Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T12:25:38.226Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Autobiographical memory and default mode network function in schizophrenia: an fMRI study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 November 2019

Marta Martin-Subero
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Madrid, Spain Department of Psychiatry and Forensic Medicine, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Paola Fuentes-Claramonte
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Madrid, Spain
Pilar Salgado-Pineda
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Madrid, Spain
Josep Salavert
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain Department of Psychiatry and Forensic Medicine, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain Psychiatry Department, Hospital Sant Rafael, Barcelona, Spain
Antoni Arevalo
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain Psychiatry Department, Hospital Sagrat Cor Martorell Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Clara Bosque
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain Benito Menni Centre Assistencial en Salut Mental, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
Carmen Sarri
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain Benito Menni Centre Assistencial en Salut Mental, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
Amalia Guerrero-Pedraza
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain Benito Menni Centre Assistencial en Salut Mental, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
Aniol Santo-Angles
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
Antoni Capdevila
Affiliation:
Radiology Unit, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau (HSCSP), Barcelona, Spain CIBER-BBN (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red en Bioingeniería, Biomateriales y Nanomedicina), Zaragoza, Spain
Salvador Sarró
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Madrid, Spain
Raymond Salvador
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Madrid, Spain
Peter J. McKenna*
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Madrid, Spain
Edith Pomarol-Clotet
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Madrid, Spain
*
Author for correspondence: Peter J. McKenna, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The brain functional correlates of autobiographical recall are well established, but have been little studied in schizophrenia. Additionally, autobiographical memory is one of a small number of cognitive tasks that activates rather than de-activates the default mode network, which has been found to be dysfunctional in this disorder.

Methods

Twenty-seven schizophrenic patients and 30 healthy controls underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging while viewing cue words that evoked autobiographical memories. Control conditions included both non-memory-evoking cues and a low level baseline (cross fixation).

Results

Compared to both non-memory evoking cues and low level baseline, autobiographical recall was associated with activation in default mode network regions in the controls including the medial frontal cortex, the posterior cingulate cortex and the hippocampus, as well as other areas. Clusters of de-activation were seen outside the default mode network. There were no activation differences between the schizophrenic patients and the controls, but the patients showed clusters of failure of de-activation in non-default mode network regions.

Conclusions

According to this study, patients with schizophrenia show intact activation of the default mode network and other regions associated with recall of autobiographical memories. The finding of failure of de-activation outside the network suggests that schizophrenia may be associated with a general difficulty in de-activation rather than dysfunction of the default mode network per se.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019

Autobiographical recall refers to the conscious re-imagining of events from one's past, with the memories typically being accompanied by some of their original sensory and emotional qualities (Rubin, Reference Rubin1996; Svoboda et al., Reference Svoboda, McKinnon and Levine2006). Autobiographical memory forms part of the broader construct of episodic memory, but differently from standard episodic memory tasks, which employ experimenter-generated stimuli such as lists of words, it is tested by asking subjects to recall memorable events in their own lives. These may be elicited in response to cue words like ‘river’ or ‘puppy’ (the Crovitz task; Crovitz and Schiffman, Reference Crovitz and Schiffman1974), or by means of prompts about events such as starting a new job or attending a wedding (the Autobiographical Memory Test, AMI; Kopelman et al., Reference Kopelman, Wilson and Baddeley1989). Autobiographical memory has been linked conceptually to the ability to imagine future events, and together they form the concept of ‘mental time travel’ (Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2007). Autobiographical memory has also been argued to play a key role in the construction of one's sense of self (Conway and Pleydell-Pearce, Reference Conway and Pleydell-Pearce2000).

As expected, given the evidence for episodic memory impairment in the disorder (e.g. Palmer et al., Reference Palmer, Dawes and Heaton2009), autobiographical memory has been found to be impaired in schizophrenia. A meta-analysis of 20 studies (Berna et al., Reference Berna, Potheegadoo, Aouadi, Ricarte, Alle, Coutelle, Boyer, Cuervo-Lombard and Danion2016) found significantly poorer performance compared to healthy controls in all aspects of autobiographical recall examined; effect sizes were large for richness of detail and specificity of memories, and moderate for conscious recollection, i.e. the degree of personal awareness of participating in the re-experienced events.

To date, there has been only one study of the brain functional correlates of autobiographical recall in schizophrenia: Cuervo-Lombard et al. (Reference Cuervo-Lombard, Lemogne, Gierski, Bera-Potelle, Tran, Portefaix, Kaladjian, Pierot and Limosin2012) compared 13 schizophrenic patients and 14 healthy controls using a task where they saw cue words and pressed a button when they recalled a personal event associated with them. The control task consisted of a button press in response to instructions to use either the middle or index finger to do this. Whole-brain functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with correction for multiple comparisons revealed no major clusters of significant difference between the groups in the cortex, but there were small clusters of reduced activation in the patients in the lateral ventral tegmental area, the right cerebellum and both caudate nuclei. Uncorrected comparisons within a mask comprising the areas activated by the patients and/or the controls, however, revealed additional areas of reduced activation in the patients in the medial frontal cortex, the precuneus, the left lateral prefrontal cortex, the left medial temporal lobe and the occipital cortex.

