Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-08T04:45:53.324Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Taste, olfactory and food texture reward processing in the brain and the control of appetite

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 September 2012

Edmund T. Rolls*
Affiliation:
Department of Computer Science, Oxford Centre for Computational Neuroscience, Oxford CV4 7AL, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Complementary neuronal recordings and functional neuroimaging in human subjects show that the primary taste cortex in the anterior insula provides separate and combined representations of the taste, temperature and texture (including fat texture) of food in the mouth independently of hunger and thus of reward value and pleasantness. One synapse on, in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), these sensory inputs are for some neurons combined by learning with olfactory and visual inputs, and these neurons encode food reward in that they only respond to food when hungry, and in that activations correlate with subjective pleasantness. Cognitive factors, including word-level descriptions, and attention modulate the representation of the reward value of food in the OFC and a region to which it projects, the anterior cingulate cortex. Further, there are individual differences in the representation of the reward value of food in the OFC. It is argued that over-eating and obesity are related in many cases to an increased reward value of the sensory inputs produced by foods, and their modulation by cognition and attention that over-ride existing satiety signals. It is proposed that control of all rather than one or several of these factors that influence food reward and eating may be important in the prevention and treatment of overeating and obesity.

Type
70th Anniversary Conference on ‘Body weight regulation – food, gut and brain signalling’
Copyright
Copyright © The Author 2012

Abbreviations:
ACC

anterior cingulate cortex

MSG

monosodium glutamate

OFC

orbitofrontal cortex

The aims of this paper are to describe the rules of the cortical processing of taste and smell, how the pleasantness (or affective or reward value) of the taste, smell and texture of food are represented in the brain, how cognitive factors and attention modulate these affective representations, and how these food reward representations play an important role in the control of appetite, food intake and obesity. To make the results relevant to understanding the control of human food intake, complementary evidence is provided by neurophysiological studies in non-human primates and by functional neuroimaging studies in human subjects. A broad perspective on brain processing involved in emotion and in hedonic aspects of the control of food intake is provided by Rolls in Emotion and Decision-Making Explained ( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 2 ).

A reason why it is important to understand the brain systems for food reward is that the reward value of food (i.e. whether we will work for a food), measures our appetite for a food, and whether we will eat a food. Thus normally we want food (will work for it and will eat it) when we like it. ‘We want because we like’: the goal value, the food reward value, makes us want it. For example, neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and lateral hypothalamus described later respond to the reward value of a food when it is for example shown, and these neuronal responses predict whether that food will be eaten( Reference Rolls 1 Reference Rolls, Murzi and Yaxley 5 ). Similarly in a whole series of studies on sensory specific satiety in human subjects based on these discoveries, the reported pleasantness in human subjects of a food is closely correlated with whether it will then be eaten, and even with how much is eaten( Reference Rolls, Van Duijenvoorde and Rowe 6 Reference Rolls, Van Duijvenvoorde and Rolls 8 ). (The situation when it has been suggested that wanting is not a result of liking( Reference Berridge, Robinson and Aldridge 9 ) is when behaviour becomes a habit, that is becomes a stimulus–response type of behaviour that is no longer under control of the goal, but of an overlearned conditioned stimulus( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 2 ).) The concept here is that food reward normally drives appetite and eating, and it is therefore important to understand the brain mechanisms involved in food reward, the main subject of this paper, in order to understand the control of food intake. Moreover, individual differences in these reward systems may lead to differences in appetite, overeating and obesity; and environmental factors such as the palatability and variety of food available in our modern environment may tend to drive overeating, as described in the section ‘Implications for understanding, preventing and treating obesity’.

Taste processing in the primate brain

Pathways

A diagram of the taste and related olfactory, somatosensory, and visual pathways in primates is shown in Fig. 1. The multimodal convergence that enables single neurons to respond to different combinations of taste, olfactory, texture, temperature and visual inputs to represent different flavours produced often by new combinations of sensory input is a theme of recent research that will be described.

Fig. 1. Schematic diagram showing some of the gustatory, olfactory, visual and somatosensory pathways to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and some of the outputs of the OFC, in primates. The secondary taste cortex and the secondary olfactory cortex are within the OFC. V1, primary visual cortex; V4, visual cortical area V4; PreGen Cing, pregenual cingulate cortex. ‘Gate’ refers to the finding that inputs such as the taste, smell and sight of food in some brain regions only produce effects when hunger is present( Reference Rolls 1 ). The column of brain regions including and below the inferior temporal visual cortex represents brain regions in which what stimulus is present is made explicit in the neuronal representation, but not its reward or affective value which are represented in the next tier of brain regions, the OFC and amygdala, and in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). In areas beyond these such as medial prefrontal cortex area 10, choices or decisions about reward value are taken, with the mechanisms described elsewhere( Reference Rolls 2 , Reference Rolls and Deco 10 , Reference Rolls 11 ). Medial PFC area 10, medial prefrontal cortex area 10; VPL, Ventralposterolateral nucleus of the thalamus; VPMpc, ventralposteromedial thalamic nucleus.

The primary taste cortex

Rolls and co-workers have shown that the primary taste cortex in the primate anterior insula and adjoining frontal operculum contains not only taste neurons tuned to sweet, salt, bitter, sour( Reference Scott, Yaxley, Sienkiewicz and Rolls 12 Reference Rolls, Scott and Doty 14 ) and umami as exemplified by monosodium glutamate (MSG)( Reference Baylis and Rolls 15 , Reference Rolls, Critchley and Wakeman 16 ) but also other neurons that encode oral somatosensory stimuli including viscosity, fat texture, temperature and capsaicin( Reference Verhagen, Kadohisa and Rolls 17 ). Some neurons in the primary taste cortex respond to particular combinations of taste and oral texture stimuli, but do not respond to olfactory stimuli or visual stimuli such as the sight of food( Reference Verhagen, Kadohisa and Rolls 17 ) Neurons in the primary taste cortex do not represent the reward value of taste, that is, the appetite for a food, in that their firing is not decreased to zero by feeding the taste to satiety( Reference Rolls, Scott and Sienkiewicz 18 , Reference Yaxley, Rolls and Sienkiewicz 19 ).

The secondary taste cortex

A secondary cortical taste area in primates was discovered by Rolls et al. ( Reference Rolls, Yaxley and Sienkiewicz 20 ) in the OFC, extending several millimetres in front of the primary taste cortex. Neurons in this region respond not only to each of the four classical prototypical tastes sweet, salt, bitter and sour( Reference Rolls, Scott and Doty 14 , Reference Rolls 21 ) but also to umami tastants such as glutamate (which is present in many natural foods such as tomatoes, mushrooms and milk)( Reference Baylis and Rolls 15 ) and inosine monophosphate (which is present in meat and some fish such as tuna)( Reference Rolls, Critchley and Wakeman 16 ). This evidence, taken together with the identification of glutamate taste receptors( Reference Zhao, Zhang and Hoon 22 , Reference Maruyama, Pereira and Margolskee 23 ), leads to the view that there are five prototypical types of taste information channels, with umami contributing, often in combination with corresponding olfactory inputs( Reference Rolls, Critchley and Browning 24 Reference Rolls 26 ) to the flavour of protein. In addition, other neurons respond to water and others to somatosensory stimuli including astringency as exemplified by tannic acid( Reference Critchley and Rolls 27 ) and capsaicin( Reference Rolls, Verhagen and Kadohisa 28 , Reference Kadohisa, Rolls and Verhagen 29 ). Taste responses are found in a large mediolateral extent of the OFC( Reference Critchley and Rolls 27 , Reference Rolls 30 Reference Rolls and Grabenhorst 32 ).

The pleasantness of the taste of food, sensory-specific satiety, and the effects of variety on food intake

The modulation of the reward value of a sensory stimulus such as the taste of food by motivational state, for example hunger, is one important way in which motivational behaviour is controlled( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 33 ). The subjective correlate of this modulation is that food tastes pleasant when hungry, and tastes hedonically neutral when it has been eaten to satiety. Following Edmund Rolls’ discovery of sensory-specific satiety revealed by the selective reduction in the responses of lateral hypothalamic neurons to a food eaten to satiety( Reference Rolls 3 , Reference Rolls, Murzi and Yaxley 5 ), it has been shown that this is implemented in a region that projects to the hypothalamus, the orbitofrontal (secondary taste) cortex, for the taste, odour and sight of food( Reference Rolls, Sienkiewicz and Yaxley 4 , Reference Critchley and Rolls 34 ).

