Understanding how early environments affect trajectories of social, emotional and behavioural problems is the core objective of several lines of research in human development. There is substantial heterogeneity at the root of these trajectories, even among individuals exposed to similar living circumstances; although some individuals seem to thrive under harsh living conditions, others are affected by minimal disruptions in their environments. Reference Boyce and Ellis1,Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn2 Adverse environments such as maltreatment lead to antisocial outcomes only in a fraction of individuals. Reference Wilson, Stover and Berkowitz3 Identifying the mechanisms underlying this heterogeneity is an important step towards improving our understanding of antisocial behaviour and informing prevention efforts. Genetically informative studies indicate that genes and environments explain individual differences in antisocial behaviour. Reference Rhee and Waldman4 However, these contributions may not be independent. Reference Hicks, South, Dirago, Iacono and McGue5 For example, twins with the highest genetic risk at age 5 showed the greatest impact of maltreatment on conduct disorder. Reference Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, Dodge, Rutter and Taylor6 Phenotypic heterogeneity following exposure to maltreatment may lie, to some extent, in differences present at the DNA level. Genes×environment interaction (GE) occurs when the effect of an environment on a phenotype is conditional on a person's genotype. Reference Moffitt, Caspi and Rutter7 In a seminal paper published by Caspi and colleagues in 2002, the impact of childhood maltreatment on antisocial personality, conduct disorder and violent crimes was magnified by an allele conferring low activity of the monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene. Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin and Craig8 Importantly, differences between low- and high-allele carriers were present only when exposed to childhood maltreatment, but not otherwise. MAOA is an enzyme that preferentially degrades monoamines such as noradrenaline, serotonin and dopamine and is presumed to affect antisocial behaviour indirectly, through the disruption of neurotransmitter balance in brain structures and neural pathways involved in emotional regulation and behavioural inhibition. Reference Buckholtz, Callicott, Kolachana, Hariri, Goldberg and Genderson9
The moderating impact of the MAOA gene on the association linking childhood maltreatment to antisocial outcomes has been replicated, Reference Foley, Eaves, Wormley, Silberg, Maes and Kuhn10–Reference Fergusson, Boden, Horwood, Miller and Kennedy12 but not consistently. Reference Haberstick, Lessem, Hopfer, Smolen, Ehringer and Timberlake13,Reference Young, Smolen, Hewitt, Haberstick, Stallings and Corley14 Several methodological issues may explain these discrepancies, including diverse sample populations (for example clinical or forensic v. population-based), Reference Young, Smolen, Hewitt, Haberstick, Stallings and Corley14 population genetic heterogeneity Reference Haberstick, Lessem, Hopfer, Smolen, Ehringer and Timberlake13 and exposure to distinct adverse environments, varying from maltreatment, Reference Weder, Yang, Douglas-Palumberi, Massey, Krystal and Gelernter15 to type of residence Reference Nilsson, Sjoberg, Damberg, Leppert, Ohrvik and Alm16 and stressful life events. Reference Enoch, Steer, Newman, Gibson and Goldman17 Even when only maltreatment is considered, inconsistency remains that may partially lie in how this variable was measured (such as official reports, structured interviews, self-reports) and later analysed. Specifically, exposure to violence is a quantitative variable that is, in its natural state, continuously distributed in the population. Most studies have thus far used artificially truncated measures, making it difficult to determine whether the moderating effect of the MAOA gene takes place across the full distribution of exposure or emerges whenever a yet unknown level of violence has been reached, as the diathesis–stress model suggests. Importantly, the level of exposure at which the moderating effect of the MAOA gene likely emerges cannot be deduced from studies assuming a constant increment of the interaction across the distribution of violence. Past investigations may thus not provide a comprehensive account of the moderating role of the MAOA gene to antisocial behaviour. The aim of the study was to investigate whether the MAOA gene moderates the impact of childhood exposure to violence on antisocial outcomes in a population-based sample of males. The objectives were threefold. First, we examined the main and interaction effects of the MAOA gene across the full distribution of violence. Second, we tested whether the MAOA gene affects risks of antisocial outcomes at higher level of exposure. Third, we investigated whether similar findings emerge for distinct antisocial outcomes.
