Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T01:06:42.225Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Survival of the Scheming: A Genetically Informed Link Between the Dark Triad and Mental Toughness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 September 2013

Michael Onley
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
Livia Veselka
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
Julie Aitken Schermer
Affiliation:
Management and Organizational Studies, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
Philip A. Vernon*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
*
address for correspondence: Philip A. Vernon, Department of Psychology, Social Science Centre, Room 7418, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The present study is the first behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad traits of personality, consisting of Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy, and the variable of mental toughness, reflecting individual differences in the ability to cope when under pressure. The purpose of this investigation was to explore a potential explanation for the success of individuals exhibiting the Dark Triad traits in workplace and social settings. Participants were adult twins who completed the MACH-IV, the Narcissistic Personality Inventory, and the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale assessing Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy, respectively, as well as the MT48, measuring mental toughness. Correlational analyses of the data revealed significant positive phenotypic associations between mental toughness and narcissism. Psychopathy and Machiavellianism, however, both showed some significant negative phenotypic correlations with mental toughness. Bivariate behavioral genetic analyses of the data were conducted to assess the extent to which these significant phenotypic correlations were attributable to common genetic and/or common environmental factors. Results indicate that correlations between narcissism and mental toughness were attributable primarily to common non-shared environmental factors, correlations between Machiavellianism and mental toughness were influenced by both common genetic and common non-shared environmental factors, and the correlations between psychopathy and mental toughness were attributable entirely to correlated genetic factors. Implications of these findings in the context of etiology and organizational adaptation are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2013 

The Dark Triad is a personality cluster, defined at the subclinical level, and composed of three distinct, but overlapping personality traits: narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism (Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002). Though conventionally seen as socially malevolent, these three traits have also been linked to socially desirable outcomes in the workforce and in short-term relationships (e.g., Jonason et al., Reference Jonason, Li, Webster and Schmitt2009, Reference Jonason, Li and Teicher2010; Young & Pinksy, Reference Young and Pinksy2006). In an effort to explore the potentially adaptive nature of the Dark Triad traits, the present study assesses the personality cluster in relation to mental toughness, which has been identified as a key trait in human resiliency (Clough et al., Reference Clough, Earl, Sewell and Cockerill2001). Though associations between these sets of traits have not yet been examined, previous studies of mental toughness have shown that this construct's patterns of correlations with existing personality traits are sometimes reminiscent of those exhibited by the Dark Triad variables (e.g., Horsburgh et al., Reference Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka and Vernon2009; Vernon, et al., Reference Vernon, Villani, Vickers and Harris2008), suggesting that the Dark Triad and mental toughness may exhibit significant associations. To explore the potential etiological factors underlying these possible correlations, behavioral genetic analyses will also be carried out.

The Dark Triad Traits: Description and Correlations

Narcissism

Subclinical narcissism, measured with the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Hall, Reference Raskin and Hall1979), is characterized by vanity, grandiosity, a sense of entitlement, self-enhancement, self-deception, an inflated sense of self-worth, and manipulativeness (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Hoffman, Campbell and Marchisio2011; Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002; Watson et al., Reference Watson, Grisham, Trotter and Biderman1984). Narcissism has been shown to fit within a conventional framework of personality through its associations with the dimensions of the Big Five model of personality (Vernon et al., Reference Vernon, Villani, Vickers and Harris2008). Narcissistic individuals tend to obtain low scores on the Big Five dimension of Agreeableness, but high scores on the model's Extraversion and Openness to Experience factors (Vernon et al., Reference Vernon, Villani, Vickers and Harris2008). This pattern of associations provides an accurate reflection of the manner in which narcissism is presently understood: a mixture of both maladjusted and adjusted tendencies that have both been shown to augment the resilience of narcissistic individuals Paulhus (Reference Paulhus1998).

Psychopathy

Individuals exhibiting high levels of subclinical psychopathy can be described as impulsive, uninhibited, and often self-enhancing, showing little regard for others (Cooke & Michie, Reference Cooke and Michie2001; Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002). Such individuals exhibit low anxiety, paired with high thrill-seeking behavior, and show limited capability of taking the perspective of others (Hare, Reference Hare1985; Petrides et al., Reference Petrides, Vernon, Schermer and Veselka2011). Psychopathy has been associated with low scores on the Big Five dimensions of Agreeableness, Neuroticism, and Conscientiousness, as well as high scores on Extraversion and Openness to Experience (Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002).

Machiavellianism

Machiavellian individuals, as measured by the MACH-IV inventory (Christie & Geis, Reference Christie and Geis1970), are characterized by their frequent and extreme use of deception and manipulation (Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002). They are often perceived as being charming and attractive by their peers (Cherulnik et al., Reference Cherulnik, Way, Ames and Hutto1981), but individuals scoring high on Machiavellianism convey insincere emotions in order to engage others and manipulate them for their own benefit (Jakobwitz & Egan, Reference Jakobwitz and Egan2006). Machiavellianism has been found to correlate negatively with Agreeableness and Conscientiousness in the Big Five model (Paulhus and Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002).

