Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T13:40:42.113Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Factors Associated with the Development of High Nutrition Risk: Data from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2023

Christine Marie Mills*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health Sciences, School of Rehabilitation Therapy, Aging and Health Program, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
Heather H. Keller
Affiliation:
Schlegel-UW Research Institute for Aging and Department of Kinesiology & Health Sciences, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada
Vincent Gerard DePaul
Affiliation:
School of Rehabilitation Therapy and Health Services and Policy Research Institute, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
Catherine Donnelly
Affiliation:
School of Rehabilitation Therapy and Health Services and Policy Research Institute, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
*
Corresponding author: La correspondance et les demandes de tirés-à-part doivent être adressées à : / Correspondence and requests for offprints should be sent to: Christine Marie Mills, 200 University Avenue West, Waterloo, ON, Canada N2L 3G1 ([email protected]).
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study aimed to determine which social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables were able to predict the development of high nutrition risk in Canadian adults at midlife and beyond, using data from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Multivariable binomial logistic regression was used to examine the predictors of the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up, 3 years after baseline. At baseline, 35.0 per cent of participants were at high nutrition risk and 42.2 per cent were at high risk at follow-up. Lower levels of social support, lower social participation, depression, and poor self-rated healthy aging were associated with the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up. Individuals showing these factors should be screened proactively for nutrition risk.

Résumé

Résumé

Cette étude basée sur des données de l’Étude longitudinale canadienne sur le vieillissement visait à déterminer quelles variables liées au réseau social, aux données démographiques et aux indicateurs de santé pouvaient permettre de prédire le développement d’un risque nutritionnel élevé chez les adultes canadiens d’âge mûr et plus âgés. Une régression logistique binomiale multivariée a été utilisée pour examiner les facteurs prédictifs du développement d’un risque nutritionnel élevé lors du suivi, trois ans après le début de l’étude. Au départ, 35,0 % des participants présentaient un risque nutritionnel élevé contre 42,2 % lors du suivi. Des niveaux inférieurs de soutien social, une participation sociale plus faible, la dépression et un niveau médiocre de vieillissement en bonne santé auto-évalué ont été associés au développement d’un risque nutritionnel élevé lors du suivi. Les personnes présentant ces facteurs devraient faire l’objet d’un dépistage proactif de risque nutritionnel.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is included and the original work is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.
Copyright
© Canadian Association on Gerontology 2023

Introduction

Nutrition throughout the lifespan has an influence on health and well-being (Herman et al., Reference Herman, Baer, Adams, Cunningham-Sabo, Duran, Johnson and Yakes2014). Diets that provide the appropriate amount of energy and essential nutrients are important for the maintenance of good health and well-being (Herman et al., Reference Herman, Baer, Adams, Cunningham-Sabo, Duran, Johnson and Yakes2014). Nutrition at midlife and beyond, in particular, can help prevent the development of many chronic diseases or can help in their management (Lambrinoudaki et al., Reference Lambrinoudaki, Ceasu, Depypere, Erel, Rees and Schenck-Gustafsson2013). However, as people enter midlife (ages 45 to 65) and older adulthood (ages 65 and older), physiological, psychological, and social changes can lead to changes in dietary habits and food intake (de Boer, Ter Horst, & Lorist, Reference de Boer, Ter Horst and Lorist2013; Elsner, Reference Elsner2002; Herman et al., Reference Herman, Baer, Adams, Cunningham-Sabo, Duran, Johnson and Yakes2014). When these changes lead to inadequate intake, nutrition risk and the risk of poor nutritional status can develop (Ramage-Morin & Garriguet, Reference Ramage-Morin and Garriguet2013). Nutrition risk lies on a continuum between good nutritional health and malnutrition (Ramage-Morin & Garriguet, Reference Ramage-Morin and Garriguet2013). Depending on how nutrition risk is measured, individuals can be at no/low, moderate, or high nutrition risk (Craven, Pelly, Lovell, & Isenring, Reference Craven, Pelly, Lovell and Isenring2018; de Groot, Beck, Schroll, & van Staveren, Reference de Groot, Beck, Schroll and van Staveren1998; Keller, Goy, & Kane, Reference Keller, Goy and Kane2005; Roberts, Wolfson, & Payette, Reference Roberts, Wolfson and Payette2007). Moderate to high nutrition risk can lead to several negative outcomes, including decreased quality of life, frailty, hospitalization, institutionalization, and mortality (Keller & Østbye, Reference Keller and Østbye2003; Keller, Østbye, & Goy, Reference Keller, Østbye and Goy2004; Payette, Coulombe, Boutier, & Gray-Donald, Reference Payette, Coulombe, Boutier and Gray-Donald2000; Ramage-Morin, Gilmour, & Rotermann, Reference Ramage-Morin, Gilmour and Rotermann2017). High nutrition risk is prevalent in community-dwelling Canadians, with approximately one-third of adults ages 50 and older at high nutrition risk (Morrison, Laur, & Keller, Reference Morrison, Laur and Keller2019; Ramage-Morin & Garriguet, Reference Ramage-Morin and Garriguet2013).

A variety of factors have been associated with high nutrition risk in cross-sectional studies of community-dwelling older adults; however, relatively few studies have examined nutrition risk longitudinally or examined nutrition risk in adults at midlife. A Canadian longitudinal study by Roberts et al. (Reference Roberts, Wolfson and Payette2007), that lasted 1 year, examined nutrition risk in 839 older adults residing in Montreal, Quebec, Canada. The authors found that for individuals who were at low nutrition risk at baseline, only poor self-rated health was a predictor of elevated risk 12 months later (Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Wolfson and Payette2007). In this study, Roberts et al. (Reference Roberts, Wolfson and Payette2007) used the Elderly Nutrition Screening tool to determine nutrition risk status and logistic regression to determine the predictors of elevated nutrition risk. Another longitudinal Canadian study, by Keller (Reference Keller2006), that lasted 18 months examined the relationship between meal programs (such as Meals on Wheels and other meal programs that include a social component, such as congregate dining or supportive housing with dining) and nutrition risk scores in older adults living in southwestern Ontario, Canada. She found that meal programs improved nutrition risk scores between baseline and follow-up 18 months later, and that depression at baseline was associated with increased nutrition risk 18 months later (Keller, Reference Keller2006). Keller (Reference Keller2006) used Seniors in the Community: Risk Evaluation for Eating and Nutrition to determine nutrition risk and self-reported frequency of depression to determine depression status. In a longitudinal study looking at nutrition risk in men only, five trajectories of nutrition risk were identified over the course of 4 years (Lengyel, Jiang, & Tate, Reference Lengyel, Jiang and Tate2017). These trajectory groups differed on mental health, physical aging, self-perceived successful aging, and living alone (Lengyel et al., Reference Lengyel, Jiang and Tate2017).

Outside of Canada there have also been longitudinal studies examining nutrition risk. A study in the United Kingdom found an association between baseline nutrition risk scores and lower grip strength at follow-up (Bloom et al., Reference Bloom, Pilgrim, Jameson, Dennison, Sayer and Roberts2021). In a study from Taiwan, being at malnutrition risk at baseline was longitudinally associated with an increased risk of depression at two follow-up points, 4 and 6 years after baseline (Tsai, Reference Tsai2013). A study from New Zealand by Wham, Curnow, and Towers (Reference Wham, Curnow and Towers2022) looked at nutrition risk over the course of 4 years. They found that, at baseline, those at high nutrition risk had poorer mental and physical health than those not at high risk, and those at high risk also had higher depression scores (Wham et al., Reference Wham, Curnow and Towers2022). Those at high nutrition risk also had lower social connection scores than those not at high risk (Wham et al., Reference Wham, Curnow and Towers2022). Four years later, those who were at high risk improved their mental health scores, whereas their physical health scores deteriorated (Wham et al., Reference Wham, Curnow and Towers2022). Depression scores increased for those not at high risk and remained stable for those who were at high risk (Wham et al., Reference Wham, Curnow and Towers2022).