Autobiographical recall is also of interest from the functional imaging point of view because it has been found to activate the so-called default mode network (Buckner et al., Reference Buckner, Andrews-Hanna and Schacter2008; Raichle, Reference Raichle2015). This network consists of a set of brain regions that are normally active at rest but which de-activate during performance of a wide range of attention-demanding tasks. It includes prominently two midline areas, the medial prefrontal cortex and the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus, as well as parts of the parietal and temporal lobe cortex and the hippocampus (Gusnard and Raichle, Reference Gusnard and Raichle2001; Raichle et al., Reference Raichle, MacLeod, Snyder, Powers, Gusnard and Shulman2001; Buckner et al., Reference Buckner, Andrews-Hanna and Schacter2008). The small number of tasks that have been found to activate rather than de-activate the default mode network regions include imagining the future (Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner2007), making judgements about oneself and others (van der Meer et al., Reference van der Meer, Costafreda, Aleman and David2010; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Schaer and Debbane2012), making moral judgements (Boccia et al., Reference Boccia, Dacquino, Piccardi, Cordellieri, Guariglia, Ferlazzo, Ferracuti and Giannini2017), engaging in theory of mind-type reasoning (Schurz et al., Reference Schurz, Radua, Aichhorn, Richlan and Perner2014) and autobiographical memory. With respect to this last paradigm, Svoboda et al. (Reference Svoboda, McKinnon and Levine2006) meta-analysed 24 positron-emission tomography (PET) and fMRI studies using autobiographical memory tasks and found pooled evidence of activations in the medial frontal cortex and the retrosplenial/posterior cingulate cortex, i.e. the two midline cortical ‘nodes’ of the default mode network, as well as other regions including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, other lateral prefrontal regions, the medial and lateral temporal cortex, the temporoparietal junction and the cerebellum.

Default mode network dysfunction during performance of various cognitive tasks has been reported in schizophrenia since 2007. Two initial studies (Garrity et al., Reference Garrity, Pearlson, McKiernan, Lloyd, Kiehl and Calhoun2007; Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Yucel, Pujol and Pantelis2007) found increased de-activation or a mixed pattern of increased activation and failure of de-activation, respectively. Since then, however, the almost invariable finding has been failure of de-activation, which is typically seen in the medial frontal cortex (Pomarol-Clotet et al., Reference Pomarol-Clotet, Salvador, Sarro, Gomar, Vila, Martinez, Guerrero, Ortiz-Gil, Sans-Sansa, Capdevila, Cebamanos and McKenna2008; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli, Thermenos, Milanovic, Tsuang, Faraone, McCarley, Shenton, Green, Nieto-Castanon, LaViolette, Wojcik, Gabrieli and Seidman2009; Mannell et al., Reference Mannell, Franco, Calhoun, Canive, Thoma and Mayer2010; Salgado-Pineda et al., Reference Salgado-Pineda, Fakra, Delaveau, McKenna, Pomarol-Clotet and Blin2011; Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Royer, Grosselin, Pellet, Barral, Laurent, Brouillet and Lang2011; Dreher et al., Reference Dreher, Koch, Kohn, Apud, Weinberger and Berman2012; Haatveit et al., Reference Haatveit, Jensen, Alnaes, Kaufmann, Brandt, Thoresen, Andreassen, Melle, Ueland and Westlye2016), although the posterior cingulate gyrus/precuneus has also sometimes been found to be affected (Salgado-Pineda et al., Reference Salgado-Pineda, Fakra, Delaveau, McKenna, Pomarol-Clotet and Blin2011; Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Royer, Grosselin, Pellet, Barral, Laurent, Brouillet and Lang2011). There appear to be only two exceptions: using a visual working memory task with various levels of difficulty, Hahn et al. (Reference Hahn, Harvey, Gold, Ross and Stein2017) found that 21 schizophrenic patients and 16 controls showed no differences in de-activation across 13 regions of interest placed in the default mode network, and at the two hardest levels de-activation was significantly greater in the patients. In another study using a task requiring direction of attention to visual stimuli that either predicted or did not predict the location of a subsequent target, the same group (Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Harvey, Gold, Fischer, Keller, Ross and Stein2016) again found no differences in default mode network de-activation in 20 schizophrenic patients compared to 20 healthy controls (when the cue was predictive) or greater de-activation (when the cue was non-predictive).

Given the evidence for failure of default mode de-activation (and perhaps increased de-activation in some circumstances) in schizophrenia, how the network behaves during a task like autobiographical memory, which normally activates it, is clearly of some interest. In the current study, we examined both activations and de-activations associated with autobiographical recall in schizophrenia, using a larger sample of patients and controls than in Cuervo-Lombard et al.'s (Reference Cuervo-Lombard, Lemogne, Gierski, Bera-Potelle, Tran, Portefaix, Kaladjian, Pierot and Limosin2012) study and employing whole-brain analysis with correction for multiple comparisons.

Methods

Subjects

The patient sample consisted of 27 right-handed patients meeting DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia, recruited from three psychiatric hospitals in Barcelona (Benito Menni CASM, Hospital Sagrat Cor de Martorell and Sant Rafael Hospital). The diagnosis was established using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Disorders (SCID) (First et al., Reference First, Spitzer, Miriam and Williams2002). Patients were excluded if they (a) were younger than 18 or older than 65, (b) had a history of brain trauma or neurological disease or (c) had shown alcohol/substance abuse/dependence within 12 months prior to participation. With respect to the last criterion, all participants were questioned about alcohol and drug use during the previous year, and we also excluded those who reported habitual use of cannabis. Social use of alcohol was permitted, as was non-habitual use of cannabis. All the patients were taking antipsychotic treatment (23 on atypical neuroleptics, one on typical neuroleptics and three on both).

The control sample consisted of 30 right-handed healthy individuals recruited from non-clinical staff working in the hospitals, their relatives and acquaintances, plus independent sources in the community. They met the same exclusion criteria as the patients and they were also interviewed using the SCID to exclude current and past psychiatric disorders. They were questioned and also excluded if they reported a history of treatment with psychotropic medication beyond non habitual use of night sedation.