Method
Participants
The Québec Longitudinal Study of Kindergarten Children (QLSKC) was created from a larger sample of children. A first subsample comprised 2000 children (999 girls and 1001 boys) selected randomly to represent children attending kindergarten in French-speaking state schools of the province of Québec, Canada. The second subsample included all remaining children who scored at the eightieth percentile or higher (n = 1017, 424 girls and 593 boys, with gender-specific cut-offs) on disruptive behaviour. Teachers and mothers rated these behaviours using 13 items from the Social Behavior Questionnaire, Reference Tremblay, Loeber, Gagnon, Charlebois, Larivée and LeBlanc18 covering physical aggression, opposition, hyperactivity and antisociality (i.e. lying, cheating and stealing). Factor analyses suggested that these items belonged to a single factor for which high internal consistency was noted (Cronbach's alpha from 0.86 to 0.90 and 0.82 to 0.89 for mothers and teachers respectively). A total initial sample of 3017 children was thus created. At 21 years old, men were invited to a follow-up assessment. Participants for whom DNA was not collected were more likely to be from a family with a low socioeconomic status (t(1, 469.76) = −6.40, P<0.001) and have externalising problems as children (t(1, 528.25) = −3.70, P<0.001). All statistical analyses have thus been conducted using weights to compensate for this selective attrition. We only include self-reported White participants to reduce genetic heterogeneity. The study was approved by the research ethics board of Université de Montréal and McGill University. Written informed consents were obtained.
Measures
Genotyping
Common tag single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs, minor allele frequency >5%) and SNPs located 5 kbp upstream of the transcription site were selected. Tag SNPs were obtained using Hapmap for the Utah residents with ancestry from northern and western Europe (CEU) 19 and the Multimarker tagging procedure (r 2>0.8) using Tagger. Reference de Bakker, Yelensky, Pe'er, Gabriel, Daly and Altshuler20 Additionally, 44 anonymous markers spread across the genome and located outside of gene-coding regions were genotyped to detect population stratification. We used a high-throughput, 768-SNP Illumina platform and GoldenGate panel based on BeadArray technology. The initial genotyping success rate for the SNPs was 95.4%. SNPs less than 60 base pairs apart were eliminated, 33 SNPs were eliminated because of call rate <0.90 and 14 participants were discarded because of low average call rates (<0.95). Based on the pattern of linkage disequilibrium between the 23 genotyped SNPs (online Fig. DS1), we selected a maximally informative set of SNPs (rs5906893, rs5906957, rs2283725, rs2072744, rs979605) using an algorithm published by Carlson et al Reference Carlson, Eberle, Rieder, Yi, Kruglyak and Nickerson21 and a stringent threshold (r 2⩾0.90) to capture most individual variation. Each SNP was tested separately to enhance the sensitivity of our investigations.
Population stratification
Even though the French Canadian population has descended from a small number of founders, Reference Desjardins22 we identified 12 population outliers using the genotype log likelihood test statistic with a standard cut-off of −2.33, corresponding to a P-value of 0.001 of n(0,1). The final sample thus included 327 participants.
Exposure to violence
Exposure to violence was based on four indicators collected retrospectively when the participants were 21 years of age. Mother's and father's maltreatment encompassed psychological aggression and physical assaults that have occurred before age 18 using an adapted version of the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales. Reference Strauss, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore and Runyan23 Good internal reliability was noted (Cronbach's α = 0.85 and 0.83 respectively) and this measure was associated to expected outcomes in this cohort (such as depression and suicide attempts). Scores varied from 0 to 64 (mean 8.47, s.d. = 8.73) and 0 to 41 (mean 8.66, s.d. = 8.01) respectively. Sexual abuse was assessed using five items adapted from the Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz and Edwards24 and the Sexually Victimized Children Questionnaires, Reference Hébert25 reflecting experiences of unwanted sexual acts (such as exhibitionism, sexual fondling and completed or attempted sexual intercourse; Cronbach's α = 0.65). This measure predicted later sexual problems and high-risk sexual behaviour in this cohort. Reference Lacelle, Hebert, Lavoie, Vitaro and Tremblay26 Participants who answered ‘yes’ to any of these questions were classified as having experienced sexual abuse. Physical abuse experienced before age 18 at the hands of a mother or father figure was measured using eight items of the Parent–Child Conflict Tactics Scale (Cronbach's α = 0.64). Reference Strauss, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore and Runyan23 Physical abuse was associated with later verbal and physical aggression in this cohort. Reference Pagani, Tremblay, Nagin, Zoccolillo, Vitaro and McDuff27 A total of 23 (7.0%) and 80 participants (24.5%) reported sexual and physical abuse respectively. A confirmatory factorial analysis confirmed that these indicators could be grouped into one factor (root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.045; comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.918; Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI) = 0.901). Except when otherwise mentioned, we have conducted the analyses according to five equal groups exposed to incremental levels of violence to avoid making comparisons on small numbers of observations.