Theoretical Framework

This study uses social network theory, described by Berkman, Glass, Brissette, and Seeman (Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000), as its theoretical framework. Using a theoretical framework helps provide structure and support for research (Osanloo & Grant, Reference Osanloo and Grant2016) and helps guide research questions (Herek, Reference Herek, Pequegnat, Stover and Boyce2011); however, much of the nutrition risk literature does not use a framework. Social network theory describes how social networks influence health outcomes (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000) and suggests that social and cultural contexts (the macro level) influence social networks (the mezzo level), which in turn affect psychosocial mechanisms that comprise social and interpersonal behaviours (the micro level) (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000). These psychosocial mechanisms then influence health outcomes (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000).

In Berkman et al.’s (Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000) social network theory, social networks have characteristics that include their size and range. The ties within these networks include frequency of contact with network members and frequency of participation in community activities (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000). Social networks then influence behaviour through psychosocial mechanisms that include the provision of social support, social engagement and attachment, and access to resources and material goods (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000).

Social networks may also change as people enter midlife and older adulthood (Infurna, Gerstorf, & Lachman, Reference Infurna, Gerstorf and Lachman2020; Luo & Li, Reference Luo and Li2022). At midlife, individuals may be caring for aging parents, having their relationships with their children change, and may become grandparents (Infurna et al., Reference Infurna, Gerstorf and Lachman2020). These changes may affect their dietary intake (Booth et al., Reference Booth, Sallis, Ritenbaugh, Hill, Birch and Frank2009; Chen & Antonelli, Reference Chen and Antonelli2020). As people continue to age into older adulthood, social networks continue to change (Ayalon & Levkovich, Reference Ayalon and Levkovich2019), frequently becoming smaller (Cornwell, Laumann, & Schumm, Reference Cornwell, Laumann and Schumm2008; English & Carstensen, Reference English and Carstensen2014) while connections within those networks may become closer or more intimate (English & Carstensen, Reference English and Carstensen2014). These changes may lead to social isolation (Luo & Li, Reference Luo and Li2022), which may lead to a decrease in food intake (Vesnaver & Keller, Reference Vesnaver and Keller2011).

A variety of health outcomes, both physical and psychological, have been associated with social networks in the literature (Berkman, Reference Berkman2000; Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000; Smith & Christakis, Reference Smith and Christakis2008; Tsai & Papachristos, Reference Tsai and Papachristos2015); therefore, it is hypothesized that social networks may also be associated with the development of high nutrition risk. Many of the social network characteristics and psychosocial mechanisms from social network theory (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000) have been associated with nutrition risk and food intake in cross-sectional studies of adults ages 65 and older (Vesnaver & Keller, Reference Vesnaver and Keller2011). Several studies have shown that eating with others improves dietary intake and reduces nutrition risk in older adults (Vesnaver & Keller, Reference Vesnaver and Keller2011), whereas eating alone is associated with high nutrition risk (Bloom et al., Reference Bloom, Lawrence, Barker, Baird, Dennison and Sayer2017; Keller & McKenzie, Reference Keller and McKenzie2003). Social relationships may also improve nutritional risk by encouraging compliance with dietary norms (Vesnaver & Keller, Reference Vesnaver and Keller2011). Eating with others may provide “social cues for when and what to eat” (Vesnaver & Keller, Reference Vesnaver and Keller2011, p. 15). An individual’s social support system may also encourage healthy behaviours, such as consuming adequate amounts of nutrient-rich foods (Locher & Sharkey, Reference Locher, Sharkey, Bales and Ritchie2009). Studies have also found that social support helps reduce nutrition risk (Keller, Reference Keller2005; Vesnaver & Keller, Reference Vesnaver and Keller2011), whereas low levels of social support are associated with increased nutrition risk (Locher & Sharkey, Reference Locher, Sharkey, Bales and Ritchie2009). Individuals with higher levels of social support may have greater assistance with food-related activities, such as meal preparation and grocery shopping (Vesnaver & Keller, Reference Vesnaver and Keller2011).

While the current literature has identified several factors that contribute to changes in nutrition risk in adults over time, what has not been done is to comprehensively explore the social network factors associated with high nutrition risk or the development of high nutrition risk. This study focuses on the mezzo and micro levels of Berkman et al.’s (Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000) social network theory, as social network factors at these levels may be direct upstream determinants of nutrition risk. This study also seeks to add to the literature on longitudinal nutrition risk by examining social network factors associated with the development of high nutrition risk, using data from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). By identifying factors that lead to the development of high nutrition risk, this study hopes to provide guidance for identifying people who should be proactively screened for nutrition risk and informing programs and policies designed to prevent high nutrition risk.

The goal is to determine which social network characteristics and psychosocial mechanisms, collectively referred to as social network factors, predict the development of high nutrition risk in individuals who were not originally at high risk.

Methods

Data Source

This study uses data from baseline and first follow-up of the CLSA. The CLSA is a large, longitudinal study and participants are followed every 3 years for 20 years or until participants’ death (Kirkland et al., Reference Kirkland, Griffith, Menec, Wister, Payette and Wolfson2015). CLSA participants were between the ages of 45 and 85 when recruited between 2010 and 2015, and baseline data were gathered at this time (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009). First follow-up data, referred to as follow-up, were gathered 3 years later (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009).

The detailed CLSA study design is available elsewhere (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009). There are 21,241 tracking participants who are followed by telephone interview only, and there are 30,097 comprehensive participants who are interviewed in person, undergo physical assessments, and provide urine and blood samples (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009). The CLSA randomly selected participants in the tracking cohort within sex and age strata in each Canadian province. The proportion of individuals in the tracking cohort from each province is proportional to the province’s population to allow CLSA data to be generalized to a given province’s population and the Canadian population (Kirkland et al., Reference Kirkland, Griffith, Menec, Wister, Payette and Wolfson2015; Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009). For this reason, this study uses data from the tracking cohort.

The selection and recruitment process for the CLSA is described elsewhere (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009). Briefly, the CLSA used three sampling frames for the tracking cohort: a subset of participants from Statistics Canada’s Canadian Community Health Survey – Healthy Aging (CCHS-HA), registries from provincial health care systems, and random digit dialling (RDD) of landline telephones (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). There was an attempt to over-sample certain regions identified from census data to ensure representation of individuals with lower socio-economic status and less education, since they are often underrepresented in population-based studies (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). Residents of the Canadian territories and some remote regions, individuals living on First Nation reserves and settlements, regular force members of the Canadian Armed Forces, and individuals living in institutions, including long-term care homes, are not included in the CLSA (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). Participants were required to understand English or French and be physically and cognitively able to answer study questions by themselves (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019).

The core set of questionnaires in the CLSA is common across both cohorts. Questionnaires ask about demographics, social and psychological measures, health and functional status, lifestyle, and behaviour (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). To use a theoretically informed model, CLSA measures were mapped onto the social network factors in Berkman et al.’s (Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000) social network theory (Table 1), as several of the social measures gathered by the CLSA had a clear and obvious fit with social network theory. These social network variables were the predictor variables in all analyses, with high nutrition risk as the outcome variable and demographic and health-indicator variables as potential covariates.

Table 1. Mapping CLSA variables onto social network theory

The complete list of CLSA measures has been reported elsewhere (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009). Data from baseline were used for the social network, demographic, health-indicator variables, and data from both baseline and follow-up for nutrition risk.

Social network variables

Social network size. CLSA participants indicated the number of people in each of these groups: children (biological, adopted, step), siblings, close friends, relatives, and neighbours. Participants also reported the number of people known through work or school, through community involvement, and through other activities.