The two groups were selected to be matched for age, sex and estimated IQ (premorbid IQ in the patients). This latter was measured using the Word Accentuation Test (Test de Acentuación de Palabras, TAP; Del Ser et al., Reference Del Ser, Gonzalez-Montalvo, Martinez-Espinosa, Delgado-Villapalos and Bermejo1997; Gomar et al., Reference Gomar, Ortiz-Gil, McKenna, Salvador, Sans-Sansa, Sarro, Guerrero and Pomarol-Clotet2011). All patients were scanned when in a relatively stable condition.

All participants gave written informed consent. All the study procedures were approved by the local research ethics committee.

Autobiographical memory task

The task used was based on the one developed by Oertel-Knochel et al. (Reference Oertel-Knochel, Reinke, Hornung, Knochel, Matura, Knopf, Prvulovic, O'Dwyer, Lindner, Rau, Hampel, Haenschel and Linden2012), which employed personalised cues that had previously been found to evoke autobiographical memories in the subjects. While they employed a control which involved completing a sentence with a semantically appropriate word, we changed this to one involving viewing of cues that had previously been found not to evoke autobiographical memories.

Before the fMRI session, each participant was administered the cue words from the Crovitz test (Crovitz and Schiffman, Reference Crovitz and Schiffman1974) and the autobiographical prompt phrases from the AMI (Kopelman et al., Reference Kopelman, Wilson and Baddeley1989), in order to generate between four and six autobiographical memories for each of the time periods covering childhood, adolescence, adulthood and the preceding year. The stimuli chosen for the fMRI paradigm consisted of groups of three words personalised for each participant. The first word in the group referred to one of the above four time periods, and the other two words were chosen on the basis that they had previously evoked autobiographical memories (e.g. childhood-grandmother-cake; adult-car-robbery). All memories were required to have been given the maximum score of 3 in the AMI, indicating that they were clearly specified in time and place and descriptively rich. For the control conditions, we randomly selected groups of three words from the words that did not evoke autobiographical memories.

Ten blocks of non-memory-evoking stimuli were alternated with ten blocks of memory-evoking stimuli; all blocks lasted 20 s. Each block contained two cue sentences of the appropriate type. Subjects were instructed to recollect the memory previously evoked by the three word phrase, or in the case of the non-memory-evoking phrase, to read the phrase with no further requirements. A low level baseline condition was also employed, cross fixation. This was presented between blocks for 16 s.

At the end of the scanning session all participants were asked what they had been thinking about during each condition. Specifically, we asked if they could recollect the memories that they reported in the previous interview during the memory-evoking condition, and whether they were wide awake and focused during the session. Participants who responded negatively to either of these questions were excluded.

Image acquisition

Images were acquired with a 3T Philips Achieva scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands). Functional data were acquired using a T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence with the following acquisition parameters: TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 78°, in-plane resolution = 3 × 3 mm, FOV = 240 mm, slice thickness = 3 mm, inter-slice gap = 1 mm. The autobiographical memory task consisted of 360 volumes. Slices (32 per volume) were acquired with an interleaved order parallel to the AC-PC plane. Before the functional sequences, a high-resolution anatomical 3D volume was acquired using a Turbo Field Echo sequence for anatomical reference and inspection (TR = 8.15 ms; TE = 3.73 ms; flip angle = 8°; voxel size = 0.9375 × 0.9375 mm; slice thickness = 1 mm; slice number = 160; FOV = 240 mm).

Any subjects with excessive head movement during the fMRI sequence, defined as an estimated maximum absolute movement >3.0 mm or an average absolute movement >0.3 mm, were excluded.

Image preprocessing and analysis

Preprocessing and analysis was carried out with the FEAT module included in the FSL (FMRIB Software Library) software (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Woolrich, Beckmann, Behrens, Johansen-Berg, Bannister, De Luca, Drobnjak, Flitney, Niazy, Saunders, Vickers, Zhang, De Stefano, Brady and Matthews2004). The first 20 s, corresponding to signal stabilisation, were discarded. Preprocessing included motion correction (using the MCFLIRT algorithm) and co-registration and normalisation to a common stereotactic space (MNI template). Before group analyses, normalised images were spatially filtered with a Gaussian filter (FWHM = 5 mm).

Statistical analysis was performed by means of a general linear model (GLM). Two regressors of interest were defined at the single-subject level analysis (memory-evoking blocks and non-memory-evoking blocks) and the GLM was fitted to generate activation maps of each condition compared to baseline and the comparison between conditions. Group comparisons between patients and controls were performed within the FEAT module, with mixed-effects GLMs (Beckmann et al., Reference Beckmann, Jenkinson, Woolrich, Behrens, Flitney, Devlin and Smith2006).

Statistical tests were carried out at the cluster level with a corrected p value of 0.05 using Gaussian random field methods. A threshold of z = 3.1 was used to define the initial set of clusters.

Contrasts used in the analysis

In order to examine autobiographical memory associated activations, the main contrast employed was that between cues that evoked and did not evoke autobiographical memories, something that should eliminate ‘noise’ due to aspects of performance common to both tasks.

For de-activations, we focused on the contrast between memory-evoking cues and the low level baseline. This was to avoid a methodological problem identified by Gusnard and Raichle (Reference Gusnard and Raichle2001), that examination of relative changes between two active tasks will not necessarily reveal the true picture of activations and de-activations. Specifically, because fMRI analysis is subtractive, task-associated decreases in activation will be obtained not only when there is greater de-activation from baseline levels in the task of interest (in this case memory evoking cues) than in the control task (in this case non-memory-evoking cues), but also if there is greater activation from baseline levels in the control task than in the task of interest. It follows that de-activations can only be confidently identified with respect to a low level baseline.

