Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:14:52.820Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Trying to name what doesn't change: Neural nonresponse to Cognitive Therapy for depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 May 2023

Marlene V. Strege*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
John A. Richey
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, VA, USA
Greg J. Siegle
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Marlene V. Strege, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Theoretical models of neural mechanisms underlying Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT) for major depressive disorder (MDD) propose that psychotherapy changes neural functioning of prefrontal cortical structures associated with cognitive-control processes (DeRubeis, Siegle, & Hollon, 2008); however, MDD is persistent and characterized by long-lasting vulnerabilities to recurrence after intervention, suggesting that underlying neural mechanisms of MDD remain despite treatment. It follows that identification of treatment-resistant aberrant neural processes in MDD may inform clinical and research efforts targeting sustained remission. Thus, we sought to identify brain regions showing aberrant neural functioning in MDD that either (1) fail to exhibit substantive change (nonresponse) or (2) exhibit functional changes (response) following CBT.

Methods

To identify treatment-resistant neural processes (as well as neural processes exhibiting change after treatment), we collected functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data of MDD patients (n = 58) before and after CBT as well as never-depressed controls (n = 35) before and after a similar amount of time. We evaluated fMRI data using conjunction analyses, which utilized several contrast-based criteria to characterize brain regions showing both differences between patients and controls at baseline and nonresponse or response to CBT.

Results

Findings revealed nonresponse in a cerebellar region and response in prefrontal and parietal regions.

Conclusions

Results are consistent with prior theoretical models of CBT's direct effect on cortical regulatory processes but expand on them with identification of additional regions (and associated neural systems) of response and nonresponse to CBT.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Psychotherapy is associated with neural changes in major depressive disorder (MDD) (Franklin, Carson, & Welch, Reference Franklin, Carson and Welch2016; Frewen, Dozois, & Lanius, Reference Frewen, Dozois and Lanius2008; Marwood, Wise, Perkins, & Cleare, Reference Marwood, Wise, Perkins and Cleare2018; Sankar et al., Reference Sankar, Melin, Lorenzetti, Horton, Costafreda and Fu2018); however, despite observed neural changes, the course of treated MDD remains chronic and impairing, featuring persistent symptoms and vulnerability to future depressive experiences (Judd et al., Reference Judd, Akiskal, Maser, Zeller, Endicott, Coryell and Keller1998; Kennedy, Abbott, & Paykel, Reference Kennedy, Abbott and Paykel2004; Strege, Richey, & Siegle, Reference Strege, Richey and Siegle2022). The observation of neural changes does not inform potential neural disease-relevant mechanisms that are resistant to change (i.e. neural markers distinct to depressed individuals that persist despite intervention). Complementary knowledge of treatment-resistant processes could speed progress toward interventions that provide sustained remission, as these markers may equally represent plausible targets for intervention to the extent that they are both pathognomonic of depression and treatment-resistant. Accordingly, our objective was to identify brain regions (and related neural systems) of nonresponse as well as treatment change. We conducted functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) whole-brain conjunction analyses in MDD patients before and after Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT) and never-depressed individuals at two time points equal in duration to MDD patients, in order to characterize brain regions that were both divergent from controls at baseline and either responsive to treatment or remained measurably stable.

Several theoretical models of CBT's influence on neural functioning suggest CBT targets cognitive-control processes and related cortical regions, that may relate to depressive symptomatology via regulatory influence of structures involved in more automatic threat- or salience-related reactivity (Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010; DeRubeis, Siegle, & Hollon, Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008; Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004; Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003). The proposed interactions between cognitive-control and salience-related neural systems are consistent with CBT theory and practice. According to cognitive theory, depression involves recurrent maladaptive information processing tendencies, such as prioritized attention for negative stimuli. CBT targets maladaptive cognitive biases with therapeutic exercises, during which the individual practices examining the accuracy and utility of their thoughts and generating more realistic and adaptive thoughts (Beck, Reference Beck2008; Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010). Through repeated exercises, one develops skills at recognizing, challenging, and reducing reactivity to negative thinking patterns, thus strengthening cognitive-control abilities during negative emotional states. The theorized cognitive-control neural systems affected by CBT involve prefrontal regions (e.g. lateral/dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), medial prefrontal cortex, and orbitofrontal cortex), and may also involve subregions of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), such as the subgenual ACC (sgACC); however, theories differ in interpretation of ACC involvement, with some potential for salience-related or salience-moderating properties (Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010). Subcortical structures in these models primarily include the amygdala and/or hippocampus, viewed as salience-related and associated with strong emotional experiences. Thus, theoretical frameworks suggest CBT may affect subcortical salience functioning as an indirect effect of increased cortical regulation.

Research on neural effects of CBT in depression provides only partial support for cortical regulation models. One review observed CBT and interpersonal psychotherapy for depression primarily affected prefrontal (dorsolateral, ventrolateral, and medial) and cingulate (anterior and posterior) cortical functioning (Frewen et al., Reference Frewen, Dozois and Lanius2008), whereas a more recent review of pre- v. post-CBT neuroimaging studies in depression found that the ACC (multiple subregions) most consistently showed change, instead of the DLPFC or other prefrontal regions. They also reported change (although less frequently) of subcortical regions associated with salience-related processes [e.g. amygdala and hippocampus (Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Carson and Welch2016; Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Anderson, Leal, Shestyuk, Gulsen, Mnatsakanyan and Lin2017)]. Functional change of the ACC (rostral) after psychotherapy (CBT, behavioral activation, and psychodynamic) for depression was also reported by a recent meta-analysis (Sankar et al., Reference Sankar, Melin, Lorenzetti, Horton, Costafreda and Fu2018). Another recent meta-analysis that also collapsed across psychotherapy types in depression and anxiety similarly found only partial support for prior theories, reporting most robust changes in anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortical regions as well as the insula (Marwood et al., Reference Marwood, Wise, Perkins and Cleare2018). Mixed findings in the literature may reflect heterogeneous study methodologies (e.g. task type, neuroimaging modality, and sample characteristics) as well as heterogeneous review methodologies (e.g. treatment and sample inclusion criteria), suggesting CBT changes neural functioning, but its effects may be more nuanced than indicated by prior theoretical models.

Despite observed neural changes, depressive symptoms and vulnerabilities persist in MDD after treatment. Long-term intervention follow-up research suggests that the course of MDD involves persistent (most often low-grade) depressive symptoms (Judd et al., Reference Judd, Akiskal, Maser, Zeller, Endicott, Coryell and Keller1998; Kennedy et al., Reference Kennedy, Abbott and Paykel2004; Strege et al., Reference Strege, Richey and Siegle2022). Even individuals considered ‘remitted’ often exhibit at least one residual symptom, with some estimates indicating over 90% of remitted patients (Nierenberg et al., Reference Nierenberg, Husain, Trivedi, Fava, Warden, Wisniewski and Rush2010). Common residual depressive symptoms after treatment consist of sleep disturbance and fatigue (Conradi, Ormel, & de Jonge, Reference Conradi, Ormel and de Jonge2011; McClintock et al., Reference McClintock, Husain, Wisniewski, Nierenberg, Stewart, Trivedi and Rush2011; Nierenberg et al., Reference Nierenberg, Husain, Trivedi, Fava, Warden, Wisniewski and Rush2010; Romera et al., Reference Romera, Pérez, Ciudad, Caballero, Roca, Polavieja and Gilaberte2013; Taylor, Walters, Vittengl, Krebaum, & Jarrett, Reference Taylor, Walters, Vittengl, Krebaum and Jarrett2010), sad mood (McClintock et al., Reference McClintock, Husain, Wisniewski, Nierenberg, Stewart, Trivedi and Rush2011; Romera et al., Reference Romera, Pérez, Ciudad, Caballero, Roca, Polavieja and Gilaberte2013; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Walters, Vittengl, Krebaum and Jarrett2010), and concentration difficulties (Conradi et al., Reference Conradi, Ormel and de Jonge2011; McClintock et al., Reference McClintock, Husain, Wisniewski, Nierenberg, Stewart, Trivedi and Rush2011). Individuals with MDD also experience high rates of relapse and recurrence, with some estimates over 90% when followed for 25 years (Gotlib & Hammen, Reference Gotlib and Hammen2008), suggesting that even during periods of reduced symptoms, vulnerability for more severe psychopathology persists throughout the lifespan. The persistence of symptoms as well as risk of relapse/recurrence suggests underlying neurobiological mechanisms of depression maintenance may continue to persist despite treatment.