Antisocial outcomes
Conduct disorder symptoms were assessed at age 15 using a semi-structured interview based on the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC) Reference Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan and Schwab-Stone28 and conducted by trained research assistants. The reliability of the French version of the DISC is satisfactory. Reference Valla, Breton, Bergeron, Gaudet, Berthiaume and Saint-Georges29 A total was created by summing up the criteria (range 0–11, mean 1.47 (s.d. = 1.81)). Antisocial personality symptoms were measured at age 21 in a similar interview, the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS) for adults, also based on the DSM-III-R criteria (such as illegal activities, impulsivity and remorselessness). Reference Robins, Helzer, Cottler and Goldring30 A total was obtained by summing up the criteria (range 0–11, mean 1.47 (s.d. = 1.81)). Property-violent crimes was assessed using 10 items (such as stealing, fraud and burglary) included in the life events calendar conducted when the participants were 21 years old. Reference Horney, Meier, Kennedy and Sacco31 We scored each behaviour as absent or present in the past year and identified 35 participants (10.7%) who reported at least one property crime. The occurrence of violent crimes during the past year was assessed using eight items drawn from the DIS Reference Robins, Helzer, Cottler and Goldring30 and the Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology – Basic Questionnaire. Reference Livesley and Jackson32 A total of 28 participants (8.6%) reported at least one violent crime. When combined, 58 participants (17.7%) committed either property or violent crimes. Arrest was measured using the life events calendar by asking participant to recall whether they have ever been arrested as an adult or juvenile. A total of 59 participants (18.0%) did. Physical partner violence was measured at 21 years using 15 items of the Conflict Tactics Scale, Reference Strauss, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore and Runyan23 including items such as pushed/grabbed/shoved; choked/strangled; threatened with knife/gun. A total was created by summing up the item (range 0–11, mean 1.47 (s.d. = 1.81)).
Statistical analyses
Preliminary analyses
In a series of ANOVAs, we detected comparable levels of violence between the MAOA alleles (Fs (1,325) varied from 0.07 to 0.85, all Ps >0.35), suggesting that the participants' genotype should not have affected violence experiences. However, mothers' antisocial personality (measured when the participants were age 15 using the DIS for adults) Reference Robins, Helzer, Cottler and Goldring30 was associated with their son's exposure to violence (F(2, 324) = 5.06, P = 0.007). We controlled for this potential confounder in all subsequent analyses by including this variable in the model along with the main and interaction effects to avoid bias associated with passive gene–environment correlations.
Main analyses
First, we tested the main and interaction (linear and non-linear) effects of the MAOA gene and exposure to violence on antisocial outcomes using weighted least squares analyses (with robust estimators). Second, we examined whether linear or quadratic functions best described the associations between violence and each outcome according to an SNP (rs2283725) in high linkage disequilibrium with the others. Significant quadratic associations among one group but not the other would suggest that the genetic moderation materialises only when a yet unidentified threshold of violence is reached. We then followed an analytical strategy adapted from Weder et al to test whether the interaction could take place only among individuals exposed to higher levels of violence. Reference Weder, Yang, Douglas-Palumberi, Massey, Krystal and Gelernter15 To do so, the interaction is estimated across the distribution (grouped in deciles) and then at increasing levels of violence through setting the interaction term to ‘0’ (one decile at the time). The total sample was thus always included in the analyses.
Results
Main effects of the MAOA gene and violence on antisocial outcomes
Exposure to violence was associated with more symptoms of conduct disorder in adolescence and antisocial personality in adulthood and a higher probability of arrest and partner violence (Table 1, Model 1). No clear association was noted for property-violent crimes. Two out of five MAOA SNPs uniquely contributed to conduct disorder symptoms and all five were associated with antisocial personality symptoms and partner violence once exposure to violence and maternal antisocial personality were accounted for. That is, low-frequency allele carriers had more conduct disorder and antisocial personality symptoms and reported more partner violence in comparison with high-frequency allele carriers. Online Table DS1 presents the bivariate associations between MAOA SNPs and antisocial outcomes.