Mental Toughness: Description and Correlations

Mental toughness involves being dedicated, motivated, focused, resilient, and having an affinity for challenge (Clough et al., Reference Clough, Earl, Sewell and Cockerill2001). Consequently, it can be broken down into four components: control, commitment, challenge, and confidence. Control can be further divided into two subcategories: control over life, reflecting a belief that one is in control of one's own destiny and experiences; and emotional control, defined by one's ability to remain calm and relaxed under pressure. Commitment involves being mentally and physically committed to the tasks and challenges being undertaken. Challenge is defined by the ability to be welcoming of and adept at dealing with obstacles and life changes. Lastly, confidence can further be broken down into confidence in abilities, reflecting overall self-efficacy, andinterpersonal confidence as defined by a sociable and outgoing demeanor.

Mental toughness is significantly and positively correlated with the Big Five dimensions of Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness and significantly negatively correlated with Neuroticism (Horsburgh et al., Reference Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka and Vernon2009). The one exception to the pattern of positive correlations with Agreeableness is its significant negative correlation with the interpersonal confidence factor of mental toughness.

Mental Toughness and the Dark Triad Traits

The Dark Triad traits and mental toughness have been linked in various ways to achieving goals and personal success in competitive situations, given their links to confidence, motivated actions, and a strong belief in oneself and one's own ability. Roberts and Robins (Reference Roberts and Robins2000) reported that aspects of subclinical narcissism, such as goal-striving, charisma, and high aspirations, can yield positive work outcomes. Feist (Reference Feist1993), in a study of eminent figures in the sciences, also found that narcissistic tendencies, when combined with additional skills and traits, could be quite adaptive in certain work contexts. A significant positive correlation has been noted between Machiavellianism and income in educated men, suggesting that the trait may play a role in the attainment of high work positions (Turner & Martinez, Reference Turner and Martinez1977). Furthermore, in studies of sales organizations, those scoring high on Machiavellianism tended to be most successful in unstructured environments in which they were able to manipulate others freely (Schultz, Reference Schultz1993). Lastly, those scoring high on subclinical psychopathy have been shown to be impulsive, and to feel little emotion or anxiety (Cooke & Michie, Reference Cooke and Michie2001). It is possible that these risk-taking tendencies can prove beneficial in competitive work environments that favor quick decision-making. Furthermore, the distorted, positive self-perceptions that stem from the low anxiety and exaggerated sense of control defining psychopathy may be adaptive in situations of high stress, such as those that arise in high-demand work environments (Taylor & Armor, Reference Taylor and Armor1996). Many of these features of the Dark Triad traits are echoed in the factors comprising mental toughness, suggesting that the two sets of traits may be more related than one might initially believe.

Both the Dark Triad cluster and mental toughness further share common personality trait correlates. Mental toughness and all of its defining factors are significantly and negatively related to the Big Five model's Neuroticism dimension (Horsburgh et al., Reference Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka and Vernon2009), as is psychopathy (e.g., Jakobwitz & Egan, Reference Jakobwitz and Egan2006; Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002). High scores on mental toughness, narcissism, and psychopathy are further correlated with high levels of Extraversion and Openness to Experience (Horsburgh et al., Reference Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka and Vernon2009; Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002). Extraversion and Openness to Experience, in turn, are often associated with popularity and social success (van der Linden et al., Reference van der Linden, Scholte, Cillessen and Segers2010; Watson & Clark, Reference Watson, Clark, Hogan, Johnson and Briggs1997), supporting the notion that both mental toughness and the Dark Triad cluster may be adaptive in terms of reaching life goals. Furthermore, Machiavellianism is negatively correlated with the Big Five's Agreeableness, as is the interpersonal confidence facet of mental toughness (Horsburgh et al., Reference Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka and Vernon2009; Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002). Based on these findings, it seems plausible that mental toughness and the three Dark Triad traits may themselves be positively related.

Behavioral Genetic Analyses of the Dark Triad Traits and Mental Toughness

Comparing the responses of monozygotic (MZ) and same-sex dizygotic (DZ) twins on a given trait makes it possible to determine the extent to which genetics play a role in the etiology of the trait (Rijsdijk & Sham, Reference Rijsdijk and Sham2002). Assuming equal environments among identical and fraternal twins, the only way in which they differ is via their genes, given that MZ twins share 100% of their genotype, whereas DZ share only about 50% of their genes (Rijsdijk & Sham, Reference Rijsdijk and Sham2002). Thus, if genetics play a large role in the development of a given trait, MZ twins should provide more similar responses regarding that trait than should DZ twins.

