding whether HPA axis functioning is a result of something potentially “trait-like” (i.e., how a person perceives stress in general), rather than stressor-specific effects.

Importantly, Latino/a college students may also endure the cumulative impact of multiple sources of stress. Consistent with allostatic load (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998), the accumulation of multiple stressors may be particularly deleterious for HPA axis functioning due to the “wear and tear” that results from chronic activation of biological systems. Although no studies have examined cumulative stress and physiological outcomes among Latino/a college students, existing research suggests that cumulative stress negatively impacts HPA axis functioning among minority groups (Kwak et al., Reference Kwak, Taylor, Anaya, Feng, Evich and Jones2017; Suglia et al., Reference Suglia, Staudenmayer, Cohen, Enlow, Rich-Edwards and Wright2010). For example, Suglia et al. (Reference Suglia, Staudenmayer, Cohen, Enlow, Rich-Edwards and Wright2010) found that cumulative stress (e.g., discrimination, negative life events, community violence) was associated with lower morning cortisol and a flatter DCS among Black, but not Hispanic, pregnant women, and Kwak et al. (Reference Kwak, Taylor, Anaya, Feng, Evich and Jones2017) found that Latino adolescents who reported greater cumulative family stress (e.g., financial, career, relationships, prejudice) had a lower CAR than those who endorsed low cumulative stress. Further, a longitudinal analysis of African American adolescents found that perceptions of discrimination that were high and stable across adolescence (e.g., ages 16 to 18) predicted higher levels of allostatic load (e.g., cortisol, blood pressure, BMI) in young adulthood (Brody et al., Reference Brody, Lei, Chae, Yu, Kogan and Beach2014). Although these studies do not generalize to the demographics of the current study, they provide general evidence for the link between cumulative stress and HPA axis functioning.

The present study

The present study harnessed the strengths of an ethnically homogenous design to identify within-group differences (i.e., meaningful variability) in stress perceptions and physiological stress processes among Latino/a college students (García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Crnic, Lamberty, Wasik, Jenkins, Garcia and McAdoo1996). Specifically, to untangle the complex associations between stress and HPA axis functioning, we implemented a “multi-risk model” approach that characterized stress in three different ways. First, we examined the independent effects of stress by entering all stress forms into the model together (additive model), the common and/or “shared” influence of stress, measured by estimating underlying latent factor(s) of stress (common model), and the cumulative impact of experiencing multiple stressors to a high degree (cumulative model; see Figure 1). Given that ethnic/racial minorities are known to experience multiple sources of stress during college (e.g., Arbona et al., Reference Arbona, Fan and Olvera2018; Wei et al., Reference Wei, Liao, Chao, Mallinckrodt, Tsai and Botello-Zamarron2010), this multi-risk model approach sought to identify whether the common and/or cumulative effects of multiple forms of stress provide important information about physiological stress processes, separate from what can be observed by the additive effects of stress. We hypothesized that each form of stress would predict a flatter DCS (Hypothesis 1a). Further, based on previous findings (Bai et al., Reference Bai, Robles, Reynolds and Repetti2017; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Margolin, Shapiro and Timmons2017; Skinner et al, Reference Skinner, Shirtcliff, Haggerty, Coe and Catalano2011; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Hoyt and Adam2014), we predicted that general, social, academic, financial, and bicultural stress would be associated with an increased CAR, while discrimination would be linked with a reduced CAR (Hypothesis 1b). Due to a lack of previous research examining stress as a latent variable, hypotheses regarding the common effects of stress on diurnal cortisol were exploratory (i.e., nondirectional). Lastly, in line with prior findings (Kwak et al., Reference Kwak, Taylor, Anaya, Feng, Evich and Jones2017; Suglia et al., Reference Suglia, Staudenmayer, Cohen, Enlow, Rich-Edwards and Wright2010), we hypothesized that cumulative stress would be associated with a flatter DCS (Hypothesis 2a) and a reduced CAR (Hypothesis 2b).

Figure 1. Conceptual representation of the “multi-risk model” approach.

Method

Participants

209 Hispanic/Latino/a adolescents (M age = 18.10, SD = 0.41; 64.4% female) were recruited during the spring or summer of their senior year in high school (T1; Spring, 2017) prior to enrolling at a large, ethnically/racially diverse (49.6% White, 22.8% Hispanic or Latino/a, 6.9% Asian, 4.3% Black or African American) university in the southwestern United States, as part of a longitudinal, multi-method study of Latino/a adolescents transitioning to college (see Doane et al., Reference Doane, Sladek, Breitenstein, Park, Castro and Kennedy2018b). This study utilized data from Spring 2018 (T3; N = 196; M age = 18.95, SD = .40), when the majority of participants were in their second semester of college (n = 194 enrolled in college). Participants were recruited through university orientation sessions, as well as via email, text messaging, and phone conversations in English or Spanish. Inclusion criteria required that participants: (1) had gained acceptance to the focal university and had paid an initial deposit or selected to defer payment; (2) were current seniors in high school; (3) identified as Hispanic/Latino/a; and (4) lived within 60 miles of the university when they were recruited. All participants identified broadly as Hispanic or Latino/a, with the majority of participants specifically identifying as being of Mexican descent (85.1%), followed by South or Central American (10.1%), Cuban (5.3%), and Other (4.3%) descent. Most participants (84.2%) identified as non-White Latino/a; 15.8% identified as White Latino/a. Eleven percent (10.6%) of participants were first-generation immigrants (born outside the United States), 62.0% were second generation (born in United States with at least one parent born outside the United States), and 27.4% were third generation or greater (both parents born in the United States). With regards to sexual orientation, most of our sample identified as straight (86.5%), followed by bisexual (7.2%), lesbian/gay (3.4%), unsure (1.4%), and other (1.4%). Thirty-four percent (33.7%) of the sample reported that their parents had attained less than a high school degree, 21.6% of parents earned a high school degree or GED, 22.8% of parents completed some college, 15.9% of parents had obtained a Bachelor’s degree, and 3.8% reported that their parents had a graduate education. Participants’ subjective social class was as follows: 12.6% working class, 28.2% lower-middle class, 48.5% middle class, 10.2% upper-middle class, and 0.5% upper class. More information on sample recruitment and demographics can be found in Doane et al. (Reference Doane, Sladek, Breitenstein, Park, Castro and Kennedy2018b).

Procedure

The university’s Institutional Review Board approved all procedures before data collection began. Informed consent and assent (i.e., for participants under the age of 18) were obtained from all participants prior to beginning study procedures. Study personnel traveled to participants’ homes or hosted participants in a campus lab to deliver study materials, collect survey responses, and provide instructions for saliva sampling and daily diary procedures. Participants also completed an online battery of survey measures at a time of their convenience during the semester, which included measures of demographic information, cultural values, emotional health, and stress experiences.

During the week following the home and/or lab visit, participants wore wrist-based accelerometers (e.g., actigraphy watch) to assess objective sleep throughout the week (M nights = 6.49, SD = .91) and completed 4–5 diary entries per day, across 7 days (M = 26.20, SD = 3.98). Participants also provided saliva samples via passive drool 5 times a day for 3 weekdays: immediately upon waking, 30 min after waking, twice during the day (approx. 2 hr and 8 hr from initial waking sample, to avoid mealtimes), and at bedtime. Thus, participants were asked to complete 15 saliva samples in total (i.e., 5 samples per day across 3-day cortisol protocol; M = 14.88, SD = .95). Within our analytic sample of 180 participants, there were a total of 2,667 cortisol samples. Participants were asked not to eat, drink, or brush their teeth an hour prior to saliva sampling. Participants recorded the date and time of each sample, but also used a track cap compliance device (MEMS 6TM (Aardex)) to objectively record the sample time upon track cap opening. Participants were instructed to press a button on the actigraphy watches each time they had completed a saliva sample or a daily diary entry (i.e., as secondary indicators). After providing each saliva sample, participants completed brief diary entries using web-based smartphones that assessed questions about stressors experienced in the last hour or across the day (e.g., bicultural stress). Further, participants reported whether they had recently eaten, exercised, used caffeine, nicotine, medication, slept, or experienced pain (i.e., as potential covariates in cortisol analysis).

Compliance with saliva sampling procedures for the waking and post-30 min waking sample was determined via participants recordings of time on vials, track cap device times, actigraphy-recorded times, and daily diary times. Because noncompliance with saliva sampling procedures can bias cortisol estimates (Kudielka et al., Reference Kudielka, Broderick and Kirschbaum2003; Stalder et al., Reference Stalder, Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Adam, Pruessner, Wüst, Dockray, Smyth, Evans, Hellhammer, Miller, Wetherell, Lupien and Clow2016), each indicator of time was carefully inspected to determine “compliant” versus “noncompliant” saliva samples (Doane & Zeiders, Reference Doane and Zeiders2014). Criteria for compliance are as follows: For the waking sample: track cap detected times were within 15 min of participants’ actigraphy-recorded times (87.9% of samples with complete compliance data; 75% of all waking samples); for the second (post 30-min waking) sample: track cap detected times were within 25 to 45 min after track cap detected times of waking sample times (85.7% of samples with valid track cap data; 82.9% of valid samples of all second samples). Additionally, these rates required that actigraphy or track cap data were available for samples to be considered compliant, resulting in noncompliance if this data were missing. Lastly, to avoid biased estimates of DCS and CAR (see Stalder et al., Reference Stalder, Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Adam, Pruessner, Wüst, Dockray, Smyth, Evans, Hellhammer, Miller, Wetherell, Lupien and Clow2016), cortisol values from noncompliant samples were treated as missing data in final analyses (4.9% of all samples).