Conduct disorder | Antisocial personality | Property-violent crimes | Arrest | Partner violence | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SNP ID | Linear | Non-linear | Linear | Non-linear | Linear | Non-linear | Linear | Non-linear | Linear | Non-linear |
rs5906893 | ||||||||||
1. Main effects | ||||||||||
rs5906893 | 4.32* | 2.84 † | 6.65** | 6.27** | 0.001 | 0.001 | 1.52 | 1.28 | 9.96** | 8.41** |
Violence | 38.56*** | 35.60*** | 21.63*** | 24.20*** | 1.99 | 6.50 | 3.74* | 10.87** | 32.98*** | 50.68*** |
2. GE | 3.38 † | 7.65 † | 0.63 | 11.75* | 0.16 | 2.29 | 0.25 | 0.68 | 7.06** | 8.23 † |
rs5906957 | ||||||||||
1. Main effects | ||||||||||
rs5906957 | 3.66 † | 2.53 | 6.64** | 6.45** | 0.42 | 0.44 | 2.50 | 2.23 | 6.70** | 5.75* |
Violence | 39.60*** | 36.07*** | 22.44*** | 24.70*** | 2.00 | 6.42 | 3.89* | 11.04** | 33.90** | 52.33*** |
2. GE | 0.98 | 5.66 | 1.01 | 8.13 † | 0.53 | 3.24 | 0.02 | 0.18 | 1.98 | 2.53 |
rs2283725 | ||||||||||
1. Main effects | ||||||||||
rs2283725 | 3.47 † | 2.94 † | 4.88* | 4.33* | 0.07 | 0.12 | 0.52 | 0.30 | 6.68** | 5.06* |
Violence | 37.80*** | 35.84*** | 20.49*** | 22.78*** | 2.06 | 6.74 | 3.71* | 11.05** | 3.11** | 48.60*** |
2. GE | 3.90* | 10.48* | 0.48 | 5.47 | 1.14 | 5.03 | 0.72 | 0.79 | 5.13* | 5.53 |
rs2072744 | ||||||||||
1. Main effects | ||||||||||
rs2072744 | 2.30 | 1.94 | 4.40* | 3.76* | 0.10 | 0.21 | 0.05 | 0.001 | 9.06** | 6.76** |
Violence | 37.18*** | 35.43*** | 19.65*** | 21.74*** | 2.09 | 6.85 | 3.83* | 11.41** | 29.71*** | 46.37*** |
2. GE | 4.09* | 11.13* | 0.57 | 7.43 | 1.78 | 3.29 | 0.76 | 0.95 | 5.93* | 4.83 |
rs979605 | ||||||||||
1. Main effects | ||||||||||
rs979605 | 3.73* | 2.52 | 6.94** | 6.68** | 0.007 | 0.001 | 0.66 | 0.54 | 13.40*** | 12.03*** |
Violence | 38.06*** | 35.57*** | 20.92*** | 23.33*** | 1.99 | 6.49 | 3.78* | 11.11** | 32.35*** | 50.24*** |
2. GE | 4.30* | 7.47 † | 0.63 | 9.36* | 0.64 | 0.64 | 0.38 | 1.38 | 9.04** | 9.60** |
GE, gene × environment interaction.
a. Model 1 presents the estimates of the main effects for MAOA SNPs and exposure to violence and Model 2 present the estimates of their interaction (the main effects obtained concurrently with the interaction are not presented). All models controlled for the mother's antisocial personality symptoms.
* P<0.05,
** P<0.01,
*** P<0.001,
† P<0.10.
Interaction between violence and MAOA gene on antisocial outcomes
Table 1 (Model 2) presents the tests of linear and non-linear interactions between the MAOA SNPs, exposure to violence and antisocial outcomes. Only conduct disorder symptoms and partner violence showed significant linear interaction between the gene and the environment (GE). Significant non-linear models were uncovered for antisocial personality symptoms, in addition to conduct disorder and partner violence. The Wald chi-squared estimates of GE also increased for property-violent crimes but did not reach significance. Also suggesting that a magnified risk of antisocial outcomes may come about at higher levels of exposure is the observation that quadratic associations tend to be present (albeit not always significant at α = 0.05) among the low-frequency allele carriers for conduct disorder (Wald χ2 = 5.61(1), P = 0.02), antisocial personality (Wald χ2 = 3.23(1), P = 0.07), property-violent crimes (Wald χ2 = 7.34(1), P = 0.007) and partner violence (Wald χ2 = 3.23(1), P = 0.07). Conversely, only linear associations were noted between violence and conduct disorder among the high-frequency allele carriers (Wald χ2 = 11.65(1), P = 0.001), antisocial personality symptoms (Wald χ2 = 12.58(1), P<0.001) and partner violence (Wald χ2 = 12.63(1), P<0.001). No linear or quadratic associations were detected for the history of arrest. Figure 1 shows that low-frequency allele carriers (rs2283725) had more symptoms of conduct disorder and antisocial personality and reported more property-violent crimes, arrests and partner violence than high-frequency allele carriers when exposed to higher levels of violence.