Previous univariate behavioral genetic studies of the Dark Triad traits have shown that individual differences in each of these variables are largely influenced by genetic factors (Vernon et al., Reference Vernon, Villani, Vickers and Harris2008; Veselka et al., Reference Veselka, Schermer and Vernon2011). In a direct univariate behavioral genetic investigation of mental toughness, Horsburgh et al. (Reference Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka and Vernon2009) concluded that individual differences in mental toughness were largely attributable to both genetic and non-shared environmental factors. The existence of underlying genetic components has also been demonstrated for specific constructs such as behavioral and cognitive resilience and task persistence, which are related to mental toughness (Deater-Deckard et al., Reference Deater-Deckard, Petrill, Thompson and DeThorne2006).

Present Study

The present investigation serves to determine whether significant relations exist between the Dark Triad traits and mental toughness factors (Clough et al., Reference Clough, Earl, Sewell and Cockerill2001). We are also interested in determining why relations between these traits may exist, by assessing the extent to which these associations are attributable to common genetic and/or common environmental factors. A bivariate behavioral genetic analysis will be employed to address this.

We predict that significant positive associations will emerge between each of the Dark Triad traits and mental toughness at the global and factor levels. Furthermore, given that primarily genetic and non-shared environmental factors underscore individual differences in each of the variables of interest at the univariate level, it is further hypothesized that these correlations will largely be attributable to genetic and non-shared environmental factors at the bivariate level.

Method

Participants

Participants in the present study were 210 same-sex twin pairs, which yielded a sample of 420 individual participants. Of these pairs, 152 were MZ — 128 female pairs and 24 male pairs. A total of 58 same-sex DZ pairs were also included in the sample — 53 female pairs (DZF) and five male pairs (DZM). Participants were from Canada and the United States, and ranged in age from 17 to 92 years (M = 41.42, SD = 17.54). Participants completed the necessary questionnaires individually. As compensation for participation, participants received $20.00 and were entered into a draw to win one of ten $100.00 prizes.

Materials

Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI)

Variation in subclinical narcissism was measured via the 40-item NPI (Raskin & Hall, Reference Raskin and Hall1979). According to a factor analysis by Emmons (Reference Emmons1984), the measure's items load on four distinct factors: exploitativeness/entitlement, leadership/authority, superiority/arrogance, and self-absorption/self-admiration. Higher scores on the NPI, therefore, are representative of a greater tendency to engage in behaviors and possess traits characteristic of these factors. Each of the items comprising the questionnaire consists of two conflicting, self-reflective statements. Participants are asked to select the one statement of the pair that best reflects their own beliefs and feelings. The NPI shows strong psychometric properties and is thus considered an accurate measure of subclinical narcissism (e.g., Emmons, Reference Emmons1984; Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002; Raskin & Terry, Reference Raskin and Terry1988).

Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (SRP-III)

Individual differences in subclinical psychopathy were measured via the 62-item Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (SRP-III; Hare, Reference Hare1985). The items comprising the measure can be divided into four subscales: interpersonal manipulation, callous affect, erratic lifestyle, and antisocial behavior (Wheeler et al., Reference Wheeler, Book and Costello2009). Each item presents a self-reflective statement to participants, to which they respond using a five-point Likert scale (where 1 = disagree strongly and 5 = agree strongly). As a result, higher scores on the SRP-III represent higher levels of subclinical psychopathy, as defined by its four subscales. Investigations of the psychometric properties of the SRP-III indicate that it is a reliable and valid measure of subclinical psychopathy (e.g., Derefinko & Lynam, Reference Derefinko and Lynam2006; Mahmut et al., Reference Mahmut, Menictas, Stevenson and Homewood2011; Nathanson et al., Reference Nathanson, Paulhus and Williams2006).

MACH-IV

The 20-item MACH-IV (Christie & Geis, Reference Christie and Geis1970) was used to measure individual differences in subclinical Machiavellianism. Four subscales comprise this particular measure: cynical view of human nature, positive view of human nature, negative interpersonal tactics primarily defined by manipulativeness, and positive interpersonal tactics chiefly identifiable through honesty (Andrew et al., Reference Andrew, Cooke and Muncer2008). Each item of the MACH-IV presents a self-reflective statement to participants. Participants respond to each of these statements via a five-point Likert scale (where 1 = disagree strongly and 5 = agree strongly). Consequently, higher scores on the MACH-IV indicate greater Machiavellian tendencies in an individual on both a global level and as defined by its subscales. The MACH-IV has been shown to be a sound measure of Machiavellianism (e.g., Kline & Cooper, Reference Kline and Cooper1983; Wrightsman, Reference Wrightsman, Robinson, Shaver and Wrightsman1991; Zook, Reference Zook1985).