Measures

Salivary cortisol

Salivary cortisol was assessed at T3. Participants were instructed to store their completed saliva samples in their refrigerator until study personnel retrieved the samples to return them to the lab (typically 4 days later). Samples were stored at -80 degrees Celsius, per existing recommendations (Nicolson, Reference Nicolson, Luecken and Gallo2008). Once the study was completed and all saliva samples had been retrieved, they were placed on dry ice and transported via courier across 3 days to the Biochemisches Labor at the University of Trier in Germany for assay. This is the preferred method for handling and transporting salivary biomarkers (Granger et al., Reference Granger, Fortunato, Beltzer, Virag, Bright and Out2012). Samples were assayed in duplicate for salivary cortisol (Dressendörfer et al., Reference Dressendörfer, Kirschbaum, Rohde, Stahl and Strasburger1992). Average concentration from both assays (excluding the samples for which only one assay was possible) was used to assess cortisol in nanomoles per liter. The intra-assay coefficient of variation ranged from 4.0% to 6.7%, and the inter-assay coefficient of variation ranged from 7.1% to 9.0%.

General stress

General stress was assessed using the 4-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4; Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983). Participants were asked to indicate their feelings and thoughts during the last month using a Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (Never) to 4 (Very Often). Sample items included “How often have you felt difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them?” and “How often have you felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life?” The four items were summed to create a measure of general stress (α = .66). Scores ranged from 0 to 16, with higher scores reflecting greater perceived stress. Previous studies have utilized the PSS-4 among Hispanic/Latino/a college student and emerging adult samples (Cano et al., Reference Cano, Marsiglia, Meca, De La Rosa, Ramírez-Ortiz, Sánchez, Colón Burgos, Alamilla, Wuyke, Parras, Ali, Forney, Varas-Rodríguez, Ruvalcaba, Contreras, Ahmed, Vega-Luna, Ochoa, Cuadra and Domínguez García2021; Ibarra-Mejia et al., Reference Ibarra-Mejia, Lusk and Umucu2022).

College stress

Academic, Social, and Financial stress were each measured using the 18-item College Stress Scale (CSS; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Myers, Morris and Cardoza2000). Participants were asked to rate how stressful certain experiences were since the beginning of the semester using a 5-point scale that ranged from 1 (does not apply) to 5 (extremely stressful). This study examined three subscales: academic stress (7-items; e.g., “Handling your academic workload”), social stress (6-items; e.g., “Handling personal relationships”), and financial stress (5-items; e.g., “Paying for bills and living expenses”). Items within each subscale were averaged to create three college-related stress scales, with higher scores indicating greater stress in each domain. Internal consistencies were good for all three subscales: academic (α = .85), social (α = .84), and financial (α = .85).

Bicultural stress

Bicultural stress was measured using participants’ nighttime daily diary reports to five items adapted from the 20-item Bicultural Stress Scale (Romero & Roberts, Reference Romero and Roberts2003). Questions were framed in a daily format (e.g., “Today I did not feel comfortable with people whose culture is different from mine”). Participants responded “yes” or “no” to each item; a frequency count of “yes” items were summed at each day to represent daily bicultural stress. An aggregate measure of average bicultural stress was created using the mean of participants’ daily scores across the week. The full week of diary data (i.e., measured across 7 days) was used to create this aggregate score, rather than limiting the measure to the 3-day cortisol protocol, as the current study aimed to assess bicultural stress across a typical week for students. This diary-based approach has been used successfully in previous waves of this sample (e.g., during high school) as a measure of adolescents’ average daily bicultural stress (see Sladek et al., Reference Sladek, Doane and Park2020a).

Discrimination stress

Ethnic/racial discrimination was assessed utilizing the Adult Discrimination and Peer Discrimination Scale (Greene et al., Reference Greene, Way and Pahl2006; Way, Reference Way1997). Participants were asked to rate the frequency of the occurrence of racial or ethnic-based discrimination by adults and peers at their school. Although the original scale does not include timing, the current study asked participants to think about these experiences during their second semester at the focal institution (e.g., Spring, 2018). Participants responded on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (all the time). Sample items include “How often do you feel that adults treat you unfairly because of your race or ethnicity?” and “How often do you feel that other students at your school insult you because of your race or ethnicity?” The current study examined perpetrator-specific experiences of discrimination (i.e., peer vs. adult-based), as is standard when using this measure (Greene et al., Reference Greene, Way and Pahl2006) and due to research suggesting the need for closer attention to variation in outcomes by perpetrator (Benner et al., Reference Benner, Wang, Shen, Boyle, Polk and Cheng2018). Peer and adult-based discrimination scores were computed by taking the average of 7 items on each scale, with higher scores indicating more experiences of peer and adult-based ethnic/racial discrimination. Internal consistencies were good for both peer (α = .93) and adult discrimination (α = .95).

Covariates

Several key demographic characteristics and health behaviors were examined as potential covariates, in an effort to isolate associations between stress and diurnal cortisol patterns (Adam & Kumari, Reference Adam and Kumari2009). Momentary covariates included whether participants ate, consumed caffeine, used nicotine, experienced pain, exercised, drank alcohol, slept, or used medication within the hour prior to sampling. Day-level covariates included actigraphy-measured sleep duration. Between-person covariates included sex designated at birth (herein referred to as sex; 0 = female, 1 = male), immigrant generation status (0 = participant, parents, and both sets of grandparents were born outside of the United States to 7 = all family members were born in the United States; Umaña-Taylor et al. Reference Umaña-Taylor, Alfaro, Bámaca and Guimond2009), whether participants completed the study during the summer (0 = school year participation, 1 = summer participation), living situation (0 = lived with parents or other family, 1 = lived away from the home in university dorms, with friends, or alone), parent education (1 = less than high school to 10 = doctorate or advanced degree), subjective social class (1 = upper class, 2 = upper-middle class, 3 = middle class, 4 = lower-middle class, 5 = working class; Rubin et al. Reference Rubin, Denson, Kilpatrick, Matthews, Stehlik and Zyngier2014), topical medication use (i.e., corticosteroids), and oral contraceptive use.

Data analytic plan

The final analytic sample was limited to participants who had at least one valid day of cortisol data at T3 (N = 180). Independent t-test and Chi-square tests were conducted to investigate whether there were differences between participants who had valid cortisol data at T3 and those who did not. Independent t-tests revealed that there were no significant group differences on any of the continuous study variables (p > .28). Chi-square tests indicated that participants who did not provide cortisol samples (n = 16) were significantly more likely to participate [in other portions of the study] during the summer than during the school year (χ 2 = 52.45 (1), p < .001). There were no significant group differences for any other categorical variables (p > .09). Within this analytic sample, data missingness ranged from 0% (e.g., general stress) to 1.7% (e.g., ethnic/racial discrimination).

Three separate models were created to characterize stress: (1) additive model; (2) common model; and (3) cumulative model. Additive effects were examined by inserting all stressors as predictors into the model simultaneously. Common effects were estimated by using exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to determine whether the seven stress variables exhibited optimal factor structure when modeled as indicators of one or more latent factors of stress, which were then included as predictor(s) of diurnal cortisol. To evaluate model fit, several fit indices were examined: chi-square test of model fit, comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), root mean square of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). Based on published criteria (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999), good model fit was determined if CFI and TLI values were above .95, RMSEA values were below .06, and SRMR values were below .08. Lastly, cumulative effects were measured by calculating a cumulative risk index (CRI). A count variable was created indicating how many stressors the participant experienced to a high degree. Being in the highest quartile of any one form of stress added “1” to the count. Possible scores ranged from 0 to 7.

To assess momentary (Level 1), daily (Level 2), and between-person (Level 3) variation in cortisol, three-level growth curve models were fit using Mplus 8.0 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998–2017). The diurnal cortisol pattern (e.g., DCS and CAR) was modeled at Level 1 by including growth parameters based on participants’ wake time (linear), the squared function of this variable (i.e., to assess curvilinear patterns; time since waking2), and a dummy variable corresponding to the CAR sample (1 = second sample). At Level 3, additive, common, and cumulative stress were included as person-specific predictors of the cortisol diurnal pattern. Each model of stress was tested separately in the analyses. Presented below is an equation for the additive stress model. In this equation, b 0di represents the intercept (average cortisol at waking), b 1di represents the CAR, and b 2di and b 3di, together, represent the cortisol slope across the day from waking to bedtime (excluding the CAR). Only the prediction of waking cortisol is presented here at Levels 2 and 3, as the other cortisol parameters were estimated equivalently at each level, with the exception of random effects at Level 2, which were only included for the intercept. In the additive model, all stress forms were allowed to covary with one another. Covariates that exhibited statistically significant associations with cortisol outcomes (DCS and CAR) at the bivariate level (i.e., tested in a multi-level framework) were retained in the final models. A full information maximum likelihood (FIML) method was utilized to account for missing data.