This evidence shows that the reduced acceptance of food that occurs when food is eaten to satiety, the reduction in the pleasantness of its taste and flavour, and the effects of variety to increase food intake( Reference Rolls, Van Duijenvoorde and Rowe 6 Reference Rolls, Van Duijvenvoorde and Rolls 8 , Reference Cabanac 35 Reference Hetherington 43 ) are produced in the OFC, but not at earlier stages of processing where the responses reflect factors such as the intensity of the taste, which is little affected by satiety( Reference Rolls and Grabenhorst 32 , Reference Rolls, Rolls and Rowe 44 ). In addition to providing an implementation of sensory-specific satiety (probably by habituation of the synaptic afferents to orbitofrontal neurons with a time course of the order of the length of a course of a meal), it is likely that visceral and other satiety-related signals reach the OFC (as indicated in Fig. 1; from the nucleus of the solitary tract, via thalamic and possibly hypothalamic nuclei) and there modulate the representation of food, resulting in an output that reflects the reward (or appetitive) value of each food( Reference Rolls 1 ).

The representation of flavour: convergence of olfactory, taste and visual inputs in the orbitofrontal cortex

Taste and olfactory pathways are brought together in the OFC where flavour is formed by learned associations at the neuronal level between these inputs (see Fig. 1)( Reference Verhagen, Kadohisa and Rolls 17 ,45–Reference Rolls 48 ). Visual inputs also become associated by learning in the OFC with the taste of food to represent the sight of food and contribute to flavour( Reference Thorpe, Rolls and Maddison 49 , Reference Rolls 50 ). The visual and olfactory as well as the taste inputs represent the reward value of the food, as shown by sensory-specific satiety effects( Reference Critchley and Rolls 34 ).

The texture of food, including fat texture

Some OFC neurons have oral texture-related responses that encode parametrically the viscosity of food in the mouth (shown using a methyl cellulose series in the range 1–10 000 centiPoise), others independently encode the particulate quality of food in the mouth, produced quantitatively for example by adding 20–100 μm microspheres to methyl cellulose( Reference Rolls, Verhagen and Kadohisa 28 ), and others encode the oral texture of fat( Reference Rolls, Critchley and Browning 51 Reference Rolls 53 ) as illustrated in Fig. 2. Somatosensory signals that transmit information about capsaicin (chilli) and astringency are also reflected in neuronal activity in these cortical areas( Reference Critchley and Rolls 27 , Reference Kadohisa, Rolls and Verhagen 29 , Reference Kadohisa, Rolls and Verhagen 54 ).

Fig. 2. (Colour online) A neuron in the primate orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) responding to the texture of fat in the mouth independently of viscosity. The cell (bk265) increased its firing rate to a range of fats and oils (the viscosity of which is shown in centipoise). The information that reaches this type of neuron is independent of a viscosity sensing channel, in that the neuron did not respond to the carboxymethyl cellulose (CMC) viscosity series. The neuron responded to the texture rather than the chemical structure of the fat in that it also responded to silicone oil (Si(CH3)2O) n ) and paraffin (mineral) oil (hydrocarbon). Some of these neurons have taste inputs (after Verhagen et al. ( Reference Verhagen, Rolls and Kadohisa 52 )).

In addition, we have shown that some neurons in the OFC reflect the temperature of substances in the mouth, and that this temperature information is represented independently of other sensory inputs by some neurons, and in combination with taste or texture by other neurons( Reference Kadohisa, Rolls and Verhagen 29 , Reference Kadohisa, Rolls and Verhagen 54 ).

Imaging studies in human subjects

Taste

In human subjects it has been shown in neuroimaging studies using functional MRI that taste activates an area of the anterior insula/frontal operculum, which is probably the primary taste cortex, and part of the OFC, which is probably the secondary taste cortex( Reference Francis, Rolls and Bowtell 55 Reference de Araujo, Kringelbach and Rolls 57 ). Within individual subjects separate areas of the OFC are activated by sweet (pleasant) and by salt (unpleasant) tastes( Reference O'Doherty, Rolls and Francis 56 ). The primary taste cortex in the anterior insula of human subjects represents the identity and intensity of taste in that activations there correlate with the subjective intensity of the taste, and the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) represents the reward value of taste, in that activations there correlate with the subjective pleasantness of taste( Reference Grabenhorst, Rolls and Bilderbeck 58 , Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 59 ).

We also found activation of the human amygdala by the taste of glucose( Reference Francis, Rolls and Bowtell 55 ). Extending this study, O'Doherty et al. ( Reference O'Doherty, Rolls and Francis 56 ) showed that the human amygdala was as much activated by the affectively pleasant taste of glucose as by the affectively negative taste of NaCl, and thus provided evidence that the human amygdala is not especially involved in processing aversive as compared with rewarding stimuli. Zald et al. ( Reference Zald, Lee and Fluegel 60 ) had shown earlier that the amygdala as well as the OFC respond to aversive (saline) taste stimuli.

Umami taste stimuli, of which an exemplar is MSG and which capture what is described as the taste of protein, activate the insular (primary), orbitofrontal (secondary) and anterior cingulate (tertiary( Reference Rolls 30 )) taste cortical areas( Reference de Araujo, Kringelbach and Rolls 61 ). When the nucleotide 0·005 m inosine 5′-monophosphate was added to MSG (0·05 m), the blood oxygenation-level dependent signal in an anterior part of the OFC showed supralinear additivity, and this may reflect the subjective enhancement of umami taste that has been described when inosine 5′-monophosphate is added to MSG( Reference Rolls 26 ). (The supra-linear additivity refers to a greater activation to the combined stimulus MSG + inosine 5′-monophosphate than to the sum of the activations to MSG and inosine 5′-monophosphate presented separately. This evidence that the effect of the combination is greater than the sum of its parts indicates an interaction between the parts to form in this case an especially potent taste of umami, which is part of what can make a food taste delicious( Reference Rolls 26 ).) Overall, these results illustrate that the responses of the brain can reflect inputs produced by particular combinations of sensory stimuli with supralinear activations, and that the combination of sensory stimuli may be especially represented in particular brain regions, and may help to make the food pleasant.

Odour

In human subjects, in addition to activation of the pyriform (olfactory) cortex( Reference Poellinger, Thomas and Lio 62 Reference Zald and Pardo 64 ), there is strong and consistent activation of the OFC by olfactory stimuli( Reference Francis, Rolls and Bowtell 55 , Reference Zatorre, Jones-Gotman and Evans 65 ), and this region appears to represent the pleasantness of odour, as shown by a sensory-specific satiety experiment with banana v. vanilla odour( Reference O'Doherty, Rolls and Francis 66 ). Further, pleasant odours tend to activate the medial, and unpleasant odours the more lateral, OFC( Reference Rolls, Kringelbach and de Araujo 67 ), adding to the evidence that it is a principle that there is a hedonic map in the OFC, and also in the ACC, which receives inputs from the OFC( Reference Rolls and Grabenhorst 32 , Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 68 ). The primary olfactory (pyriform) cortex represents the identity and intensity of odour in that activations there correlate with the subjective intensity of the odour, and the orbitofrontal and ACC represents the reward value of odour, in that activations there correlate with the subjective pleasantness of odour( Reference Rolls and Grabenhorst 32 , Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 68 , Reference Grabenhorst, Rolls and Margot 69 ).

Olfactory-taste convergence to represent flavour, and the influence of satiety

Supradditive effects indicating convergence and interactions were found for taste (sucrose) and odour (strawberry) in the orbitofrontal and ACC, and activations in these regions were correlated with the pleasantness ratings given by the participants( Reference de Araujo and Rolls 70 Reference Small and Prescott 72 ). These results provide evidence on the neural substrate for the convergence of taste and olfactory stimuli to produce flavour in human subjects, and where the pleasantness of flavour is represented in the human brain.

McCabe and Rolls( Reference McCabe and Rolls 25 ) have shown that the convergence of taste and olfactory information appears to be important for the delicious flavour of umami. They showed that when glutamate is given in combination with a consonant, savoury, odour (vegetable), the resulting flavour can be much more pleasant than the glutamate taste or vegetable odour alone, and that this reflected activations in the pregenual cingulate cortex and medial OFC. The principle is that certain sensory combinations can produce very pleasant food stimuli, which may of course be important in driving food intake; and that these combinations are formed in the brain far beyond the taste or olfactory receptors( Reference Rolls 26 ).