MT48

The MT48 (Clough et al., Reference Clough, Earl, Sewell and Cockerill2001) is a 48-item scale, which measures individual differences in mental toughness by assessing an individual's ability to withstand mental and emotional pressure in a variety of situations. Clough et al. (Reference Clough, Earl, Sewell and Cockerill2001) determined that the scale's items load onto the four distinct factors of challenge, control (control over life, emotional control), commitment, and confidence (confidence in abilities, interpersonal confidence). In the scope of the present study, the subfactors pertaining to control and confidence were not assessed. To complete this measure, participants respond to items via a five-point Likert scale (where 1 = strongly agree and 5 = strongly disagree). Higher scores on the MT48 represent higher levels of mental toughness as defined by the factors. The MT48 has been shown to be a valid scale for the measure of the mental toughness construct (e.g., Clough et al., Reference Clough, Earl, Sewell and Cockerill2001; Crust, Reference Crust2008).

Procedure

Potential participants were contacted via telephone and e-mail. Individuals who expressed interest were sent a package via standard mail, which contained the MACH-IV, the NPI, the SRP-III, the MT48, a letter of information, a consent form, and a self-addressed stamped envelope. Participants who wished to take part in the study were asked to read and sign the consent form and to send it, along with the completed questionnaires, back to the researchers using the provided self-addressed envelope. Participants were then sent a debriefing form providing further details about the present study, along with their compensation. Over 90% of the twins who were originally contacted agreed to participate and submitted their completed questionnaires.

Analyses

Even though the majority of participants completed all of the items on the four measures used in the present study, on rare occasions an item was left blank. In such instances, the missing data were replaced with the average of the Likert scale used. For the purpose of analysis, the items on the MT48 were converted into four scores, one for each of the major mental toughness dimensions. The remaining questionnaires each yielded one general score, corresponding to each of the Dark Triad variables.

To determine the extent to which correlated genetic and/or correlated environmental factors play a role in the co-occurrence of the Dark Triad traits and mental toughness factors, a multivariate behavioral genetic analysis was conducted using Mx, a software program (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Boker, Xie and Maes2006). In these analyses, MZ and DZ cross-correlations were computed, and the method of Cholesky or triangular decomposition was applied (Neale & Cardon, Reference Neale and Cardon1992). A full ACE model was first fit to the data to assess the significance of genetic (A), common environmental (C), and/or non-shared environment (E) effects. Following this step, reduced AE and CE models were further tested in order to explore more parsimonious solutions. The best-fitting models were those that yielded the lowest AIC value and the lowest χ2 change relative to the full model.

Results

Descriptive statistics for the three Dark Triad traits are presented in Table 1. Descriptive statistics are also provided for the four factors of mental toughness in the same table.

TABLE 1 Mean (M) and Standard Deviation (SD) for the NPI, SRP-III, MACH-IV, and MT48

aMeasured by the NPI; bMeasured by the SRP-III; cMeasured by the MACH-IV; dMeasured by the MT48.

Phenotypic Correlations

A correlational analysis of participants’ scores on the NPI, MACH-IV, SRP-III, and the four major dimensions of the MT48 was conducted to investigate the magnitude of any possible relations between the Dark Triad and mental toughness variables. As can be seen in Table 2, all factors of mental toughness were positively and significantly correlated with narcissism, while being negatively and significantly correlated with psychopathy, with the exception of the association between psychopathy and the challenge factors of mental toughness, which was non-significant. Correlations were more diverse for Machiavellianism, which was significantly and positively associated with commitment and control, but significantly and negatively associated with challenge and confidence. These significant effects were primarily moderate in effect sizes, and the strongest of these were the associations involving Machiavellianism.

TABLE 2 Phenotypic (rp), Genetic (rg), Shared Environmental (rc), and Non-Shared Environmental (re) Correlations Between the Dark Triad Traits and the Mental Toughness Factors

Numbers appearing in brackets represent the 95% confidence interval values. All correlations whose confidence intervals do not include zero are deemed to be significant at the 0.05 level. The best fitting model is presented.

**p < .01 (two-tailed).

Behavioral Genetic Analysis

The results of the bivariate BG analyses (see Table 2) show that the associations between narcissism and the mental toughness factors are largely influenced by non-shared environmental factors. Common non-shared environmental factors accounted for the significant correlation between narcissism and three of the four factors of mental toughness (commitment, control, and confidence). Results indicate that the correlation between narcissism and the fourth factor of mental toughness, challenge, was best explained by correlated genetic factors only. The three significant phenotypic associations between psychopathy and mental toughness were shown to be entirely attributable to common genetic factors, all of moderate effect size. Lastly, the four significant observed correlations between Machiavellianism and mental toughness were entirely accounted for by correlated genetic and correlated non-shared environmental factors. In all cases, the significant genetic correlations were larger than the corresponding non-shared environmental effects.

Discussion

To explore the nature of the Dark Triad, mental toughness, and any potential interaction between them, correlational analyses were employed. We also conducted a behavioral genetic analysis to reveal the extent to which any phenotypic correlations could be attributed to common genetic and/or common environmental factors. Along with some predicted findings, a number of unexpected, yet significant relations were found between the Dark Triad traits and the factors of mental toughness.