Level 1 (moment):

$$\begin{gathered}Cortiso{l_{{\text{mdi}}}} = {b_{{\text{0di}}}} + {b_{{\text{1di}}}}(CA{R_{{\text{mdi}}}}) + {b_{{\text{2di}}}}(Time\;Since\;Wakin{g_{{\text{mdi}}}}) \\ \!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!+ {b_{{\text{3di}}}}(Time\;Since\;Waking_{{\text{mdi}}}^2) \\ \!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!+ {b_{{\text{mdi}}}}(Momentary\;Covariate{s_{{\text{mdi}}}}) + {u_{{\text{mdi}}}} \\ \end{gathered}$$

Level 2 (day):

$${b_{\rm 0di}= \beta_{00i}+ \beta_{\rm 01i} (Prior \;Night \;Sleep \;Duration_{\rm 0di}) + u_{\rm 0di}}$$

Level 3 (person):

$$\begin{gathered}{\beta _{00i}} = {\gamma _{000}} + {\gamma _{001}}(General\;Stres{s_{\text{i}}}) \\ \,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,+ {\gamma _{002}}(Academic\;Stres{s_{\text{i}}}) + {\gamma _{003}}(Social\;Stres{s_{\text{i}}}) \\ \,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,+ {\gamma _{004}}(Financial\;Stres{s_{\text{i}}}) \\ \,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,+ {\gamma _{005}}(Bicultural\;Stres{s_{\text{i}}}) \\ \,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,+ {\gamma _{006}}(Peer\;Discriminatio{n_{\text{i}}}) \\ \,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,+ {\gamma _{007}}(Adult\;Discriminatio{n_{\text{i}}}) \\ \,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,+ {\gamma _{008}}(Person - Level\;Covariate{s_{\text{i}}}) + {u_{00{\text{i}}}} \\ \end{gathered}$$

Results

Preliminary analyses

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations are presented in Table 1. First, the data were examined for normality and outliers. Bicultural stress levels were significantly skewed (2.45) due to relatively low endorsement of daily bicultural stress (e.g., 47.2% reported no instance of bicultural stress); however, a majority of the sample reported experiencing at least one bicultural stressor across the week. Thus, this variable was transformed using the natural log function prior to inclusion in main analyses. Raw cortisol values were also transformed to account for outliers and positive skew of the cortisol distribution. First, outlier cortisol values (n = 14) were winsorized at 50 nmol/L, following standard practice (Nicolson, Reference Nicolson, Luecken and Gallo2008). Next, raw cortisol values were natural log-transformed (skew = 2.89 before transforming, –0.39 after transforming). Plots of cortisol values (prior to log transformation) are presented in Figure 2 for visualization purposes. Bivariate correlations shown in Table 1 depict the association between study variables and the average of participants’ five cortisol samples across all three assessment days. Correlations between the different stress forms ranged from small (r = .11) to moderate (r = .79).

Table 1. Bivariate correlations and descriptive statistics

Note. N = 180. Averages of raw cortisol values (nmol/L) presented for descriptive purposes. Bicultural stress levels natural log-transformed for analyses due to positive skew, but descriptive statistics presented represent original scores. Average sleep duration = average total sleep time across days that cortisol samples were provided. Sex: 1 = male, 0 = female; Immigrant generation: 0 = participant, parents, and both sets of grandparents born outside the United States, 7 = all were born in the United States; Living situation: 1 = living away from home in university dorms or apartment, 0 = living at home with parents or other relatives; Parent education: 1 = less than high school, 10 = doctorate or advanced degree; Subjective social class: 1 = upper class, 2 = upper-middle class, 3 = middle class, 4 = lower-middle class, 5 = working class; Topical medication use: 0 = no, 1 = yes; Oral contraceptive use: 0 = no, 1 = yes.

p < .10.

*p < .05,

**p < .01,

***p < .001.

Figure 2. Cortisol values (nmol/L) across the waking day. Note. Time of day is presented on a 24-hr scale (e.g., 5 = 5 a.m.; 24 = 12 a.m.). Values above 24 correspond to the next waking day (25 = 1 a.m., 30 = 6 a.m.). Extreme values winsorized to = 50.

Factor analysis of stress

EFA was conducted to investigate the factor structure of each of the stress indicators. A scree plot, number of significant items per factor, and theoretical rationale were used to determine optimal factor structure. The scree plot identified two factors with an eigenvalue exceeding one (2.79 and 1.42). The 2-factor solution showed significantly better statistical fit than the 1-factor solution (Δχ 2 (6) = 158.12, p < .001). Thus, the 2-factor solution was retained for subsequent analyses using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). Model fit for this two-factor CFA was good (χ 2 (13) = 15.19, p = .30, RMSEA = .03, CFI = .99, TLI = .99, SRMR = .04). The first factor was composed of general stress, academic stress, social stress, financial stress, and bicultural stress, and the second factor consisted of both peer and adult ethnic/racial discrimination. Factor loadings for the first latent stress factor ranged from low to high: bicultural stress (λ = .42, SE = .07, p < .001), general stress (λ = .47, SE = .07, p < .001), financial stress (λ = .57, SE = .06, p < .001), academic stress (λ = .69, SE = .06, p < .001), social stress (λ = .78, SE = .05, p < .001). Because the three highest factor loadings were subscales of the CSS (Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Myers, Morris and Cardoza2000), this latent factor was referred to as “college stress.” In the second factor, peer (λ = .88, SE = .08, p < .001) and adult-based discrimination (λ = .89, SE = .08, p < .001) were both significant with high factor loadings. This factor was referred to as “discrimination stress.” The two latent stress factors were significantly positively correlated (r = .36, p < .001).

Cumulative risk index

Percentile/quartile scores were used to determine whether participants were experiencing “high” stress of any form. Scoring above the 75th percentile for any given stress form was considered high stress and given a “1” for this stressor (“0” was assigned to participants <75th percentile; Ettekal et al., Reference Ettekal, Eiden, Nickerson and Schuetze2019; Suglia et al., Reference Suglia, Staudenmayer, Cohen, Enlow, Rich-Edwards and Wright2010). The cutoff criteria values were as follows: general stress (>9.00), academic stress (>4.00), social stress (>3.67), financial stress (>3.20), bicultural stress (>0.38), peer discrimination (>2.00), adult discrimination (>2.00). These dummy variables were then summed to create an overall CRI. The distribution of this CRI (M = 1.52, SD = 1.65) was as follows: 36.7% scored “0” (i.e., experienced no stressor to a high degree), 24.4% scored “1”, 13.3% scored “2”, 10.0% scored “3”, 10.6% scored “4”, 2.8% scored “5”, 1.1% scored “6”, and 1.1% scored “7” (i.e., experienced each form of stress to a high degree).

Diurnal pattern

A linear growth model with a dummy code to represent the CAR fit the data significantly better than an unconditional model with no predictors, χ 2 (9) = 2,386.442, p < .001. Adding a quadratic term fit the data significantly better than the linear model, χ 2 (6) = 64.513, p < .001. Growth modeling revealed the expected average diurnal cortisol pattern to have relatively high cortisol levels at waking (5.37 nmol/L), an approximate 84.04% increase 30 min after waking (CAR)Footnote 3 , and an approximate 6.8% decline in cortisol per hour estimated at waking (DCS), accounting for participants’ protocol noncompliance and adjusting for momentary, daily, and between-person covariates (Table 2, Diurnal Pattern Model). Notably, 86.7% of the variance in cortisol was attributable to within-person variance (i.e., sample-to-sample, day-to-day differences; intraclass correlation = .133).

Table 2. Fixed effects estimates from three-level growth models of diurnal cortisol

Note. N = 2,667 samples nested within 180 individuals. Cortisol values (nmol/L) transformed using the natural log function. Besides growth parameters, continuous Level 1 and Level 2 predictors centered within-person; continuous level 3 predictors grand-mean-centered.

p < .10.

*p < .05.

**p < .01.

Additive stress model

First, the additive (independent) contributions of all seven stress forms were examined as predictors of average diurnal cortisol (e.g., CAR and DCS), including covariates (Table 2, Additive Model). None of the stress forms were significantly associated with the CAR. Further, results indicated that higher general stress was associated with an approximate 0.8% flatter per hour at waking DCS (γ 201 = .008, p = .03), whereas higher bicultural stress was associated with a 6.7% steeper per hour at waking DCS, on average (γ 205 = –.069, p = .047). No other stress forms were significantly associated with the DCS, ps > .59. To aid in the interpretation of these results, we standardized the stressors, which revealed that a 1 SD increase in general stress was associated with a 2.3% flatter DCS per hour (γ = .023, p = .03), whereas a 1 SD increase in bicultural stress was linked with a 1.8% steeper DCS per hour (γ = –.018, p = .047).