In consideration of prior theory and supporting research on CBT's effects on cognitive-control and salience-related neural systems (Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010; DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008; Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004; Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003), we used a task with salient negative emotional words and prompts asking participants to rate the self-relevance of words, to encourage elaborative processing of emotional stimuli (Siegle, Granholm, Ingram, & Matt, Reference Siegle, Granholm, Ingram and Matt2001). We examined indices of sustained neural reactivity to capture disturbances in cognitive control (Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer, & Thase, Reference Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer and Thase2007). We hypothesized functional changes of the DLPFC following CBT, as it is the most consistently referenced cognitive control prefrontal region of prior theories (Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010; DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008; Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004; Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003) and because it is considered a major hub of a brain network commonly associated with cognitive control, the executive network (Menon, Reference Menon2011); however, with mixed findings in the literature, we may not observe DLPFC functional change. The literature also suggests that we may observe functional changes in other regions as well, e.g. ACC subregions. Given the novelty of our approach for identifying nonresponse, it is unclear from theory and literature which regions will show functional nonresponse. With theory stating CBT affects cognitive control and cortical regulation, we anticipate that nonresponse will not include prefrontal cortical regions. Nonresponse may include salience-related subcortical regions, as CBT is theorized not to directly affect subcortical reactivity (Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010; DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008; Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004; Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003); however, findings of subcortical functional changes in the literature also suggest the potential for this to not be the case (Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Carson and Welch2016).

Although the study focus was on processing of negative emotional stimuli, examining reactivity to other valence types would inform whether observed neural changes and nonresponse findings are specific to negative emotions or more generalized. Thus, to assess valence specificity, we conducted exploratory analyses on positive and neutral word trials also included in the task.

To identify neural systems of nonresponse to therapy and therapy-related change mechanisms, we assessed individuals with MDD (n = 58) and never-depressed controls (n = 35) at two time points, before and after CBT for individuals with MDD and before and after a comparable amount of time for never-depressed controls. We used conjunction analyses to characterize areas of nonresponse (showing differences between MDD patients and never-depressed controls prior to treatment and also showing no practical change with treatment). In an additional exploratory analysis for biomarkers, we assessed whether any of the nonresponse regions were also prognostic of treatment outcome. For assessing change mechanisms, we considered remediation of existing aberrant functioning as well as novel compensatory developments. As an exploratory aim, we assessed areas showing remediation-based change for normalization, comparing post-treatment MDD patients and never-depressed controls.

Consistent with the aim of examining nonresponse and response to therapy in the brain, we also conducted region-of-interest (ROI) analyses with regions associated with cognitive-control and salience-related neural systems. We chose the DLPFC (cognitive control), amygdala (salience related), and sgACC (potential moderator of neural processes) as representative of these brain systems because they were consistently referenced across theoretical models (Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010; DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008; Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004; Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003), and they were also associated with related neural networks (e.g. executive, (Menon, Reference Menon2011); salience, (Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Anderson, Leal, Shestyuk, Gulsen, Mnatsakanyan and Lin2017)) or have shown functional connections with regions of these networks (Disner, Beevers, Haigh, & Beck, Reference Disner, Beevers, Haigh and Beck2011; Drevets, Savitz, & Trimble, Reference Drevets, Savitz and Trimble2008). However, other ROIs (e.g. other prefrontal or anterior cingulate subregions, the insula, and the hippocampus) also could have been included as representative of networks. Our objective was not to test all empirically-supported regions but rather to get a supplementary estimate of neural processes of interest (cognitive control and salience related).

Method

Participants

Participants were adults (n = 58) with MDD who received CBT as part of prior clinical trials (MH58356; PI: Thase, MH58397; PI: Jarrett; MH074807; PI: Siegle) and underwent fMRI tasks before and after treatment. Participants also included never-depressed controls (n = 35) from MH074807, assessed at two timepoints of comparable duration to those of the MDD group. The CBT participants completed Cognitive Therapy (Beck, Reference Beck1979) protocols that consisted of 16–20 sessions. See our prior publication (Siegle et al., Reference Siegle, Thompson, Collier, Berman, Feldmiller, Thase and Friedman2012) for additional information regarding the therapy protocol. Participants met MDD DSM-IV diagnostic criteria via a structured clinical interview (SCID-IV; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1996) and also scored at least 14 on the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1960) prior to the start of treatment. In interest of contrasting pre- and post-assessments, analyses were restricted to participants who had fMRI task data for both timepoints (Consort diagram, Supplement 1). Sample demographics and clinical characteristics (Supplement 2) show that depressed individuals and never-depressed controls did not differ on demographic variables but differed on self-reported depression symptoms.

fMRI task and data preparation

We used a modified personal relevance rating task (PRRT) (Siegle, Carter, & Thase, Reference Siegle, Carter and Thase2006; Siegle et al., Reference Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer and Thase2007; Siegle et al., Reference Siegle, Thompson, Collier, Berman, Feldmiller, Thase and Friedman2012), during which trials (N = 60) began with a fixation cue (row of X's with prongs around center X), presented for one second. A word (normed or idiographic) of positive, negative, or neutral valence appeared after the fixation cue for 200 ms. Analyses were of negative word trials. After each word, a row of X's appeared and stayed on the screen for 10.8 s. Each trial contained prompt asking participants to push a button indicating perceived self-relevance (relevant, somewhat relevant, not relevant) of the word.

Our fMRI processing methods were consistent with our prior publication (Siegle et al., Reference Siegle, Thompson, Collier, Berman, Feldmiller, Thase and Friedman2012). Processing was done with locally developed NeuroImaging Software and AFNI. We applied slice-time correction, motion correction (AFNI 3dVolReg), linear detrending to eliminate scanner drift, and voxelwise winsorization of outliers. We converted data to percent-change from that voxel's median within the run. Data were additionally temporally smoothed (7-point Gaussian filter), cross-registered to the Colin-27 MNI with AIR's 32-parameter non-linear warp (Woods, Mazziotta, & Cherry, Reference Woods, Mazziotta and Cherry1993), and spatially smoothed (6-mm full width half maximum), and we normalized time series across scanners.

Contrast images were sustained neural reactivity to negative words, calculated as the difference between the mean of later (4th–7th) images for each negative-word trial and that trial's first image, acquired during the presentation of the trial's fixation cue (prior to the negative word). We did not use neutral or positive words for contrasts due to prior research showing that neutral and positive stimuli may not actually be neutral and positive for depressed individuals (Epstein et al., Reference Epstein, Pan, Kocsis, Yang, Butler, Chusid and Silbersweig2006). For example, a study looking at neural reactivity to emotional words in depressed individuals found that depressed individuals do not show neural reactivity of the ventral striatum, a reward-related region, in response to positive words. In the same study, depressed individuals actually showed greater neural reactivity of the left amygdala to neutral words than negative words (Epstein et al., Reference Epstein, Pan, Kocsis, Yang, Butler, Chusid and Silbersweig2006). These neuroimaging findings are consistent with clinical observations and behavioral data, e.g. negative interpretations of neutral experiences when depressed (Hindash & Amir, Reference Hindash and Amir2012), as well as prior self-report research, e.g. difficulty sustaining positive affect in response to positive scripts (Horner et al., Reference Horner, Siegle, Schwartz, Price, Haggerty, Collier and Friedman2014).

Conjunction analyses

To identify brain areas and related neural systems of treatment nonresponse and change mechanisms, we conducted several voxelwise whole-brain conjunction analyses (Friston Reference Friston, Holmes, Price, Büchel and Worsley1999; Friston Reference Friston, Penny and Glaser2005) in which we interpreted our hypotheses as supported when all a priori criteria were met across participants. One set of contrasts probed differences between the MDD group and never-depressed controls (voxels showing aberrant neural functioning). Other contrasts probed the effect of time, such as pre- and post-treatment for the MDD group (voxels showing change or absence of change after treatment). We calculated voxelwise Cohen's d (henceforth, d; details in Supplement 3) for each contrast image and used these effect size estimates to test the a priori criteria for establishing conjunction between images at a given voxel. Reliance on clinically significant or demonstrably null effects across all tests within the conjunction obviates common concerns regarding the interpretability of conjunction analyses in fMRI (Nichols, Brett, Andersson, Wager, & Poline, Reference Nichols, Brett, Andersson, Wager and Poline2005) without requiring significance thresholds suggested to be overly conservative (Friston, Reference Friston, Penny and Glaser2005).