Phenotypic Correlations

As predicted, narcissism correlated positively with all factors of mental toughness. However, contrary to our hypotheses, both psychopathy and Machiavellianism showed moderate to strong negative correlations with most of the mental toughness factors. One interpretation of this finding is that narcissism is qualitatively unique among the Dark Triad cluster in that it is associated with prosocial and adaptive tendencies as well as socially aversive behaviors (e.g., Veselka et al., Reference Veselka, Schermer and Vernon2012). Petrides et al. (Reference Petrides, Vernon, Schermer and Veselka2011) have reported that narcissism behaves appreciably differently from the other Dark Triad variables when examined in the context of trait emotional intelligence (trait EI). While psychopathy and Machiavellianism correlate negatively with many factors of trait EI, suggesting a deficiency in understanding and processing emotion-related information, narcissism exhibits positive associations with EI. To account for these findings, Petrides et al. suggested that the exaggerated sense of self-worth and pride associated with narcissism can render a narcissist optimistic, motivated, assertive, and successful in relationships.

An alternative explanation for the differences in the nature of the correlations found between mental toughness and the Dark Triad are potential errors in the way we understand and measure the Dark Triad traits. Previous research highlights the complex nature of the personality cluster and the potential problems faced when trying to measure it. Mcdonald et al. (Reference McDonald, Donnellan and Navarrete2012) have suggested that the questionnaires most often used to measure narcissism do not capture a unitary construct. For example, the NPI, employed in the current investigation appears to assess multiple, conflicting constructs of narcissism, which can be socially desirable (leadership skills, and social influence) or socially toxic (entitlement and exploitativeness). As a result, a single coherent construct may be difficult to investigate with this inventory. The validity of the NPI is further criticized for similar reasons by Brown et al. (Reference Brown, Budzek and Tamborski2009), who conclude that many of the tendencies measured by the NPI are more representative of healthy self-esteem, which is considered a prosocial trait, than of antisocial narcissism. Items designed to measure the more antisocial dimensions of narcissism are underrepresented in the NPI. Consequently, scores obtained using the NPI may be biased toward assessing only the more adaptive qualities of narcissism, therefore yielding results suggesting that it is indeed linked to positive outcomes, even though that may not necessarily be the case. It is possible that the positive correlations between narcissism and mental toughness — considered a prosocial trait — may be a product of such biasing.

While the common correlates of the Big Five model's Extraversion and Openness dimensions hinted at a potential positive relation between psychopathy and mental toughness, it seems that differences between these variables far outweigh their similarities. Psychopathy, a generally antisocial trait, comprises impulsivity, irresponsibility, and socially deviant behaviors (Levenson et al., Reference Levenson, Kiehl and Fitzpatrick1995). In contrast, mental toughness is characterized by the ability to stay calm and responsible in stressful situations, as well as the ability to remain committed to a goal (Clough et al., Reference Clough, Earl, Sewell and Cockerill2001). Although it appears to be the case that psychopathy can yield some positive outcomes in high-demand, competitive settings (Taylor & Armor, Reference Taylor and Armor1996), it may ultimately be the case that these outcomes stem from more conniving practices rather than from overall resilience in the face of pressure (Jonason et al., Reference Jonason, Slomski and Partyka2012).

The predicted positive correlations between Machiavellianism and the mental toughness dimensions were only partially established. In particular, positive associations were noted between Machiavellianism and the factors of commitment and control. These associations make sense on a conceptual level, and are in line with past research on Machiavellianism in organizational and workplace settings. Specifically, control is defined by a sense of power over one's own experiences and emotions, while commitment is characterized by dedication to the goals and tasks at hand (Clough et al., Reference Clough, Earl, Sewell and Cockerill2001). In the case of Machiavellianism, those who score high on the trait also tend to exhibit high self-related work commitment where the focus is on the achievement of one's own goals (Zettler et al., Reference Zettler, Friedrich and Hilbig2011), a high need for control in the workplace (Rayburn & Rayburn, Reference Rayburn and Rayburn1996), and a propensity toward manipulation, which can further be seen as an effort on the part of the Machiavellian individual to exert control over one's own outcomes (e.g., Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Bandelli, Spector, Borman, Nelson and Penney2010). Contrary to our predictions, however, Machiavellianism correlated negatively with challenge and confidence. From a theoretical perspective, these findings seem unusual, given the self-importance and general hardiness and adaptability attributed to those who score high on Machiavellianism (e.g., Den Hartog & Belschak, Reference Den Hartog and Belschak2012). That being said, past studies of personality have found inverse patterns of correlations for the two sets of constructs, suggesting that the two mental toughness dimensions and Machiavellianism may indeed be negatively related (e.g., Horsburgh et al., Reference Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka and Vernon2009; Jakobwitz & Egan, Reference Jakobwitz and Egan2006). Overall then, it seems to be the case that Machiavellian individuals exhibit confidence and task-orientation in a complex manner, and future investigations may wish to tease apart this intricate association.