Exploratory sensitivity analyses
Figure 2 presents the Wald chi-squared estimates of the interaction between the MAOA SNPs and violence once the main effects of these variables, mothers' antisocial personality and selective attrition were accounted for. After testing the interaction across the full distribution, we repeat our tests while assuming that the interaction took place only at higher levels of violence (one decile at the time). The interaction coefficients became gradually larger, and eventually significant, when no interactions were assumed to occur at lower levels of exposure for conduct disorder and antisocial personality symptoms and property-violent crimes. A similar pattern of findings was detected for arrest without reaching significance. For partner violence, significant interactions were generally noted across the full distribution of violence in two out of five MAOA SNPs (rs5906893 and rs979605).
Discussion
Our study shows evidence of a moderating effect of the MAOA gene on antisocial outcomes in a population-based sample of young males. Higher risks for antisocial outcomes were observed in males carrying the MAOA low-frequency alleles (such as rs5906957; tag SNP) in comparison with high-frequency allele carriers for most outcomes when exposed to violence. This study extends previous investigations on two accounts.
First, a number of observations indicate that the MAOA gene may affect the impact of violence on antisocial outcomes once a certain level of violence was experienced. Among them are the detection of non-linear interactions between MAOA SNPs and violence for conduct disorder, antisocial personality symptoms and partner violence. Significant quadratic associations between violence and antisocial outcomes in low-frequency allele carriers but not among high-frequency allele carriers also points to that possibility. These findings are in line with a diathesis–stress model in which genetic liabilities are more likely to be expressed under adverse circumstances. In the absence of a conceptual model specifying the levels of violence at which the genetic moderation is expected to take place, we conducted exploratory analyses testing whether the interactions between the MAOA gene and violence varied according to level of exposure. This strategy was suggested by Weder et al who found a shifting contribution of the MAOA gene on inattention, aggression and rule-breaking behaviours as a function of the levels of trauma experienced. Reference Weder, Yang, Douglas-Palumberi, Massey, Krystal and Gelernter15 Large variation in the magnitude of the interaction was noted for all outcomes, with the exception of partner violence for which the interaction remained generally stable (and significant for two SNPs out of five). Collectively, these exploratory analyses allowed the detection of significant interactions for antisocial personality symptoms and property-violent crimes that would otherwise have been missed if linear interaction was assumed to take place across the distribution of violence. The possibility that a genetic moderation emerges once a certain threshold of violence was reached may depend on whether regulatory systems are affected incrementally (in a linear fashion) or only at higher levels of exposure. Models such as the biological sensitivity to the context propose non-linear associations between the environments and physiological outcomes. Reference Boyce and Ellis1 More generally, the idea of a striking increase in behavioural problems once a certain number of risk factors is present is not new. Reference Rutter, Kent and Rolf33 For example, Greenberg and colleagues reported a strong increase in conduct problems when three or more risk factors (such as poor parenting practices) were present. Reference Greenberg, Speltz, DeKlyen and Jones34 Because these risk factors are likely to be partially genetically mediated, we speculate that these associations may conceal non-linear GE interactions, as suggested in our findings. Closer attention to the level of violence experienced by participants may help to clarify the role of gene–environment interplay in the aetiology of antisocial behaviour. Reference Rutter35
Our findings are thus partially consistent with those originally reported by Caspi and colleagues Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin and Craig8 and others, Reference Foley, Eaves, Wormley, Silberg, Maes and Kuhn10–Reference Fergusson, Boden, Horwood, Miller and Kennedy12,Reference Nilsson, Sjoberg, Damberg, Leppert, Ohrvik and Alm16 who noted the impact of the MAOA genotype on antisocial behaviour in the context of maltreatment, but not otherwise. Other studies have, however, failed to replicate this finding. Reference Haberstick, Lessem, Hopfer, Smolen, Ehringer and Timberlake13,Reference Young, Smolen, Hewitt, Haberstick, Stallings and Corley14 One source of these inconsistencies may lie in the contrast tested between individuals exposed to distinct levels of ‘high’ exposure to violence. Without an a priori hypothesis regarding the precise level of violence required to trigger the genetically mediated ‘diathesis’, caution may be warranted in creating subgroups of exposure according to arbitrary cut-off points.