To test a priori criteria, we used effect size cutoffs to determine a practical absence of effect (nonresponse) or a minimum treatment effect that was still clinically-relevant (response). In light of fMRI effect size literature and supporting Human Connectome Project data reporting smaller effect size estimates (Cremers, Wager, & Yarkoni, Reference Cremers, Wager and Yarkoni2017; Poldrack et al., Reference Poldrack, Baker, Durnez, Gorgolewski, Matthews, Munafò and Yarkoni2017), we anticipated ‘response’ to be a modest effect at best, a minimal yet clinically relevant effect (d > 0.24) (Cuijpers, Turner, Koole, van Dijke, & Smit, Reference Cuijpers, Turner, Koole, van Dijke and Smit2014), whereas we viewed nonresponse as a practical absence of effect (d < 0.05), a threshold considered to reflect approximately 98% overlap between samples (Grice & Barrett, Reference Grice and Barrett2014). A practical absence of effect (d < 0.05) is represented as ‘ = ’ below. A clinically relevant effect (d > 0.24) is represented as ‘≠’ below. Pre/post represents prior to or after treatment for depressed participants or a comparable amount of time for control participants. Conjunction ‘and’ operations are represented as ‘*’ below (tabular presentation of formulae for change mechanisms and nonresponse, Table 1).

Table 1. Defining change mechanism and nonresponse

Note. Check mark indicates samples differed (d > 0.24) at the voxel; X indicates a practical absence of effect between samples (d < 0.05) at the voxel.

Formulae

Defining nonresponse. We calculated nonresponse as: (pre-control ≠ pre-depressed) * (pre-depressed = post-depressed).

Defining biomarker. Biomarkers were areas of nonresponse that also were prognostic of treatment outcome, calculated as voxelwise R 2 change from adding depression symptoms (Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) residuals) to a scanner-only whole-brain regression.

Defining change mechanisms. We considered two potential change mechanisms, (1) remediation-based and (2) compensatory. We defined remediation-based change as: (pre-control ≠ pre-depressed) * (pre-depressed ≠ post-depressed) * (pre-post control ≠ pre-post depressed). We defined compensatory change as: (pre-control = pre-depressed) * (pre-control ≠ post-depressed) * (pre-post control ≠ pre-post depressed).

Defining normalization. We considered normalization regions to be areas that exhibited change mechanism qualities (defined above) and also showed comparable neural reactivity between post-treatment depressed patients and never-depressed controls, as suggested by voxelwise d estimates for the post-control v. post-depressed contrast.

Cluster thresholding

To control for Type I error, AFNI's 3dFWHMx and 3dClustSim's ACF model provided cluster size for cluster thresholding. 3dFWHMx's spatial autocorrelation function provided the noise smoothness values for 3dClustSim (post 2015 smoothing correction). We used the conjunctive p for the voxel p values (<0.005 for change mechanism and <0.02 for nonresponse), and we set the significance threshold to p < 0.05. We defined conjunctive p as the product of p values for each contrast in the conjunction analysis, e.g. change mechanism conjunctive p corresponds to: (p value for d1 > 0.24) * (p value for d2 > 0.24) * (p value for d3 > 0.24). 3dClustSim's ACF model provided cluster thresholds of >93 voxels for change mechanisms and >297 voxels for nonresponse for whole brain results.

A priori regions of interest

For assessing whether a priori ROIs (bilateral DLPFC, sgACC, and amygdala), representative of cognitive-control and salience-related neural systems, met criteria for change mechanism or nonresponse, we applied region masks to the results of the whole-brain voxelwise conjunction analyses. To determine appropriate cluster thresholds, we submitted each region mask to the 3dclustim function and followed the aforementioned cluster thresholding approach.

Results

Nonresponse

One vermis-centered cluster comprising primarily cerebellar regions met contrast criteria for nonresponse and survived cluster-thresholding (Table 2; Fig. 1). MDD patients showed reduced activation of the nonresponse cluster relative to control participants, which did not increase after CBT (Fig. 2a).

Fig. 1. Treatment nonresponse cluster.

Note. (a) Pre-Ctrl ≠ Pre-Dep represents brain areas where control participants differed (d > 0.24) from depressed patients prior to treatment. (b) Pre-Dep = Post-Dep represents brain areas where a practical absence of effect (d < 0.05) was observed when comparing depressed patients prior to and after treatment. (c) Nonresponse represents areas that met both A and B effect size criteria and survived cluster thresholding [>297 voxels (AFNI's NN3, 2-sided)].

Coloring represents whole-brain regression R2 Δ values on neural reactivity to negative words at the pre-treatment scan as predicted by residual BDI-II scores above and beyond scanner (i.e. extent to which the region acts as a predictor of treatment response); the overall low values suggest non-response regions are not associated with either clinical change or change in activity in association with treatment.

Fig. 2. Box and scatter plots for individual averages of nonresponse and response clusters.

Note. (a) Individual mean reactivity averages of the nonresponse cluster for control participants (Pre-Ctrl) and MDD patients prior to and after CBT (Pre-Dep, Post-Dep); (b) Individual mean reactivity averages of the change mechanism regions (all clusters) for pre and post assessments for both control participants (Pre-Ctrl, Post-Ctrl) and MDD patients (Pre-Dep, Post-Dep). Gray lines represent mean, and black lines represent median.

Table 2. Coordinates for clusters of non-response and change mechanisms

Note. Coordinates are cluster centroids reported in MNI space.

Biomarkers

R 2 changes from incorporating depression symptom residuals (BDI-II) to a whole-brain regression (neural reactivity regressed on site) ranged from 0 to 0.15 in the non-response cluster, suggesting minimal prognostic value (Fig. 1).

Change mechanisms

Three clusters met a priori contrast criteria for a remediation-based change mechanism and survived cluster-thresholding (Table 2; Fig. 3). MDD patients exhibited less reactivity to negative stimuli in prefrontal and parietal cortical change mechanism clusters relative to controls, which increased after CBT for the MDD group (Fig. 2b). No areas that met contrast criteria for a compensatory change mechanism survived cluster-thresholding.

Fig. 3. Treatment response (change mechanism) clusters.

Note. (a) Pre-Ctrl ≠ Pre-Dep represents brain areas where control participants differed (d > 0.24) from depressed patients prior to treatment. (b) Pre-Dep ≠ Post-Dep represents brain areas where depressed patients prior to and after treatment differed (d > 0.24). (c) Pre-Post Ctrl ≠ Pre-Post Dep represents brain areas where pre-post estimates differed (d > 0.24) between control participants and depressed patients. (d) Change Mechanism represents brain areas that met all prior criteria (a, b, c) and survived cluster thresholding (>93 voxels [AFNI's NN3, 2-sided]). Coloring represents d estimates (contrast of post-treatment depressed patients and never-depressed control participants), showing some instances in which effect sizes suggest normalization occurs

Normalization

For brain regions suggesting a remediation-based change mechanism, some portions of clusters exhibited normalization following CBT; effect sizes (d) ranged from 0 to 0.5 for differences between post-treatment depressed patients (Fig. 3).

A priori regions of interest

The application of nonresponse and change mechanism a priori criteria to the bilateral, functionally defined DLPFC (Siegle et al., Reference Siegle, Thompson, Collier, Berman, Feldmiller, Thase and Friedman2012) resulted in separate clusters within the DLPFC that exhibit change mechanism or nonresponse qualities (Fig. 4). The change mechanism cluster (3187.50 mm3) within the DLPFC was centered at 33, 24, 39 (MNI), and the nonresponse cluster (1718.75 mm3) within the DLPFC was centered at 45, 26, 31 (MNI). Both cluster centroids were in the middle frontal gyrus. The application of nonresponse and change mechanism criteria to the bilateral sgACC and amygdala resulted in small clusters (~2 voxels) within a priori regions that did not survive cluster correction.

Fig. 4. Clusters within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Note. The yellow cluster represents a subregion within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that met criteria for a change mechanism [pre-depressed ≠ pre-control * pre-depressed ≠ post-depressed * pre-post depressed ≠ pre-post control (≠ corresponds to d > 0.24)] and survived cluster correction (>19 voxels). The red cluster represents a subregion within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that met criteria for nonresponse [pre-depressed ≠ pre-control * pre-depressed = post-depressed (≠ corresponds to d > 0.24 and = corresponds to d < 0.05)] and met cluster correction (>43 voxels).