To assess how satiety influences the brain activations to a whole food which produces taste, olfactory and texture stimulation, we measured brain activation by whole foods before and after the food is eaten to satiety. The foods eaten to satiety were either chocolate milk or tomato juice. A decrease in activation by the food eaten to satiety relative to the other food was found in the OFC( Reference Kringelbach, O'Doherty and Rolls 73 ) but not in the primary taste cortex. This study provided evidence that the pleasantness of the flavour of food, and sensory-specific satiety, are represented in the OFC.

Fig. 3. (Colour online) Brain regions in which the activations were correlated with the subjective pleasantness of fat texture: Mid-orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) ([32 34 − 14], z = 3·38, P = 0·013) (a, yellow circle, c showing the relation between the% change in the blood oxygenation-level dependent (BOLD) signal and the rating of the pleasantness of the texture) and anterior cingulate cortex ([2 30 14], z = 3·22, P = 0·016) (a, pink circles and b). (After Grabenhorst Rolls et al. ( Reference Grabenhorst, Rolls and Parris 74 ).)

Oral viscosity and fat texture

The viscosity of food in the mouth is represented in the human primary taste cortex (in the anterior insula), and also in a mid-insular area that is not taste cortex, but which represents oral somatosensory stimuli( Reference de Araujo and Rolls 75 ). Oral viscosity is also represented in the human orbitofrontal and perigenual cingulate cortices, and it is notable that the perigenual cingulate cortex, an area in which many pleasant stimuli are represented, is strongly activated by the texture of fat in the mouth and also by oral sucrose( Reference de Araujo and Rolls 75 ). We have recently shown that the pleasantness and reward value of fat texture is represented in the mid-orbitofrontal and ACC, where activations are correlated with the subjective pleasantness of oral fat texture( Reference Rolls 26 , Reference Grabenhorst, Rolls and Parris 74 , Reference Rolls, Montmayeur and Coutre 76 ) (Fig. 3). This provides a foundation for studies of whether activations in the fat reward system are heightened in people who tend to become obese.

Fig. 4. (Colour online) Cognitive modulation of flavour reward processing in the brain. (a) The medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) was more strongly activated when a flavour stimulus was labelled ‘rich and delicious flavour’ (MSGVrich) than when it was labelled ‘boiled vegetable water’ (MSGVbasic) ([ −8 28 −20]). (The flavour stimulus, MSGV, was the taste 0·1 m MSG + 0·005 m inosine 5′ monophosphate combined with a consonant 0·4 % vegetable odour.) (b) The time course of the blood oxygenation-level dependent (BOLD) signals for the two conditions. (c) The peak values of the BOLD signal (mean across subjects (sem)) were significantly different (t = 3·06, df = 11, P = 0·01). (d) The BOLD signal in the medial OFC was correlated with the subjective pleasantness ratings of taste and flavour, as shown by the Statistical Parametric Mapping analysis, and as illustrated (mean across subjects (sem), r = 0·86, P < 0·001). (After Grabenhorst et al. ( Reference Grabenhorst, Rolls and Bilderbeck 58 ).)

The sight of food

O'Doherty et al. ( Reference O'Doherty, Deichmann and Critchley 77 ) showed that visual stimuli associated with the taste of glucose activated the OFC and some connected areas, consistent with the primate neurophysiology. Simmons, Martin & Barsalou( Reference Simmons, Martin and Barsalou 78 ) found that showing pictures of foods, compared with pictures of locations, can also activate the OFC. Similarly, the OFC and connected areas were also found to be activated after presentation of food stimuli to food-deprived subjects( Reference Wang, Volkow and Telang 79 ).

Cognitive and selective attentional effects on representations of food

To what extent does cognition influence the hedonics of food-related stimuli, and how far down into the sensory system does the cognitive influence reach? To address this, we performed a functional MRI investigation in which the delivery of a standard test odour (isovaleric acid combined with cheddar cheese odour, presented orthonasally using an olfactometer) was paired with a descriptor word on a screen, which on different trials was ‘Cheddar cheese’ or ‘Body odour’. Participants rated the affective value of the test odour as significantly more pleasant when labelled ‘Cheddar Cheese’ than when labelled ‘Body odour’, and these effects reflected activations in the medial OFC/rostral ACC that had correlations with the pleasantness ratings( Reference de Araujo, Rolls and Velazco 80 ). The implication is that cognitive factors can have profound effects on our responses to the hedonic and sensory properties of food, in that these effects are manifest quite far down into sensory processing, so that hedonic representations of odours are affected( Reference de Araujo, Rolls and Velazco 80 ). Similar cognitive effects and mechanisms have now been found for the taste and flavour of food, where the cognitive word level descriptor was for example ‘rich delicious flavour’ and activations to flavour were increased in the OFC and regions to which it projects including the pregenual cingulate cortex and ventral striatum, but were not influenced in the insular primary taste cortex where activations reflected the intensity (concentration) of the stimuli( Reference Grabenhorst, Rolls and Bilderbeck 58 ) (see Fig. 4).

In addition, we have found that with taste, flavour and olfactory food-related stimuli, selective attention to pleasantness modulates representations in the OFC (see Fig. 5), whereas selective attention to intensity modulates activations in areas such as the primary taste cortex( Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 59 , Reference Rolls, Grabenhorst and Margot 81 ). Thus, depending on the context in which tastes and odours are presented and whether affect is relevant, the brain responds to a taste and odour differently. These findings show that when attention is paid to affective value, the brain systems engaged to represent the stimulus are different from those engaged when attention is directed to the physical properties of a stimulus such as its intensity. This differential biasing by prefrontal cortex attentional mechanisms( Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 82 , Reference Ge, Feng and Grabenhorst 83 ) of brain regions engaged in processing a sensory stimulus depending on whether the cognitive demand is for affect-related v. more sensory-related processing may be an important aspect of cognition and attention which have implications for how strongly the reward system is driven by food, and thus for eating and the control of appetite( Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 59 , Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 68 , Reference Rolls, Grabenhorst and Margot 81 ).

Fig. 5. (Colour online) Effect of paying attention to the pleasantness v. the intensity of a taste stimulus. Top: a significant difference related to the taste period was found in the medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) at [−6 14 −20], z= 3·81, P < 0·003 (towards the back of the area of activation shown) and in the pregenual cingulate cortex at [−4 46 −8], z = 2·90, P < 0·04 (at the cursor). Middle: medial OFC. Right: the parameter estimates (mean (sem) across subjects) for the activation at the specified coordinate for the conditions of paying attention to pleasantness or to intensity. The parameter estimates were significantly different for the OFC t = 7·27, df = 11, P < 10−4. Left: the correlation between the pleasantness ratings and the activation (% blood oxygenation-level dependent (BOLD) change) at the specified coordinate (r = 0·94, df = 8, P < < 0·001). Bottom: pregenual cingulate cortex. Conventions as previously. Right: the parameter estimates were significantly different for the pregenual cingulate cortex t = 8·70, df = 11, P < 10−5. Left: the correlation between the pleasantness ratings and the activation (% BOLD change) at the specified coordinate (r=  0·89, df = 8, P = 0·001). The taste stimulus, 0·1 m monosodium glutamate, was identical on all trials (after Grabenhorst and Rolls( Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 59 )).

Beyond reward value to decision-making

Representations of the reward value of food and their subjective correlate the pleasantness of food are fundamental in determining appetite. But after the reward evaluation, a decision has to be made about whether to seek for and consume the reward. We are now starting to understand how the brain takes decisions as described in The Noisy Brain ( Reference Rolls and Deco 10 ), and this has implications for whether a reward of a particular value will be selected( Reference Rolls and Deco 10 , Reference Rolls 11 , Reference Rolls and Grabenhorst 32 , Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 68 , Reference Rolls, Cutsuridis, Hussain and Taylor 84 , Reference Deco, Rolls and Albantakis 85 ). A tier of processing beyond the OFC, in medial prefrontal cortex area 10, becomes engaged when choices are made between odour stimuli based on their pleasantness( Reference Grabenhorst, Rolls and Parris 86 Reference Rolls, Grabenhorst and Deco 89 ). The choices are made by a local attractor network in which the winning attractor represents the decision, with each possible attractor representing a different choice, and each attractor receiving inputs that reflect the evidence for that choice. (The attractor network is formed in a part of the cerebral cortex by strengthening of the recurrent collateral excitatory synapses between nearby pyramidal cells. One group of neurons with strengthened synapses between its members can form a stable attractor with high firing rates, which competes through inhibitory interneurons with other possible attractors formed by other groups of excitatory neurons( Reference Rolls 11 , Reference Rolls 90 ). The word attractor refers to the fact that inexact inputs are attracted to one of the states of high firing that are specified by the synaptic connections between the different groups of neurons. The result in this non-linear system is that one attractor wins, and this implements a mechanism for decision-making with one winner( Reference Rolls and Deco 10 , Reference Rolls 11 , Reference Deco, Rolls and Albantakis 85 , Reference Wang 91 ).) The decisions are probabilistic as they reflect the noise in the competitive non-linear decision-making process that is introduced by the random spiking times of neurons for a given mean rate that reflect a Poisson process( Reference Rolls and Deco 10 , Reference Rolls, Grabenhorst and Deco 89 ). The costs of each reward need to be subtracted from the value of each reward to produce a net reward value for each available reward before the decision is taken( Reference Rolls 11 , Reference Rolls and Grabenhorst 32 , Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 68 ). The reasoning or rational system with its long-term goals (introducing evidence such as ‘scientific studies have shown that fish oils rich in omega 3 may reduce the probability of Alzheimer's disease’) then competes with the rewards such as the pleasant flavour of food (which are gene-specified( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 2 ), though subject to conditioned effects( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Booth 92 )) in a further decision process which may itself be subject to noise( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls and Deco 10 , Reference Rolls 11 ). This can be described as a choice between the selfish phene (standing for phenotype) and the selfish gene( Reference Rolls 2 , Reference Rolls, Cutsuridis, Hussain and Taylor 84 , Reference Rolls 93 ). In this context, the findings described in this paper that the cognitive system can have a top–down influence on the food reward system are important advances in our understanding of how these decisions are reached.

