Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T11:59:20.630Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fish personality: meta-theoretical issues, personality dimensions, and applications to neuroscience and psychopathology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 October 2023

Ana Carolina Luchiari
Affiliation:
Department of Physiology & Behavior, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
Caio Maximino*
Affiliation:
Laboratório de Neurociências e Comportamento, Instituto de Estudos em Saúde e Biológicas, Universidade Federal do Sul e Sudeste do Pará, Marabá, Brazil
*
Corresponding author: Caio Maximino; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

While the field of personality neuroscience has extensively focused on humans and, in a few cases, primates and rodents, a wide range of research on fish personality has emerged in the last decades. This research is focused mainly on the ecological and evolutionary causes of individual differences and also aimed less extensively at proximal mechanisms (e.g., neurochemistry or genetics). We argue that, if consistent and intentional work is made to solve some of the meta-theoretical issues of personality research both on fish and mammals, fish personality research can lead to important advances in personality neuroscience as a whole. The five dimensions of personality in fish (shyness-boldness, exploration-avoidance, activity, aggressiveness, and sociability) need to be translated into models that explicitly recognize the impacts of personality in psychopathology, synergizing research on fish as model organisms in experimental psychopathology, personality neuroscience, and ecological-ethological approaches to the evolutionary underpinnings of personality to produce a powerful framework to understand individual differences.

Type
Review Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

The landscape of personality research has been dominated by work with humans; however, in order to understand the neurobehavioral bases of personality, the use of model organisms is a fundamental step. While most researchers are likely to look into research on closely related mammals, including primates, an expanding field has been the study of personality in fish. Since the seminal work of Huntingford (Reference Huntingford1976) on individual differences in sticklebacks, the field of fish personality has steadily grown, now involving a wide array of species, tests, and empirical data on personality dimensions (Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz, & Sih, Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall & Dingemanse, Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007). The interest in using fish sprung not only from the relatively ease of using small teleosts as laboratory model organisms (Stewart et al., 2015) but also due to the tradition of fish research in ethology. Combining approaches from both ethology and neuroscience has the potential to transform the field of personality neuroscience, but, as will be discussed in the present article, many issues still make it difficult to compare data from both fields.

The present article is a discussion of the implications of fish personality research to personality neuroscience in general. We begin by presenting some of the current conundrums in fish personality research, focusing on a meta-theoretical framework proposed by Jana Uher (Uher, Reference Uher2008, Reference Uher2011, Reference Uher2013, Reference Uher2015; Uher, Werner & Gosselt, Reference Uher, Werner and Gosselt2013). In Section 2, we focus on the empirical research that defined a five-dimensional model of personality across fish species, commenting on some of the findings on proximal mechanisms that might be relevant to personality neuroscience. We close the article by presenting implications of fish personality research to experimental psychopathology, using the relationship between personality and individual differences in anxiety-like behavior in zebrafish as a case study.

1. Current issues in fish personality research

One important issue in the field of animal personality in general is how one defines personality. This question is important because it represents a theoretical constraint on how to measure personality in non-linguistic species, as well as in non-mammals. Theory always guides judgments about what counts as data in a given field, and how these data can be analyzed and interpreted (Cervone, Shadel & Jencius, Reference Cervone, Shadel and Jencius2001; Køppe, Reference Køppe2012; Uher, Reference Uher2015); theories of personality always contain not only a theory of individual differences but also a theory of how to assess these differences (at least implicitly). Thus, theories of personality always should ask: which kinds of phenomena should be considered in assessing individual differences? Should the focus be on behavior, emotionality, cognition, motivation, or something else? What kind of differences are important: between-subject differences, consistency in these differences, or within-subject differences?