Results

Demographic data

Age, sex and TAP-estimated IQ data for the patients and controls are shown in Table 1. As can be seen, the two groups were matched on all three variables. None of the patients and two of the controls reported sporadic use of cannabis.

Table 1. Demographic data for the patients and controls

Values for age and premorbid IQ are means (sd)

fMRI findings: memory-evoking v. non-memory evoking cues

In this contrast the healthy controls showed a large cluster of greater activation to the memory-evoking cues than the non-memory-evoking cues in the medial frontal cortex extending to the orbitofrontal cortex and temporal poles bilaterally, as well as to the thalamus, the basal ganglia, the hippocampus and the parahippocampal cortex. This cluster also extended posteriorly to include a portion of the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus and the calcarine cortex. Other clusters of activation included the left temporo-parietal junction (comprising the posterior portion of middle temporal cortex, the angular gyrus and the middle occipital cortex), the left middle temporal cortex, and the right angular gyrus (see Fig. 1, top panel, and online Supplementary Table S1).

Fig. 1. Areas of significant differences between the autobiographical memory-evoking and non-evoking cue conditions for the healthy subjects (a) and the schizophrenic patients (b). Warm colours represent autobiographical recall > non-memory-evoking cues, cold colours represent non-memory-evoking cues > autobiographical recall. Bottom row (c) shows areas of significant differences between the patients and controls in this contrast. Colour bars depict Z values. Images are displayed in neurological convention (right is right).

The healthy controls also showed three clusters of increased activation to non-memory-evoking than to memory-evoking cues. These were in the occipital cortex bilaterally, the lateral parietal cortex bilaterally, more in the right hemisphere, the superior temporal cortex bilaterally and the right frontal polar cortex.

The schizophrenic patients (Fig. 1, middle panel) showed a broadly similar pattern of activation, although this appeared visually less extensive in the medial frontal cortex and subcortical regions. Unlike the controls, they showed no regions where there was relatively greater activation in response to non-memory-evoking cues.

Significant group differences (Fig. 1, bottom panel) were observed in four relatively small clusters: the right lingual gyrus [294 voxels, peak activation at BA 19, MNI (26, −54, −4), z score = 4.52, p = 0.003], the right cuneus [279 voxels, peak activation at BA 18, MNI (6, −90, 26), z score = 3.86, p = 0.004], the left middle temporal cortex [263 voxels, peak activation at BA 21, MNI (−68, −10, −2), z score = 4.48, p = 0.006] and the right angular gyrus [199 voxels, peak activation at BA 40, MNI (54, −52, 38), z score = 4.03, p = 0.02]. As can be seen from Fig. 1, these clusters of significant difference were all in regions where the healthy controls showed greater activation to non-memory-evoking cues than to memory-evoking cues. Box plots of mean activations within these four clusters are shown in online Supplementary Fig. S1. This confirmed that they all represented regions of relatively greater activation in the patients.

To investigate the possible influence of antipsychotic treatment on the above findings, the within-group analysis for the schizophrenic patients was repeated adding medication dose (in chlorpromazine equivalents) as a covariate. The findings in this group remained closely similar (see online Supplementary Material, Fig. S2A).

fMRI findings: memory-evoking cues v. low level baseline

Compared to cross fixation, the healthy controls showed a similar but more extensive pattern of activation than in the autobiographical memory-evoking v. non-evoking cue contrast. A large cluster encompassed the posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus, the left angular gyrus, the middle temporal cortex bilaterally, parts of the lateral prefrontal cortex and anterior insula bilaterally and the medial prefrontal cortex, and also extended to the occipital cortex, the hippocampus and parahippocampus, the thalamus, the basal ganglia and the cerebellum. A second cluster covered the posterior portion of the right middle temporal cortex and right angular gyrus. The third cluster of activation was located in the precuneus. The findings are shown in Fig. 2, top panel; further details are given in online Supplementary Table S2.

Fig. 2. Activation map for the autobiographical memory-evoking cues v. fixation condition in the healthy subjects (a) and the schizophrenic patients (b). Warm colours represent autobiographical cues > baseline. Cold colours represent baseline > autobiographical cues. The third panel (c) shows areas of significant differences between the patients and the controls in this contrast. Colour bars depict Z values. Images are displayed in neurological convention (right is right).

As can also be seen in Fig. 2, the healthy controls also showed clusters of de-activation compared to cross fixation. There were bilateral clusters in the superior temporal gyrus extending to the postcentral gyrus; the cluster on the right also extended to the superior parietal cortex and parts of the posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus. Two more bilateral clusters were seen in the inferior temporal cortex extending to the lateral occipital cortex. A fifth cluster was in the left superior parietal cortex.

The patients showed a broadly similar pattern of activation to the healthy controls, with large clusters in the medial prefrontal cortex and the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus, hippocampus and parahippocampus, as well as in the lateral prefrontal cortex bilaterally, the left temporal and parietal cortex and parts of the occipital cortex bilaterally. However, they showed no clusters of de-activation (see Fig. 2, middle panel, and online Supplementary Table S2). As in the contrast between memory-evoking and non-evoking cues, repeating the within-group analysis for the schizophrenic patients adding medication dose (in chlorpromazine equivalents) as a covariate made little difference to the findings (see Supplementary Material, Fig. S2B).