Synthesis

These investigations show that a principle of brain function is that representations of the reward/hedonic value and pleasantness of sensory including food-related stimuli are formed separately from representations of what the stimuli are. The pleasantness/reward value is represented in areas such as the OFC and pregenual cingulate cortex, and it is here that hunger/satiety signals modulate the representations of food to make them implement reward. The satiety signals that help in this modulation may reach the OFC from the hypothalamus, and in turn, the OFC projects to the hypothalamus where neurons are found that respond to the sight, smell and taste of food if hunger is present( Reference Rolls and Grabenhorst 32 , Reference Rolls 33 ). We have seen earlier some of the principles that help to make the food pleasant, including particular combinations of taste, olfactory, texture, visual and cognitive inputs.

I develop a hypothesis later (section ‘Brain processing of the sensory properties and pleasantness of food’) that obesity is associated with overstimulation of these reward systems by very rewarding combinations of taste, odour, texture, visual and cognitive inputs.

Implications for understanding, preventing and treating obesity

Understanding the mechanisms that control appetite is becoming an increasingly important issue, given the increasing incidence of obesity (a threefold increase in the UK since 1980 to a figure of 20% defined by a BMI > 30) and the realisation that it is associated with major health risks (with 1000 deaths each week in the UK attributable to obesity). It is important to understand and thereby be able to minimise and treat obesity because many diseases are associated with a body weight that is much above normal. These diseases include hypertension, CVD, hypercholesterolaemia and gall bladder disease; and in addition obesity is associated with some deficits in reproductive function (e.g. ovulatory failure), and with an excess mortality from certain types of cancer( Reference Schwartz and Porte 94 , Reference Guyenet and Schwartz 95 ).

There are many factors that can cause or contribute to obesity in human subjects( Reference Morton, Cummings and Baskin 96 , Reference O'Rahilly 97 ) that are investigated with approaches within or related to neuroscience and psychology( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 33 , Reference Rolls 98 , Reference Rolls 99 ). Rapid progress is being made in understanding many of these factors at present with the aim of leading to better ways to minimise and treat obesity. These factors include the following( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 2 , Reference Rolls 100 ).

Genetic factors

These are of some importance, with some of the variance in weight and RMR in a population of human subjects attributable to inheritance( Reference Morton, Cummings and Baskin 96 , Reference O'Rahilly 97 ). However, the ‘obesity epidemic’ that has occurred since 1990 cannot be attributed to genetic changes, for which the time scale is far too short, but instead to factors such as the increased palatability, variety and availability of food which are some of the crucial drivers of food intake and the amount of food that is eaten in our changed modern environment( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 33 , Reference Rolls 98 , Reference Rolls 99 , Reference Heitmann, Westerterp and Loos 101 ) and that are described later.

Endocrine factors and their interaction with brain systems

A small proportion of cases of obesity can be related to gene-related dysfunctions of the peptide systems in the hypothalamus, with for example 4% of obese people having deficient melanocortin 4 receptors for melanocyte stimulating hormone( Reference Morton, Cummings and Baskin 96 , Reference O'Rahilly 97 ). Cases of obesity that can be related to changes in the leptin hormone satiety system are very rare( Reference O'Rahilly 97 ). Further, obese people generally have high levels of leptin, so leptin production is not the problem, and instead leptin resistance (i.e. insensitivity) may be somewhat related to obesity, with the resistance perhaps related in part to smaller effects of leptin on arcuate nucleus NPY/AGRP (neuropeptide Y/Agouti-related peptide) neurons( Reference Munzberg and Myers 102 ). However, although there are similarities in fatness within families, these are as strong between spouses as they are between parents and children, so that these similarities cannot be attributed to genetic influences, but presumably reflect the effect of family attitudes to food and weight.

Brain processing of the sensory properties and pleasantness of food

The way in which the sensory factors produced by the taste, smell, texture and sight of food interact in the brain with satiety signals (such as gastric distension and satiety-related hormones) to determine the pleasantness and palatability of food, and therefore whether and how much food will be eaten, is described earlier and shown in Figs. 1 and 6. The concept is that convergence of sensory inputs produced by the taste, smell, texture and sight of food occurs in the OFC to build a representation of food flavour. The OFC is where the pleasantness and palatability of food are represented, as shown by the discoveries that these representations of food are only activated if hunger is present, and correlate with the subjective pleasantness of the food flavour( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 2 , Reference Rolls and Grabenhorst 32 , Reference Rolls 33 , Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 68 , Reference Rolls 98 , Reference Rolls 99 ). The OFC representation of whether food is pleasant (given any satiety signals present) then drives brain areas such as the striatum and cingulate cortex that then lead to eating behaviour.

Fig. 6. Schematic diagram to show how sensory factors interact in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) with satiety signals to produce the hedonic, rewarding value of food, which leads to appetite and eating. Cognitive and attentional factors directly modulate the reward system in the brain. The neural mechanisms involved are considered elsewhere( Reference Rolls 2 , Reference Rolls and Deco 10 , Reference Rolls 11 ).

The fundamental concept this leads to about some of the major causes of obesity is that, over the last 30 years, sensory stimulation produced by the taste, smell, texture and appearance of food, as well as its availability, have increased dramatically, yet the satiety signals produced by stomach distension, satiety hormones, etc.( Reference Cummings and Overduin 103 ) have remained essentially unchanged, so that the effect on the brain's control system for appetite (shown in Figs. 1 and 5) is to lead to a net average increase in the reward value and palatability of food which over rides the satiety signals, and contributes to the tendency to be overstimulated by food and to overeat.

In this scenario, it is important to understand much better the rules used by the brain to produce the representation of the pleasantness of food and how the system is modulated by eating and satiety. This understanding, and how the sensory factors can be designed and controlled so as not to override satiety signals, are important research areas in the understanding, prevention and treatment of obesity. Advances in understanding the receptors that encode the taste and olfactory properties of food( Reference Zhao, Zhang and Hoon 22 , Reference Buck, Kandel, Schwartz and Jessel 104 ), and the processing in the brain of these properties( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 99 , Reference Rolls 105 ), are also important in providing the potential to produce highly palatable food that is at the same time nutritious and healthy.

An important aspect of this hypothesis is that different human subjects may have reward systems that are especially strongly driven by the sensory and cognitive factors that make food highly palatable. In a test of this, we showed that activation to the sight and flavour of chocolate in the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortex were much higher in chocolate cravers than non-cravers( Reference Rolls and McCabe 106 ). This concept that individual differences in responsiveness to food reward are reflected in brain activations in regions related to the control food intake( Reference Rolls and McCabe 106 , Reference Beaver, Lawrence and Ditzhuijzen 107 ) may provide a way for understanding and helping to control food intake.

Food palatability

A factor in obesity (as described in section ‘Brain processing of the sensory properties and pleasantness of food’) is food palatability, which with modern methods of food production can now be greater than would have been the case during the evolution of our feeding control systems. These brain systems evolved so that internal signals from for example gastric distension and glucose utilisation could act to decrease the pleasantness of the sensory sensations produced by feeding sufficiently by the end of a meal to stop further eating( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 99 , Reference Rolls 105 ). However, the greater palatability of modern food may mean that this balance is altered, so that there is a tendency for the greater palatability of food to be insufficiently decreased by a standard amount of food eaten, so that extra food is eaten in a meal (see Fig. 6).