Closely related to these issues is the methodological question of the unit of analysis: while exploration of differences between populations is fundamental to identify what is specific to each individual (Uher, Reference Uher2013), in the end the question of individual differences is also targeted at exploring individual consistency (Allport & Allport, Reference Allport and Allport1921; Uher, Reference Uher2011). Thus, one important methodological question in the field of personality research is how to define patterns of phenomena (e.g., behaviors) both at the population and individual levels (Carter, Feeney, Marshall, Cowlishaw & Heinsohn, Reference Carter, Feeney, Marshall, Cowlishaw and Heinsohn2013; Sánchez-Tójar, Moiron & Niemelä, Reference Sánchez-Tójar, Moiron and Niemelä2022). It has been argued (Kaiser & Müller, Reference Kaiser and Müller2021) that, in order to speak of non-human personality, population-level and between-individual differences are necessary to identify broader patterns, but these data must in turn be able to define a behavioral trait in individual animals: the individual must behave differently than others (i.e., show individual differences); these behavioral differences must be stable over a certain time (i.e., temporal stability), and they must be consistent in different contexts (contextual consistency) (Castanheira, Herrera, Costas, Conceição & Martins, Reference Castanheira, Herrera, Costas, Conceição and Martins2013; Kaiser & Müller, Reference Kaiser and Müller2021).

An example of research that demonstrated that a specific stability of correlations between behavioral variables at the population levels did not hold at the individual level is that made by Lee and Bereijikian (Reference Lee and Bereijikian2008) on brown rockfish (Sebastes auriculatus): positive correlations were found at the population level between predator inspection and feeding in absence and presence of predators, and these correlations still held 10 days later. While this would usually be interpreted as a sign of a “personality,” the individuals’ rank order along the different behavioral variables changed considerably. This is not an indication of individual behavioral phenotype and, as a result, not consistent with the term “personality” (Uher, Reference Uher2011).

Most of the research that is discussed in the present article is relevant to understanding fish personality even though individual consistencies are not always evaluated. Especially when the final aim is to describe proximal causes and mechanisms, most studies on fish personality measure differences at the population level, without looking at individual differences and consistencies (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Toms, Echevarria & Jouandot, Reference Toms, Echevarria and Jouandot2010). As a result, while certainly relevant as hypothetical-generating devices, these studies do not target personality per se.

Finally, the question of mechanism is highly important to the field of personality. The field of animal personality can be approached both from the point of view of distal (e.g., evolutionary) mechanisms and from the point of view of proximal mechanisms (e.g., biological basis) (Weiss, Reference Weiss2018). However, the particular mechanisms that are looked for depend heavily on the conceptual and methodological issues (Uher, Reference Uher2015).

One of the implicit decisions that is apparent in fish personality research is the assumption that the traits or dimensions that are observed are evolved. As remarked above, the field of fish personality research blossomed in the hands of researchers who were trying to understand the distal mechanisms of personality. Following the dominant adaptationist paradigm, this research usually understands personality traits as adaptive and “selected for,” and part of the research in the field revolves around trying to find the selective pressures that led to the evolution of the specific trait or traits (Schuett, Tregenza & Dall, Reference Schuett, Tregenza and Dall2010; Sih, Bell, & Johnson, Reference Sih, Bell and Johnson2004). Again, variation across personality dimensions is usually assessed as behavioral variation between populations and not as variation between individuals (i.e., within-population variability) (MacKay & Haskell, Reference MacKay and Haskell2015). While such variation is certainly important if we are to understand the evolutionary bases of personality, within-individual consistency is necessary to define a behavioral trait in individual animals, and individual variation is important to understand the proximal mechanisms which are relevant to personality neuroscience (MacKay & Haskell, Reference MacKay and Haskell2015; Pervin, Reference Pervin1994; Uher, Reference Uher2011).

A closely related question is that of selection towards one or more optimal strategies (Carter et al., Reference Carter, Feeney, Marshall, Cowlishaw and Heinsohn2013): if selection acts towards optimality, how is variation in animal behavior maintained? However, most studies on fish personality so far have not explicitly tested adaptationist hypotheses that are related to the specific contexts in which species-specific selective pressures are taking place, instead looking for pressures that appear to be universal. This, of course, facilitates a comparative framework, but it also assumes the universality of traits. Uher (Reference Uher2008) proposed that assuming a behavioral repertoire approach in non-human personality research will necessitate explicitly recognizing that the dimensions or traits that will be investigated are not necessarily universal nor unique; in fact, because “all species have a phylogenetic history and show adaptations to a particular ecological niche, most species exhibit both universal and unique trait dimensions in their personality structure” (Uher, Reference Uher2008, p. 479).