Discussion

The study objective was to identify brain areas (in context of related neural systems) resistant to as well as responsive to change following CBT. Exploratory aims were to evaluate the extent to which identified nonresponse regions acted as biomarkers (prognostic of treatment outcome) and the extent to which change mechanism regions normalized (comparable after treatment to never-depressed controls). Conjunction analyses resulted in one vermis-centered cluster that differed between depressed and control participants and did not change following CBT, as well as three prefrontal and parietal clusters that changed after treatment. Smaller observed biomarker and normalization effect sizes suggest the cerebellar nonresponse region shows minimal prognostic potential for CBT, and subregions of prefrontal and parietal change mechanism regions appear to normalize after CBT. Analyses specific to regions often-cited in related theoretical models and associated with neural systems of interest (e.g. DLPFC, sgACC, amygdala) yielded subregions of the DLPFC meeting criteria for nonresponse or change mechanism.

Study findings were largely consistent with prior theoretical models of cognitive regulatory processes (Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010; DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008; Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004; Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003), yet expanded on prior work with consideration of additional regions/subregions. We found functional DLPFC subregions that showed different responses (change mechanism v. nonresponse) to CBT. This observation could be considered consistent with prior work suggesting functional subdivisions of the DLPFC exist (Cieslik et al., Reference Cieslik, Zilles, Caspers, Roski, Kellermann, Jakobs and Eickhoff2012) and may be differentially associated with treatment (Rosen et al., Reference Rosen, Bhat, Cardenas, Ehrlich, Horwege, Mathalon and Yesavage2021), or it could be consistent with a partial response model in which DLPFC functioning was somewhat, but incompletely affected by CBT. The change mechanism DLPFC subregion was more ventral than the nonresponse region and had a portion of the cluster extending farther in the anterior direction, thus appearing more consistent with the meta-analytically derived anterior-ventral subregion (Cieslik et al., Reference Cieslik, Zilles, Caspers, Roski, Kellermann, Jakobs and Eickhoff2012). The anterior-ventral subregion has shown stronger association with the ACC (Cieslik et al., Reference Cieslik, Zilles, Caspers, Roski, Kellermann, Jakobs and Eickhoff2012), subregions of which have shown strong connections with subcortical salience-related structures (Disner et al., Reference Disner, Beevers, Haigh and Beck2011; Drevets et al., Reference Drevets, Savitz and Trimble2008), suggesting this DLPFC subregion's potential influence of salience-driven processes. In support of this division, targeting the DLPFC subregion showing greater connectivity to the sgACC is associated with better depression response to transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) (Rosen et al., Reference Rosen, Bhat, Cardenas, Ehrlich, Horwege, Mathalon and Yesavage2021).

Additional change mechanism findings of increased activation of the precuneus and angular gyrus suggest that the cognitive-control regulation processes of CBT may also involve functional changes of default mode network (DMN) regions (Raichle, Reference Raichle2015). A wealth of research suggests aberrant DMN functioning in MDD (Hamilton et al., Reference Hamilton, Farmer, Fogelman and Gotlib2015; Scalabrini et al., Reference Scalabrini, Vai, Poletti, Damiani, Mucci, Colombo and Northoff2020; Williams, Reference Williams2016), as it is believed to contribute to depression symptoms and disease maintenance features, e.g. rumination (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Chen, Shen, Li, Chen, Zhu and Yan2020). Often-observed inverse correlations of DMN regions with the task network would not predict this observation, but it is consistent with more nuanced conceptions of DMN and prefrontal interactions, with region activity and functional connectivity contingent on task requirement and stimulus type (Beaty, Benedek, Silvia, & Schacter, Reference Beaty, Benedek, Silvia and Schacter2016; Bluhm et al., Reference Bluhm, Clark, McFarlane, Moores, Shaw and Lanius2011; Hearne, Cocchi, Zalesky, & Mattingley, Reference Hearne, Cocchi, Zalesky and Mattingley2015; Mayer, Roebroeck, Maurer, & Linden, Reference Mayer, Roebroeck, Maurer and Linden2010; Piccoli et al., Reference Piccoli, Valente, Linden, Re, Esposito, Sack and Di Salle2015; Spreng, Stevens, Chamberlain, Gilmore, & Schacter, Reference Spreng, Stevens, Chamberlain, Gilmore and Schacter2010; Sreenivas, Boehm, & Linden, Reference Sreenivas, Boehm and Linden2012). For example, goal-directed tasks that involve internally focused processes (e.g. self-referential), show activity of and positive functional associations between DMN and prefrontal regions, with greater functional coupling of DMN and prefrontal regions associated with optimal performance (Beaty et al., Reference Beaty, Benedek, Silvia and Schacter2016; Konishi, McLaren, Engen, & Smallwood, Reference Konishi, McLaren, Engen and Smallwood2015; Straub et al., Reference Straub, Plener, Sproeber, Sprenger, Koelch, Groen and Abler2015). The finding of increased activity of key DMN regions and the task network during a self-relevance rating task suggests that CBT's proposed mechanisms of enhanced cognitive control may also involve dynamic cooperation of neural systems (task-based and default mode), necessary for flexible allocation of cognitive resources (Cocchi, Zalesky, Fornito, & Mattingley, Reference Cocchi, Zalesky, Fornito and Mattingley2013; Zabelina & Andrews-Hanna, Reference Zabelina and Andrews-Hanna2016).

Areas that did not show change in response to CBT, but were different in depressed and never-depressed individuals before treatment, may give particular insight into why depression symptoms return, despite remission in therapy, as they could represent ongoing vulnerabilities to recurrence. In particular, the vermis (and cerebellum more broadly) is largely not referenced in popular theoretical models of CBT (Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010; DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008; Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004; Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003); however, there is substantial literature on its involvement in emotion processes (Adamaszek et al., Reference Adamaszek, D'Agata, Ferrucci, Habas, Keulen, Kirkby and Verhoeven2017; Pierce & Péron, Reference Pierce and Péron2020; Sacchetti, Scelfo, & Strata, Reference Sacchetti, Scelfo and Strata2009; Schmahmann, Reference Schmahmann2010; Strata, Reference Strata2015) and MDD (Villanueva, Reference Villanueva2012). The vermis, is specifically referred to as a key region of cerebellar affective processing (Baumann & Mattingley, Reference Baumann and Mattingley2012; Pierce & Péron, Reference Pierce and Péron2020), the ‘limbic cerebellum’ (Stoodley & Schmahmann, Reference Stoodley and Schmahmann2010). It is theorized to contribute to the detection of a stimulus’ emotional relevance by modulating reactivity of limbic structures within the salience network (Habas et al., Reference Habas, Kamdar, Nguyen, Prater, Beckmann, Menon and Greicius2009), potentially suggesting that while CBT targets cortical regulators of the salience network, it may not address other regulators.

The vermis is also implicated in a wide variety of body-relevant processing such as postural control (Colnaghi, Honeine, Sozzi, & Schieppati, Reference Colnaghi, Honeine, Sozzi and Schieppati2017) and expression of emotion in the body (Sokolov et al., Reference Sokolov, Zeidman, Erb, Pollick, Fallgatter, Ryvlin and Pavlova2020). Increasing data suggests profound roles for how people relate to their body in emotional information processing (Colombetti, Reference Colombetti2014; Damasio, Reference Damasio1999; De Gelder, Reference De Gelder2016; van der Kolk, Reference van der Kolk1994; Wassmann, Reference Wassmann2010), particularly with regard to posture (Dael, Mortillaro, & Scherer, Reference Dael, Mortillaro and Scherer2012; Gilbert, Martin, & Coulson, Reference Gilbert, Martin and Coulson2011; Strata, Reference Strata2015). Potentially, CBT, especially CT focuses on cognitions (the mind), leaving vulnerabilities to continued symptomatology in the arena of mind/body relationships, and the body's role in emotion. This theory would suggest that mind-body interventions such as yoga, which affect the cerebellum (van Aalst et al., Reference van Aalst, Ceccarini, Schramm, Van Weehaeghe, Rezaei, Demyttenaere and Van Laere2020, Reference van Aalst, Jennen, Demyttenaere, Sunaert, Koole, Ceccarini and Van Laere2021), could be of interest as adjuncts to CBT.