Frequency of contact with network members. CLSA participants reported when they last got together with members of each of the following groups: children, siblings, close friends, relatives, and neighbours. Responses included: live with me, more than one year ago, within the past year, within the past six months, within the past month, within the last week or two, and within the last day or two. Like previous studies (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Kobayashi, Tong, Davison, Arora and Fuller-Thomson2020; Mills, Keller, DePaul, & Donnelly, Reference Mills, Keller, DePaul and Donnelly2023), these were collapsed into two categories: “low contact” for more than one year ago, within the past year, and within the past six months; and “high contact” for within the last month, within the last week or two, within the last day or two, and live with me.

Social participation. CLSA participants reported how often they participated in eight different types of activities over the past 12 months. These were: family/friend activities, religious activities, sports or physical activities with others, education or cultural activities, clubs or fraternal organizations, association activities, volunteer or charity work, and other recreational activities. Participants could respond, never, at least once a year, at least once a month, at least once a week, and at least once a day. Like Harasemiw et al. (Reference Harasemiw, Newall, Shooshtari, Mackenzie and Menec2018), the responses for each of these categories were summed to create a social participation variable that could range from 0 to 32, with higher numbers indicating increased frequency of participation.

Social support. Social support was measured using the 19-item MOS Social Support Survey. It measures multiple components of social support, including affection, emotional and informational support, tangible social support, and positive social interaction. The MOS has excellent internal consistency (overall and subscale Cronbach alphas ranging from 0.91 to 0.97) and test–retest reliability (ICC = 0.78 after 1 year) (Sherbourne & Stewart, Reference Sherbourne and Stewart1991).

Self-rated social standing. Participants were asked to think of a ladder with 10 steps as representing where people stand in their communities. At the top of the ladder (or Step 10) are the people who have the highest standing in their community. At the bottom (or Step 1) are the people who have the lowest standing in their community. Participants were asked, “On which step would you place yourself on this ladder?”

Household income. Participants were asked about their household income from all sources, using the following categories: “less than $20,000,” “$20,000–$49,999,” “$50,000–$99,999,” and “$100,000 or more.”

Demographic variables

Demographic measures in the CLSA include age, sex assigned at birth, living situation (alone or with others), marital status, education, and income. For marital status, participants could use one of the following options: single (never married), married/common-law, widowed, divorced, and separated. These were collapsed into three categories: “married/common law,” “single (including single, divorced, or separated),” and “widowed.” Participants were asked about their highest level of education. This study used the level of education (four levels) variable: “less than secondary school graduation,” “secondary school graduation (but no post-secondary),” “some post-secondary,” and “post-secondary degree/diploma.” Participants were asked about the number of people living in their household, other than the participant. Individuals were categorized as living alone if they indicated no other person resided in their household.

Health-indicator variables

Self-rated general health, mental health, healthy aging, and oral health. These were measured by asking participants: Would you say your health/mental health/healthy aging/oral health is excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor? Like previous studies (Mills et al., Reference Mills, Keller, DePaul and Donnelly2023; Morrison et al., Reference Morrison, Laur and Keller2019; Wister, Cosco, Mitchell, Menec, & Fyffe, Reference Wister, Cosco, Mitchell, Menec and Fyffe2019), these were collapsed into three categories: “fair/poor,” “good,” and “very good/excellent.”

Depression. Depression was measured using the short form of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression (CES-D10) Scale. The CES-D10 has been used in many large studies and has shown good reliability and validity in adult and older adult populations (Andresen, Malmgren, Carter, & Patrick, Reference Andresen, Malmgren, Carter and Patrick1994). The CES-D10 has a positive correlation with poor health status and a negative correlation with positive affectivity (Andresen et al., Reference Andresen, Malmgren, Carter and Patrick1994).

Disability. Basic activities of daily living (ADL) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) were measured using modifications of the questions of the Older Americans Resources and Services (OARS) Multidimensional Assessment Questionnaire. The OARS scale is a valid and reliable tool for use in older adult populations (Fillenbaum, Reference Fillenbaum1985, Reference Fillenbaum1988). Participants were classified into one of five categories, ranging from no functional impairment to total impairment. Like the other health-indicator variables, these were collapsed into three categories: “total/severe impairment,” “moderate impairment,” and “mild/no impairment.”

Nutrition risk

Nutrition risk was measured using the abbreviated version of Seniors in the Community: Risk Evaluation for Eating and Nutrition II (rebranded as SCREEN-8) (Keller, Reference Kellern.d.). Eight questions ask about typical daily eating habits and include questions on weight change, meal skipping, appetite, swallowing, servings of fruit and vegetables, fluid intake, eating with others, and meal preparation (Keller, Reference Kellern.d.). Scores range from 0 to 48 (Keller et al., Reference Keller, Goy and Kane2005). When compared to registered dietitians’ assessment of nutritional risk, SCREEN-8 has good specificity and sensitivity with an AUC ≥ 78 per cent. The test–retest reliability of SCREEN-8 with adults ages 50 and older is good, with ICC = 0.84, as is the inter-rater reliability, with ICC = 0.79. A SCREEN-8 score less than 38 indicates that an individual is at high nutrition risk (Keller et al., Reference Keller, Goy and Kane2005). The development of high nutrition risk (SCREEN-8 score < 38) at follow-up was the outcome of interest.

Data Analysis

The primary analytic strategy was hierarchical multivariable binomial regression, as the outcome variable for all analyses was the presence or absence of high nutrition risk at follow-up, thus the outcome variable had two levels (Osborne, Reference Osborne2015). For all analyses, weighting was applied to the data, as recommended by the CLSA (Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging, 2011). Due to the large sample size, the significance level was set at 𝛼 ≤ 0.001, and confidence intervals (CI), odds ratios (OR), and effect sizes were reported (Khalilzadeh & Asli, Reference Khalilzadeh and Asli2017). Data were analysed using IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 28 (IBM Corp, 2022) and RStudio Version 2022.02.3 (RStudio Team, 2022).

First, univariate analyses were run in SPSS to obtain descriptive statistics for each variable used in the regression analyses. Individuals in the sample were then classified into two groups: those who were at high nutrition risk and those who were not at high nutrition risk at baseline. For the social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables, comparisons were made between those at high risk at baseline and those not at high risk. Independent sample t-tests were used for continuous variables and χ2 tests for categorical variables. Effect sizes were calculated, using Cohen’s d for continuous variables and Cramer’s V for categorical variables (Durlak, Reference Durlak2009). The proportion of missing data for all variables and the patterns of missingness were examined in RStudio using the package naniar (Tierney & Cook, Reference Tierney and Cook2023). This package and Little’s (Reference Little1988) test were also used to determine whether cases were missing completely at random.

For the logistic regression analyses, the assumption of linearity of the continuous variables with respect to the logit of the dependent variables was tested using the Box-Tidwell procedure (Box & Tidwell, Reference Box and Tidwell1962). Two multivariable binomial logistic regression analyses were run for all participants; the first had the presence or absence of high nutrition risk at baseline as the outcome variable, and the second had the presence or absence of high nutrition risk at follow-up as the outcome variable. The social network variables were the predictor variables, and potential covariates included the demographic and health-indicator variables.

Development of High Nutrition Risk at Follow-up In Those Not at High Risk at Baseline

Using data from participants who were not at high nutrition risk at baseline, bivariate odds ratios between the presence of high nutrition risk at follow-up and social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables at baseline were calculated. Hierarchical binomial logistic regression was conducted, to first examine which social network variables were associated with the development of high nutrition risk, then to examine which social network variables were associated with the development of high nutrition risk when demographic variables were added as potential covariates, and, finally, to examine which social network variables were associated with the development of high nutrition risk when both demographic and health-indicator variables were added as potential covariates. Therefore, three binomial logistic regression analyses were run with the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up as the outcome variable. The first analysis had the social network variables at baseline as the predictors, the second analysis added demographic variables as potential covariates to the social network variables, and the third analysis added health-indicator variables as potential covariates to the social network and demographic variables. The sensitivity, specificity, and accuracy for each of the analyses were calculated. The McFadden pseudo-R 22) was used to calculate whether the analyses were a good fit, as McFadden states that a ρ2 between .4 and .6 indicates an excellent fit (McFadden, Reference McFadden, Hensher and Stopher R.1979). Odds ratios were reported for all the predictor variables and covariates.