There were no regions where the patients showed less activation compared to cross fixation than the controls (Fig. 2, bottom panel). However, the patients showed greater activation than the controls in seven clusters: the largest was in the right parietal cortex [1336 voxels, peak activation at BA 40, MNI (38, −44, 50), z score = 4.86, p < 0.001]; a roughly symmetrical but smaller cluster was in the left parietal cortex [563 voxels, peak activation at BA 40, MNI (−42, −46, 52), z score = 4.39, p < 0.001]; other clusters were in the postcentral gyrus [324 voxels, peak activation at BA 48, MNI (−64, −18, 22), z score = 4.08, p < 0.001]; the left middle temporal cortex [255 voxels, peak activation at BA 22, MNI (−62, −12, −2), z score = 5.01, p = 0.00329]; the left inferior temporal cortex [191 voxels, peak activation at BA 37, MNI (−48, −56, −8), z score = 4.58, p = 0.0145]; the left superior occipital cortex [163 voxels, peak activation at BA 19, MNI (−20, −74, 40), z score = 4.22, p = 0.029]; and the insula [148 voxels, peak activation at BA 48, MNI (−36, −20, 12), z score = 4.56, p = 0.0426].

Box plots of the mean activations within these seven clusters confirmed that in six cases they represented failure of de-activation in the schizophrenic patients (see Supplementary Fig. S3). The seventh cluster (in the superior occipital cortex) was in a region where the controls showed no significant activation or de-activation.

Discussion

This study examined the brain functional correlates of autobiographical recall in schizophrenia, comparing it to two control tasks, viewing of non-memory evoking cues and cross fixation. Under both conditions the healthy controls showed activation within the territory of the default mode network, particularly in its two midline cortical regions. The patients did not differ significantly from the healthy controls in the degree of activation in these regions. However, they did show evidence of changes, which took the form mainly of failure of de-activation, in regions outside the default mode network.

The healthy controls in our study showed a pattern of autobiographical memory associated activations that was reasonably consistent with that found by Svoboda et al. (Reference Svoboda, McKinnon and Levine2006) in their meta-analysis of 24 studies. The most important difference was that, whereas Svoboda et al. (Reference Svoboda, McKinnon and Levine2006) found evidence of lateral frontal activation during task performance, in our study this was only seen under the memory cue evoking condition v. low level baseline (cross fixation), and not in the memory cue evoking v. non-evoking contrast. One possible explanation for this difference might be that the participants actively engaged in search (i.e. executive) strategies during both the active conditions, before either locating or failing to locate a relevant autobiographical memory. Some support for this explanation comes from a study of healthy subjects by Cabeza et al. (Reference Cabeza, Prince, Daselaar, Greenberg, Budde, Dolcos, LaBar and Rubin2004) which, as in our study, contrasted cues designed to both elicit and not elicit autobiographical memories (in this case, photographs of a university campus taken by the subject him-or herself or by others). Both conditions were found to activate the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex compared to cross fixation, with no differences being found between the two active conditions.

Our findings in schizophrenic patients differ markedly from those of the only other study to date, that of Cuervo-Lombard et al. (Reference Cuervo-Lombard, Lemogne, Gierski, Bera-Potelle, Tran, Portefaix, Kaladjian, Pierot and Limosin2012). These authors found reduced activation in the patients in the medial frontal cortex and the precuneus, and also in the left lateral prefrontal cortex, the left medial temporal lobe and other areas, whereas we found no evidence of reduced activation in any region. However, there is an obvious potential explanation for this discrepancy: as noted in the introduction, Cuervo-Lombard et al.'s (Reference Cuervo-Lombard, Lemogne, Gierski, Bera-Potelle, Tran, Portefaix, Kaladjian, Pierot and Limosin2012) findings of reduced cortical activation in the schizophrenic patients were only obtained when a masked analysis using an uncorrected threshold was employed; whole brain corrected analysis revealed differences between the patients and the controls only in small clusters located in non-cortical regions.

Nevertheless, the schizophrenic patients in our study did show evidence of brain functional abnormality at the whole-brain corrected level. Clusters of significant difference were seen in both the memory-evoking v. non-memory evoking cues and in the memory-evoking cues v. low level contrasts, which took the form of relatively greater activation in both cases. As pointed out by Gusnard and Raichle (Reference Gusnard and Raichle2001, see Methods), the interpretation of relative changes between two active tasks can be difficult, but the findings in the contrast between autobiographical memory-evoking cues and low-level baseline were clear: they represented failure of de-activation in the patients in six of the seven clusters of significant difference that emerged (the pattern in the seventh cluster, in the superior occipital cortex, was one of activation in a region where the controls showed neither activation nor de-activation). All these seven clusters were outside the regions usually considered to form part of the default mode network as identified by de-activations in studies using attention-demanding tasks (Buckner et al., Reference Buckner, Andrews-Hanna and Schacter2008) or based on resting state connectivity (Yeo et al., Reference Yeo, Krienen, Sepulcre, Sabuncu, Lashkari, Hollinshead, Roffman, Smoller, Zollei, Polimeni, Fischl, Liu and Buckner2011).