These assumptions, of course, determine which behaviors will be selected as endpoints in personality research. Uher (Reference Uher2008) identifies two rationales for selecting endpoints: a bottom-up approach, which usually starts from the exhaustive measurement and cataloging of observable behavior in a population, followed by subsequent theorizing and analysis of Tinbergen’s four questions (function, evolution, causation, and development), and a top-down approach, which usually starts from theories of personality that were created based mainly on human data (e.g., the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST); Corr, Reference Corr2004) and attempts to apply it to other species. In fish personality research, the bottom-up approach is much more common (Toms et al., Reference Toms, Echevarria and Jouandot2010) – in fact, the majority of non-human personality studies which claim the shyness-boldness dimension that will be discussed below were made first in fish species (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Sih, Bell, & Johnson, Reference Sih, Bell and Johnson2004; Sih, Bell, Johnson, et al., Reference Sih, Bell, Johnson and Ziemba2004). However, some attempts have been made to “map” the personality dimensions empirically described in fish to other theories, such as RST (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Oliveira, Rosemberg, Batista, Herculano, Oliveira and Blaser2012).

2. Dimensionality of personality in fish

Many authors have pointed out that the lack of consensus on measurement reflects the lack of consensus on the definitions of specific dimensions or traits in non-human personality (Carter et al., Reference Carter, Feeney, Marshall, Cowlishaw and Heinsohn2013; Toms et al., Reference Toms, Echevarria and Jouandot2010). This is a meta-theoretical issue as well (Uher, Reference Uher2013, Reference Uher2015): should a continuum such as shyness-boldness be considered as a dimension, with possible values falling all over the continuum, or as a trait, with possible values falling on limited portions of the continuum? While partially solvable by empirical research using appropriate statistical methods (Toms & Echevarria, Reference Toms and Echevarria2014), this meta-theoretical issue is commonly left implicit in the field of fish personality research. Moreover, researchers of “(non-human) animal personality” usually study specific personality traits or dimensions, such as shyness-boldness, sociability, or aggressiveness, rather than personalities in general (Kaiser & Müller, Reference Kaiser and Müller2021). Nonetheless, the use of terms such as “personality dimensions” implies the existence of a “global” personality that is decomposed into these dimensions (Kaiser & Müller, Reference Kaiser and Müller2021).

This problem gave rise to differences in terminology in the field. The terms “personality,” “temperament,” and “behavioral syndrome” are sometimes treated as interchangeable, leading to conceptual confusion. We follow Kaiser and Müller (Reference Kaiser and Müller2021) by defining personality as the individual variation, consistent across time and contexts, across all possible dimensions/axes. A personality dimension is understood as a single axis (e.g., aggressiveness or shyness-boldness) in which population-level variation can be mapped. Finally, we apply the term “behavioral syndrome,” introduced by Alison Bell (Bell, Reference Bell2007; Sih, Bell, & Johnson, Reference Sih, Bell and Johnson2004; Sih, Bell, Johnson, et al., Reference Sih, Bell, Johnson and Ziemba2004), in the same sense that was proposed by MacKay and Haskell (Reference MacKay and Haskell2015) as the link between “two or more dimensions across a population” (p. 41). Thus, a behavioral syndrome is a suite of correlated behaviors across situations and contexts which exists within a population (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Sih, Bell, & Johnson, Reference Sih, Bell and Johnson2004; Sih, Bell, Johnson, et al., Reference Sih, Bell, Johnson and Ziemba2004). For example, boldness-aggression syndromes have been shown within some populations of zebrafish or sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus), but not in other populations (Bell, Reference Bell2005; Martins & Bhat, Reference Martins and Bhat2014; Roy & Bhat, Reference Roy and Bhat2018a, Reference Roy and Bhat2018b). This is a necessary definition to allow for comparability of research made by ethologists and animal behaviorists with that made by psychologists, who would certainly not consider within-population variation in a single dimension as evidence for a “personality” (Pervin, Reference Pervin1994; Uher, Reference Uher2011). Mere evidence of within-population correlations between dimensions, without evidence of stable within-individual correlations, is not sufficient to affirm a “personality” (see Uher, Reference Uher2011, for a thorough discussion of this topic).