Common stress model

Next, the two-factor latent stress variable (e.g., college stress, discrimination stress) was entered as the focal predictor of the diurnal pattern (Table 2, Common Model). This third model was composed solely of the two stress factors and previously included covariates. Results from this model indicated that greater college stress was significantly associated with an approximate 8.5% lower CAR (γ 109 = –.089, p = .045), but the discrimination-specific stress factor was not significantly associated with the CAR (γ 108 = –.022, p = .71). Neither latent stress factor was significantly associated with the DCS (ps > .82).

Cumulative stress model

In the final model, the CRI was examined as a predictor of diurnal cortisol, accounting for covariates (Table 2, Cumulative Model). Results indicated that a one unit increase on the CRI (e.g., experiencing one more stressor to a high degree) was associated with an approximate 5.4% lower CAR (γ 110 = –.055, p = .007), but was not significantly related to the DCS (γ 210 = –.002, p = .74).

Sensitivity analyses

Although not a primary focus of this study, additional sensitivity analyses were conducted to examine whether stress predicted another common summary indicator of the diurnal cortisol pattern: area under the curve (AUCg; calculated following (Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid and Hellhammer2003), averaged across the three study days. Results were as follows: in the additive model, no stress forms were significantly associated with the AUCg (ps > .11); in the common model, neither college stress (p = .57) nor discrimination stress (p = .89) were significantly related to the AUCg; in the cumulative model, the CRI was not significantly associated with the AUCg (p = .79).

Discussion

Latino/a adolescents transitioning to college are at increased risk for experiences of stress, including college demands, social stressors, and ethnic/racial stigma (Huynh & Fuligni, Reference Huynh and Fuligni2012; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Myers, Morris and Cardoza2000). The present study utilized a “multi-risk model” approach to investigate additive, common, and cumulative effects of stress on diurnal cortisol among first-year Latino/a college students. Results indicated that, in the additive model, no stress forms were associated with the CAR, however, general stress was associated with a flatter DCS and bicultural stress was linked with a steeper DCS. In the common model, the latent college stress factor was associated with a lower CAR, but was not related to the DCS. Further, greater cumulative stress was also linked with a reduced CAR, but not the DCS. These findings provide insight into the unique links between specific stress forms and HPA axis functioning during Latino/a students’ transition to college, accounting for other relevant stressors experienced during this time. Differences and commonalities across the three models contribute to the complex literature surrounding stress and HPA axis linkages. Importantly, findings support theoretical frameworks positing that chronic and cumulative stress exposure can lead to alterations in physiological functioning that, accumulated over time, can result in maladaptive diurnal patterns (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998), which may underlie existing ethnic/racial disparities in HPA axis functioning (DeSantis et al., Reference DeSantis, Adam, Doane, Mineka, Zinbarg and Craske2007), developmental psychopathology (e.g., Adam et al., Reference Adam, Doane, Zinbarg, Mineka, Craske and Griffith2010; Adam et al., Reference Adam, Vrshek-Schallhorn, Kendall, Mineka, Zinbarg and Craske2014), and health and disease (Myers, Reference Myers2009; Steptoe & Serwinski, Reference Steptoe, Serwinski and Fink2016). These findings may be harnessed as evidence to promote services and mechanisms that support Latino/a students during this transition, including resources for coping and stress management (Bottaccioli et al., Reference Bottaccioli, Bottaccioli, Carosella, Cofini, Muzi and Bologna2020; Sladek et al., Reference Sladek, Doane, Luecken and Eisenberg2016) and increased efforts to promote diversity and inclusion across campus (Sladek et al., Reference Sladek, Doane, Luecken, Gonzales and Grimm2020b).

Additive model findings

Findings indicated that general stress and bicultural stress were both significantly associated with the DCS, accounting for other stress forms. The relation between increased general stress and a flatter DCS is not surprising, as a flatter diurnal rhythm is a common indicator of altered HPA axis activity, and increased stress, in general, is related to this pattern (e.g., Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007). However, it was unexpected that general stress would be the only stress form related to a flatter rhythm. This is an interesting finding that prompts the examination of what differentiates this stressor from others. General stress was assessed using the PSS-4 (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983), a global measure of stress that assesses the degree to which individuals perceive nonspecific events in their lives to be stressful (e.g., unpredictable, uncontrollable, overloading). Thus, it is possible that this measurement was tapping into something unique to participants’ stress perceptions (i.e., their appraisal of stress, rather than frequency/type of stress). Further, perhaps the most compelling rationale for the distinct effect of general stress is the PSS-4’s assessment of stress as uncontrollable. In their meta-analysis, Miller et al. (Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007) identified the “controllability of stress” as a major characteristic influencing how chronic stress relates to HPA axis activity, with greater uncontrollability linked with alterations in HPA axis functioning, including flatter rhythms.

The finding that increased bicultural stress was associated with a steeper DCS is an unexpected result of the current study. Whereas no previous research has directly examined the relation between bicultural stress and diurnal cortisol, other studies examining related cultural stressors (e.g., microaggressions, acculturative stress) among Latino/a adolescents and adults led us to expect that greater bicultural stress would be linked with maladaptive patterns of HPA axis activity (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Wilborn and Mangold2017; Torres et al., Reference Torres, Mata-Greve and Harkins2018; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Landor, Flores and Brown2018). One important consideration that may help explain this finding is the timing in which bicultural stress was measured. Compared to the other stress forms, which asked over the past month or semester, bicultural stress was an average of the daily count of stress experiences that occurred over 1 week, alongside the measurement of cortisol. Thus, it is possible that average daily bicultural stress was associated with what appeared to be an “adaptive” diurnal pattern due to the body adapting to the stressor in the short-term (i.e., steeper slopes resulting from adaptive cortisol activity across days/week). Indeed, a previous study found that Mexican American adolescents who reported higher biculturalism exhibited greater cortisol reactivity in the face of a stressor (i.e., adaptive short-term response; Gonzales et al., Reference Gonzales, Johnson, Shirtcliff, Tein, Eskenazi and Deardorff2018). Given that more bicultural youth likely encounter a greater frequency of bicultural stressors (e.g., Love & Buriel, Reference Love and Buriel2007), it could be that these individuals were responding to these stressors in an adaptive manner. Future studies may choose to examine stress–HPA axis linkages over a longer period of time to better elucidate the short-term versus chronic effects of bicultural stress (i.e., whether “adaptive” diurnal patterns persist in future months/years).

Contrary to our expectations, the additive model did not yield support for independent contributions of academic, social, financial, or discrimination stress on diurnal cortisol. These results contrast previous work linking similar stressors with HPA axis activity. For example, previous studies have linked social stress (e.g., peer problems) with a flatter DCS and greater waking cortisol (Bai et al., Reference Bai, Robles, Reynolds and Repetti2017) and academic stress (e.g., academic problems, student status) with greater morning cortisol and a smaller CAR (Bai et al., Reference Bai, Robles, Reynolds and Repetti2017; McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Murphy, Albano and Ceballos2016). However, it is worth noting that these studies were conducted in younger (Bai et al., Reference Bai, Robles, Reynolds and Repetti2017) and older (McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Murphy, Albano and Ceballos2016) student samples and were not framed within the undergraduate college context, as they were in the present study. Importantly, the current study focused on Latino/a college students, a population for whom these questions had not yet been studied to the authors’ knowledge. Furthermore, the present study estimated these stress–diurnal cortisol associations while accounting for various other forms of stress, which is not as commonly practiced in the literature and may underlie these observed differences.

The nonsignificant findings for ethnic/racial discrimination were particularly unexpected, given the accumulation of evidence linking discrimination with HPA axis functioning in adolescence and young adulthood for Latino/a and ethnically/racially diverse youth (Huynh et al., Reference Huynh, Guan, Almeida, McCreath and Fuligni2016; Skinner et al., Reference Skinner, Shirtcliff, Haggerty, Coe and Catalano2011; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Doane and Roosa2012; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Hoyt and Adam2014). One potential explanation is that the timing of ethnic/racial discrimination was more recent than the timing of discrimination scales used in previous studies (e.g., lifetime, past year; Huynh et al., Reference Huynh, Guan, Almeida, McCreath and Fuligni2016; Skinner et al., Reference Skinner, Shirtcliff, Haggerty, Coe and Catalano2011; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Hoyt and Adam2014). In addition, there is evidence that experiences of discrimination may decrease across the transition to college for Latino students (Castro et al., Reference Castro, Sasser, Sills and Doane2022; Huynh & Fuligni, Reference Huynh and Fuligni2012). Given that previous research found associations between high and stable trajectories of discrimination on African American young adults’ physiological functioning (Brody et al., Reference Brody, Lei, Chae, Yu, Kogan and Beach2014), it could be that, in our sample, recent experiences of discrimination in college were not related to cortisol the same way observed in previous studies that were longitudinal (Brody et al., Reference Brody, Lei, Chae, Yu, Kogan and Beach2014) or asked about discrimination across a longer time frame (Skinner et al., Reference Skinner, Shirtcliff, Haggerty, Coe and Catalano2011; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Hoyt and Adam2014).