Importantly, few studies have investigated interactions between the MAOA gene, violence and antisocial outcomes using markers other than the promoter MAOA variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR), functionally related with enzyme expression and amine concentration. Reference Haberstick, Lessem, Hopfer, Smolen, Ehringer and Timberlake13 Our findings suggest that five other loci (rs5906893, rs5906957, rs2283725, rs2072744 and rs979605) may act in a similar way for antisocial outcomes although direct comparisons could not be performed as information about the VNTR was not available for our participants. Two lines of evidence suggest that the VNTR and these loci are linked. First, Ducci et al genotyped the VNTR as well as several other MAOA SNPs and found that rs979605 and the VNTR were in high linkage disequilibrium (D’ 0.95). Reference Ducci, Enoch, Hodgkinson, Xu, Catena and Robin36 Of note, this study was conducted in Southwest American Indians, which is not an isolated population with a defined founder effect, such as the French Canadians. Among French Canadians, linkage disequilibrium values between these variants are likely to be higher. Second, in Haploview (4.2), the MAOA region (HapMap v2 Release 21) in the CEU sample, and two of our selected SNPs (rs5906893 and rs5906957) are both in a haplotype block that extends far upstream of the MAOA gene (beyond the VNTR). Future studies exploring the extent to which non-linear interactions may exist would benefit from examining haplotypes including both the identified SNPs and the VNTR.
Second, we observed distinct patterns of GE between antisocial outcomes. Whereas symptoms of conduct disorder and antisocial personality, property-violent crimes and to a certain extent the history of arrest (albeit not significant) followed a pattern of findings suggesting that the genetic diathesis is not present across the distribution of violence but is only restricted to higher levels of violence, distinct findings were noted for partner violence. For that phenotype, the effect of the MAOA genotype appeared to be incremental, with no inflexion point at higher levels of exposure. To the best of our knowledge, no studies have yet tested the moderating role of the MAOA gene on violence occurring in the context of intimate relationships. It is, however, possible that the genetic aetiology of intimate partner violence may be different from other antisocial outcomes. To begin with, the heritability of partner violence is less than half of those generally reported for antisocial behaviour (16% v. 40–50%). Reference Hines and Saudino37 Also, enduring vulnerabilities for partner violence, such as depression, antisocial behaviour and hostility Reference Ouellet-Morin, Fisher, York-Smith, Fincham-Campbell, Moffitt and Arseneault38 (all of which being under partial genetic influence) were shown to influence partner violence through couple conflicts and negative relationship attributions, Reference Marshall, Jones and Feinberg39 which may complicate even more the description of the pathways by which genes influence these behaviours. Additional studies investigating specifically partner violence are needed to shed further light on its genetic basis.
Limitations
A few features of the study might have constrained the findings. First, a number of measures of antisocial outcomes and exposure to violence have been collected concurrently through self-reports. The use of a mono-method strategy and recall biases thus could have inflated associations between them. However, these potential biases could not explain away the main effects of the MAOA gene and should not have otherwise tainted the findings obtained for the symptoms of conduct disorder and antisocial personality (assessed in semi-structured interviews). By and large, the similar patterns of findings emerge across antisocial outcomes suggesting that the impact of such bias should be minimal. Second, because our measure of violence encompassed all experiences that have occurred prior to 18 years of age, we could not contrast the effect of childhood v. adolescence exposure to violence. Finally, despite evidence suggesting that the selected SNPs relate to the MAOA promoter VNTR, their functional role on MAOA activity remains to be demonstrated. In the same vein, the recurrence of similar patterns of findings across the selected SNPs should be interpreted cautiously given their level of linkage disequilibrium. Nevertheless, the emergence of distinct findings between the SNPs suggests that our analytical strategy allowed a sensitive test of the impact of genetically based differences within the MAOA gene on antisocial outcomes.
Directions for future research
This study extends previous research by showing that the genetic moderation of the MAOA gene may not be present across the full distribution of violence but emerges at a certain level and vary according to different antisocial outcomes. Future studies should investigate the cognitive, emotional and neurophysiological mechanisms detecting and translating the individual's vulnerability to violence, test whether they catalyse the expression of genetically based differences in antisocial behaviour and whether these mechanisms are themselves likely to vary across individuals.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.