Sensitivity Analysis

Multiple imputation of missing data (Rubin, Reference Rubin2004) was performed using RStudio and the package mice (van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011) to assess the potential for bias due to missing values (Supplemental Table 4). The mice package uses fully conditional specification (FCS) to conduct multiple imputation. This method is also known as multivariate imputation by chained equations (MICE) (van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011). MICE is practical and flexible and uses a series of regression models for each variable with missing data (Azur, Stuart, Frangakis, & Leaf, Reference Azur, Stuart, Frangakis and Leaf2011; White, Royston, & Wood, Reference White, Royston and Wood2011). This method can handle a variety of different variable types, including continuous and categorical variables (Azur et al., Reference Azur, Stuart, Frangakis and Leaf2011; van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011; White et al., Reference White, Royston and Wood2011). All variables included in the regression analyses were included in the imputation model, as recommended (White et al., Reference White, Royston and Wood2011; Wulff & Ejlskov, Reference Wulff and Ejlskov2017). Twenty imputations were used as recommended in the literature (Sterne et al., Reference Sterne, White, Carlin, Spratt, Royston and Kenward2009; White et al., Reference White, Royston and Wood2011). After multiple imputation, the third analysis (social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables as predictors) was re-run. The package mice (van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011) was used to obtain the pooled regression results and odds ratios.

Results

Sample Description

Of the 21,241 participants in the baseline tracking sample, 17,051 provided data at follow-up, and that sample is used here. There were confirmed deaths for 1,165 participants (5.5%). For sex assigned at birth, there were approximately equal percentages of males and females (47.8% males). The mean age of the participants was 59.46 (SD = 9.94). Most participants were married or partnered (76.5%). At baseline, 36.5 per cent of all participants were at nutrition risk and 42.2 per cent were at risk at follow-up. A paired sample t-test found that there was a difference between SCREEN-8 scores at baseline and at follow-up between individuals who were not at high nutrition risk and those who were at high nutrition risk at baseline, and the effect sizes were very large (Table 2). Among those who were not at high nutrition risk at baseline, 27.4 per cent developed high nutrition risk at follow-up. Further details on the social network, demographic, and health-related variables are shown in Table 2. Comparing those who were not at high nutrition risk at baseline to those who were at high risk, individuals who were not at high risk had higher levels of social participation, higher self-rated social standing, greater social support, and better self-rated health and healthy aging, although the effect sizes were small (Cohen, Reference Cohen1992; McHugh, Reference McHugh and Book2018).

Table 2. Population characteristics, overall and by nutrition risk

Notes:

a Population estimates calculated using trimmed inflation weights.

b Raw data, not weighted.

c Effect size for the difference between those at high nutrition risk at baseline and those not at high risk – Cohen’s d for continuous variables and Cramer’s V for categorical variables.

* Small effect size.

** Very large effect size.

The proportion of missing data was examined. For nutrition risk, 13.1 per cent of the sample was missing SCREEN-8 scores at baseline and 19.7 per cent at follow-up (Supplemental Table 1). Out of 17,051 participants for whom there were data, 9,437 (44.4%) participants had complete data on all the variables of interest. Little’s Missing Completely at Random test (Little, Reference Little1988) was not statistically significant (χ2 = 29703.91, p = .394), therefore the complete case analysis is reported here.

Logistic Regression Analyses

There was no multicollinearity among the variables used in the analyses for all the analyses conducted, as all variables had a variance inflation factor value below 4 (Harris, Reference Harris2021). All continuous independent variables were linearly related to the logit of the dependent variables (Osborne, Reference Osborne2015). There were no outliers (no cases with standardized residuals above three standard deviations) (Osborne, Reference Osborne2015).

High Nutrition Risk at Baseline

At baseline, among all participants, 36.5 per cent were at high nutrition risk. The results of the analysis examining the variables at baseline that examined the presence or absence of high nutrition risk at baseline can be found in the supplemental materials (Supplemental Table 3). Among the social network variables, lower levels of social participation, lower self-rated social standing, and lower levels of social support at baseline were associated with increased odds of being at high nutrition risk at baseline.

High Nutrition Risk at Follow-Up

At follow-up, among all participants, 42.2 per cent were at high nutrition risk. The results of the analysis examining the variables at baseline that examined the presence or absence of high nutrition risk at follow-up can be found in the supplemental materials (Supplemental Table 4). Among the social network variables, lower levels of social participation, lower self-rated social standing, and lower levels of social support at baseline were associated with increased odds of being at high nutrition risk at follow-up.

Bivariate Analysis

Among those who were not at high nutrition risk at baseline, 27.4 per cent developed high nutrition risk at follow-up (see Table 2). In the bivariate analyses examining the relationship between the predictor variables at baseline and the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up in those who were not at high nutrition risk at baseline, the SCREEN-8 score at baseline was associated with the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up (Supplemental Table 5). All the social network variables were associated with the development of high nutrition risk, except for the number of children and siblings, and all the frequency of contact variables. Among the demographic variables, all were associated with the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up, except for age and sex assigned at birth. All the health-indicator variables were associated with the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up.

Development of High Nutrition Risk at Follow-Up in Those Not at High Risk at Baseline

The first analysis, which used the social network variables to examine the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up in those not at high risk at baseline, was statistically significant (χ2 = 5294.7, p < .0001). The McFadden pseudo-R 2 was .406, indicating an excellent fit (McFadden, Reference McFadden, Hensher and Stopher R.1979). The accuracy was 77.4 per cent, the sensitivity was 40.9 per cent, and the specificity was 77.5 per cent. Higher levels of social participation, social support, and household income were associated with lower odds of developing high nutrition risk (Table 3).

Table 3. Logistic regression analyses examining the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up in those not at risk at baseline

Notes:

* p ≤ 0.001.

** p ≤ 0.0001.

The second analysis added demographic variables as potential covariates to the social network variables. This analysis examining the development of high nutrition risk was statistically significant (χ2 = 5344.0, p < .0001). The McFadden pseudo-R 2 was .410, indicating an excellent fit (McFadden, Reference McFadden, Hensher and Stopher R.1979). The accuracy was 77.4 per cent, the sensitivity was 46.5 per cent, and the specificity was 77.6 per cent. Higher social participation, self-rated social standing, and social support were associated with lower odds of developing high nutrition risk (see Table 3).

The third analysis added health-indicator variables as potential covariates to the social network variables and demographic covariates. This analysis examining the development of high nutrition risk was statistically significant (χ2 = 5515.1, p < .0001). The McFadden pseudo-R 2 was .423, indicating an excellent fit (McFadden, Reference McFadden, Hensher and Stopher R.1979). The accuracy was 77.4 per cent, the sensitivity was 50.0 per cent, and the specificity was 77.9 per cent.

Among the social network variables, in the analysis that included the social network, as well as the demographic and health-indicator variables as potential covariates, social support was a statistically significant predictor. For every 1-point increase in social support, the odds of developing high nutrition risk decreased by .99. None of the demographic variables were associated with the development of high nutrition risk. Among the health-indicator variables, depression, self-rated healthy aging, and self-rated oral health were associated with the development of high nutrition risk. Those who screened positive for depression had 1.36 odds of developing high nutrition risk, compared to those who screened negative for depression. Those with a self-rated healthy aging of “good” or “fair/poor,” compared to those with a self-rated healthy aging of “very good/excellent,” had 1.49 and 1.69 odds, respectively, of developing high nutrition risk. Those with a self-rated oral health of “good,” compared to “very good/excellent,” had 1.27 odds of developing high nutrition risk (see Table 3).