In this review, we follow the framework by Réale et al. (Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007) to describe the behavioral dimensions of fish personality: sociability, aggressiveness, exploration-avoidance, activity, and shyness-boldness (Fig. 1); however, we also echo the concerns of Conrad et al. (Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011) that simply referring to the term used for a specific dimension is not enough, and grasping the full meaning of that dimension/trait is only possible by considering the context and methods of each study. In what follows, we briefly define these dimensions, describe some of the behavioral tests that are used to assess them (Table 1), and then present some results on eco-ethological research and on mechanistic research that are likely to be relevant to the field of personality neuroscience.

Figure 1. Five-dimensional model of fish personality and observed or inferred correlations between dimensions. Blue arrows denote positive correlations, while red arrows denote negative correlations. The “classical” approach in fish personality research is to look for mechanisms in single dimensions, while a “system” approach involves looking for mechanisms for the interplay between dimensions (i.e., for behavioral syndromes). Abbreviations: ExpAv: Exploration-avoidance; Bold: Shyness-boldness; Act: Activity; Soc: Sociability; Agr: Aggressiveness.

Table 1. Summary of behavioral tests commonly used to measure each dimension of fish personality. Note that some tests can measure more than one dimension

2.1 Boldness-shyness

Réale et al. (Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007) define “boldness” as behavior in situations perceived as dangerous, excluding reactions to novelty. This definition was based on studies that were unable to find correlations between responses to threatening novel stimuli with responses to non-threatening novel stimuli (e.g., food in Coleman & Wilson, Reference Coleman and Wilson1998). However, this definition is also over-reductive, as it ignores the possibility that novelty is also threatening (Kaiser & Müller, Reference Kaiser and Müller2021). In fact, there is a long tradition of research on exploratory behavior in rats that recognizes that at least two factors underline this behavior: an approach motivation (“curiosity” or “exploration”) and an avoidance motivation (“fear”) (Hughes, Reference Hughes1997; Montgomery, Reference Montgomery1955; Montgomery & Monkman, Reference Montgomery and Monkman1955; Russell, Reference Russell1973). These two-factor theories were also highly influential in motivating theoretical models, such as RST (Corr, Reference Corr2004; Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr and McNaughton2012). In contrast to Réale et al. (Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007), Toms et al. (Reference Toms, Echevarria and Jouandot2010) argued that tests involving novelty are best suited for capturing a personality dimension of “shyness-boldness,” while measuring risk-taking in the presence of predators would capture situationally defined reactions but not necessarily boldness.

To further complicate the issue, boldness is commonly assessed using emergence tests, in which the key measure is the latency to leave a “safe” chamber and enter a “risky” arena, which nonetheless has no predators or other clearly threatening stimuli. In these situations, the “risky” arena represents a potentially threatening place, but it is not clear how this maps to Réale et al.’s (Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007) definition of boldness. In some studies (e.g., Ferrari et al., Reference Ferrari, Horri, Allal, Vergnet, Benhaim, Vandeputte, Chatain and Bégout2016), traits defined as “boldness” using variations of the emergence test have been used to define “coping styles,” with bold individuals described as “proactive” and shy individuals described as “reactive.” However, definitions of coping commonly used in fish research are one-dimensional (Koolhaas et al., Reference Koolhaas, Korte, De Boer, Van Der Vegt, Van Reenen, Hopster, De Jong, Ruis and Blokhuis1999), and coping is highly plastic (Øverli et al., Reference Øverli, Sørensen, Pulman, Pottinger, Korzan, Summers and Nilsson2007). Another source of confusion is that some researchers (e.g., Sih, Bell, & Johnson, Reference Sih, Bell and Johnson2004) treat coping as an “axis” while simultaneously defining proactive individuals as “both aggressive and bold,” suggesting a confusion between a personality dimension and a behavioral syndrome.