Sensory-specific satiety and the effects of variety on food intake

Sensory-specific satiety is the decrease in the appetite for a particular food as it is eaten in a meal, without a decrease in the appetite for different foods( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 99 , Reference Rolls 105 ), as shown earlier. It is an important factor influencing how much of each food is eaten in a meal, and its evolutionary significance may be to encourage eating of a range of different foods, and thus obtaining a range of nutrients. As a result of sensory-specific satiety, if a wide variety of foods is available, overeating in a meal can occur. Given that it is now possible to make available a very wide range of food flavours, textures and appearances, and that such foods are readily available, this variety effect may be a factor in promoting excess food intake.

Fixed meal times and the availability of food

Another factor that could contribute to obesity is fixed meal times, in that the normal control of food intake by alterations in inter-meal interval is not readily available in human subjects, and food may be eaten at a meal-time even if hunger is not present( Reference Rolls 1 ). Even more than this, because of the high and easy availability of food (in the home and workplace) and stimulation by advertising, there is a tendency to start eating again when satiety signals after a previous meal have decreased only a little, and the consequence is that the system again becomes overloaded.

Food saliency and portion size

Making food salient, for example by placing it on display, may increase food selection particularly in the obese( Reference Schachter 108 , Reference Rodin 109 ), and portion size is a factor, with more being eaten if a large portion of food is presented( Reference Kral and Rolls 110 ), though it is not yet clear whether this is a factor that can lead to obesity and not just alter meal size. The driving effects of visual and other stimuli, including the effects of advertising, on the brain systems that are activated by food reward may be different in different individuals, and may contribute to obesity.

Energy density of food

Although gastric emptying rate is slower for high-energy density foods, this does not fully compensate for the energy density of the food( Reference Hunt and Stubbs 111 , Reference Hunt 112 ). The implication is that eating energy dense foods (e.g. high-fat foods) may not allow gastric distension to contribute sufficiently to satiety. Because of this, the energy density of foods may be an important factor that influences how much energy is consumed in a meal( Reference Kral and Rolls 110 , Reference Rolls 113 ). Indeed, it is notable that obese people tend to eat foods with high-energy density, and to visit restaurants with high-energy density (e.g. high-fat) foods. It is also a matter of clinical experience that gastric emptying is faster in obese than in thin individuals, so that gastric distension may play a less effective role in contributing to satiety in the obese. It is also important to remember that the flavour of a food can be conditioned to its energy density, leading over a few days to more eating of low than high-energy dense foods, in the phenomenon known as conditioned satiety( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Booth 92 ).

Eating rate

A factor related to the effects described in section ‘Energy density of food’ is eating rate, which is typically fast in the obese, and may provide insufficient time for the full effect of satiety signals as food reaches the intestine to operate.

Stress

Another potential factor in obesity is stress, which can induce eating and could contribute to a tendency to obesity. (In a rat model of this, mild stress in the presence of food can lead to overeating and obesity. This overeating is reduced by anti-anxiety drugs.)

Food craving

Binge eating has some parallels to addiction. In one rodent model of binge eating, access to sucrose for several hours daily can lead to binge-like consumption of the sucrose over a period of days( Reference Berner, Bocarsly and Hoebel 114 ). The binge-eating is associated with the release of dopamine. This model brings binge eating close to an addictive process, at least in this model, in that after the binge-eating has become a habit, sucrose withdrawal decreases dopamine release in the ventral striatum (a part of the brain involved in addiction to drugs such as amphetamine), altered binding of dopamine to its receptors in the ventral striatum is produced, and signs of withdrawal from an addiction occur including teeth chattering. In withdrawal, the animals are also hypersensitive to the effects of amphetamine. Another rat model is being used to investigate the binge eating of fat, and whether the reinforcing cues associated with this can be reduced by the gamma-aminobutyric acid B receptor agonist baclofen( Reference Berner, Bocarsly and Hoebel 114 ).

Energy output

If energy intake is greater than energy output, body weight increases. Energy output is thus an important factor in the equation. However, studies in human subjects show that although exercise has health benefits, it does not have very significant effects on body weight gain and adiposity in the obese or those who become obese( Reference Wilks, Besson and Lindroos 115 , Reference Thomas, Bouchard and Church 116 ). These findings help to emphasise the importance of understanding the factors that lead to overeating, including factors such as increased responsiveness of the reward system for food in some individuals, and the effects described here that contribute to reward signals produced in modern society being greater than the satiety signals, which have not changed from those in our evolutionary history( Reference Heitmann, Westerterp and Loos 101 ).

Cognitive factors and attention

As described earlier, cognitive factors, such as preconceptions about the nature of a particular food or odour, can reach down into the olfactory and taste system in the OFC which controls the palatability of food to influence how pleasant an olfactory, taste, or flavour stimulus is( Reference Grabenhorst, Rolls and Bilderbeck 58 , Reference de Araujo, Rolls and Velazco 80 ). This has implications for further ways in which food intake can be controlled by cognitive factors, and this needs further investigation. For example, the cognitive factors that have been investigated in these studies are descriptors of the reward value of the food, such as ‘rich and delicious’. But it could be that cognitive descriptions of the consequences of eating a particular food, such as ‘this food tends to increase body weight’, ‘this food tends to alter your body shape towards fatness’, ‘this food tends to make you less attractive’, ‘this food will reduce the risk of a particular disease’, etc., could also modulate the reward value of the food as it is represented in the OFC. If so, these further types of cognitive modulation could be emphasised in the prevention and treatment of obesity.

Further, attention to the affective properties of food modulates processing of the reward value of food in the OFC( Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls 59 , Reference Rolls, Grabenhorst and Margot 81 ), and this again suggests that how attention is directed may be important in the extent to which food over-stimulates food intake. Not drawing attention to the reward properties of food, or drawing attention to other properties such as its nutritional value and energy content, could reduce the activation of the brain's reward system by the food, and could be another useful way to help prevent and treat obesity.

The neuroscience and psychology of compliance with information about risk factors for obesity

It is important to develop better ways to provide information that will be effective in the long term in decreasing food intake while maintaining a healthy diet, and in promoting an increase in energy expenditure by for example encouraging exercise. In this respect, the individual differences in the brain's response to the reward value of a food, found for example in our study with chocolate cravers and non-cravers( Reference Rolls and McCabe 106 ), is one type of factor that may influence whether an individual can comply. But there are individual differences in other factors that may influence compliance, such as impulsiveness, and the OFC is implicated in this( Reference Berlin, Rolls and Kischka 117 Reference Cools, Blackwell and Clark 119 ). It is important to better understand possible individual differences in the ability for an individual to stop, and be influenced by the reasoning system with its long-term interests in comparison with the immediate rewards specified by genes( Reference Rolls 1 , Reference Rolls 2 , Reference Rolls 11 , Reference Rolls, Cutsuridis, Hussain and Taylor 84 ). It could also be that substances such as alcohol shift this balance, making an individual temporarily or possibly in the long term more impulsive and less under control of the reasoning executive system( Reference Crews and Boettiger 120 ), and therefore more likely to eat, and to eat unhealthily. These effects of alcohol on impulsiveness may be complemented by hormonal processes( Reference Barson, Karatayev and Chang 121 ). Understanding these processes, and enabling individuals to benefit from this understanding, may also be useful in the prevention and treatment of obesity.