Sensitivity Analysis

Compared to the complete cases, those with missing data differed on all the social network variables, except for the frequency of contact with neighbours; however, the effect sizes were all small or trivial (Cohen, Reference Cohen1992; Ferguson, Reference Ferguson2009). Among the demographic variables, age, marital status, living situation, and education attainment differed between the two groups, but again the effect sizes were small or trivial (Ferguson, Reference Ferguson2009). SCREEN-8 scores at baseline and at follow-up also differed between the complete cases and those with missing data; however, the effect size was trivial (Ferguson, Reference Ferguson2009) (Supplemental Table 2).

Development of High Nutrition Risk in Those Not at Risk at Baseline

After multiple imputation, the logistic regression analysis that included the social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables, that examined the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up in those not at high risk at baseline, had several differences compared to the complete case analysis (Supplemental Table 6). After multiple imputation, social participation, sex assigned at birth, educational attainment, marital status, and self-rated general health became associated with the development of high nutrition risk.

Discussion

This study builds on the literature examining nutrition risk longitudinally by using a theoretical framework to explore factors that contribute to the development of high nutrition risk in individuals who were originally not at high risk, using a nationally representative sample of Canadian adults ages 45 and older. This study found that 36.5 per cent of the sample was at high nutrition risk at baseline, similar to previous Canadian studies that used SCREEN-8 and found that 32.5 per cent of community-dwelling Canadians ages 55 and older (Morrison et al., Reference Morrison, Laur and Keller2019) and 34.2 per cent of community-dwelling Canadians ages 65 and older (Ramage-Morin & Garriguet, Reference Ramage-Morin and Garriguet2013) were at high nutrition risk. Other studies, using SCREEN-14 (which adds six questions to the questions in SCREEN-8), have found a higher prevalence of high nutrition risk in community-dwelling adults ages 65 and older: 61.5 per cent in the Netherlands, 68.2 per cent in New Zealand, and 70.1 per cent in Canada (Borkent, Keller, Wham, Wijers, & de van der schueren, Reference Borkent, Keller, Wham, Wijers and de van der schueren2020); and 65 per cent in Sweden (Westergren, Khalaf, & Hagell, Reference Westergren, Khalaf and Hagell2015) and 67 per cent in the Netherlands (Haakma & Wham, Reference Haakma and Wham2015). These studies, using SCREEN-14, all found a higher prevalence of high nutrition risk than the current study did at follow-up (42.2%). A 2023 study that used the CLSA comprehensive cohort (the study presented here used the tracking cohort) found that 29.0 per cent of participants were at high nutrition risk at baseline, 35.6 per cent were at high nutrition risk at follow-up, and 17.3 per cent of those who were not at high risk at baseline developed a high risk at follow-up (Keller & Trinca, Reference Keller and Trinca2023). While these percentages are lower than those found for the tracking cohort, participants in the comprehensive cohort are more educated and have higher household incomes compared to the Canadian population, whereas the tracking cohort is representative of each province’s population (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). Further, participants in the comprehensive CLSA cohort had to be able to visit the urban academic test centres, and thus underrepresent Canadians who live in small urban and rural communities (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019).

In this study, social support was associated with the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up in individuals who were not at high risk at baseline. Higher levels of social support at baseline were associated with lower odds of developing high nutrition risk at follow-up in all three analyses. Social participation was also a predictor of the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up in the analysis that included only the social network variables and in the analysis that included the social network and demographic variables, but not in the analysis that included the social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables. Greater social participation was associated with lower odds of developing high nutrition risk.

Previous Canadian studies have found that low social support (Johnson, Reference Johnson2005; Ramage-Morin & Garriguet, Reference Ramage-Morin and Garriguet2013) and infrequent social participation (Ramage-Morin & Garriguet, Reference Ramage-Morin and Garriguet2013) were associated with nutrition risk in adults ages 65 and older. Other studies have also found that having low levels of social support is associated with increased nutrition risk (Locher et al., Reference Locher, Ritchie, Roth, Baker, Bodner and Allman2005; Locher & Sharkey, Reference Locher, Sharkey, Bales and Ritchie2009). Social support may affect nutrition risk in several ways. First, healthy behaviours such as consuming adequate amounts of nutrient-rich foods may be encouraged by an individual’s social support system (Locher & Sharkey, Reference Locher, Sharkey, Bales and Ritchie2009). Additionally, if an individual requires assistance with food-related activities, such as grocery shopping or meal preparation, adequate social support can ensure these needs are met (Keller, Reference Keller2005). Higher levels of social support and increased social participation may also provide an individual with more opportunities to eat with others. Eating with others improves food intake (de Castro, Reference de Castro1994), whereas eating alone is a well-known risk factor for poor nutrition and food intake (Bloom et al., Reference Bloom, Lawrence, Barker, Baird, Dennison and Sayer2017; Keller & McKenzie, Reference Keller and McKenzie2003; Vesnaver & Keller, Reference Vesnaver and Keller2011). Eating with others can reinforce social norms around food and eating, for example, by providing cues for mealtimes and appropriate food intake (Vesnaver & Keller, Reference Vesnaver and Keller2011). Even after adjusting for demographic and health-related variables that have been shown to influence nutrition risk, social support remained a significant predictor of the development of high nutrition risk, lending further support to the idea that social relationships play a key role in food and eating behaviours.

Social participation may also be associated with health status and therefore affect nutrition risk. As adequate nutrition is important for the maintenance of good health and well-being (Herman et al., Reference Herman, Baer, Adams, Cunningham-Sabo, Duran, Johnson and Yakes2014), individuals who are not at high nutrition risk may have better health than those at risk (see Table 2). Here, those who were not at high nutrition risk at baseline had higher self-rated general health than those who were at high risk. Therefore, individuals who are not at high nutrition risk may have better health and may therefore be able to participate in a greater number of community activities.

This study found that screening positive for depression was associated with higher odds of developing high nutrition risk in the analysis that included the social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables. Previous longitudinal studies have found an association between depression and increased nutrition risk (Keller, Reference Keller2006; Tsai, Reference Tsai2013). Other studies have also found this association (Wham et al., Reference Wham, Teh, Moyes, Dyall, Kepa, Hayman and Kerse2015; Wham et al., Reference Wham, Curnow and Towers2022; Wham, Carr, & Heller, Reference Wham, Carr and Heller2011). Depression may lead to a loss of appetite and reduced food intake (Ávila-Funes, Gray-Donald, & Payette, Reference Ávila-Funes, Gray-Donald and Payette2008; de Boer et al., Reference de Boer, Ter Horst and Lorist2013), resulting in weight loss and nutrition risk. Those with fewer social connections and lower social support may also be at increased risk for developing depression (Courtin & Knapp, Reference Courtin and Knapp2017; Gariépy, Honkaniemi, & Quesnel-Vallée, Reference Gariépy, Honkaniemi and Quesnel-Vallée2016). Similarly, those who are depressed may engage in fewer social activities, and thus have fewer opportunities to eat with others. There is therefore a complex relationship between social support, social participation, and depression, and these can all affect nutrition risk.

Self-rated healthy aging was associated with the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up in the analysis that included the social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables. Those who rated their self-rated healthy aging lower had higher odds of developing high nutrition risk at follow-up. A previous Canadian study also found an association between increased nutrition risk and lower self-rated successful aging (Lengyel, Tate, & Bayomi, Reference Lengyel, Tate and Bayomi2014). Individuals with higher self-rated healthy aging may find it easier to complete food-related tasks, such as meal preparation and grocery shopping. The consumption of a healthy diet, and therefore the absence of high nutrition risk, could also lead an individual to rate their healthy aging as higher. Not being at high nutrition risk may be both a cause and a consequence of healthy aging.