Even if only studies of behavior under distal or proximal threat are considered, a shyness-boldness dimension has been documented in a variety of fish species. Studies in sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) show that the ontogenetic stability of boldness is variable between populations (Bell & Stamps, Reference Bell and Stamps2004) and that boldness varies across habitats as a function of predation risk (Álvarez & Bell, Reference Álvarez and Bell2007). These results were conceptually replicated in populations of poeciliids (B. episcopi) (Brown & Braithwaite, Reference Brown and Braithwaite2004; Brown, Jones & Braithwaite, Reference Brown, Jones and Braithwaite2005; Brown, Jones & Braithwaite, Reference Brown, Jones and Braithwaite2007) and zebrafish (Danio rerio) (Roy & Bhat, Reference Roy and Bhat2018b, Reference Roy and Bhat2018a). In all cases, individuals from habitats with higher predation risk are bolder, illustrating the “paradox of risk allocation” (Ferrari, Sih & Chivers, Reference Ferrari, Sih and Chivers2009).

Boldness has been considered a trait with high heritability in fishes (Brown, Burgess & Braithwaite, Reference Brown, Burgess and Braithwaite2007; Ferrari et al., Reference Ferrari, Horri, Allal, Vergnet, Benhaim, Vandeputte, Chatain and Bégout2016; Mazué, Dechaume-Moncharmont & Godin, Reference Mazué, Dechaume-Moncharmont and Godin2015) as in other animals, although the environment plays an important part in shaping individual levels of boldness (Polverino, Cigliano, Nakayama & Mehner, Reference Polverino, Cigliano, Nakayama and Mehner2016; Stamps & Groothuis, Reference Stamps and Groothuis2010). Longitudinal studies followed the ontogenesis of this trait in search of some temporal consistency, but it is worth noting that behavioral plasticity figures as a valuable element for individual fitness. For example, sex, age, body size, and hierarchical status are known factors that affect boldness (Castanheira et al., Reference Castanheira, Cerqueira, Millot, Gonçalves, Oliveira, Conceição and Martins2016; King, Fürtbauer, Mamuneas, James & Manica, Reference King, Fürtbauer, Mamuneas, James and Manica2013; Philip, Dellinger & Benhaïm, Reference Philip, Dellinger and Benhaïm2022). Moreover, several environmental factors experienced during early development, such as food availability, pH variation, and temperature, affect boldness later in life (Bell & Stamps, Reference Bell and Stamps2004; Stamps & Groothuis, Reference Stamps and Groothuis2010; Stamps & Groothuis, Reference Stamps and Groothuis2010).

The shyness-boldness continuum is sometimes correlated with aggressiveness, forming a boldness-aggression syndrome. In Fig. 1, this is represented by individuals A and B showing higher levels of both boldness and aggressiveness, and individual C showing low levels of both boldness and aggressiveness. The stability of these associations seems to depend on the population. For example, studying zebrafish populations collected in the wild, Martins and Bhat (Reference Martins and Bhat2014) found major population differences in the levels of aggression and boldness (using a simulated predator attack), but found correlations between aggression and boldness in only one of the five populations. In sticklebacks, positive correlations between aggression and boldness were also described. Associations at the population level were found by Bell, Henderson and Huntingford. (Reference Bell, Henderson and Huntingford2010), who found that sticklebacks derived from populations from high predation sites had higher boldness (measured by predator inspection) and conspecific aggression. Bell and Sih (Reference Bell and Sih2007) found that sticklebacks which were exposed to predation show correlations between boldness (measured by a simulated strike assay) and aggression, while animals which were not experimentally exposed to aggression did not show these correlations. Roy and Bhat (Reference Roy and Bhat2018a) also found in zebrafish that predation levels are related to associations between boldness (using a predator inspection assay) and other dimensions; a negative relationship was found between activity and boldness only within two low-predation populations. In general, then, associations that are found at the population level do not always translate to individual differences and therefore might be more related to population-level selective pressures than individual variability.