Overall, I suggest that understanding of all the afore-mentioned processes, and their use in combination rather than purely individually, may provide new avenues to the control of overeating and body weight. I have outlined in this paper a number of factors that may tend to promote overeating and obesity in our modern society, for example by increasing the impact of reward signals on the brain's appetite control system, or by making it difficult for individuals to resist the increased hedonic value of food. It is possible that any one of these, or a few in combination, could produce overeating and obesity. In these circumstances, to prevent and treat obesity it is unlikely to be sufficient to reduce and focus on or test just one or a few of these factors. As there are many factors, there may always be others that apply and that tend to promote overeating and obesity. The conclusion I therefore reach is that to prevent and treat obesity, it may be important to address all of the afore-mentioned factors together, given that any one, or a few, could tend to lead to overeating and obesity. The science I have described suggests that taking this overall approach, minimising the impact of all these factors, could be an important aim for future research and strategy.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Medical Research Council. The participation of many colleagues in the studies cited is sincerely acknowledged. The author declares no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Corresponding author: Professor E. T. Rolls, email [email protected], url www.oxcns.org

References

1. Rolls, ET (2005) Emotion Explained. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
2. Rolls, ET (2013) Emotion and Decision-Making Explained. Oxford: Oxford University Press (In the Press).Google Scholar
3. Rolls, ET (1981) Central nervous mechanisms related to feeding and appetite. Br Med Bull 37, 131134.Google Scholar
4. Rolls, ET, Sienkiewicz, ZJ & Yaxley, S (1989) Hunger modulates the responses to gustatory stimuli of single neurons in the caudolateral orbitofrontal cortex of the macaque monkey. Eur J Neurosci 1, 5360.Google Scholar
5. Rolls, ET, Murzi, E, Yaxley, S, et al. (1986) Sensory-specific satiety: food-specific reduction in responsiveness of ventral forebrain neurons after feeding in the monkey. Brain Res 368, 7986.Google Scholar
6. Rolls, BJ, Van Duijenvoorde, PM & Rowe, EA (1983) Variety in the diet enhances intake in a meal and contributes to the development of obesity in the rat. Physiol Behav 31, 2127.Google Scholar
7. Rolls, BJ, Rowe, EA, Rolls, ET et al. (1981) Variety in a meal enhances food intake in man. Physiol Behav 26, 215221.Google Scholar
8. Rolls, BJ, Van Duijvenvoorde, PM & Rolls, ET (1984) Pleasantness changes and food intake in a varied four-course meal. Appetite 5, 337348.Google Scholar
9. Berridge, KC, Robinson, TE & Aldridge, JW (2009) Dissecting components of reward: ‘liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol 9, 6573.Google Scholar
10. Rolls, ET & Deco, G (2010) The Noisy Brain: Stochastic Dynamics as a Principle of Brain Function. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
11. Rolls, ET (2008) Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making: A Unifying Computational Neuroscience Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
12. Scott, TR, Yaxley, S, Sienkiewicz, ZJ & Rolls, ET (1986) Gustatory responses in the frontal opercular cortex of the alert cynomolgus monkey. J Neurophysiol 56, 876890.Google Scholar
13. Yaxley, S, Rolls, ET & Sienkiewicz, ZJ (1990) Gustatory responses of single neurons in the insula of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 63, 689700.Google Scholar
14. Rolls, ET & Scott, TR (2003) Central taste anatomy and neurophysiology. In Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation, pp. 679705 [Doty, RL, editor). New York: Dekker.Google Scholar
15. Baylis, LL & Rolls, ET (1991) Responses of neurons in the primate taste cortex to glutamate. Physiol Behav 49, 973979.Google Scholar
16. Rolls, ET, Critchley, H, Wakeman, EA et al. (1996) Responses of neurons in the primate taste cortex to the glutamate ion and to inosine 5′-monophosphate. Physiol Behav 59, 9911000.Google Scholar
17. Verhagen, JV, Kadohisa, M & Rolls, ET (2004) The primate insular/opercular taste cortex: neuronal representations of the viscosity, fat texture, grittiness, temperature and taste of foods. J Neurophysiol 92, 16851699.Google Scholar
18. Rolls, ET, Scott, TR, Sienkiewicz, ZJ et al. (1988) The responsiveness of neurones in the frontal opercular gustatory cortex of the macaque monkey is independent of hunger. J Physiol 397, 112.Google Scholar
19. Yaxley, S, Rolls, ET & Sienkiewicz, ZJ (1988) The responsiveness of neurons in the insular gustatory cortex of the macaque monkey is independent of hunger. Physiol Behav 42, 223229.Google Scholar
20. Rolls, ET, Yaxley, S & Sienkiewicz, ZJ (1990) Gustatory responses of single neurons in the caudolateral orbitofrontal cortex of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 64, 10551066.Google Scholar
21. Rolls, ET (1997) Taste and olfactory processing in the brain and its relation to the control of eating. Crit Rev Neurobiol 11, 263287.Google Scholar
22. Zhao, GQ, Zhang, Y, Hoon, MA et al. (2003) The receptors for mammalian sweet and umami taste. Cell 115, 255266.Google Scholar
23. Maruyama, Y, Pereira, E, Margolskee, RF et al. (2006) Umami responses in mouse taste cells indicate more than one receptor. J Neurosci 26, 22272234.Google Scholar
24. Rolls, ET, Critchley, HD, Browning, A et al. (1998) The neurophysiology of taste and olfaction in primates, and umami flavor. Ann N Y Acad Sci 855, 426437.Google Scholar
25. McCabe, C & Rolls, ET (2007) Umami: a delicious flavor formed by convergence of taste and olfactory pathways in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci 25, 18551864.Google Scholar
26. Rolls, ET (2009) Functional neuroimaging of umami taste: what makes umami pleasant. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 803S814S.Google Scholar
27. Critchley, HD & Rolls, ET (1996) Responses of primate taste cortex neurons to the astringent tastant tannic acid. Chem Senses 21, 135145.Google Scholar
28. Rolls, ET, Verhagen, JV & Kadohisa, M (2003) Representations of the texture of food in the primate orbitofrontal cortex: neurons responding to viscosity, grittiness and capsaicin. J Neurophysiol 90, 37113724.Google Scholar
29. Kadohisa, M, Rolls, ET & Verhagen, JV (2004) Orbitofrontal cortex neuronal representation of temperature and capsaicin in the mouth. Neuroscience 127, 207221.Google Scholar
30. Rolls, ET (2008) Functions of the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortex in taste, olfaction, appetite and emotion. Acta Physiol Hung 95, 131164.Google Scholar
31. Pritchard, TC, Edwards, EM, Smith, CA, et al. (2005) Gustatory neural responses in the medial orbitofrontal cortex of the old world monkey. J Neurosci 25, 60476056.Google Scholar
32. Rolls, ET & Grabenhorst, F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect to decision-making. Prog Neurobiol 86, 216244.Google Scholar
33. Rolls, ET (2007) Sensory processing in the brain related to the control of food intake. Proc Nutr Soc 66, 96112.Google Scholar
34. Critchley, HD & Rolls, ET (1996) Hunger and satiety modify the responses of olfactory and visual neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 75, 16731686.Google Scholar
35. Cabanac, M (1971) Physiological role of pleasure. Science 173, 11031107.Google Scholar
36. Rolls, ET & Rolls, BJ (1977) Activity of neurones in sensory, hypothalamic and motor areas during feeding in the monkey. In Food Intake and Chemical Senses, pp. 525549 [Katsuki, Y Sato, M Takagi, S and Oomura, Y, editors]. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press.Google Scholar
37. Rolls, BJ, Rolls, ET, Rowe, EA et al. (1981) Sensory specific satiety in man. Physiol Behav 27, 137142.Google Scholar
38. Rolls, BJ, Rowe, EA & Rolls, ET (1982) How sensory properties of foods affect human feeding behavior. Physiol Behav 29, 409417.Google Scholar
39. Rolls, ET & Rolls, BJ (1982) Brain mechanisms involved in feeding. In Psychobiology of Human Food Selection, pp. 3362 [Barker, editor LM). Westport, CT: AVI Publishing Company.Google Scholar
40. Rolls, BJ, Rolls, ET & Rowe, EA (1983) Body fat control and obesity. Behav Brain Sci 4, 744745.Google Scholar
41. Rolls, BJ & Hetherington, M (1989) The role of variety in eating and body weight regulation. In Handbook of the Psychophysiology of Human Eating, pp. 5784 [Shepherd, R, editor]. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
42. Rolls, ET & Rolls, JH (1997) Olfactory sensory-specific satiety in humans. Physiol Behav 61, 461473.Google Scholar
43. Hetherington, MM (2007) Cues to overeat: psychological factors influencing overconsumption. Proc Nutr Soc 66, 113123.Google Scholar
44. Rolls, ET, Rolls, BJ & Rowe, EA (1983) Sensory-specific and motivation-specific satiety for the sight and taste of food and water in man. Physiol Behav 30, 185192.Google Scholar
45. Rolls, ET & Baylis, LL (1994) Gustatory, olfactory, and visual convergence within the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci 14, 54375452.Google Scholar
46. Critchley, HD & Rolls, ET (1996) Olfactory neuronal responses in the primate orbitofrontal cortex: analysis in an olfactory discrimination task. J Neurophysiol 75, 16591672.Google Scholar
47. Rolls, ET, Critchley, HD & Treves, A (1996) The representation of olfactory information in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 75, 19821996.Google Scholar
48. Rolls, ET (2011) Chemosensory learning in the cortex. Front Syst Neurosci 5, 78.Google Scholar
49. Thorpe, SJ, Rolls, ET & Maddison, S (1983) Neuronal activity in the orbitofrontal cortex of the behaving monkey. Exp Brain Res 49, 93115.Google Scholar
50. Rolls, ET (1996) The orbitofrontal cortex. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 351, 14331444.Google Scholar
51. Rolls, ET, Critchley, HD, Browning, AS et al. (1999) Responses to the sensory properties of fat of neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci 19, 15321540.Google Scholar
52. Verhagen, JV, Rolls, ET & Kadohisa, M (2003) Neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex respond to fat texture independently of viscosity. J Neurophysiol 90, 15141525.Google Scholar
53. Rolls, ET (2011) The neural representation of oral texture including fat texture. J Texture Stud 42, 137156.Google Scholar
54. Kadohisa, M, Rolls, ET & Verhagen, JV (2005) Neuronal representations of stimuli in the mouth: the primate insular taste cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and amygdala. Chem Senses 30, 401419.Google Scholar
55. Francis, S, Rolls, ET, Bowtell, R et al. (1999) The representation of pleasant touch in the brain and its relationship with taste and olfactory areas. Neuroreport 10, 453459.Google Scholar
56. O'Doherty, J, Rolls, ET, Francis, S et al. (2001) The representation of pleasant and aversive taste in the human brain. J Neurophysiol 85, 13151321.Google Scholar
57. de Araujo, IET, Kringelbach, ML, Rolls, ET et al. (2003) Human cortical responses to water in the mouth, and the effects of thirst. J Neurophysiol 90, 18651876.Google Scholar
58. Grabenhorst, F, Rolls, ET & Bilderbeck, A (2008) How cognition modulates affective responses to taste and flavor: top down influences on the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortices. Cereb Cortex 18, 15491559.Google Scholar
59. Grabenhorst, F & Rolls, ET (2008) Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes taste stimuli. Eur J Neurosci 27, 723729.Google Scholar
60. Zald, DH, Lee, JT, Fluegel, KW et al. (1998) Aversive gustatory stimulation activates limbic circuits in humans. Brain 121, 11431154.Google Scholar
61. de Araujo, IET, Kringelbach, ML, Rolls, ET et al. (2003) The representation of umami taste in the human brain. J Neurophysiol 90, 313319.Google Scholar
62. Poellinger, A, Thomas, R, Lio, P et al. (2001) Activation and habituation in olfaction - an fMRI study. Neuroimage 13, 547560.Google Scholar