There were many more social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables associated with the development of high nutrition risk in the bivariate analyses. It is possible that some of these variables are consequences of nutrition risk, as opposed to potential causes. It is also possible that there are additional factors affecting these social network, demographic, and health-related variables, and nutrition risk. Other factors have been associated with high nutrition risk in cross-sectional studies, such as driving status, number of medications used daily, type of housing, satisfaction with income, and satisfaction with social support (Ramage-Morin et al., Reference Ramage-Morin, Gilmour and Rotermann2017; Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Wolfson and Payette2007). These variables were not included in the analyses presented here as they either were not available in the CLSA data or not part of Berkman et al.’s (Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000) theoretical framework. It is likely that some of these additional variables could also be predictors of high nutrition risk.

Strengths and Limitations

One strength of this study is the use of CLSA data, which is a large, representative sample of the Canadian population ages 45 and older (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009). As there are currently two waves of CLSA data available, nutrition risk was able to be examined longitudinally. As CLSA will follow participants every 3 years for 20 years or until participants’ death, additional longitudinal analyses can be completed in the future. This longitudinal data will also allow trajectories of nutrition risk to be explored in future research.

Another strength is that the CLSA measures were chosen in collaboration with expert working groups (Kirkland et al., Reference Kirkland, Griffith, Menec, Wister, Payette and Wolfson2015; Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009; Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). The measures were chosen based on many factors, including feasibility, practicality, and availability of tools in English and French (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009, Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). Other considerations included relevance across the age groups included in the CLSA (45 to 85) and the psychometric properties of the measures, including sensitivity, specificity, and responsiveness (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009, Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). The CLSA used validated questionnaires when available in both English and French (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009, Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). When there was no validated questionnaire available, the CLSA used established questionnaires from national surveys such as Statistics Canada’s CCHS (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Oremus and Patterson2009, Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). Thus, the measures used in the research presented here all have good validity and reliability or have been used in national Canadian surveys.

Despite these strengths, there are some limitations to the CLSA data. For example, not all social activities were captured in the CLSA data set. Similarly, the CLSA only captured frequency of face-to-face contact and not other forms of contact such as telephone calls, e-mails, or connecting through social media sites or other Internet programs. This may affect the generalizability of the results to other populations.

While the CLSA is representative of Canadian provincial residents, several groups were excluded. Full-time members of the Canadian Armed Forces, those living in the territories and in some remote areas, and those living on First Nation reserves and settlements (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019) were not included in the CLSA. Additionally, only those who can speak English or French and those with the capability to answer the questions themselves were included in the CLSA (Raina et al., Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, Griffith, Balion and Cossette2019). The tracking cohort also consists primarily of Canadians who indicated that their cultural/racial background is white (97.40%) (Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging, n.d.).

While several CLSA measures mapped onto the factors in Berkman et al.’s social network theory (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000), there are other social network factors that were not captured in the CLSA. Inclusion of additional mezzo and micro level factors may have resulted in better models and should be explored in future research. Factors at the macro level could also affect nutrition risk, and these should be explored in future research, as should additional factors that have been associated with nutrition risk in other studies.

It should be noted this study included adults ages 45 and older, whereas most other studies included only adults ages 65 and older – although a previous study using CLSA data included adults ages 55 and older (Morrison et al., Reference Morrison, Laur and Keller2019). This younger age group was purposely included so future research can explore how their nutrition risk continues to change, particularly as they enter older adulthood. Future research will explore how the development of high nutrition risk varies for different age groups.

Conclusions

This study examined factors that were associated with the development of high nutrition risk in Canadian adults ages 45 and older, using data from the CLSA. The analyses, based on Berkman et al.’s social network theory (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000), found that low social participation and social support were associated with the development of high nutrition risk in the analysis that looked at only social network variables and in the analysis that looked at social network and demographic variables, that low social support was associated with the development of high nutrition risk in all three analyses (social network; social network and demographic; and social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables), and that depression and fair/poor self-rated healthy aging were associated with the development of high nutrition in the analysis that included social network, demographic, and health-indicator variables. Designing programs and policies that encourage social participation, provide social support, address depression, and encourage healthy aging may therefore help prevent the development of high nutrition risk. Canada’s Food Guide (Health Canada, 2019), for example, recognizes the importance of eating with others. The Canadian Malnutrition Task Force (Keller, Donnelly, Laur, Goharian, & Nasser, Reference Keller, Donnelly, Laur, Goharian and Nasser2021) also recommends referral to meal-based programs or shopping and meal preparation assistance when an individual is at high nutrition risk. Congregate meal programs have been shown to improve nutritional status (Keller, Reference Keller2006) and provide opportunities for socialization. Further research should evaluate additional interventions designed to prevent high nutrition risk.

As high nutrition risk is associated with increased morbidity and mortality (Keller & Østbye, Reference Keller and Østbye2003; Ramage-Morin et al., Reference Ramage-Morin, Gilmour and Rotermann2017), it is important to help individuals who are not at high nutrition risk to remain not at risk. Individuals with low social participation, low social support, depression, and low self-rated healthy aging should also be screened proactively for high nutrition risk. Future research should examine additional factors associated with the development of high nutrition risk and should use future waves of CLSA data to continue to explore predictors of nutrition risk longitudinally.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0714980823000545.

Acknowledgements

This research was made possible using the data/biospecimens collected by the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). Funding for the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA) is provided by the Government of Canada through the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) under grant reference: LSA 94473 and the Canada Foundation for Innovation, as well as the following provinces, Newfoundland, Nova Scotia, Quebec, Ontario, Manitoba, Alberta, and British Columbia. This research has been conducted using the CLSA Baseline Tracking Dataset - Version 3.7, and Follow-up 1 Tracking Dataset - Version 2.2, under Application Number 2104008. The CLSA is led by Drs. Parminder Raina, Christina Wolfson, and Susan Kirkland.The AB SCREENTM II, rebranded as SCREEN-8, assessment tool is owned by Dr. Heather Keller. Use of the AB SCREENTM II assessment tool was made under license from the University of Guelph.

Disclaimer

The opinions expressed in this manuscript are the authors’ own and do not reflect the views of the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging.