The obvious interpretation of this finding is that schizophrenic patients show failure of de-activation outside the default mode network during performance of a task that normally activates it. Clearly, however, such an interpretation depends on to what extent autobiographical recall can be considered to be normally associated with a pattern of non-default mode network de-activation. Unfortunately, this question is difficult to answer, as most studies of autobiographical recall in healthy subjects have not reported de-activations. Four early studies of autobiographical recall using PET noted both task related activations (i.e. recall > rest) and de-activations (i.e. rest > recall) (Andreasen et al., Reference Andreasen, O'Leary, Cizadlo, Arndt, Rezai, Watkins, Ponto and Hichwa1995; Fink et al., Reference Fink, Markowitsch, Reinkemeier, Bruckbauer, Kessler and Heiss1996; Gemar et al., Reference Gemar, Kapur, Segal, Brown and Houle1996; Andreasen et al., Reference Andreasen, O'Leary, Paradiso, Cizadlo, Arndt, Watkins, Ponto and Hichwa1999), but the sample sizes were mostly small (7–19 subjects) in these studies and the areas of de-activation varied widely. Only two fMRI studies appear to have reported de-activations. Ino et al. (Reference Ino, Nakai, Azuma, Kimura and Fukuyama2011) examined 21 healthy subjects and found greater activation under a ‘no thinking’ condition than during autobiographical recall in the temporal poles, orbitofrontal cortex, posterior insula, and portions of the bilateral middle/superior temporal, inferior parietal, and occipital cortex. De-activation was also observed in the mid-cingulate cortex and precuneus, extending into the superior parietal cortex. Bado et al. (Reference Bado, Engel, de Oliveira-Souza, Bramati, Paiva, Basilio, Sato, Tovar-Moll and Moll2014) examined 18 healthy subjects and found greater activation during a ‘relax and stay awake’ condition than during recall of both emotional and neutral autobiographical memories in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, the ventral striatum and the hypothalamus/septal area, and additionally in the inferior parietal lobe bilaterally during recall of emotional memories. Collectively, these findings support the idea that autobiographical recall is associated with de-activations, and there are hints of an overlap with the findings from our own group of healthy controls.

The only other relevant data come from a study by DuPre et al. (Reference DuPre, Luh and Spreng2016) in which 31 healthy adults viewed emotional pictures and engaged in autobiographical recall, prospection or theory of mind reasoning based on their content. Data from this study are publicly available at NeuroVault (https://neurovault.org/collections/1866/). Contrasting self-generated thought (i.e. combining all three task conditions) to a baseline consisting of viewing a scrambled image followed by a button press, clusters of de-activation were seen in the parietal and temporal regions not dissimilar to those we observed in our study, as well as in the occipital cortex.

If replicated, the current study's finding of intact default mode activation but failure of de-activation outside the default mode network in schizophrenia would appear to have two main implications for the pathophysiology of the disorder. The first is that, rather than there being dysfunction of the default mode network specifically in schizophrenia, it may be that there is a more general problem with de-activation, which manifests itself in different regions (i.e. within or outside the default mode network) depending on the task used. Just such a proposal has recently been made by Allen et al. (Reference Allen, Sommer, Jardri, Eysenck and Hugdahl2019), who argue that instead of schizophrenia being associated with ‘static’ dysfunctions in the prefrontal cortex and the default mode network, in reality the underlying brain dysfunction involves the dynamic balance between the ‘task positive’ networks (one of which is the fronto-parietal, executive or cognitive control network) and the ‘task negative’ or default mode network with which they are normally anticorrelated (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Snyder, Vincent, Corbetta, Van Essen and Raichle2005). Allen et al. (Reference Allen, Sommer, Jardri, Eysenck and Hugdahl2019) go on to relate such a dysfunction to a change in the balance between the main excitatory and inhibitory transmitters in the brain, glutamate and GABA, although the evidence for dysfunction in the latter of these transmitter systems in schizophrenia is currently slender.

Second, the current study's findings bear on of the issue of how far default mode network dysfunction might underlie the cognitive impairment seen in schizophrenia. Anticevic et al. (Reference Anticevic, Cole, Murray, Corlett, Wang and Krystal2012) reviewed evidence on the relationship between default mode network activity and cognitive function in healthy subjects, noting that lower default mode network activity has been found to be associated with better performance across a number of cognitive tasks, and that higher levels of activity are correlated with lapses of attention. Based on this they suggested that the relevance of default mode network de-activation to cognition in schizophrenia warranted further investigation. So far, however, this investigation has been extremely limited. In what appears to be the only study to directly address this question, Ortiz-Gil et al. (Reference Ortiz-Gil, Pomarol-Clotet, Salvador, Canales-Rodriguez, Sarro, Gomar, Guerrero, Sans-Sansa, Capdevila, Junque and McKenna2011) examined 18 cognitively impaired and 19 (relatively) cognitively preserved schizophrenic patients during performance of the n-back working memory task. They found that while the cognitively impaired patients showed hypoactivation compared to the cognitively preserved patients in the DLPFC and other regions, no differences in de-activation between the two groups were seen, even though the group of patients as a whole showed the expected failure of de-activation in the medial frontal cortex. The current study's finding of intact activation during autobiographical recall in schizophrenia adds a further, if currently not entirely clear, dimension to this debate.

In conclusion, the current study finds no evidence of altered default mode network function in schizophrenia during performance of a task which normally activates it, autobiographical recall. This finding needs to be viewed in the context of (a) conflicting findings in the only other study to use such a task in schizophrenia; and (b) the current lack of certainty that autobiographical recall is associated with de-activation outside the default mode network in healthy subjects. Further studies using not only autobiographical memory but perhaps also other tasks that activate the default mode network and, crucially, employing a low-level baseline in order to examine de-activations, would therefore be desirable. Some limitations need to be acknowledged. The sample sizes we employed were relatively small, and it is possible that activation differences between schizophrenic patients and controls may have emerged if these were larger. The schizophrenic patients were taking antipsychotic medication, and this potential confounding factor is not easy to fully address in a study making comparisons with healthy subjects. We did not measure autobiographical memory performance in the patients and so there is an unanswered question about whether and to what extent task related activations (and de-activations) were influenced by poor task performance.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719003052.

Financial support

This work was supported by CIBERSAM and the Catalonian Government (2017 SGR 1271 and 2017 SGR 1265). Also by a grant from the Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades: Juan de la Cierva-formación contract (FJCI-2015-25278 to PF-C) and a Research Project Grant (FFI2016-77647-C2-2-P to PS-P). And by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III, co-funded by European Union (ERDF/ESF, ‘Investing in your future’): Miguel Servet Research contract (MSII16/00018 to EP-C), Rio Hortega contract (CM15/00024 to MM-S), and Research Project Grants (PI18/00877 to RS, PI18/00880 to PM).