Lastly, it is important to consider the college context that the majority of adolescents in our sample were attending when interpreting these findings. The university that participants attended had considerable Hispanic/Latino/a student representation (22.8%), which was the second most represented ethnic/racial group following White students (49.6% White; ASU Institutional Analysis, 2017). As such, these students’ experiences may not reflect those of Latino/a students attending universities with less co-ethnic representation. Future studies may choose to examine these associations in contexts where Latino/a students are less represented and, in turn, may experience more frequent experiences of discrimination (e.g., Bellmore et al., Reference Bellmore, Nishina, You and Ma2012). In sum, findings of the additive model provide additional evidence for links between higher stress perceptions and flatter DCSs, and offer new findings linking bicultural stress with steeper slopes in the short-term. These results may suggest that students’ appraisals of stress are particularly linked with maladaptive patterns of diurnal cortisol, whereas the frequency of stressors may have more complex relationships with HPA axis activity.

Common model findings

The present study’s examination of stress as a latent variable uncovered two distinct stress factors: (1) college stress, which consisted of social, academic, financial, general, and bicultural stress; and (2) discrimination stress, which consisted of peer and adult ethnic/racial discrimination. To the authors’ knowledge, this was the first study to conduct a factor analysis using multiple stress indicators during the college years. These results highlight distinct differences between stress experiences relating to ethnic/racial discrimination, as opposed to normative college stress, general stress, and daily bicultural stress. Notably, the EFA suggested that college-specific stressors (e.g., social, academic, financial) loaded highest onto the first latent factor, demonstrating that context-specific stressors seemed to be carrying the weight of these stress perceptions. These findings support theoretical and empirical research suggesting that ethnic/racial minority students encounter minority-specific stress that is distinct from general college stress, with the latter thought of as experienced by all students (Arbona et al., Reference Arbona, Fan and Olvera2018; Wei et al., Reference Wei, Liao, Chao, Mallinckrodt, Tsai and Botello-Zamarron2010). However, in this study, bicultural stress contributed to the college stress, rather than the discrimination stress latent factor, which may point to important differences between experiences of discrimination/prejudice, as compared to other manifestations of bicultural stress (e.g., dual language demands, inter/intra-group pressures). Indeed, whereas discrimination is often an interaction between ethnic groups resulting from ethnocentrism (e.g., White youth as majority group), bicultural stress represents dual processes stemming from both pressures to maintain adolescents’ native culture (e.g., within-ethnic group stress), as well as pressures to adapt mainstream behaviors (Romero & Van Campen, Reference Romero, Van Campen and Levesque2018). Further, this finding may be due, in part, to bicultural stress being assessed daily, as these experiences are likely nested within college contexts (e.g., interactions with classmates). In sum, these results underscore the importance of examining interrelations between stressors, rather than assuming that all forms of stress are capturing the same underlying construct.

When examining associations between the two latent factors and diurnal cortisol, findings revealed that greater college stress was associated with a lower CAR, but not the DCS, whereas discrimination stress was not significantly related to the CAR or DCS. The null findings for discrimination are surprising from a theoretical perspective, and inconsistent with previous work linking discrimination and diurnal cortisol. However, these results are similar to what was observed in the additive model, and may point to important commonalities among nondiscrimination stress forms as they relate to the CAR. Although no specific hypotheses were made for the common model, we expected that the stress forms that contributed to the college stress factor would be additively related to a larger CAR, due to the recency and predictability of these stressors (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007), which may elicit an “adaptive” boost of cortisol upon waking (Adam et al., Reference Adam2006). In contrast, we found that the common contributions of these stressors were associated with a lower CAR, a pattern linked with fatigue and burnout (Chida & Steptoe, Reference Chida and Steptoe2009). This finding suggests that these developmentally salient stressors may correlate with maladaptive neuroendocrine processes (e.g., blunted CAR), even within the first year of college. However, none of these stressors were individually related to the CAR, suggesting that this association was driven primarily by something all stress forms had in common (i.e., underlying unobserved latent construct), and thus moves beyond additive expectations for stress–HPA axis linkages. Given that this latent construct was comprised of college, general, and minority-specific stressors, it could be that these findings capture the chronic, multiple stress experiences that Latino/a students experience during the transition to college, which would be expected to result in disruptions in diurnal cortisol activity.

In sum, findings from the common model further highlight ethnic/racial discrimination stress as distinct from general and college-specific stress, and link developmentally salient stressors (i.e., relating to or nested in college contexts) with diminished biological functioning. These results may be particularly important for intervention and prevention programs aiming to reduce student stress and burnout, particularly among Latino/a students, which may seek to target stress immediately upon entry into college settings.

Cumulative model findings

A primary objective of this study was to examine whether the cumulative impact of multiple stress forms was associated with diurnal cortisol in ways that were distinct from the additive or common impact of these stressors. The present study observed that cumulative stress was associated with a blunted CAR for first-year Latino/a students. This finding is consistent with the study hypothesis, as well as previous literature examining cumulative risk and altered HPA axis functioning (Kwak et al., Reference Kwak, Taylor, Anaya, Feng, Evich and Jones2017; Suglia et al., Reference Suglia, Staudenmayer, Cohen, Enlow, Rich-Edwards and Wright2010). Specifically, our findings closely relate to those of Kwak et al. (Reference Kwak, Taylor, Anaya, Feng, Evich and Jones2017), in which higher cumulative family stress was linked with a lower CAR among Latino adolescents. In addition, these results are consistent with allostatic load (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998) and models of cumulative vulnerability and minority health (Myers, Reference Myers2009), such that the cumulative effects of various stressors, including general, college, and minority-specific stress, were related to a diurnal pattern indicative of overactivation of the stress response systems (e.g., lower CAR). Despite our predictions, we did not find evidence for an association between cumulative stress and a flatter DCS – a pattern often considered the “hallmark” of stress and strain (Adam et al., Reference Adam, Quinn, Tavernier, McQuillan, Dahlke and Gilbert2017). Though surprising, this finding may help bolster support for the responsivity of the CAR in the context of cumulative stress. Importantly, these findings may help inform future research on the determinants of fatigue and burnout among first-year Latino/a college students. Specifically, they corroborate the notion that students who are subjected to cumulative experiences of multiple forms of stress, which are often initiated and perpetuated by broader systemic and societal racism and marginalization, are at-risk for alterations in stress responsive systems that have been linked with disease onset and mental health problems (Mangold et al., Reference Mangold, Marino and Javors2011; Steptoe & Serwinski, Reference Steptoe, Serwinski and Fink2016).

Multi-risk model approach: differences and takeaways

The primary goal of the multi-risk model approach was to elucidate the complex links between experiences of stress and HPA axis functioning by testing the effects of stress in three different ways (e.g., additive, common, cumulative). Though we cannot make direct comparisons across these models, it is worthwhile to examine clear commonalities and differences observed. Perhaps the most striking difference was the aspect of diurnal cortisol that was associated with stress across the models. Specifically, in the additive model, general and bicultural stress were significantly associated with the DCS, whereas common and cumulative stress were not. This difference was unexpected, as previous work has consistently linked chronic stress with a flatter DCS (e.g., Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007), which led us to expect significant links between common and cumulative stress and the DCS. However, given that time since stress onset is associated with more altered HPA axis functioning (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007), it is possible that the proximity of stress forms in the current study (e.g., daily, monthly, semesterly) may explain these null findings.

Importantly, findings from the additive model may also provide insight into nonsignificant findings across models. For example, in the common model, general and bicultural stress had the lowest factor loadings on the college stress factor (below .50), indicating that there was more variance not attributed to these stressors, which may explain why this latent factor was not associated with the DCS. Further, in the additive model, general and bicultural stress were differentially linked with the DCS (i.e., higher stress linked with flatter and steeper slopes, respectively). Thus, it is possible that the combination of these two stressors into one construct contributed to the nonsignificant DCS findings in the common and cumulative models. This discrepancy across models points to a potential strength of the multi-risk model approach, as the removal of the additive model may have led to substantially different conclusions (e.g., DCS not impacted by stress, general and bicultural stress not as influential on HPA axis activity).

In addition, there were also important similarities across models, with the most notable being that college stress and cumulative stress were both significantly associated with a lower CAR. This similarity is consistent with previous work demonstrating that both observed-score (e.g., CRI) and variable-centered (e.g., latent factor analysis) methods can be used to assess cumulative risk with multiple indicators (Ettekal et al., Reference Ettekal, Eiden, Nickerson and Schuetze2019), and that these two techniques hold unique advantages and disadvantages. For example, the current study found that the common model was less robust with regards to the statistical significance of this effect (p = .045 as compared to p = .007). This highlights a potential strength of the CRI, as it allows for the inclusion of distinct risk processes (e.g., discrimination), as compared to latent factor analysis, which imposes that all stressors are interrelated. On the other hand, the common model provides more specificity regarding which stressors are tapping into the same underlying stress construct, which can aid in interpretation when pinpointing the combined impact of a specific set of stress forms. Indeed, findings from the common model point to college-specific stress forms (e.g., social, academic, financial) as potential drivers of the negative association between cumulative stress and the CAR, a specificity not provided by the cumulative model alone.