Other potentially adjunctive interventions (e.g. antidepressant medications, exercise, neuromodulation) also demonstrate functional cerebellar change. In an MDD intervention study, increased cerebellar activity was identified as a unique effect of paroxetine, an effect absent from the CBT comparison group (Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004). Several imaging studies examining antidepressant medication effects in MDD samples have also found functional changes of the cerebellum (Cullen et al., Reference Cullen, Klimes-Dougan, Vu, Westlund Schreiner, Mueller, Eberly and Lim2016; Delaveau et al., Reference Delaveau, Jabourian, Lemogne, Guionnet, Bergouignan and Fossati2011; Frodl et al., Reference Frodl, Scheuerecker, Schoepf, Linn, Koutsouleris, Bokde and Meisenzahl2011; Fu et al., Reference Fu, Williams, Cleare, Brammer, Walsh, Kim and Bullmore2004, Reference Fu, Williams, Brammer, Suckling, Kim, Cleare and Bullmore2007; Mayberg et al., Reference Mayberg, Brannan, Tekell, Silva, Mahurin, McGinnis and Jerabek2000). In addition to antidepressant medication, there is preliminary support of repeated aerobic exercise altering cerebellar functioning (Leddy et al., Reference Leddy, Cox, Baker, Wack, Pendergast, Zivadinov and Willer2013; Won et al., Reference Won, Faroqi-Shah, Callow, Williams, Awoyemi, Nielson and Smith2021). A more targeted cerebellar intervention may involve neuromodulation via electrical or magnetic stimulation. For an early example of cerebellar stimulation, patients of heterogeneous treatment-resistant psychiatric conditions saw symptom improvements following implantation of a vermis pacemaker (Heath, Reference Heath1977). Less-invasive options for modulating cerebellar functioning include TMS and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). For example, repetitive TMS of the vermis has been found to improve depressive symptoms in schizophrenia relative to a sham condition (Garg, Sinha, Tikka, Mishra, & Goyal, Reference Garg, Sinha, Tikka, Mishra and Goyal2016), and tDCS of the cerebellum has been shown to enhance emotion recognition and improve symptoms of treatment-resistant obsessive-compulsive disorder when combined with SSRIs (Ferrucci et al., Reference Ferrucci, Giannicola, Rosa, Fumagalli, Boggio, Hallett and Priori2012).

Study findings were regarding negative emotional stimuli, but task design allowed for exploratory analyses of valence type. With a negative emotion emphasis of prior theoretical frameworks (Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010; DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008; Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004; Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003), as well as a study Cognitive Therapy protocol consisting primarily of cognitive restructuring of beliefs around negative information (instead of focusing on savoring, behavioral activation, or other techniques for increasing positive affect), our primary questions regarded neural reactivity to negative words. That said, the task's positive and neutral word trials provided the ability to examine valence specificity. Thus, we conducted exploratory parallel analyses for positive and neutral trials (Supplements 4 and 5, respectively) as well as a mixed-effects analysis including all trial types (Supplement 6). Parallel analyses for positive and neutral words showed that some effects may be unique to negative emotional stimuli (e.g. prefrontal change mechanism cluster), whereas other effects show some similarities between valence types (e.g. negative and neutral words for the nonresponse region). The absence of any change mechanism cluster for positive words may reflect the negative emotion focus of the treatment protocol or the complicated nature of assessing positive emotion processing in depression (e.g. positive stimuli being interpreted as negative or neutral for some depressed individuals (Horner et al., Reference Horner, Siegle, Schwartz, Price, Haggerty, Collier and Friedman2014)). The partial overlap between negative and neutral trials for nonresponse is consistent with literature showing neutral stimuli may actually be processed as negative by depressed individuals (Epstein et al., Reference Epstein, Pan, Kocsis, Yang, Butler, Chusid and Silbersweig2006). The more widespread nonresponse findings for neutral words could be due to neutral words having more interpretative variability in association with depression, in contrast to negative words, which are thought to be ‘negative’ for all people, thus perhaps processed more similarly. Valence-related general linear tests within a mixed-effects analysis illustrate other potential valence specific and general effects, e.g. some overlapping regions and some specific regions within the medial and lateral prefrontal cortex for positive v. negative words for related conjunction criteria (online Supplementary Fig. S6.2). These regions are not functionally distinct enough in the literature to allow easy functional interpretation, but they support the potential usefulness of using multiple types of stimuli in future work that aims to elucidate nonresponse. This perspective is bolstered by the substantial literature on positive emotions in depression (Carl, Soskin, Kerns, & Barlow, Reference Carl, Soskin, Kerns and Barlow2013; Keren et al., Reference Keren, O'Callaghan, Vidal-Ribas, Buzzell, Brotman, Leibenluft and Stringaris2018; Vanderlind, Millgram, Baskin-Sommers, Clark, & Joormann, Reference Vanderlind, Millgram, Baskin-Sommers, Clark and Joormann2020), including prior neuroimaging studies examining neural reactivity to rewarding/positive stimuli within the context of CBT for depression (Chuang et al., Reference Chuang, Whitaker, Murray, Elliott, Hagan, Graham and Suckling2016; Dichter et al., Reference Dichter, Felder, Petty, Bizzell, Ernst and Smoski2009; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Olino, Ryan, Birmaher, Axelson, Moyles and Dahl2010; Hanuka et al., Reference Hanuka, Olson, Admon, Webb, Killgore, Rauch and Pizzagalli2022; Ritchey, Dolcos, Eddington, Strauman, & Cabeza, Reference Ritchey, Dolcos, Eddington, Strauman and Cabeza2011; Straub et al., Reference Straub, Plener, Sproeber, Sprenger, Koelch, Groen and Abler2015). These studies show some overlapping regions with negative emotion research (e.g. prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortical regions), but they also show distinct regions related to reward processing in the brain, e.g. ventral striatum.

The current study has multiple methodological limitations. Results are contingent on a priori effect size thresholds for ‘response’ and ‘nonresponse,’ and it is unclear whether the thresholds selected are optimal. Thresholds were defined with the intent of representing a minimal effect that was still clinically-relevant (Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, Turner, Koole, van Dijke and Smit2014) and a practical absence of effect (~98% group overlap (Grice & Barrett, Reference Grice and Barrett2014)); however, there is a literature lacuna regarding appropriate effect size thresholds for neural changes following MDD treatment, making the decision of optimal thresholds particularly challenging. In addition, despite the study's relatively large sample for task-based neuroimaging and treatment studies, a larger replication sample would allow confidence in the replicability of findings, with differentiation of potentially responsive subtypes (Beijers, Wardenaar, van Loo, & Schoevers, Reference Beijers, Wardenaar, van Loo and Schoevers2019; Price et al., Reference Price, Lane, Gates, Kraynak, Horner, Thase and Siegle2017), and potentially better generalization to the larger depressed population. Neural reactivity was measured in response to negative words and prompts of self-relevance, which may not capture the complexities of real-world stressors of living with MDD; replication of the observed associations with other tasks may help to address issues of generalization.

Study limitations notwithstanding, findings provide additional support for and expand upon prior theory of neural mechanisms associated with CBT (Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010; DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008; Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004; Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003). The direct effect of CBT on cortical regulatory processes was largely supported. Study results added to the literature with the suggestion of different effects of CBT on functional subdivisions of the DLPFC (DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008). Increased DMN reactivity could highlight the role of CBT in changing the nature of self-relevant processing, e.g. allowing more functional task-related self-awareness. The vermis (and cerebellum more broadly) exhibited aberrant neural reactivity to negative emotional stimuli in MDD and did not respond to CBT, potentially suggesting that CBT's direct effects on neural reactivity are specific to cortical regulatory regions. Accordingly, aberrant cerebellar functioning may serve as a target for future depression intervention research aimed at addressing neurobiological vulnerabilities that persist in treated MDD.

The study finding of nonresponse of the vermis to CBT also speaks to a broader concerning message, that aberrant neural functioning persists in treated-MDD, even when symptoms improve, making a person with a history of MDD neurally vulnerable for a return to depressive experiences. Persistent vulnerability to depression is also supported by clinical long-term (9–14 years) follow-up data that we collected on a subset of this sample. When assessing cross-year, cross-severity depression symptom estimates in the years after CBT, every individual eventually experienced a return to symptoms. Moreover, the near-universal trajectory was persistent symptoms for several years with persistent quality of life deficits across multiple life domains, which is consistent with other depressed samples and similar follow-up methods (Judd et al., Reference Judd, Akiskal, Maser, Zeller, Endicott, Coryell and Keller1998; Kennedy et al., Reference Kennedy, Abbott and Paykel2004). The neuroimaging nonresponse finding from this study, within the context of bleak clinical outcomes, supports a conceptualization of MDD that is truly chronic, even with rigorously-implemented, gold-standard evidence-based treatment.