References

Andresen, E. M., Malmgren, J. A., Carter, W. B., & Patrick, D. L. (1994). Screening for depression in well older adults: Evaluation of a short form of the CES-D. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 10(2), 7784. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0749-3797(18)30622-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ávila-Funes, J. A., Gray-Donald, K., & Payette, H. (2008). Association of nutritional risk and depressive symptoms with physical performance in the elderly: The Quebec longitudinal study of nutrition as a determinant of successful aging (NuAge). Journal of the American College of Nutrition, 27(4), 492498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayalon, L., & Levkovich, I. (2019). A systematic review of research on social networks of older adults. Gerontologist, 59(3), e164e176. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnx218CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Azur, M. J., Stuart, E. A., Frangakis, C., & Leaf, P. J. (2011). Multiple imputation by chained equations: What is it and how does it work? International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 20(1), 4049. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.329CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkman, L. F. (2000). Social support, social networks, social cohesion and health. Social Work in Health Care, 31(2), 314. https://doi.org/10.1300/j010v31n02_02CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkman, L. F., Glass, T., Brissette, I., & Seeman, T. E. (2000) From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new Millennium. Social Science & Medicine, 51(6), 843857. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00065-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, I., Lawrence, W., Barker, M., Baird, J., Dennison, E., Sayer, A. A., et al. (2017). What influences diet quality in older people? A qualitative study among community-dwelling older adults from the hertfordshire cohort study, UK. Public Health Nutrition, 20(15), 26852693. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980017001203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, I., Pilgrim, A., Jameson, K. A., Dennison, E. M., Sayer, A. A., Roberts, H. C., et al. (2021). The relationship of nutritional risk with diet quality and health outcomes in community-dwelling older adults. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 33(10), 27672776. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40520-021-01824-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, S. L., Sallis, J. F., Ritenbaugh, C., Hill, J. O., Birch, L. L., Frank, L. D., et al. (2009). Environmental and societal factors affect food choice and physical activity: Rationale, influences, and leverage points. Nutrition Reviews, 59(3), S21S36. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1753-4887.2001.tb06983.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borkent, J. W., Keller, H. H., Wham, C., Wijers, F., & de van der schueren, M. A. E. (2020). Cross-country differences and similarities in undernutrition prevalence and risk as measured by SCREEN II in community-dwelling older adults. Healthcare, 8(2), 151. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare8020151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Box, G. E. P., & Tidwell, P. W. (1962). Transformation of the independent variables. Technometrics, 4(4), 531550. 10.1080/00401706.1962.10490038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. (2011). Sampling and computation of response rates and sample weights for the tracking (Telephone Interview) participants and comprehensive participants.Google Scholar
Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. (n.d.) Cultural/Racial Background. Retrieved April 25, 2023, from https://datapreview.clsa-elcv.ca/mica/variable/trm%3ASDC_DCGT_TRM%3ACollected#/.Google Scholar
Chen, P.-J., & Antonelli, M. (2020). Conceptual models of food choice: Influential factors related to foods, individual differences, and society. Foods, 9(12), 1898. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9121898CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornwell, B., Laumann, E. O., & Schumm, L. P. (2008). The social connectedness of older adults: A national profile. American Sociological Review, 73(2), 185203. https://doi.org/10.1177/000312240807300201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtin, E., & Knapp, M. (2017). Social isolation, loneliness and health in old age: A scoping review. Health & Social Care in the Community, 25(3), 799812. https://doi.org/10.1111/hsc.12311CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craven, D. L., Pelly, F. E., Lovell, G. P., & Isenring, E. (2018). Nutrition risk measured online in community-living older Australians. Journal of Nutrition in Gerontology and Geriatrics, 37(3–4), 241254. https://doi.org/10.1080/21551197.2018.1490680CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Boer, A., Ter Horst, G. J., & Lorist, M. M. (2013). Physiological and psychosocial age-related changes associated with reduced food intake in older persons. Ageing Research Reviews, 12(1), 316328. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2012.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Castro, J. M. (1994). Family and friends produce greater social facilitation of food intake than other companions. Physiology & Behavior, 56(3), 445455. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(94)90286-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Groot, L. C., Beck, A. M., Schroll, M., & van Staveren, W. A. (1998). Evaluating the DETERMINE your nutritional health checklist and the mini nutritional assessment as tools to identify nutritional problems in elderly Europeans. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 52(12), 877883. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600658CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durlak, J. A. (2009). How to select, calculate, and interpret effect sizes. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34(9), 917928. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsp004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elsner, R. J. F. (2002). Changes in eating behavior during the aging process. Eating Behaviors, 3(1), 1543. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1471-0153(01)00041-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
English, T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2014). Selective narrowing of social networks across adulthood is associated with improved emotional experience in Daily Life. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(2), 195202. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413515404CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. J. (2009). An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 40(5), 532538. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015808CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fillenbaum, G. (1988). Multidiminsional functional assessment of older adults: The Duke older Americans resources and services procedures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fillenbaum, G. G. (1985). Screening the elderly. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 33(10), 698706. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.1985.tb01779.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gariépy, G., Honkaniemi, H., & Quesnel-Vallée, A. (2016). Social support and protection from depression: Systematic review of current findings in Western Countries. British Journal of Psychiatry, 209(4), 284293. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.115.169094CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haakma, T. A., & Wham, C. A. (2015). High prevalence of nutrition risk among community living older people in Woerden, the Netherlands. Journal of Aging Research and Lifestyle, 4, 230234. https://doi.org/10.14283/jarcp.2015.77Google Scholar
Harasemiw, O., Newall, N., Shooshtari, S., Mackenzie, C., & Menec, V. (2018). From social integration to social isolation: The relationship between social network types and perceived availability of social support in a national sample of older CanadiansResearch on Aging40(8), 715739. 10.1177/0164027517734587CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. K. (2021). Primer on binary logistic regression. Family Medicine and Community Health, 9(Suppl 1), e001290. https://doi.org/10.1136/fmch-2021-001290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Health Canada. (2019). Canada’s Dietary Guidelines for Health Professionals and Policy Makers. Ottawa, ON: Health Canada.Google Scholar
Herek, G. M. (2011). Developing a theoretical framework and rationale for a research proposal. In Pequegnat, W., Stover, E., and Boyce, C. A. (Eds.), How to Write a Successful Research Grant Application: A Guide for Social and Behavioral Scientists (pp. 137145) Boston, MA: Springer US.Google Scholar
Herman, D. R., Baer, M. T., Adams, E., Cunningham-Sabo, L., Duran, N., Johnson, D. B., & Yakes, E. (2014). Life course perspective: Evidence for the role of nutrition. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 18(2), 450461. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-013-1280-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IBM Corp. (2022). IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, Version 28.0.Google Scholar
Infurna, F. J., Gerstorf, D., & Lachman, M. E. (2020). Midlife in the 2020s: Opportunities and challenges. American Psychologist, 75(4), 470. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000591CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, C. S. J. (2005). Psychosocial correlates of nutritional risk in older adults. Canadian Journal of Dietetic Practice and Research, 66(2), 9597. https://doi.org/10.3148/66.2.2005.95CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, H. H. (2005). Reliance on others for food-related activities of daily living. Journal of Nutrition for the Elderly, 25(1), 4359. https://doi.org/10.1300/J052v25n01_05CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, H. H. (2006). Meal programs improve nutritional risk: A longitudinal analysis of community-living seniors. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 106(7), 10421048. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2006.04.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H. H. (n.d.) SCREEN tools. Older adult nutrition screening. Retrieved March 29, 2022, from https://olderadultnutritionscreening.com/.Google Scholar
Keller, H. H., Donnelly, R., Laur, C., Goharian, L., & Nasser, R. (2021). Consensus-based nutrition care pathways for hospital-to-community transitions and older adults in primary and community care. Journal of Parenteral and Enteral Nutrition, 46, 141152. https://doi.org/10.1002/jpen.2068CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, H. H., Goy, R., & Kane, S. L. (2005). Validity and reliability of SCREEN II (Seniors in the community: Risk evaluation for eating and nutrition, Version II). European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 59(10), 11491157. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602225CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, H. H., & McKenzie, J. D. D. (2003). Nutritional risk in vulnerable community-living seniors. Canadian Journal of Dietetic Practice and Research, 64(4), 195201. https://doi.org/10.3148/64.4.2003.195CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, H. H., & Østbye, T. (2003). Nutritional risk and time to death; Predictive validity of SCREEN (seniors in the community risk evaluation for eating and nutrition). Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 7(4), 274279.Google ScholarPubMed