Taken together, findings from this multi-risk model approach provide evidence that general, college-related, and cumulative stress experiences (which may disproportionally affect ethnic/racial minority students; Phinney & Haas, Reference Phinney and Haas2003; Wei et al., Reference Wei, Ku and Liao2011) were linked with alterations in HPA axis activity, which has been hypothesized to underlie subsequent health disparities (Myers, Reference Myers2009). Future studies may seek to extend these findings by investigating HPA axis functioning as a mechanism underlying longitudinal links between stress and maladaptive mental and physical health outcomes. Importantly, compared to the robust evidence linking the DCS with mental and physical health (Adam et al., Reference Adam, Quinn, Tavernier, McQuillan, Dahlke and Gilbert2017), the literature is more mixed regarding the CAR (e.g., both heightened and blunted CAR linked with illness; Adam et al., Reference Adam, Doane, Zinbarg, Mineka, Craske and Griffith2010; Mangold et al., Reference Mangold, Marino and Javors2011; Steptoe & Serwinski, Reference Steptoe, Serwinski and Fink2016). Therefore, it will be important for future research to disentangle how this pattern of diurnal cortisol is longitudinally related to maladaptive developmental outcomes such as anxiety and depression, and whether cortisol serves as a mechanism through which additive, common, and cumulative stress experiences are linked with developmental psychopathology.

Limitations and strengths

The findings of the present study should be interpreted alongside its limitations. First, participants in the present study attended a large, 4-year public university in the Southwestern United States and had lived near it at the time of study recruitment. Thus, findings may not generalize well to students attending colleges that differ in geographic location, size, or who choose to attend community or 2-year college contexts. Next, there were more females than males in the current study, which could have impacted study findings, as previous research has found sex differences in average levels of the CAR, as well as stress–CAR linkages (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Margolin, Shapiro and Timmons2017). Further, because our sample was limited to students who provided valid cortisol data, our final sample size was 180, which could have resulted in a reduction of statistical power for models with multiple predictors (e.g., additive model). Additionally, this study was conducted within one college semester (Spring, 2018); therefore, we were unable to capture the stability or enduring effects of first-year stress on subsequent HPA axis functioning across months or years later. Similarly, although the incorporation of stress forms that were specific and varied with regards to timing was a strength of the additive model (e.g., helped clarify potential time-related effects of stress on diurnal cortisol), the presence of different time scales could have influenced results of the common and cumulative models, as stress forms were essentially treated the same in these approaches. Thus, future studies examining common or cumulative stress may seek to measure multiple stressors on the same time scale, to avoid unequal weight of more proximal and/or distant stress forms. Further, the current study utilized the PSS-4 to examine general stress. Although the PSS-4 has been validated for use in adolescent populations, the abbreviated nature of the scale may have resulted in lower reliability of this measure, as was shown in the current study (α = .66). Thus, future studies may seek to incorporate longer versions of the PSS (e.g., PSS-10, PSS-14) or other comprehensive stress assessments that have been validated for use among Latino/a adolescents. Lastly, whereas the current study focused on minority-specific stress forms that were specific to participants’ ethnic/racial background and Latino/a heritage, future studies would be strengthened by examining how minority stressors due to other identities (e.g., gender, sexual orientation) may influence these relationships.

Despite these limitations, the current study harnessed the strengths of an ethnically homogenous, multi-method study design to examine seven distinct forms of stress and their additive, common, and cumulative relation to HPA axis functioning using gold standard salivary cortisol collection procedures (Stalder et al., Reference Stalder, Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Adam, Pruessner, Wüst, Dockray, Smyth, Evans, Hellhammer, Miller, Wetherell, Lupien and Clow2016) and advanced statistical procedures (e.g., three-level growth curve models). Further, this study design helped provide an important snapshot into proximal stress–HPA axis linkages within the first year of college, a time when students are at increased risk for stress experiences (Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Johnson, Gans and Krumrine2004). This was the first known study to examine links between general and minority-specific stressors and diurnal cortisol among Latino/a college students, and importantly, the first to utilize a “multi-risk model” to investigate stress–HPA axis linkages, which represents a major contribution to the literature on physiological stress processes among Latino/a adolescents and young adults.

Conclusions & implications

Chronic or repeated stress exposure, which may be especially common during a major sociocultural shift such as the transition to college (Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Johnson, Gans and Krumrine2004), can alter typical HPA axis functioning, which has lasting consequences on health and well-being (Adam et al., Reference Adam, Quinn, Tavernier, McQuillan, Dahlke and Gilbert2017). The present study provided evidence for additive, common, and cumulative effects of stress on diurnal cortisol in a sample of first-year Latino/a college students. Specifically, there were unique effects of certain stressors on students’ DCS, whereas common and cumulative stress were related to a blunted CAR, providing preliminary evidence that college stress, which is often viewed as “normative,” may have short-term negative effects on students’ stress responsive systems. Furthermore, the finding that common and cumulative stress were linked with a lower CAR, a pattern closely tied to fatigue, exhaustion, and burnout (Chida & Steptoe, Reference Chida and Steptoe2009), suggests that Latino/a students may be experiencing burnout within their first year of college, which may have lasting consequences for mental and physical health.

The implications of the present study findings span multiple levels of influence (e.g., individual, educational, systemic). From an intervention standpoint, these findings point to stress management/reduction techniques as especially promising for first-year college students. Evidence from randomized controlled trials indicate that stress management programs can result in reductions in basal morning cortisol and cortisol reactivity among undergraduate students (Bottaccioli et al., Reference Bottaccioli, Bottaccioli, Carosella, Cofini, Muzi and Bologna2020; Hammerfald et al., Reference Hammerfald, Eberle, Grau, Kinsperger, Zimmermann, Ehlert and Gaab2006), which may protect against maladaptive alterations in diurnal patterns. From a prevention standpoint, educational institutions may seek to reduce the burden of stress on ethnic/racial minority students by promoting a culture that values diversity, as evidence among Latino students suggests that an institution’s commitment to diversity and inclusion can reduce physiological responses to psychosocial stress, specifically for students with greater Latino values (Sladek et al., Reference Sladek, Doane, Luecken, Gonzales and Grimm2020b). Lastly, broader implications of these findings include nationwide efforts to reduce systemic racism and inequalities in the United States by targeting mechanisms of social disadvantage (e.g., access to healthcare, educational resources; Caldwell et al., Reference Caldwell, Ford, Wallace, Wang and Takahashi2017; Hanushek & Rivkin, Reference Hanushek and Rivkin2006), which may alleviate the cumulative burden of general, college, and minority-specific stressors for Latino/a students’ transitioning to college.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the participants of the Transiciones study, who have been a part of the study for over five years. We are also grateful for the help of our research assistants and staff who helped with data collection. A special thanks to Jennifer Kennedy for all of her contributions.

Author contributions

Jeri Sasser conceived of the study, participated in its design, performed statistical analysis, and drafted the manuscript; Leah D. Doane obtained funding for the broader project, designed and coordinated all parts of the study, supervised statistical analysis, and reviewed and edited writing. Jinni Su participated in the framing of the study and reviewed and edited writing; Kevin J. Grimm supervised statistical analysis and reviewed and edited writing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding statement

This research was supported by a William T. Grant Foundation Scholars Award (184370) to Dr. Leah D. Doane.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Ethical standards

All study procedures involving human participants were approved and in accordance with the ethical standards of the Arizona State University Institutional Review Board.

Footnotes

1 The word “Latino/a” is used here to refer to an individual who is of Latin American origin or ancestry. We use this term, rather than the nonbinary “Latinx,” to honor the self-identification of the study participants who self-selected into the study based on Hispanic/Latino/a descent. Consistent with recommended best practices (Buchanan et al., Reference Buchanan, Perez, Prinstein and Thurston2021), when discussing other studies, we adhere to the label that was originally used to describe that study’s sample (e.g., Hispanic, Latino, Latino/a, Latinx).

2 For the purposes of this paper, perceptions of stress will be referred to broadly as “stressors.” However, it should be noted that because we are using self-report data, it is likely that some of the constructs we are examining may better capture students’ perceptions of stress (e.g., academic stress, social stress).

3 Because cortisol values were log-transformed prior to analyses, effect sizes can be interpreted after using the following formula: β% change = ([e^β] – 1).