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed equally to this study.

The online version of this article has been updated since original publication. A notice detailing the changes has also been published at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720001361

References

Allen, P, Sommer, IE, Jardri, R, Eysenck, MW and Hugdahl, K (2019) Extrinsic and default mode networks in psychiatric conditions: relationship to excitatory-inhibitory transmitter balance and early trauma. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 99, 90100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, NC, O'Leary, DS, Cizadlo, T, Arndt, S, Rezai, K, Watkins, GL, Ponto, LL and Hichwa, RD (1995) Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. American Journal of Psychiatry 152, 15761585.Google ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, NC, O'Leary, DS, Paradiso, S, Cizadlo, T, Arndt, S, Watkins, GL, Ponto, LL and Hichwa, RD (1999) The cerebellum plays a role in conscious episodic memory retrieval. Human Brain Mapping 8, 226234.3.0.CO;2-4>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anticevic, A, Cole, MW, Murray, JD, Corlett, PR, Wang, XJ and Krystal, JH (2012) The role of default network deactivation in cognition and disease. Trends in Cognitive Science 16, 584592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bado, P, Engel, A, de Oliveira-Souza, R, Bramati, IE, Paiva, FF, Basilio, R, Sato, JR, Tovar-Moll, F and Moll, J (2014) Functional dissociation of ventral frontal and dorsomedial default mode network components during resting state and emotional autobiographical recall. Human Brain Mapping 35, 33023313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckmann, CF, Jenkinson, M, Woolrich, MW, Behrens, TE, Flitney, DE, Devlin, JT and Smith, SM (2006) Applying FSL to the FIAC data: model-based and model-free analysis of voice and sentence repetition priming. Human Brain Mapping 27, 380391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berna, F, Potheegadoo, J, Aouadi, I, Ricarte, JJ, Alle, MC, Coutelle, R, Boyer, L, Cuervo-Lombard, CV and Danion, JM (2016) A meta-analysis of autobiographical memory studies in schizophrenia spectrum disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 5666.Google ScholarPubMed
Boccia, M, Dacquino, C, Piccardi, L, Cordellieri, P, Guariglia, C, Ferlazzo, F, Ferracuti, S and Giannini, AM (2017) Neural foundation of human moral reasoning: an ALE meta-analysis about the role of personal perspective. Brain Imaging and Behavior 11, 278292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, RL, Andrews-Hanna, JR and Schacter, DL (2008) The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124, 138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabeza, R, Prince, SE, Daselaar, SM, Greenberg, DL, Budde, M, Dolcos, F, LaBar, KS and Rubin, DC (2004) Brain activity during episodic retrieval of autobiographical and laboratory events: an fMRI study using a novel photo paradigm. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16, 15831594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, MA and Pleydell-Pearce, CW (2000) The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review 107, 261288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crovitz, HF and Schiffman, H (1974) Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4, 517518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuervo-Lombard, C, Lemogne, C, Gierski, F, Bera-Potelle, C, Tran, E, Portefaix, C, Kaladjian, A, Pierot, L and Limosin, F (2012) Neural basis of autobiographical memory retrieval in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 201, 473480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Ser, T, Gonzalez-Montalvo, JI, Martinez-Espinosa, S, Delgado-Villapalos, C and Bermejo, F (1997) Estimation of premorbid intelligence in Spanish people with the word accentuation test and its application to the diagnosis of dementia. Brain and Cognition 33, 343356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dreher, JC, Koch, P, Kohn, P, Apud, J, Weinberger, DR and Berman, KF (2012) Common and differential pathophysiological features accompany comparable cognitive impairments in medication-free patients with schizophrenia and in healthy aging subjects. Biological Psychiatry 71, 890897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DuPre, E, Luh, WM and Spreng, RN (2016) Multi-echo fMRI replication sample of autobiographical memory, prospection and theory of mind reasoning tasks. Scientific Data 3, 160116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fink, GR, Markowitsch, HJ, Reinkemeier, M, Bruckbauer, T, Kessler, J and Heiss, WD (1996) Cerebral representation of one's own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. Journal of Neuroscience 16, 42754282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, MB, Spitzer, R, Miriam, M and Williams, JBW (2002) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
Fox, MD, Snyder, AZ, Vincent, JL, Corbetta, M, Van Essen, DC and Raichle, ME (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U S A 102, 96739678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrity, AG, Pearlson, GD, McKiernan, K, Lloyd, D, Kiehl, KA and Calhoun, VD (2007) Aberrant ‘default mode’ functional connectivity in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 450457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gemar, MC, Kapur, S, Segal, ZV, Brown, GM and Houle, S (1996) Effects of self-generated sad mood on regional cerebral activity: a pet study in normal subjects. Depression 4, 8188.3.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomar, JJ, Ortiz-Gil, J, McKenna, PJ, Salvador, R, Sans-Sansa, B, Sarro, S, Guerrero, A and Pomarol-Clotet, E (2011) Validation of the word accentuation test (TAP) as a means of estimating premorbid IQ in Spanish speakers. Schizophrenia Research 128, 175176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gusnard, DA and Raichle, ME (2001) Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2, 685694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haatveit, B, Jensen, J, Alnaes, D, Kaufmann, T, Brandt, CL, Thoresen, C, Andreassen, OA, Melle, I, Ueland, T and Westlye, LT (2016) Reduced load-dependent default mode network deactivation across executive tasks in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Neuroimage Clinical 12, 389396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, B, Harvey, AN, Gold, JM, Fischer, BA, Keller, WR, Ross, TJ and Stein, EA (2016) Hyperdeactivation of the default mode network in people with schizophrenia when focusing attention in space. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 11581166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, B, Harvey, AN, Gold, JM, Ross, TJ and Stein, EA (2017) Load-dependent hyperdeactivation of the default mode network in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 185, 190196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, BJ, Yucel, M, Pujol, J and Pantelis, C (2007) Task-induced deactivation of midline cortical regions in schizophrenia assessed with fMRI. Schizophrenia Research 91, 8286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ino, T, Nakai, R, Azuma, T, Kimura, T and Fukuyama, H (2011) Brain activation during autobiographical memory retrieval with special reference to default mode network. Open Neuroimaging Journal 5, 1423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopelman, MD, Wilson, BA and Baddeley, AD (1989) The autobiographical memory interview: a new assessment of autobiographical and personal semantic memory in amnesic patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 11, 724744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mannell, MV, Franco, AR, Calhoun, VD, Canive, JM, Thoma, RJ and Mayer, AR (2010) Resting state and task-induced deactivation: a methodological comparison in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Human Brain Mapping 31, 424437.Google ScholarPubMed
Murray, RJ, Schaer, M and Debbane, M (2012) Degrees of separation: a quantitative neuroimaging meta-analysis investigating self-specificity and shared neural activation between self- and other-reflection. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36, 10431059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oertel-Knochel, V, Reinke, B, Hornung, A, Knochel, C, Matura, S, Knopf, M, Prvulovic, D, O'Dwyer, L, Lindner, M, Rau, A, Hampel, H, Haenschel, C and Linden, DE (2012) Patterns of autobiographical memory in bipolar disorder examined by psychometric and functional neuroimaging methods. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 200, 296304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortiz-Gil, J, Pomarol-Clotet, E, Salvador, R, Canales-Rodriguez, EJ, Sarro, S, Gomar, JJ, Guerrero, A, Sans-Sansa, B, Capdevila, A, Junque, C and McKenna, PJ (2011) Neural correlates of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 199, 202210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, BW, Dawes, SE and Heaton, RK (2009) What do we know about neuropsychological aspects of schizophrenia? Neuropsychological Review 19, 365384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pomarol-Clotet, E, Salvador, R, Sarro, S, Gomar, J, Vila, F, Martinez, A, Guerrero, A, Ortiz-Gil, J, Sans-Sansa, B, Capdevila, A, Cebamanos, JM and McKenna, PJ (2008) Failure to deactivate in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: dysfunction of the default mode network? Psychological Medicine 38, 11851193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raichle, ME (2015) The brain's default mode network. Annual Review of Neuroscience 38, 433447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raichle, ME, MacLeod, AM, Snyder, AZ, Powers, WJ, Gusnard, DA and Shulman, GL (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U S A 98, 676682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, C (ed.) (1996) Remembering our Past: Studies in Autobiographical Memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salgado-Pineda, P, Fakra, E, Delaveau, P, McKenna, PJ, Pomarol-Clotet, E and Blin, O (2011) Correlated structural and functional brain abnormalities in the default mode network in schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research 125, 101109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, DL, Addis, DR and Buckner, RL (2007) Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8, 657661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, FC, Royer, A, Grosselin, A, Pellet, J, Barral, FG, Laurent, B, Brouillet, D and Lang, F (2011) Modulation of the default mode network is task-dependant in chronic schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research 125, 110117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schurz, M, Radua, J, Aichhorn, M, Richlan, F and Perner, J (2014) Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 42, 934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, SM, Jenkinson, M, Woolrich, MW, Beckmann, CF, Behrens, TE, Johansen-Berg, H, Bannister, PR, De Luca, M, Drobnjak, I, Flitney, DE, Niazy, RK, Saunders, J, Vickers, J, Zhang, Y, De Stefano, N, Brady, JM and Matthews, PM (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(Suppl 1), S208S219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Svoboda, E, McKinnon, MC and Levine, B (2006) The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 21892208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Meer, L, Costafreda, S, Aleman, A and David, AS (2010) Self-reflection and the brain: a theoretical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications for schizophrenia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34, 935946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S, Thermenos, HW, Milanovic, S, Tsuang, MT, Faraone, SV, McCarley, RW, Shenton, ME, Green, AI, Nieto-Castanon, A, LaViolette, P, Wojcik, J, Gabrieli, JD and Seidman, LJ (2009) Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of persons with schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U S A 106, 12791284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeo, BT, Krienen, FM, Sepulcre, J, Sabuncu, MR, Lashkari, D, Hollinshead, M, Roffman, JL, Smoller, JW, Zollei, L, Polimeni, JR, Fischl, B, Liu, H and Buckner, RL (2011) The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology 106, 11251165.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic data for the patients and controls

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Areas of significant differences between the autobiographical memory-evoking and non-evoking cue conditions for the healthy subjects (a) and the schizophrenic patients (b). Warm colours represent autobiographical recall > non-memory-evoking cues, cold colours represent non-memory-evoking cues > autobiographical recall. Bottom row (c) shows areas of significant differences between the patients and controls in this contrast. Colour bars depict Z values. Images are displayed in neurological convention (right is right).

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Activation map for the autobiographical memory-evoking cues v. fixation condition in the healthy subjects (a) and the schizophrenic patients (b). Warm colours represent autobiographical cues > baseline. Cold colours represent baseline > autobiographical cues. The third panel (c) shows areas of significant differences between the patients and the controls in this contrast. Colour bars depict Z values. Images are displayed in neurological convention (right is right).

Supplementary material: File

Martin-Subero et al. supplementary material

Martin-Subero et al. supplementary material

Download Martin-Subero et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.5 MB