Keller, H. H., Østbye, T., & Goy, R. (2004). Nutritional risk predicts quality of life in elderly community-living Canadians. Journal of Gerontology, 59A(1), 6874. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/59.1.m68Google Scholar
Keller, H. H., & Trinca, V. (2023). Determinants of a decline in a nutrition risk measure differ by baseline high nutrition risk status: Targeting nutrition risk screening for frailty prevention in the Canadian longitudinal study on aging (CLSA). Canadian Journal of Public Health 114, 593612. https://doi.org/10.17269/s41997-023-00745-wCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khalilzadeh, J., & Asli, D. A. T. (2017). Large sample size, significance level, and the effect size: Solutions to perils of using big data for academic research. Tourism Management, 62, 8996. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2017.03.026CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirkland, S., Griffith, L. E., Menec, V., Wister, A., Payette, H., Wolfson, C., et al. (2015). Mining a unique Canadian resource: The Canadian longitudinal study on aging. Canadian Journal on Aging/La Revue Canadienne Du Vieillissement, 34(3), 366377. https://doi.org/10.1017/S071498081500029XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambrinoudaki, I., Ceasu, I., Depypere, H., Erel, T., Rees, M., Schenck-Gustafsson, K., et al. (2013). EMAS position statement: Diet and health in midlife and beyond. Maturitas, 74(1), 99104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.maturitas.2012.10.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lengyel, C. O., Jiang, D., & Tate, R. B. (2017). Trajectories of nutritional risk: The Manitoba follow-up study. Journal of Nutrition, Health and Aging, 21(6), 604609. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12603-016-0826-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lengyel, C. O., Tate, R. B., & Bayomi, D. J. (2014). Nutritional risk in community-dwelling older men: The manitoba follow-up study. Canadian Journal of Dietetic Practice and Research, 75(2), 8488. https://doi.org/10.3148/75.2.2014.84CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, S. (Lamson), Kobayashi, K., Tong, H., Davison, K. M., Arora, S. R. A., & Fuller-Thomson, E. (2020). Close relations matter: The association between depression and refugee status in the Canadian longitudinal study on aging (CLSA). Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health, 22(5), 946956. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10903-020-00980-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 11981202. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locher, J. L., Ritchie, C. S., Roth, D. L., Baker, P. S., Bodner, E. V., & Allman, R. M. (2005). Social isolation, support, and capital and nutritional risk in an older sample: Ethnic and gender differences. Social Science and Medicine, 60(4), 747761. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locher, J. L., & Sharkey, J. R. (2009). An ecological perspective on older adult eating behavior. In Bales, C. W. and Ritchie, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of clinical nutrition and aging (pp. 317). Totowa: Humana Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, M., & Li, L. (2022). Social isolation trajectories in midlife and later-life: Patterns and associations with health. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 37(5), 5715. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5715CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McFadden, D. (1979). Quantitative methods for analysing travel behaviour of individuals: Some recent developments. In Hensher, D. A. & Stopher R., P. (Eds.), Behavioural travel modelling (pp. 279318). London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
McHugh, M. L. (2018). Cramér’s V coefficient. In Book, B. F. (Ed.), The SAGE encyclopedia of educational research, measurement, and evaluation. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
Mills, C. M., Keller, H. H., DePaul, V. G., & Donnelly, C. (2023). Social Network Factors Affect Nutrition Risk in Middle-Aged and Older Adults: Results from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 27(1), 4658. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12603-022-1877-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, J. M., Laur, C., & Keller, H. H. (2019). SCREEN III: Working towards a condensed screening tool to detect nutrition risk in community-dwelling older adults using CLSA data. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 73(9), 12601269. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41430-019-0411-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osanloo, A., & Grant, C. (2016). Understanding, selecting, and integrating a theoretical framework in dissertation research: Creating the blueprint for your ‘house’. Administrative Issues Journal: Connecting Education, Practice, and Research, 4(2), 1226.Google Scholar
Osborne, J. W. (2015). Best practices in logistic regression. London: SAGE Publications, Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payette, H., Coulombe, C., Boutier, V., & Gray-Donald, K. (2000). Nutrition risk factors for institutionalization in a free-living functionally dependent elderly population. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 53(6), 579587. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0895-4356(99)00186-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raina, P., Wolfson, C., Kirkland, S., Griffith, L. E., Balion, C., Cossette, B. C., et al. (2019). Cohort profile: The Canadian longitudinal study on aging (CLSA). International Epidemiological Association International Journal of Epidemiology, 48(6), 17521753. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyz173Google ScholarPubMed
Raina, P., Wolfson, C., Kirkland, S. A., Griffith, L. E., Oremus, M., Patterson, C., et al. (2009). The Canadian longitudinal study on aging (CLSA) Canadian Journal on Aging / La Revue Canadienne Du Vieillissement, 28(3), 221229. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0714980809990055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramage-Morin, P. L., & Garriguet, D. (2013). Nutritional risk among older Canadians. Health Reports, 24(3), 313.Google ScholarPubMed
Ramage-Morin, P. L., Gilmour, H., & Rotermann, M. (2017). Nutritional risk, hospitalization and mortality among community-dwelling Canadians aged 65 or older. Health Reports, 28(9), 1727.Google ScholarPubMed
Roberts, K. C., Wolfson, C., & Payette, H. (2007). Predictors of nutritional risk in community-dwelling seniors. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 98(4), 331336. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03405413CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RStudio Team. (2022). RStudio: Integrated Development for R.Boston, MA. Retrieved March 29, 2022, from http://www.rstudio.com/.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. B. (2004). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Sherbourne, C. D., & Stewart, A. L. (1991). The MOS social support survey. Social Science and Medicine, 32(6), 705714. https://doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(91)90150-BCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K. P., & Christakis, N. A. (2008). Social networks and health. Annual Review of Sociology, 34(1), 405429. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.34.040507.134601CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterne, J. A. C., White, I. R., Carlin, J. B., Spratt, M., Royston, P., Kenward, M. G., et al. (2009). Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: Potential and pitfalls. BMJ, 338, b2393b2393. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b2393CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tierney, N., & Cook, D. (2023). Expanding tidy data principles to facilitate missing data exploration, visualization and assessment of imputationsJournal of Statistical Software105(7), 131https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v105.i07CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, A. C., & Papachristos, A. V. (2015). From social networks to health: Durkheim after the turn of the Millennium. Social Science & Medicine ( 1982), 125, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsai, H.-J. (2013). Nutrition risk, functional dependence, and co-morbidities affect depressive symptoms in Taiwanese aged 53 years and over: A population-based longitudinal study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 75(2), 173177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.04.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). Mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45, 167. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v045.i03Google Scholar
Vesnaver, E., & Keller, H. H. (2011). Social influences and eating behavior in later life: A review. Journal of Nutrition in Gerontology and Geriatrics, 30(1), 223. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639366.2011.545038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westergren, A., Khalaf, A., & Hagell, P. (2015). A Swedish version of the SCREEN II for malnutrition assessment among community-dwelling elderly. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 43(6), 667671. https://doi.org/10.1177/1403494815575339CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wham, C., Carr, R., & Heller, F. (2011). Country of origin predicts nutrition risk among community living older people. Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 15(4), 253258. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12603-010-0305-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wham, C., Curnow, J., & Towers, A. (2022). Malnutrition risk: Four year outcomes from the health, work and retirement study 2014 to 2018. Nutrients, 14(11), 2205. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14112205CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wham, C. A., Teh, R., Moyes, S., Dyall, L., Kepa, M., Hayman, K., & Kerse, N. (2015). Health and social factors associated with nutrition risk: Results from life and living in advanced age: A cohort study in New Zealand (LILACS NZ). Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 19(6), 637645. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12603-015-0514-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, I. R., Royston, P., & Wood, A. M. (2011). Multiple imputation using chained equations: Issues and guidance for practice. Statistics in Medicine, 30(4), 377399. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.4067CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wister, A., Cosco, T., Mitchell, B., Menec, V., & Fyffe, I. (2019). “Development and concurrent validity of a composite social isolation index for older adults using the CLSA. Canadian Journal on Aging / La Revue Canadienne Du Vieillissement, 38(2), 180192. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0714980818000612CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wulff, J. N., & Ejlskov, L. (2017). Multiple imputation by chained equations in Praxis: Guidelines and Review. Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods, 15(1), 17.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Mapping CLSA variables onto social network theory

Figure 1

Table 2. Population characteristics, overall and by nutrition risk

Figure 2

Table 3. Logistic regression analyses examining the development of high nutrition risk at follow-up in those not at risk at baseline

Supplementary material: File

Mills et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S6

Download Mills et al. supplementary material(File)
File 79.5 KB