Behavioral Genetic Analysis

Predictions regarding the behavioral genetic analysis of relations between mental toughness and the Dark Triad were partially supported by the results. As predicted, based on previous behavioral genetic studies of personality traits (Blonigen et al., Reference Blonigen, Hicks, Krueger, Patrick and Iacono2006; Horsburgh et al., Reference Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka and Vernon2009; Vernon et al., Reference Vernon, Villani, Vickers and Harris2008), phenotypic correlations between mental toughness and the Dark Triad traits of psychopathy and Machiavellianism showed some underlying genetic effects. The associations between psychopathy and mental toughness were entirely accounted for by common genetic factors. These findings support previous investigations of both psychopathy and mental toughness at the univariate level, which have implicated genetic effects in individual differences in these traits (e.g., Horsburgh et al., Reference Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka and Vernon2009; Vernon et al., Reference Vernon, Villani, Vickers and Harris2008). Genetic correlations imply the existence of biological structures that contribute to the co-occurrence of these traits, and it may be fruitful to investigate these structures in subsequent work.

The observed associations between mental toughness and Machiavellianism were attributable not only to common genetic factors but also to common non-shared environmental factors, indicating that experiences and social interactions outside of the family likely play a role in the co-development of Machiavellianism and low mental toughness. In support of genetic underpinnings, the evolutionary game theory proposed by Wilson et al. (Reference Wilson, Near and Miller1996) is relevant. According to this theory, social strategies — that is, stable patterns of interacting with others — are not adopted consciously by rational choice, but rather they compete in a Darwinian fashion for manifestation and prevalence. Because unique strategies are believed to facilitate adaptation, they are thought to be naturally selected for and therefore passed along from parent to offspring. In the case of Machiavellianism, the corresponding social strategy is one that involves manipulation of others to maximize short-term gains. In line with this theory, it has been noted that individuals high on Machiavellianism tend to exhibit an exploitative and self-serving social strategy (Jonason et al., Reference Jonason, Li and Teicher2010), and report using short-term and emotionally distant mating styles (Jonason & Kavanagh, Reference Jonason and Kavanagh2010). Because Machiavellian individuals strive to employ the right behavior in the right situation in order to reap maximum benefits, however, they may also be cooperative and convivial if it is advantageous (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Near and Miller1996). Consequently, they may share aspects of the social strategy employed by individuals high on mental toughness — one defined by sociability, loyalty, and self-assurance — in addition to exhibiting tendencies that lie in contrast to these prosocial inclinations. This may account for the complex phenotypic correlations and corresponding common genetic underpinnings observed in the current study.

It was expected based on previous research (Horsburgh et al., Reference Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka and Vernon2009; Vernon et al., Reference Vernon, Villani, Vickers and Harris2008) that an analysis of the correlation between narcissism and mental toughness would reveal a strong underlying genetic component. However, a significant genetic correlation was only found between narcissism and the challenge factor of mental toughness. All remaining associations were attributable to common non-shared environmental factors. This finding is contrary to various behavioral genetic research studies on personality traits that suggest that while common non-shared environmental effects often contribute to variations in traits, genetic factors also play an important part (Livesley et al., Reference Livesley, Jang, Jackson and Vernon1993; Vernon et al., Reference Vernon, Villani, Vickers and Harris2008). This finding may be the result of the potential mismeasure of narcissism using the NPI, as discussed earlier, which may have yielded misleading findings in the current sample. More likely, however, it may be the case that, when assessing the joint manifestation of narcissism and mental toughness, rather than addressing each set of variables separately, non-shared environmental effects may indeed play a role in the extent to which these traits are exhibited. Washburn and Paskar (Reference Washburn, Paskar, Barry, Kerig, Stellwagen and Barry2011) describe an ecological systems theory encompassing four distinct interpersonal systems thought to influence narcissism (the microsystem, the mesosystem, the exosystem, and the macrosystem), which highlights the many levels at which environmental factors can be influential to the trait. By extension, these same factors may also contribute to higher levels of mental toughness. Similarly, in samples of athletes, studies have shown that mental toughness is shaped by individuals’ training and support networks (i.e., teammates and coaches) and the motivational climate in which they find themselves (e.g., Bull et al., Reference Bull, Shambrook, James and Brooks2005; Thelwell et al., Reference Thelwell, Such, Weston, Such and Greenlees2010), suggesting numerous levels of social influence on this trait as well. Consequently, it may be the case that, while specific predispositions account for the independent display of narcissism and mental toughness, the extent to which they are jointly exhibited and balanced against one another is primarily determined by the unique environments that individuals seek out.