References

Adam, E. K. (2006). Transactions among adolescent trait and state emotion and diurnal and momentary cortisol activity in naturalistic settings. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 664679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.01.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adam, E. K. (2012). Emotion–cortisol transactions occur over multiple time scales in development: Implications for research on emotion and the development of emotional disorders. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77, 1727. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2012.00657.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adam, E. K., Doane, L. D., Zinbarg, R. E., Mineka, S., Craske, M. G., & Griffith, J. W. (2010). Prospective prediction of major depressive disorder from cortisol awakening responses in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 921931. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.12.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adam, E. K., Hawkley, L. C., Kudielka, B. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Day-to-day dynamics of experience–cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 1705817063. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0605053103 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adam, E. K., & Kumari, M. (2009). Assessing salivary cortisol in large- scale, epidemiological research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 14231436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adam, E. K., Quinn, M. E., Tavernier, R., McQuillan, M. T., Dahlke, K. A., & Gilbert, K. E. (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 83, 2541. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adam, E. K., Vrshek-Schallhorn, S., Kendall, A. D., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R. E., & Craske, M. G. (2014). Prospective associations between the cortisol awakening response and first onsets of anxiety disorders over a 6-year follow-up–2013 Curt Richter Award Winner. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 44, 4759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbona, C., Fan, W., & Olvera, N. (2018). College stress, minority status stress, depression, grades, and persistence intentions among Hispanic female students: A mediation model. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 40, 414430. https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986318799077 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbona, C., & Jiménez, C. (2014). Minority stress, ethnic identity, and depression among Latino/a college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 61, 162168. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034914 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arizona State University Institutional Analysis. (2017). Facts at a Glance Fall 2017. Arizona State University Institutional Analysis. Retrieved February 23, 2022, from https://uoia.asu.edu/sites/default/files/asu_facts_at_a_glance_-_fall_2017_final.pdf Google Scholar
Bai, S., Robles, T. F., Reynolds, B. M., & Repetti, R. L. (2017). Children’s diurnal cortisol responses to negative events at school and home. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 83, 150158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.027 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellmore, A., Nishina, A., You, J. I., & Ma, T. L. (2012). School context protective factors against peer ethnic discrimination across the high school years. American Journal of Community Psychology, 49, 98111. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-011-9443-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benner, A. D., Wang, Y., Shen, Y., Boyle, A. E., Polk, R., & Cheng, Y. P. (2018). Racial/ethnic discrimination and well-being during adolescence: A meta-analytic review. American Psychologist, 73, 855883. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000204 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bottaccioli, A. G., Bottaccioli, F., Carosella, A., Cofini, V., Muzi, P., & Bologna, M. (2020). Psychoneuroendocrinoimmunology-based meditation (PNEIMED) training reduces salivary cortisol under basal and stressful conditions in healthy university students: Results of a randomized controlled study. Explore, 16, 189198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.explore.2019.10.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., Lei, M. K., Chae, D. H., Yu, T., Kogan, S. M., & Beach, S. R. (2014). Perceived discrimination among African American adolescents and allostatic load: A longitudinal analysis with buffering effects. Child Development, 85, 9891002. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12213 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, N. T., Perez, M., Prinstein, M. J., & Thurston, I. B. (2021). Upending racism in psychological science: Strategies to change how science is conducted, reported, reviewed, and disseminated. American Psychologist, 76, 10971112. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000905 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldwell, J. T., Ford, C. L., Wallace, S. P., Wang, M. C., & Takahashi, L. M. (2017). Racial and ethnic residential segregation and access to health care in rural areas. Health & Place, 43, 104112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.11.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cano, M. Á., Marsiglia, F. F., Meca, A., De La Rosa, M., Ramírez-Ortiz, D., Sánchez, M., Colón Burgos, J. F., Alamilla, S. G., Wuyke, G., Parras, D., Ali, S. Z., Forney, D. J., Varas-Rodríguez, E., Ruvalcaba, Y., Contreras, A., Ahmed, S., Vega-Luna, B., Ochoa, L. G., Cuadra, R., & Domínguez García, M. J. (2021). Psychosocial stress, bicultural identity integration, and bicultural self-efficacy among Hispanic emerging adults. Stress and Health, 37, 392398. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2993 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castro, S. A., Sasser, J., Sills, J., & Doane, L. D. (2022). Reciprocal associations of perceived discrimination, internalizing symptoms, and academic achievement in Latino students across the college transition. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000528 Google ScholarPubMed
Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Lee, R. M. (2017). Culture and biology interplay: An introduction. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 23, 14. https://doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000121 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2009). Cortisol awakening response and psychosocial factors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 80, 265278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.10.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clow, A., Thorn, L., Evans, P., & Hucklebridge, F. (2004). The awakening cortisol response: Methodological issues and significance. Stress, 7, 2937. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890410001667205 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeSantis, A. S., Adam, E. K., Doane, L. D., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R. E., & Craske, M. G. (2007). Racial/ethnic differences in cortisol diurnal rhythms in a community sample of adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, 313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.03.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355391. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doane, L. D., Sladek, M. R., & Adam, E. K. (2018a). An introduction to cultural neurobiology: Evidence from physiological stress systems. In Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H. & Gonzales, N. A. (Eds.), The handbook of culture and biology (pp. 227254). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Doane, L. D., Sladek, M. R., Breitenstein, R. S., Park, H., Castro, S. A., & Kennedy, J. L. (2018b). Cultural neurobiology and the family: Evidence from the daily lives of Latino adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 17791796. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418001104 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doane, L. D., & Zeiders, K. H. (2014). Contextual moderators of momentary cortisol and negative affect in adolescents’ daily lives. Journal of Adolescent Health, 54, 536542. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.10.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dressendörfer, R. A., Kirschbaum, C., Rohde, W., Stahl, F., & Strasburger, C. J. (1992). Synthesis of a cortisol-biotin conjugate and evaluation as a tracer in an immunoassay for salivary cortisol measurement. The Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, 43, 683692. https://doi.org/10.1016/0960-0760(92)90294-S CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ettekal, I., Eiden, R. D., Nickerson, A. B., & Schuetze, P. (2019). Comparing alternative methods of measuring cumulative risk based on multiple risk indicators: Are there differential effects on children’s externalizing problems? PloS ONE, 14, e0219134. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219134 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flentje, A., Heck, N. C., Brennan, J. M., & Meyer, I. H. (2019). The relationship between minority stress and biological outcomes: A systematic review. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 43(5), 673694. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-019-00120-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallo, L. C., Roesch, S. C., Fortmann, A. L., Carnethon, M. R., Penedo, F. J., Perreira, K., Birnbaum-Weitzman, O., Wassertheil-Smoller, S., Castañeda, S. F., Talavera, G. A., Sotres-Alvarez, D., Daviglus, M. L., Schneiderman, N., & Isasi, C. R. (2014). Associations of chronic stress burden, perceived stress, and traumatic stress with cardiovascular disease prevalence and risk factors in the HCHS/SOL sociocultural ancillary study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 76, 468475. https://doi.org/10.1097/psy.0000000000000069 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, A. F., Wilborn, K., & Mangold, D. L. (2017). The cortisol awakening response mediates the relationship between acculturative stress and self- reported health in Mexican Americans. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 51, 787798. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-017-9901-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García Coll, C. G., Crnic, K., Lamberty, G., Wasik, B. H., Jenkins, R., Garcia, H. V., & McAdoo, H. P.(1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Development, 67, 18911914. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01834 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzales, N. A., Johnson, M., Shirtcliff, E. A., Tein, J. Y., Eskenazi, B., & Deardorff, J. (2018). The role of bicultural adaptation, familism, and family conflict in Mexican American adolescents’ cortisol reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 15711587. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418001116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granger, D. A., Fortunato, C. K., Beltzer, E. K., Virag, M., Bright, M. A., & Out, D. (2012). Focus on methodology: salivary bioscience and research on adolescence: An integrated perspective. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 10811095. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, M. L., Way, N., & Pahl, K. (2006). Trajectories of perceived adult and peer discrimination among Black, Latino, and Asian American adolescents: Patterns and psychological correlates. Developmental Psychology, 42, 218236. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammerfald, K., Eberle, C., Grau, M., Kinsperger, A., Zimmermann, A., Ehlert, U., & Gaab, J. (2006). Persistent effects of cognitive-behavioral stress management on cortisol responses to acute stress in healthy subjects — a randomized controlled trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 333339 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.08.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanushek, E. A., & Rivkin, S. G. (2006). School quality and the black-white achievement gap (NBER working paper no. 12651). National Bureau of Economic Research CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heissel, J. A., Levy, D. J., & Adam, E. K. (2017). Stress, sleep, and performance on standardized tests: Understudied pathways to the achievement gap. AERA Open, 3, 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858417713488 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huynh, V. W., & Fuligni, A. J. (2012). Perceived ethnic stigma across the transition to college. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41, 817830. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-011-9731-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huynh, V. W., Guan, S. S. A., Almeida, D. M., McCreath, H., & Fuligni, A. J. (2016). Everyday discrimination and diurnal cortisol during adolescence. Hormones and Behavior, 80, 7681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.01.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ibarra-Mejia, G., Lusk, M., & Umucu, E. (2022). Mental health among college students during the COVID-19 pandemic at a Hispanic-serving institution. Health Promotion Practice. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/15248399221092750 Google Scholar