In consideration of persistent neural vulnerabilities in MDD, several avenues for future research and clinical applications follow. Further exploring nonresponse in the brain following CBT, with different tasks and/or neuroimaging measures, would be helpful to better understand neural vulnerabilities that remain. In addition, expanding this research to examine nonresponse following other treatment modalities, such as antidepressant medications, other forms of psychotherapy, or recent alternative interventions (e.g. ketamine), would be important. We anticipate that there may be at least some differences in nonresponse among modalities, given suggested differences in neural mechanisms of treatment (DeRubeis et al., Reference DeRubeis, Siegle and Hollon2008; Goldapple et al., Reference Goldapple, Segal, Garson, Lau, Bieling, Kennedy and Mayberg2004; Mayberg, Reference Mayberg2003). Moreover, with regular combined use of psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy being commonplace, examining neural nonresponse differences between combined and monotherapy approaches would also be beneficial. Clinically, better understanding of neural nonresponse following treatment provides opportunities to improve existing interventions, adjusting our treatment modalities to now address previously unaddressed vulnerabilities. There's the additional opportunity to combine different treatment modalities to treat remaining vulnerabilities. More immediate clinical recommendations involve treating MDD as the chronic condition (with persistent vulnerabilities) that it is, thus we advise continued assessment after completing treatment and receipt of mental health services upon symptom return. Moreover, it may be beneficial to engage in discussion with the patient about MDD's chronicity, to stress the importance of continued symptom monitoring and also to normalize the persistence of symptoms and persistence of maladaptive affective responses to depressive stressors.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723000727.

Financial support

Supported by the National Institute of Mental Health MH082998, MH074807, MH58356, MH69618, and the Pittsburgh Foundation, Emmerling Fund M2007-0114.