63. Sobel, N, Prabkakaran, V, Zhao, Z et al. (2000) Time course of odorant-induced activation in the human primary olfactory cortex. J Neurophysiol 83, 537551.Google Scholar
64. Zald, DH & Pardo, JV (1997) Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94, 41194124.Google Scholar
65. Zatorre, RJ, Jones-Gotman, M, Evans, AC et al. (1992) Functional localization of human olfactory cortex. Nature 360, 339340.Google Scholar
66. O'Doherty, J, Rolls, ET, Francis, S et al. (2000) Sensory-specific satiety related olfactory activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroreport 11, 893897.Google Scholar
67. Rolls, ET, Kringelbach, ML & de Araujo, IET (2003) Different representations of pleasant and unpleasant odors in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci 18, 695703.Google Scholar
68. Grabenhorst, F & Rolls, ET (2011) Value, pleasure, and choice in the ventral prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 15, 5667.Google Scholar
69. Grabenhorst, F, Rolls, ET, Margot, C et al. (2007) How pleasant and unpleasant stimuli combine in different brain regions: odor mixtures. J Neurosci 27, 1353213540.Google Scholar
70. de Araujo, IET, Rolls, ET et al. (2003) Taste-olfactory convergence, and the representation of the pleasantness of flavour, in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci 18, 20592068.Google Scholar
71. Small, DM, Voss, J, Mak, YE et al. (2004) Experience-dependent neural integration of taste and smell in the human brain. J Neurophysiol 92, 18921903.Google Scholar
72. Small, DM & Prescott, J (2005) Odor/taste integration and the perception of flavor. Exp Brain Res 166, 345357.Google Scholar
73. Kringelbach, ML, O'Doherty, J, Rolls, ET et al. (2003) Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb Cortex 13, 10641071.Google Scholar
74. Grabenhorst, F, Rolls, ET, Parris, BA et al. (2010) How the brain represents the reward value of fat in the mouth. Cereb Cortex 20, 10821091.Google Scholar
75. de Araujo, IET & Rolls, ET (2004) The representation in the human brain of food texture and oral fat. J Neurosci 24, 30863093.Google Scholar
76. Rolls, ET (2010) Neural representation of fat texture in the mouth. In Fat Detection: Taste, Texture, and Postingestive Effects, pp. 197223 [Montmayeur, J-P and Coutre, LJ, editors]. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
77. O'Doherty, JP, Deichmann, R, Critchley, HD et al. (2002) Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron 33, 815826.Google Scholar
78. Simmons, WK, Martin, A & Barsalou, LW (2005) Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. Cereb Cortex 15, 16021608.Google Scholar
79. Wang, GJ, Volkow, ND, Telang, F et al. (2004) Exposure to appetitive food stimuli markedly activates the human brain. Neuroimage 21, 17901797.Google Scholar
80. de Araujo, IET, Rolls, ET, Velazco, MI et al. (2005) Cognitive modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron 46, 671679.Google Scholar
81. Rolls, ET, Grabenhorst, F, Margot, C et al. (2008) Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes olfactory stimuli. J Cogn Neurosci 20, 18151826.Google Scholar
82. Grabenhorst, F & Rolls, ET (2010) Attentional modulation of affective vs sensory processing: functional connectivity and a top-down biased activation theory of selective attention. J Neurophysiol 104, 16491660.Google Scholar
83. Ge, T, Feng, J, Grabenhorst, F et al. (2012) Componential Granger causality, and its application to identifying the source and mechanisms of the top-down biased activation that controls attention to affective vs sensory processing. Neuroimage 59, 18461858.Google Scholar
84. Rolls, ET (2011) Consciousness, decision-making, and neural computation. In Perception-Action Cycle: Models, Algorithms and Systems, pp. 287333 [Cutsuridis, V Hussain, A and Taylor, JG, editors]. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
85. Deco, G, Rolls, ET, Albantakis, L et al. (2012) Brain mechanisms for perceptual and reward-related decision-making. Prog. Neurobiol, (Epublication ahead of print version 2 February).Google Scholar
86. Grabenhorst, F, Rolls, ET & Parris, BA (2008) From affective value to decision-making in the prefrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci 28, 19301939.Google Scholar
87. Rolls, ET, Grabenhorst, F & Parris, BA (2010) Neural systems underlying decisions about affective odors. J Cogn Neurosci 22, 10691082.Google Scholar
88. Rolls, ET, Grabenhorst, F & Deco, G (2010) Choice, difficulty, and confidence in the brain. Neuroimage 53, 694706.Google Scholar
89. Rolls, ET, Grabenhorst, F & Deco, G (2010) Decision-making, errors, and confidence in the brain. J Neurophysiol 104, 23592374.Google Scholar
90. Rolls, ET (2010) Attractor networks. WIREs Cogn Sci 1, 119134.Google Scholar
91. Wang, XJ (2008) Decision making in recurrent neuronal circuits. Neuron 60, 215234.Google Scholar
92. Booth, DA (1985) Food-conditioned eating preferences and aversions with interoceptive elements: learned appetites and satieties. Ann N Y Acad Sci 443, 2237.Google Scholar
93. Rolls, ET (2012) Neuroculture. On the Implications of Brain Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
94. Schwartz, MW & Porte, D (2005) Diabetes, obesity, and the brain. Science 307, 375379.Google Scholar
95. Guyenet, SJ & Schwartz, MW (2012) Clinical review: Regulation of food intake, energy balance, and body fat mass: implications for the pathogenesis and treatment of obesity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 97, 745755.Google Scholar
96. Morton, GJ, Cummings, DE, Baskin, DG et al. (2006) Central nervous system control of food intake and body weight. Nature 443, 289295.Google Scholar
97. O'Rahilly, S (2009) Human genetics illuminates the paths to metabolic disease. Nature 462, 307314.Google Scholar
98. Rolls, ET (2006) Brain mechanisms underlying flavour and appetite. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 361, 11231136.Google Scholar
99. Rolls, ET (2005) Taste, olfactory, and food texture processing in the brain, and the control of food intake. Physiol Behav 85, 4556.Google Scholar
100. Rolls, ET (2011) Taste, olfactory, and food texture reward processing in the brain and obesity. Int J Obes 35, 550561.Google Scholar
101. Heitmann, BL, Westerterp, KR, Loos, RJ, et al. (2012) Obesity: lessons from evolution and the environment. Obesity Reviews (Epublication ahead of print, June 2012).Google Scholar
102. Munzberg, H & Myers, MG (2005) Molecular and anatomical determinants of central leptin resistance. Nature Neurosci 8, 566570.Google Scholar
103. Cummings, DE & Overduin, J (2007) Gastrointestinal regulation of food intake. J Clin Invest 117, 1323.Google Scholar
104. Buck, L (2000) Smell and taste: the chemical senses. In Principles of Neural Science, pp. 625647 [Kandel, ER Schwartz, JH and Jessel, editors TH]. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
105. Rolls, ET (2004) Smell, taste, texture and temperature multimodal representations in the brain, and their relevance to the control of appetite. Nutr Rev 62, 193204.Google Scholar
106. Rolls, ET & McCabe, C (2007) Enhanced affective brain representations of chocolate in cravers vs non-cravers. Eur J Neurosci 26, 10671076.Google Scholar
107. Beaver, JD, Lawrence, AD, Ditzhuijzen, JV et al. (2006) Individual differences in reward drive predict neural responses to images of food. J Neurosci 26, 51605166.Google Scholar
108. Schachter, S (1971) Importance of cognitive control in obesity. Am Psychol 26, 129144.Google Scholar
109. Rodin, J (1976) The role of perception of internal and external signals in the regulation of feeding in overweight and non-obese individuals. Life Sciences Research Report. 2, pp. 265281. Berlin: Dahlem Konferenzen.Google Scholar
110. Kral, TV & Rolls, BJ (2004) Energy density and portion size: their independent and combined effects on energy intake. Physiol Behav 82, 131138.Google Scholar
111. Hunt, JN & Stubbs, DF (1975) The volume and energy content of meals as determinants of gastric emptying. J Physiol 245, 209225.Google Scholar
112. Hunt, JN (1980) A possible relation between the regulation of gastric emptying and food intake. Am J Physiol 239, G1G4.Google Scholar
113. Rolls, BJ (2010) Plenary lecture 1: dietary strategies for the prevention and treatment of obesity. Proc Nutr Soc 69, 7079.Google Scholar
114. Berner, LA, Bocarsly, ME, Hoebel, BG et al. (2011) Pharmacological interventions for binge eating: lessons from animal models, current treatments, and future directions. Curr Pharm Des 17, 11801187.Google Scholar
115. Wilks, DC, Besson, H, Lindroos, AK et al. (2011) Objectively measured physical activity and obesity prevention in children, adolescents and adults: a systematic review of prospective studies. Obes Rev 12, e119e129.Google Scholar
116. Thomas, DM, Bouchard, C, Church, T et al. (2012) Why do individuals not lose more weight from an exercise intervention at a defined dose? An energy balance analysis. Obes Rev (Epublication ahead of print).Google Scholar
117. Berlin, H, Rolls, ET & Kischka, U (2004) Impulsivity, time perception, emotion, and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Brain 127, 11081126.Google Scholar
118. Berlin, H, Rolls, ET & Iversen, SD (2005) Borderline Personality Disorder, impulsivity and the orbitofrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry 162, 23602373.Google Scholar
119. Cools, R, Blackwell, A, Clark, L, et al. (2005) Tryptophan depletion disrupts the motivational guidance of goal-directed behavior as a function of trait impulsivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 13621373.Google Scholar
120. Crews, FT & Boettiger, CA (2009) Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for addiction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 93, 237247.Google Scholar
121. Barson, JR, Karatayev, O, Chang, GQ et al. (2009) Positive relationship between dietary fat, ethanol intake, triglycerides, and hypothalamic peptides: counteraction by lipid-lowering drugs. Alcohol Fayetteville, NY 43, 433441.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Schematic diagram showing some of the gustatory, olfactory, visual and somatosensory pathways to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and some of the outputs of the OFC, in primates. The secondary taste cortex and the secondary olfactory cortex are within the OFC. V1, primary visual cortex; V4, visual cortical area V4; PreGen Cing, pregenual cingulate cortex. ‘Gate’ refers to the finding that inputs such as the taste, smell and sight of food in some brain regions only produce effects when hunger is present(1). The column of brain regions including and below the inferior temporal visual cortex represents brain regions in which what stimulus is present is made explicit in the neuronal representation, but not its reward or affective value which are represented in the next tier of brain regions, the OFC and amygdala, and in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). In areas beyond these such as medial prefrontal cortex area 10, choices or decisions about reward value are taken, with the mechanisms described elsewhere(2,10,11). Medial PFC area 10, medial prefrontal cortex area 10; VPL, Ventralposterolateral nucleus of the thalamus; VPMpc, ventralposteromedial thalamic nucleus.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. (Colour online) A neuron in the primate orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) responding to the texture of fat in the mouth independently of viscosity. The cell (bk265) increased its firing rate to a range of fats and oils (the viscosity of which is shown in centipoise). The information that reaches this type of neuron is independent of a viscosity sensing channel, in that the neuron did not respond to the carboxymethyl cellulose (CMC) viscosity series. The neuron responded to the texture rather than the chemical structure of the fat in that it also responded to silicone oil (Si(CH3)2O)n) and paraffin (mineral) oil (hydrocarbon). Some of these neurons have taste inputs (after Verhagen et al.(52)).