The present study is not without limitations. Although we employed conventional measures of the constructs of interests in our investigation, recent research suggests that these measures may not be effective in capturing adequately the full complexity of the constructs. Of particular concern may be the NPI, as used to assess narcissism. Although there is support for use of this measure (e.g., Emmons, Reference Emmons1984; Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002; Raskin & Terry, Reference Raskin and Terry1988), there also exist compelling criticisms (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Budzek and Tamborski2009). Future investigations may wish to replicate the current findings using alternative inventories. A further limitation is the present study's reliance on self-report responding, particularly in the context of the assessment of antisocial traits. Although studies have found that even in investigations of pathological socially aversive variables, social desirability bias does significantly impact upon the results (e.g., Mills et al., Reference Mills, Loza and Kroner2003); subsequent studies may wish to apply other methods to their data collection. Lastly, the sample employed in the current study was composed primarily of female participants, and therefore the findings may not be generalizable to all individuals. Specifically, because it has been shown that males more strongly exhibit Dark Triad tendencies, scoring higher on all three traits relative to females (e.g., Paulhus & Williams, Reference Paulhus and Williams2002), it is possible that some of the effects presented in the present report are more muted than may actually be the case. Subsequent studies may wish to replicate the current findings with a more balanced sample.

By exploring the nature of the Dark Triad and its interaction with more prosocial traits, such as mental toughness, we begin to better understand both the adaptive and maladaptive qualities associated with the Dark Triad. It may be beneficial in the future to further examine the nature of the individual traits of the Dark Triad, in order to fully understand these constructs and to fine-tune the way in which we measure them. With a better understanding of the traits, it may become easier to minimize the negative effects and increase the positive impact that individuals exhibiting higher levels of these behaviors have on themselves and others.