Conflict of interest

Author GJS receives royalty payments on a patent regarding a novel depression intervention licensed to Apollo Neurosciences, which is not relevant to this article, and consults for Johnson and Johnson on novel pharmacology unrelated to this project. The other authors report nothing to disclose.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Adamaszek, M., D'Agata, F., Ferrucci, R., Habas, C., Keulen, S., Kirkby, K. C., … Verhoeven, J. (2017). Consensus paper: Cerebellum and emotion. Cerebellum (London, England), 16(2), 552576. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-016-0815-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumann, O., & Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Functional topography of primary emotion processing in the human cerebellum. NeuroImage, 61(4), 805811. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(2), 8795. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T. (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(8), 969977. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beijers, L., Wardenaar, K. J., van Loo, H. M., & Schoevers, R. A. (2019). Data-driven biological subtypes of depression: Systematic review of biological approaches to depression subtyping. Molecular Psychiatry, 24(6), 888900. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0385-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bluhm, R. L., Clark, C. R., McFarlane, A. C., Moores, K. A., Shaw, M. E., & Lanius, R. A. (2011). Default network connectivity during a working memory task. Human Brain Mapping, 32(7), 10291035. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carl, J. R., Soskin, D. P., Kerns, C., & Barlow, D. H. (2013). Positive emotion regulation in emotional disorders: A theoretical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(3), 343360. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.01.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chuang, J.-Y., Whitaker, K. J., Murray, G. K., Elliott, R., Hagan, C. C., Graham, J. M., … Suckling, J. (2016). Aberrant brain responses to emotionally valent words is normalised after cognitive behavioural therapy in female depressed adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders, 189, 5461. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cieslik, E. C., Zilles, K., Caspers, S., Roski, C., Kellermann, T. S., Jakobs, O., … Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Is there ‘one’ DLPFC in cognitive action control? Evidence for heterogeneity from co-activation-based parcellation. Cerebral Cortex, 23(11), 26772689. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (2010). Cognitive theory and therapy of anxiety and depression: Convergence with neurobiological findings. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(9), 418424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cocchi, L., Zalesky, A., Fornito, A., & Mattingley, J. B. (2013). Dynamic cooperation and competition between brain systems during cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(10), 493501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colnaghi, S., Honeine, J.-L., Sozzi, S., & Schieppati, M. (2017). Body sway increases after functional inactivation of the cerebellar vermis by cTBS. The Cerebellum, 6(1), 114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-015-0758-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombetti, G. (2014). The feeling body: Affective science meets the enactive mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conradi, H. J., Ormel, J., & de Jonge, P. (2011). Presence of individual (residual) symptoms during depressive episodes and periods of remission: A 3-year prospective study. Psychological Medicine, 41(6), 11651174. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291710001911.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cremers, H. R., Wager, T. D., & Yarkoni, T. (2017). The relation between statistical power and inference in fMRI. PLoS One, 12(11), 120. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P., Turner, E. H., Koole, S. L., van Dijke, A., & Smit, F. (2014). What is the threshold for a clinically relevant effect? The case of major depressive disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 31(5), 374378. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cullen, K. R., Klimes-Dougan, B., Vu, D. P., Westlund Schreiner, M., Mueller, B. A., Eberly, L. E., … Lim, K. O. (2016). Neural correlates of antidepressant treatment response in adolescents with major depressive disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26(8), 705712. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2015.0232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dael, N., Mortillaro, M., & Scherer, K. R. (2012). Emotion expression in body action and posture. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 12(5), 1085. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
De Gelder, B. (2016). Emotions and the body. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delaveau, P., Jabourian, M., Lemogne, C., Guionnet, S., Bergouignan, L., & Fossati, P. (2011). Brain effects of antidepressants in major depression: A meta-analysis of emotional processing studies. Journal of Affective Disorders, 130(1-2), 6674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.09.032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeRubeis, R. J., Siegle, G. J., & Hollon, S. D. (2008). Cognitive therapy versus medication for depression: Treatment outcomes and neural mechanisms. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(10), 788796. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dichter, G. S., Felder, J. N., Petty, C., Bizzell, J., Ernst, M., & Smoski, M. J. (2009). The effects of psychotherapy on neural responses to rewards in major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 66(9), 886897. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Disner, S. G., Beevers, C. G., Haigh, E. A. P., & Beck, A. T. (2011). Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 12(8), 467477. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drevets, W. C., Savitz, J., & Trimble, M. (2008). The subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in mood disorders. CNS Spectrums, 13(8), 663681. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1092852900013754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, J., Pan, H., Kocsis, J. H., Yang, Y., Butler, T., Chusid, J., … Silbersweig, D. A. (2006). Lack of ventral striatal response to positive stimuli in depressed versus normal subjects. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(10), 17841790. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2006.163.10.1784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrucci, R., Giannicola, G., Rosa, M., Fumagalli, M., Boggio, P. S., Hallett, M., … Priori, A. (2012). Cerebellum and processing of negative facial emotions: Cerebellar transcranial DC stimulation specifically enhances the emotional recognition of facial anger and sadness. Cognition & Emotion, 26(5), 786799. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.619520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1996). Structured clinical interview for DSM IV Axis I disorders–patient edition. (Vol. 20). New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
Forbes, E. E., Olino, T. M., Ryan, N. D., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D., Moyles, D. L., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). Reward-related brain function as a predictor of treatment response in adolescents with major depressive disorder. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(1), 107118. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.10.1.107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, G., Carson, A. J., & Welch, K. A. (2016). Cognitive behavioural therapy for depression: Systematic review of imaging studies. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 28(2), 6174. https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2015.41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J. A., & Lanius, R. A. (2008). Neuroimaging studies of psychological interventions for mood and anxiety disorders: Empirical and methodological review. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(2), 228246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.05.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Price, C. J., Büchel, C., & Worsley, K. J. (1999). Multisubject fMRI studies and conjunction analyses. Neuroimage, 10(4), 385396. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J., Penny, W. D., & Glaser, D. E. (2005). Conjunction revisited. Neuroimage, 25(3), 661667. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frodl, T., Scheuerecker, J., Schoepf, V., Linn, J., Koutsouleris, N., Bokde, A. L. W., … Meisenzahl, E. (2011). Different effects of mirtazapine and venlafaxine on brain activation: An open randomized controlled fMRI study. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 72(4), 448457. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.09m05393blu.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fu, C. H., Williams, S. C., Cleare, A. J., Brammer, M. J., Walsh, N. D., Kim, J., & …Bullmore, E. T. (2004). Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: A prospective, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(9), 877889. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fu, C. H. Y., Williams, S. C. R., Brammer, M. J., Suckling, J., Kim, J., Cleare, A. J., … Bullmore, E. T. (2007). Neural responses to happy facial expressions in major depression following antidepressant treatment. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(4), 599607. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garg, S., Sinha, V. K., Tikka, S. K., Mishra, P., & Goyal, N. (2016). The efficacy of cerebellar vermal deep high frequency (theta range) repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in schizophrenia: A randomized rater blind-sham controlled study. Psychiatry Research, 243, 413420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.07.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, T., Martin, R., & Coulson, M. (2011). Attentional biases using the body in the crowd task: Are angry body postures detected more rapidly? Cognition & Emotion, 25(4), 700708. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.495881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldapple, K., Segal, Z., Garson, C., Lau, M., Bieling, P., Kennedy, S., & Mayberg, H. S. (2004). Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: Treatment-specific effects of cognitive behavior therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(1), 3441. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.1.34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, I. H., & Hammen, C. L. (2008). Handbook of depression (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Grice, J. W., & Barrett, P. T. (2014). A note on Cohen's overlapping proportions of normal distributions. Psychological Reports, 115(3), 741747. https://doi.org/10.2466/03.PR0.115c29z4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Habas, C., Kamdar, N., Nguyen, D., Prater, K., Beckmann, C. F., Menon, V., & Greicius, M. D. (2009). Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(26), 85868594. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1868-09.2009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, J. P., Farmer, M., Fogelman, P., & Gotlib, I. H. (2015). Depressive rumination, the default-mode network, and the dark matter of clinical neuroscience. Biological Psychiatry, 78(4), 224230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 23(1), 56. doi:10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanuka, S., Olson, E. A., Admon, R., Webb, C. A., Killgore, W. D. S., Rauch, S. L., … Pizzagalli, D. A. (2022). Reduced anhedonia following internet-based cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression is mediated by enhanced reward circuit activation. Psychological Medicine, 110. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722001106.Google ScholarPubMed
Hearne, L., Cocchi, L., Zalesky, A., & Mattingley, J. B. (2015). Interactions between default mode and control networks as a function of increasing cognitive reasoning complexity. Human Brain Mapping, 36(7), 27192731. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, R. G. (1977). Modulation of emotion with a brain pacemaker: Treatment for intractable psychiatric illness. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 165(5), 300317. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-197711000-00002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hindash, A. H. C., & Amir, N. (2012). Negative interpretation bias in individuals with depressive symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(5), 502511. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9397-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horner, M. S., Siegle, G. J., Schwartz, R. M., Price, R. B., Haggerty, A. E., Collier, A., & Friedman, E. S. (2014). C'Mon get happy: Reduced magnitude and duration of response during a positive-affect induction in depression. Depression and Anxiety, 31(11), 952960. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Judd, L. L., Akiskal, H. S., Maser, J. D., Zeller, P. J., Endicott, J., Coryell, W., … Keller, M. B. (1998). A prospective 12-year study of subsyndromal and syndromal depressive symptoms in unipolar major depressive disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(8), 694700. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.55.8.694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, N., Abbott, R., & Paykel, E. S. (2004). Longitudinal syndromal and sub-syndromal symptoms after severe depression: 10-year follow-up study. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 184, 330336. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.184.4.330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keren, H., O'Callaghan, G., Vidal-Ribas, P., Buzzell, G. A., Brotman, M. A., Leibenluft, E., … Stringaris, A. (2018). Reward processing in depression: A conceptual and meta-analytic review across fMRI and EEG Studies. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(11), 11111120. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17101124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konishi, M., McLaren, D. G., Engen, H., & Smallwood, J. (2015). Shaped by the past: The default mode network supports cognition that is independent of immediate perceptual input. PLoS One, 10(6), 118. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leddy, J. J., Cox, J. L., Baker, J. G., Wack, D. S., Pendergast, D. R., Zivadinov, R., & Willer, B. (2013). Exercise treatment for postconcussion syndrome: A pilot study of changes in functional magnetic resonance imaging activation, physiology, and symptoms. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 28(4), 241249. https://doi.org/10.1097/HTR.0b013e31826da964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marwood, L., Wise, T., Perkins, A. M., & Cleare, A. J. (2018). Meta-analyses of the neural mechanisms and predictors of response to psychotherapy in depression and anxiety. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 95, 6172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayberg, H. S. (2003). Positron emission tomography imaging in depression: A neural systems perspective. Neuroimaging Clinics of North America, 13(4), 805815. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1052-5149(03)00104-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayberg, H. S., Brannan, S. K., Tekell, J. L., Silva, J. A., Mahurin, R. K., McGinnis, S., & Jerabek, P. A. (2000). Regional metabolic effects of fluoxetine in major depression: Serial changes and relationship to clinical response. Biological Psychiatry, 48(8), 830843. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01036-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayer, J. S., Roebroeck, A., Maurer, K., & Linden, D. E. J. (2010). Specialization in the default mode: Task-induced brain deactivations dissociate between visual working memory and attention. Human Brain Mapping, 31(1), 126139. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClintock, S. M., Husain, M. M., Wisniewski, S. R., Nierenberg, A. A., Stewart, J. W., Trivedi, M. H., … Rush, A. J. (2011). Residual symptoms in depressed outpatients who respond by 50% but do not remit to antidepressant medication. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 31(2), 180. https://doi.org/10.1097/JCP.0b013e31820ebd2c.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 483506. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nichols, T., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T., & Poline, J.-B. (2005, April 15). Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. NeuroImage, 25(3), 653660. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nierenberg, A. A., Husain, M. M., Trivedi, M. H., Fava, M., Warden, D., Wisniewski, S. R., … Rush, A. J. (2010). Residual symptoms after remission of major depressive disorder with citalopram and risk of relapse: A STAR*D report. Psychological Medicine, 40(1), 4150. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291709006011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piccoli, T., Valente, G., Linden, D. E. J., Re, M., Esposito, F., Sack, A. T., & Di Salle, F. (2015). The default mode network and the working memory network are not anti-correlated during all phases of a working memory task. PLoS One, 10(4), 116. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0123354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierce, J. E., & Péron, J. (2020). The basal ganglia and the cerebellum in human emotion. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(5), 599613. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa076.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poldrack, R. A., Baker, C. I., Durnez, J., Gorgolewski, K. J., Matthews, P. M., Munafò, M. R., … Yarkoni, T. (2017). Scanning the horizon: Towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(2), 115126. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, R. B., Lane, S., Gates, K., Kraynak, T. E., Horner, M. S., Thase, M. E., & Siegle, G. J. (2017). Parsing heterogeneity in the brain connectivity of depressed and healthy adults during positive mood. Biological Psychiatry, 81(4), 347357. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.06.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raichle, M. E. (2015). The brain's default mode network. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 38, 433447. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchey, M., Dolcos, F., Eddington, K. M., Strauman, T. J., & Cabeza, R. (2011). Neural correlates of emotional processing in depression: Changes with cognitive behavioral therapy and predictors of treatment response. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(5), 577587. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.09.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romera, I., Pérez, V., Ciudad, A., Caballero, L., Roca, M., Polavieja, P., & Gilaberte, I. (2013). Residual symptoms and functioning in depression, does the type of residual symptom matter? A post-hoc analysis. BMC Psychiatry, 13(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, A. C., Bhat, J. V., Cardenas, V. A., Ehrlich, T. J., Horwege, A. M., Mathalon, D. H., … Yesavage, J. A. (2021). Targeting location relates to treatment response in active but not sham rTMS stimulation. Brain Stimulation, 14(3), 703709. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2021.04.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sacchetti, B., Scelfo, B., & Strata, P. (2009). Cerebellum and emotional behavior. Neuroscience, 162(3), 756762. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sankar, A., Melin, A., Lorenzetti, V., Horton, P., Costafreda, S. G., & Fu, C. H. Y. (2018). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the neural correlates of psychological therapies in major depression. Psychiatry Research Neuroimaging, 279, 3139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.07.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scalabrini, A., Vai, B., Poletti, S., Damiani, S., Mucci, C., Colombo, C., … Northoff, G. (2020). All roads lead to the default-mode network—global source of DMN abnormalities in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 45(12), 20582069. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0785-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmahmann, J. D. (2010). The role of the cerebellum in cognition and emotion: Personal reflections since 1982 on the dysmetria of thought hypothesis, and its historical evolution from theory to therapy. Neuropsychology Review, 20(3), 236260. https://doi.org/0.1007/s11065-010-9142-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegle, G. J., Carter, C. S., & Thase, M. E. (2006). Use of FMRI to predict recovery from unipolar depression with cognitive behavior therapy. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(4), 735738. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegle, G. J., Granholm, E., Ingram, R. E., & Matt, G. E. (2001). Pupillary and reaction time measures of sustained processing of negative information in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 49(7), 624636. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01024-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegle, G. J., Thompson, W., Carter, C. S., Steinhauer, S. R., & Thase, M. E. (2007). Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: Related and independent features. Biological Psychiatry, 61(2), 198209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegle, G. J., Thompson, W. K., Collier, A., Berman, S. R., Feldmiller, J., Thase, M. E., & Friedman, E. S. (2012). Toward clinically useful neuroimaging in depression treatment: Prognostic utility of subgenual cingulate activity for determining depression outcome in cognitive therapy across studies, scanners, and patient characteristics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(9), 913924. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokolov, A. A., Zeidman, P., Erb, M., Pollick, F. E., Fallgatter, A. J., Ryvlin, P., … Pavlova, M. A. (2020). Brain circuits signaling the absence of emotion in body language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117(34), 2086820873. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2007141117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Chamberlain, J. P., Gilmore, A. W., & Schacter, D. L. (2010). Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. NeuroImage, 53(1), 303317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sreenivas, S., Boehm, S. G., & Linden, D. E. J. (2012). Emotional faces and the default mode network. Neuroscience Letters, 506(2), 229234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.11.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoodley, C. J., & Schmahmann, J. D. (2010). Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. Cortex, 46(7), 831844. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strata, P. (2015). The emotional cerebellum. Cerebellum (London, England), 14(5), 570577. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-015-0649-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Straub, J., Plener, P. L., Sproeber, N., Sprenger, L., Koelch, M. G., Groen, G., & Abler, B. (2015). Neural correlates of successful psychotherapy of depression in adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders, 183, 239246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.05.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strege, M. V., Richey, J. A., & Siegle, G. J. (2022). What does ‘staying well’ after depression mean? Chronic low grade symptomatology after treatment for depression is common. Journal of Affective Disorders, 317, 228235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.08.075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, D. J., Walters, H. M., Vittengl, J. R., Krebaum, S., & Jarrett, R. B. (2010). Which depressive symptoms remain after response to cognitive therapy of depression and predict relapse and recurrence? Journal of Affective Disorders, 123(1-3), 181187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.08.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Aalst, J., Ceccarini, J., Schramm, G., Van Weehaeghe, D., Rezaei, A., Demyttenaere, K., … Van Laere, K. (2020). Long-term Ashtanga yoga practice decreases medial temporal and brainstem glucose metabolism in relation to years of experience. EJNMMI Research, 10(1), 50. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13550-020-00636-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Aalst, J., Jennen, L., Demyttenaere, K., Sunaert, S., Koole, M., Ceccarini, J., & Van Laere, K. (2021). Twelve-week yoga vs. aerobic cycling initiation in sedentary healthy subjects: A behavioral and multiparametric interventional PET/MR study. Frontiers in Psychiatry/Frontiers Research Foundation, 12, 739356. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.739356.Google ScholarPubMed
van der Kolk, B. A. (1994). The body keeps the score: Memory and the evolving psychobiology of posttraumatic stress. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 1(5), 253265. https://doi.org/10.3109/10673229409017088.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanderlind, W. M., Millgram, Y., Baskin-Sommers, A. R., Clark, M. S., & Joormann, J. (2020). Understanding positive emotion deficits in depression: From emotion preferences to emotion regulation. Clinical Psychology Review, 76, 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Villanueva, R. (2012). The cerebellum and neuropsychiatric disorders. Psychiatry Research, 198(3), 527532. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.02.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wassmann, C. (2010). Reflections on the ‘body loop’: Carl Georg Lange's theory of emotion. Cognition & Emotion, 24(6), 974990. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903052744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, L. M. (2016). Precision psychiatry: A neural circuit taxonomy for depression and anxiety. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 3(5), 472480. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00579-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Won, J., Faroqi-Shah, Y., Callow, D. D., Williams, A., Awoyemi, A., Nielson, K. A., … Smith, J. C. (2021). Association between greater cerebellar network connectivity and improved phonemic fluency performance after exercise training in older adults. The Cerebellum, 20, 114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-020-01218-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, R. P., Mazziotta, J. C., & Cherry, S. R. (1993). MRI-PET registration with automated algorithm. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography, 17(4), 536546. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004728-199307000-00004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabelina, D. L., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Dynamic network interactions supporting internally-oriented cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 40, 8693. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zheng, J., Anderson, K. L., Leal, S. L., Shestyuk, A., Gulsen, G., Mnatsakanyan, L., … Lin, J. J. (2017). Amygdala-hippocampal dynamics during salient information processing. Nature Communications, 8(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, H.-X., Chen, X., Shen, Y.-Q., Li, L., Chen, N.-X., Zhu, Z.-C., … Yan, C.-G. (2020). Rumination and the default mode network: Meta-analysis of brain imaging studies and implications for depression. NeuroImage, 206, 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Defining change mechanism and nonresponse