Figure 2

Fig. 3. (Colour online) Brain regions in which the activations were correlated with the subjective pleasantness of fat texture: Mid-orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) ([32 34 − 14], z = 3·38, P = 0·013) (a, yellow circle, c showing the relation between the% change in the blood oxygenation-level dependent (BOLD) signal and the rating of the pleasantness of the texture) and anterior cingulate cortex ([2 30 14], z = 3·22, P = 0·016) (a, pink circles and b). (After Grabenhorst Rolls et al.(74).)

Figure 3

Fig. 4. (Colour online) Cognitive modulation of flavour reward processing in the brain. (a) The medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) was more strongly activated when a flavour stimulus was labelled ‘rich and delicious flavour’ (MSGVrich) than when it was labelled ‘boiled vegetable water’ (MSGVbasic) ([ −8 28 −20]). (The flavour stimulus, MSGV, was the taste 0·1 m MSG + 0·005 m inosine 5′ monophosphate combined with a consonant 0·4 % vegetable odour.) (b) The time course of the blood oxygenation-level dependent (BOLD) signals for the two conditions. (c) The peak values of the BOLD signal (mean across subjects (sem)) were significantly different (t = 3·06, df = 11, P = 0·01). (d) The BOLD signal in the medial OFC was correlated with the subjective pleasantness ratings of taste and flavour, as shown by the Statistical Parametric Mapping analysis, and as illustrated (mean across subjects (sem), r = 0·86, P < 0·001). (After Grabenhorst et al.(58).)

Figure 4

Fig. 5. (Colour online) Effect of paying attention to the pleasantness v. the intensity of a taste stimulus. Top: a significant difference related to the taste period was found in the medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) at [−6 14 −20], z= 3·81, P < 0·003 (towards the back of the area of activation shown) and in the pregenual cingulate cortex at [−4 46 −8], z = 2·90, P < 0·04 (at the cursor). Middle: medial OFC. Right: the parameter estimates (mean (sem) across subjects) for the activation at the specified coordinate for the conditions of paying attention to pleasantness or to intensity. The parameter estimates were significantly different for the OFC t = 7·27, df = 11, P < 10−4. Left: the correlation between the pleasantness ratings and the activation (% blood oxygenation-level dependent (BOLD) change) at the specified coordinate (r = 0·94, df = 8, P < < 0·001). Bottom: pregenual cingulate cortex. Conventions as previously. Right: the parameter estimates were significantly different for the pregenual cingulate cortex t = 8·70, df = 11, P < 10−5. Left: the correlation between the pleasantness ratings and the activation (% BOLD change) at the specified coordinate (r=  0·89, df = 8, P = 0·001). The taste stimulus, 0·1 m monosodium glutamate, was identical on all trials (after Grabenhorst and Rolls(59)).

Figure 5

Fig. 6. Schematic diagram to show how sensory factors interact in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) with satiety signals to produce the hedonic, rewarding value of food, which leads to appetite and eating. Cognitive and attentional factors directly modulate the reward system in the brain. The neural mechanisms involved are considered elsewhere(2,10,11).