References

Andrew, J., Cooke, M., & Muncer, S. J. (2008). The relationship between empathy and Machiavellianism: An alternative to empathizing-systemizing theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 12031211.Google Scholar
Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. F., Patrick, C. J., & Iacono, W. G. (2006). Continuity and change in psychopathic traits as measured via normal-range personality: A longitudinal-biometric study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 8595.Google Scholar
Brown, R. P., Budzek, K., & Tamborski, M. (2009). On the meaning and measure of narcissism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 951964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bull, S. J., Shambrook, C. J., James, W., & Brooks, J. E. (2005). Toward an understanding of mental toughness in elite English cricket. Journal of Applies Sport Psychology, 17, 209227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, W. K., Hoffman, B. J., Campbell, S. M., & Marchisio, G. (2011). Narcissism in organizational contexts. Human Resource Management Review, 21, 268284.Google Scholar
Cherulnik, P. D., Way, J. H., Ames, S., & Hutto, D. B. (1981). Impressions of high and low Machiavellian men. Journal of Personality, 49, 388400.Google Scholar
Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clough, P., Earl, K., & Sewell, D. (2001). Mental toughness: The concept and its measurement. In Cockerill, I. (Ed.), Solutions in sport psychology (pp. 3242). London: Thomson.Google Scholar
Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (2001). Refining the construct of psychopath: Towards a hierarchical model. Psychological Assessment, 13, 171188.Google Scholar
Crust, L. (2008). A review and conceptual re-examination of mental toughness: Implications for future research. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 576583.Google Scholar
Deater-Deckard, K., Petrill, S. A., Thompson, L. A., & DeThorne, L. S. (2006). A longitudinal behavioral genetic analysis of task persistence. Developmental Science, 9, 498504.Google Scholar
Den Hartog, D. N., & Belschak, F. D. (2012). Work engagement and Machiavellianism in the ethical leadership process. Journal of Business Ethics, 107, 3547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derefinko, K. J., & Lynam, D. R. (2006). Convergence and divergence among self-report psychopathy measures: A personality-based approach. Journal of Personality Disorders, 20, 261280.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 291300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feist, G. J. (1993). A structural model of scientific eminence. Psychological Science, 4, 366371.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1985). Comparison of procedures for the assessment of psychopathy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 716.Google Scholar
Horsburgh, V. A., Schermer, J. A., Veselka, L., & Vernon, P. A. (2009). A behavioural genetic study of mental toughness and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 100105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakobwitz, S., & Egan, V. (2006). The Dark Triad and normal personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 331339.Google Scholar
Jonason, P. K., & Kavanagh, P. (2010). The dark side of love: Love styles and the Dark Triad. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 606610.Google Scholar
Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., & Teicher, E. A. (2010). Who is James Bond?: The Dark Triad as an agentic social style. Individual Differences Research, 8, 111120.Google Scholar
Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., Webster, G. D., & Schmitt, D. P. (2009). The Dark Triad: Facilitating a short-term mating strategy in men. European Journal of Personality, 23, 518.Google Scholar
Jonason, P. K., Slomski, S., & Partyka, J. (2012). The Dark Triad at work: How toxic employees get their way. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 449–453.Google Scholar
Kessler, S. R., Bandelli, A. C., Spector, P. E., Borman, W. C., Nelson, C. E., & Penney, L. M. (2010). Re-examining Machiavelli: A three-dimensional model of Machiavellianism in the workplace. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 18681896.Google Scholar
Kline, P., & Cooper, C. (1983). A factor-analytic study of measures of Machiavellianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 569571.Google Scholar
Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalised population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 151158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., Jackson, D. N., & Vernon, P. A. (1993). Genetic and environmental contributions to dimensions of personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 18261831.Google ScholarPubMed
Mahmut, M. K., Menictas, C., Stevenson, R. J., & Homewood, J. (2011). Validating the factor structure of the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale in a community sample. Psychological Assessment, 23, 670678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, M. M., Donnellan, M. B., & Navarrete, C. D. (2012). A life history approach to understanding the Dark Triad. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 601605.Google Scholar
Mills, J. F., Loza, W., & Kroner, D. G. (2003). Predictive validity despite social desirability: Evidence for the robustness of self-report among offenders. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 13 (2), 140150.Google Scholar
Nathanson, C., Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2006). Predictors of a behavioral measure of scholastic cheating: Personality and competence but not demographics. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31, 97122.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (2006). Mx: Statistical modeling manual (7th ed.). Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Virginia.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: A mixed blessing?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1197.Google Scholar
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 556563.Google Scholar
Petrides, K. V., Vernon, P. A., Schermer, J. A., & Veselka, L. (2011). Trait emotional intelligence and the Dark Triad traits of personality. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 14, 3541.Google Scholar
Raskin, R., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A Narcissistic personality inventory. Psychological Reports, 45, 590.Google Scholar
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principle-component analysis of the Narcissistic personality inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 890902.Google Scholar
Rayburn, J. M., & Rayburn, L. G. (1996). Relationship between Machiavellianism and type A personality and ethical-orientation. Journal of Business Ethics, 15, 12091219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rijsdijk, F. V., & Sham, P. C. (2002). Analytic approaches to twin data using structural equation models. Briefings in Bioinformatics, 3, 119133.Google Scholar
Roberts, R. W., & Robins, R. W. (2000). Broad dispositions, broad aspirations: The intersection of personality traits and major life goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 12841296.Google Scholar
Schultz, J. S. (1993). Situational and dispositional predictors of performance: A test of the hypothesized Machiavellianism X structure interaction among sales persons. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 478498.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., & Armor, D. A. (1996). Positive illusions and coping with adversity. Journal of Personality, 64, 873898.Google Scholar
Thelwell, R. C., Such, B. A., Weston, N. J., Such, J. D., & Greenlees, I. A. (2010). Developing mental toughness: Perceptions of elite female gymnasts. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 8, 170188.Google Scholar
Turner, C. F., & Martinez, D. C. (1977). Socioeconomic achievement and Machiavellian personality. Social Psychology Quarterly, 40, 325336.Google Scholar
van der Linden, D., Scholte, R. H. J., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Segers, E. (2010). Classroom ratings of likeability and popularity are related to the Big Five and the general factor of personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 669672.Google Scholar
Vernon, P. A., Villani, V. C., Vickers, L. C., & Harris, J. A. (2008). A behavioural genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 445452.Google Scholar
Veselka, L., Schermer, J. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2011). Beyond the Big Five: The dark triad and the supernumerary personality inventory. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 14, 158168.Google Scholar
Veselka, L., Schermer, J. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2012). The Dark Triad and an expanded framework of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 417425.Google Scholar
Washburn, J. J., & Paskar, L. D. (2011). Moving beyond parents in the etiology of narcissistic traits. In Barry, C. T., Kerig, P. K., Stellwagen, K. K. & Barry, T. D. (Eds.), Narcissism and Machiavellianism in youth: Implications for the development of adaptive and maladaptive behavior (pp. 145157). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Extraversion and its positive emotional core. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J. A. & Briggs, S. R. (Eds.), Handbook of Personality Psychology (pp. 767793). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Watson, P. J., Grisham, S. O., Trotter, M. V., & Biderman, M. D. (1984). Narcissism and empathy: Validity evidence for the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 301305.Google Scholar
Wheeler, S., Book, A., & Costello, K. (2009) Psychopathic traits and perceptions of victim vulnerability. Criminal Justice and Behaviour, 36, 635.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., Near, D., & Miller, R. R. (1996). Machiavellianism: A synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 285299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Interpersonal trust and attitudes toward human nature. In Robinson, J. P., Shaver, P. R. & Wrightsman, L. S. (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 373412). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Young, S. M., & Pinksy, D. (2006). Narcissism and celebrity. Journal of Personality Research, 40, 463471.Google Scholar
Zettler, I., Friedrich, N., & Hilbig, B. E. (2011). Dissecting work commitment: The role of Machiavellianism. Career Development International, 16, 2035.Google Scholar
Zook, A. (1985). On measurement of Machiavellianism. Psychological Reports, 57, 982.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Mean (M) and Standard Deviation (SD) for the NPI, SRP-III, MACH-IV, and MT48

Figure 1

TABLE 2 Phenotypic (rp), Genetic (rg), Shared Environmental (rc), and Non-Shared Environmental (re) Correlations Between the Dark Triad Traits and the Mental Toughness Factors