Kerr, S., Johnson, V. K., Gans, S. E., & Krumrine, J. (2004). Predicting adjustment during the transition to college: Alexithymia, perceived stress, and psychological symptoms. Journal of College Student Development, 45, 593611. https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.2004.0068 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirschbaum, C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2000). Salivary cortisol. In Fink, G. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of stress (Vol. 3, pp. 379384). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kudielka, B. M., Broderick, J. E., & Kirschbaum, C. (2003). Compliance with saliva sampling protocols: Electronic monitoring reveals invalid cortisol daytime profiles in noncompliant subjects. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 313319. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000058374.50240.bf CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwak, Y., Taylor, Z. E., Anaya, L. Y., Feng, Y., Evich, C. D., & Jones, B. L. (2017). Cumulative family stress and diurnal cortisol responses in Midwest Latino families. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 39, 8297. https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986316684130 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Love, J. A., & Buriel, R. (2007). Language brokering, autonomy, parent-child bonding, biculturalism, and depression: A study of Mexican American adolescents From immigrant families. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 29, 472491. https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986307307229 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mangold, D., Marino, E., & Javors, M. (2011). The cortisol awakening response predicts subclinical depressive symptomatology in Mexican American adults. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 902909. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.01.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mangold, D., Wand, G., Javors, M., & Mintz, J. (2010). Acculturation, childhood trauma and the cortisol awakening response in Mexican–American adults. Hormones and Behavior, 58, 637646. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.06.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, C. G., Bruce, J., & Fisher, P. A. (2012). Racial and ethnic differences in diurnal cortisol rhythms in preadolescents: The role of parental psychosocial risk and monitoring. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 661668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.02.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, K., Schwartz, J., Cohen, S., & Seeman, T. (2006). Diurnal cortisol decline is related to coronary calcification: CARDIA study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 657661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840, 3344. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S., & Seeman, T. (1999). Protective and damaging effects of mediators of stress: elaborating and testing the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 896, 3047. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08103.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McFarland, J., Hussar, B., de Brey, C., Snyder, T., Wang, X., Wilkinson-Flicker, S., Gebrekristos, S., Zhang, J., Rathbun, A., Barmer, A., Bullock Mann, F., & Hinz, S. (2017). The condition of education 2017 (NCES 2017-144). National Center for Education Statistics. https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2017/2017144.pdf Google Scholar
McGregor, B. A., Murphy, K. M., Albano, D. L., & Ceballos, R. M. (2016). Stress, cortisol, and B lymphocytes: A novel approach to understanding academic stress and immune function. Stress, 19, 185191. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2015.1127913 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Zhou, E. S. (2007). If it goes up, must it come down? Chronic stress and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in humans. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 2545. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.25 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, K. F., Margolin, G., Shapiro, L. S., & Timmons, A. C. (2017). Adolescent life stress and the cortisol awakening response: The moderating roles of attachment and sex. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 27, 3448. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12250 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morin-Major, J. K., Marin, M. F., Durand, N., Wan, N., Juster, R. P., & Lupien, S. J. (2016). Facebook behaviors associated with diurnal cortisol in adolescents: Is befriending stressful? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 63, 238246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.10.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Myers, H. F. (2009). Ethnicity-and socio-economic status-related stresses in context: An integrative review and conceptual model. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32, 919. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-008-9181-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolson, N. A. (2008). Measurement of cortisol. In Luecken, L. J. & Gallo, L. C. (Eds.), Handbook of physiological research methods in health psychology (pp. 3774). Sage Publications, Inc. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412976244.n3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obradović, J., & Armstrong-Carter, E. (2021). Addressing educational inequalities and promoting learning through studies of stress physiology in elementary school students. Development and Psychopathology, 32, 18991913.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phinney, J. S., & Haas, K. (2003). The process of coping among ethnic minority first-generation college freshmen: A narrative approach. The Journal of Social Psychology, 143, 707726. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224540309600426 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (1999). Burnout, perceived stress, and cortisol responses to awakening. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 197204. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006842-199903000-00012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 916931. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00108-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez, N., Myers, H. F., Morris, J. K., & Cardoza, D. (2000). Latino college student adjustment: Does an increased presence offset minority-status and acculturative stresses? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 15231550. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02534.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero, A. J., & Roberts, R. E. (2003). Stress within a bicultural context for adolescents of Mexican descent. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology, 9, 171184. https://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.9.2.171 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romero, A. J., & Van Campen, K. (2018). Bicultural stress. In Levesque, R. J. R. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (pp. 386398). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-33228-4_158 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, M., Denson, N., Kilpatrick, S., Matthews, K. E., Stehlik, T., & Zyngier, D. (2014). “I am working-class” subjective self-definition as a missing measure of social class and socioeconomic status in higher education research. Educational Researcher, 43, 196200. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X14528373 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidman, E., & French, S. E. (2004). Developmental trajectories and ecological transitions: A two-step procedure to aid in the choice of prevention and promotion interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 11411159. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579404040179 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shirtcliff, E. A., Skinner, M. L., Obasi, E. M., & Haggerty, K. P. (2017). Positive parenting predicts cortisol functioning 6 years later in young adults. Developmental Science, 20, e12461. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12461 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, M. L., Shirtcliff, E. A., Haggerty, K. P., Coe, C. L., & Catalano, R. F. (2011). Allostasis model facilitates understanding race differences in the diurnal cortisol rhythm. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 11671186. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941100054X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sladek, M. R., Doane, L. D., Luecken, L. J., & Eisenberg, N. (2016). Perceived stress, coping, and cortisol reactivity in daily life: A study of adolescents during the first year of college. Biological Psychology, 117, 815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.02.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sladek, M. R., Doane, L. D., Luecken, L. J., Gonzales, N. A., & Grimm, K. J. (2020b). Reducing cultural mismatch: Latino students’ neuroendocrine and affective stress responses following cultural diversity and inclusion reminder. Hormones and Behavior, 120, 104681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104681 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sladek, M. R., Doane, L. D., & Park, H. (2020a). Latino adolescents’ daily bicultural stress and sleep: Gender and school context moderation. Health Psychology, 39, 179189. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000824 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snyder, T. D., de Brey, C., & Dillow, S. A. (2019). Digest of education statistics 2017, NCES 2018-070. National Center for Education Statistics. https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2018/2018070.pdf Google Scholar
Stalder, T., Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Adam, E. K., Pruessner, J. C., Wüst, S., Dockray, S., Smyth, N., Evans, P., Hellhammer, D. H., Miller, R., Wetherell, M. A., Lupien, S. J., &  Clow, A. (2016). Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: Expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 63, 414432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.10.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steptoe, A., & Serwinski, B. (2016). Chapter 34 – Cortisol awakening response. In Fink, G. (Ed.), Stress: Concepts, cognition, emotion, and behavior (Vol. 1, pp. 277283). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-800951-2.00034-0 Google Scholar
Suglia, S. F., Staudenmayer, J., Cohen, S., Enlow, M. B., Rich-Edwards, J. W., & Wright, R. J. (2010). Cumulative stress and cortisol disruption among Black and Hispanic pregnant women in an urban cohort. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 2, 326334. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018953 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torres, L., Mata-Greve, F., & Harkins, A. (2018). A preliminary investigation of acculturative stress and diurnal cortisol among Latina women. Journal of Latina/o Psychology, 6, 149158. https://doi.org/10.1037/lat0000088 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Alfaro, E. C., Bámaca, M. Y., & Guimond, A. B. (2009). The central role of familial ethnic socialization in Latino adolescents’ cultural orientation. Journal of Marriage and Family, 71, 4660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2008.00579.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
U.S. Census Bureau. (2017). 2017 National population projections. U.S. Census Bureau. https://www.census.gov/data/tables/2017/demo/popproj/2017-summary-tables.html Google Scholar
Way, N. (1997). Adult and peer discrimination measure. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Wei, M., Ku, T. Y., & Liao, K. Y. H. (2011). Minority stress and college persistence attitudes among African American, Asian American, and Latino students: Perception of university environment as a mediator. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 17, 195203. https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986318799077 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wei, M., Liao, K. Y. H., Chao, R. C. L., Mallinckrodt, B., Tsai, P. C., & Botello-Zamarron, R. (2010). Minority stress, perceived bicultural competence, and depressive symptoms among ethnic minority college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 57, 411422. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020790 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajkowska, Z., Gullett, N., Walsh, A., Zonca, V., Pedersen, G. A., Souza, L., Kieling, C., Fisher, H. L., Kohrt, B. A., & Mondelli, V. (2022). Cortisol and development of depression in adolescence and young adulthood – a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 136, 105625. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105625 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeiders, K. H., Doane, L. D., & Roosa, M. W. (2012). Perceived discrimination and diurnal cortisol: Examining relations among Mexican American adolescents. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 541548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeiders, K. H., Hoyt, L. T., & Adam, E. K. (2014). Associations between self-reported discrimination and diurnal cortisol rhythms among young adults: The moderating role of racial–ethnic minority status. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 50, 280288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeiders, K. H., Landor, A. M., Flores, M., & Brown, A. (2018). Microaggressions and diurnal cortisol: Examining within-person associations among African-American and Latino young adults. Journal of Adolescent Health, 63, 482488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.04.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Conceptual representation of the “multi-risk model” approach.

Figure 1

Table 1. Bivariate correlations and descriptive statistics

Figure 2

Figure 2. Cortisol values (nmol/L) across the waking day. Note. Time of day is presented on a 24-hr scale (e.g., 5 = 5 a.m.; 24 = 12 a.m.). Values above 24 correspond to the next waking day (25 = 1 a.m., 30 = 6 a.m.). Extreme values winsorized to = 50.

Figure 3

Table 2. Fixed effects estimates from three-level growth models of diurnal cortisol