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Treatment nonresponse cluster.Note. (a) Pre-Ctrl ≠ Pre-Dep represents brain areas where control participants differed (d > 0.24) from depressed patients prior to treatment. (b) Pre-Dep = Post-Dep represents brain areas where a practical absence of effect (d < 0.05) was observed when comparing depressed patients prior to and after treatment. (c) Nonresponse represents areas that met both A and B effect size criteria and survived cluster thresholding [>297 voxels (AFNI's NN3, 2-sided)].Coloring represents whole-brain regression R2 Δ values on neural reactivity to negative words at the pre-treatment scan as predicted by residual BDI-II scores above and beyond scanner (i.e. extent to which the region acts as a predictor of treatment response); the overall low values suggest non-response regions are not associated with either clinical change or change in activity in association with treatment.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Box and scatter plots for individual averages of nonresponse and response clusters.Note. (a) Individual mean reactivity averages of the nonresponse cluster for control participants (Pre-Ctrl) and MDD patients prior to and after CBT (Pre-Dep, Post-Dep); (b) Individual mean reactivity averages of the change mechanism regions (all clusters) for pre and post assessments for both control participants (Pre-Ctrl, Post-Ctrl) and MDD patients (Pre-Dep, Post-Dep). Gray lines represent mean, and black lines represent median.

Figure 3

Table 2. Coordinates for clusters of non-response and change mechanisms

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Treatment response (change mechanism) clusters.Note. (a) Pre-Ctrl ≠ Pre-Dep represents brain areas where control participants differed (d > 0.24) from depressed patients prior to treatment. (b) Pre-Dep ≠ Post-Dep represents brain areas where depressed patients prior to and after treatment differed (d > 0.24). (c) Pre-Post Ctrl ≠ Pre-Post Dep represents brain areas where pre-post estimates differed (d > 0.24) between control participants and depressed patients. (d) Change Mechanism represents brain areas that met all prior criteria (a, b, c) and survived cluster thresholding (>93 voxels [AFNI's NN3, 2-sided]). Coloring represents d estimates (contrast of post-treatment depressed patients and never-depressed control participants), showing some instances in which effect sizes suggest normalization occurs

Figure 5

Fig. 4. Clusters within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.Note. The yellow cluster represents a subregion within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that met criteria for a change mechanism [pre-depressed ≠ pre-control * pre-depressed ≠ post-depressed * pre-post depressed ≠ pre-post control (≠ corresponds to d > 0.24)] and survived cluster correction (>19 voxels). The red cluster represents a subregion within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that met criteria for nonresponse [pre-depressed ≠ pre-control * pre-depressed = post-depressed (≠ corresponds to d > 0.24 and = corresponds to d < 0.05)] and met cluster correction (>43 voxels).

Supplementary material: File

Strege et al. supplementary material

Strege et al. supplementary material
Download Strege et al. supplementary material(File)
File 11.6 MB