Few attempts have been made to understand the neurobiological bases of this boldness-aggression syndrome. In both the fish and the mammalian literature, aggressiveness and responses to distal or proximal threat are both negatively related to serotonin levels (Filby, Paull, Hickmore, & Tyler, Reference Filby, Paull, Hickmore and Tyler2010; Graeff, Reference Graeff2004; Lima-Maximino et al., Reference Lima-Maximino, Pyterson, Silva, Gomes, Rocha, Herculano and Maximino2020; Olivier, Reference Olivier2004; Paul, Johnson, Shekhar & Lowry, Reference Paul, Johnson, Shekhar and Lowry2014; Paul & Lowry, Reference Paul and Lowry2013). It would not be surprising, then, to find that the boldness-aggression syndrome is related to this monoamine. Bell, Backström, Huntingford, Pottinger and Winberg (Reference Bell, Backström, Huntingford, Pottinger and Winberg2007) assessed the levels of monoamine neurotransmitters in different regions of the stickleback brain when exposed to an unfamiliar conspecific or a predator; hypothalamic serotonin was negatively correlated with frequency of attacking a conspecific and positively associated with predator inspection, suggesting this neurotransmitter as a potential link between aggression and boldness.

2.2 Exploration-avoidance

The exploration-avoidance dimension includes behaviors that involve individual differences in willingness to investigate novel environments, food items, or objects (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011). The most commonly used tests for anxiety-like behavior in zebrafish rely on exploratory behavior under conditions of novelty, but not explicit threat (Kalueff et al., Reference Kalueff, Echevarria, Homechaudhuri, Stewart, Collier, Kaluyeva and Song2016; Kysil et al., Reference Kysil, Meshalkina, Frick, Echevarria, Rosemberg, Maximino and Kalueff2017; Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, de Brito, da Silva Batista, Herculano, Morato and Gouveia2010, Reference Maximino, Oliveira, Rosemberg, Batista, Herculano, Oliveira and Blaser2012) – and, while the shyness-boldness dimension should conceptually include behavior in anxiety-inducing environments (Kaiser & Müller, Reference Kaiser and Müller2021; Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Oliveira, Rosemberg, Batista, Herculano, Oliveira and Blaser2012), by the definition of shyness-boldness and exploration-avoidance used in most of fish research these tests should be considered indexes of the latter. Typical measures of the exploration-avoidance dimension in fish include the animal’s latency to explore a novel arena or to emerge from a shelter (“emergence tests”), the latency or time spent exploring a novel object, or the latency to consume a novel food (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Réale et al., Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007). It bears repeating that a lot of research using these tests talks of boldness instead of exploration-avoidance, leading to great confusion; however, to be distinguished from shyness-boldness, exploration-avoidance should be assessed in the absence of threatening stimuli other than novelty itself (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Réale et al., Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007).

Despite these different definitions, the exploration-avoidance continuum has also been linked to neophilia or neophobia. This is important in the context of two-factor theories of exploratory behavior (Hughes, Reference Hughes1997; Montgomery, Reference Montgomery1955; Montgomery & Monkman, Reference Montgomery and Monkman1955; Russell, Reference Russell1973) that suggest that, in novel environments, exploration is controlled both by an approach motivation (“curiosity” or “exploration”) and an avoidance motivation (“fear”). From an ecological point of view, exploratory tendency has been exploited as a sign of fitness: golden shiners (Notemigonus crysoleucas) that are quicker to navigate through a novel tank were also more likely to be group leaders in a shoal (Leblond & Reebs, Reference Leblond and Reebs2006). Likewise, Nomakuchi, Park and Bell (Reference Nomakuchi, Park and Bell2009) showed that sticklebacks with higher exploration were also more likely to learn a maze through social learning. These unexpected correlations between exploration-avoidance and sociality still need to be more deeply investigated to understand whether low exploration/low sociality individuals exist within populations, which could point to different neurobiological bases for high exploration and low exploration.

The ontogenesis of the exploration-avoidance dimension has been more thoroughly investigated than boldness. Using emergence tests, some authors observed within-population differences in exploration-avoidance at very early stages of development (Alfonso, Peyrafort, Cousin & Bégout, Reference Alfonso, Peyrafort, Cousin and Bégout2020; Edenbrow & Croft, Reference Edenbrow and Croft2011; Ibarra-Zatarain, Rey, Boglino, Fatsini & Duncan, Reference Ibarra-Zatarain, Rey, Boglino, Fatsini and Duncan2020; Polverino et al., Reference Polverino, Cigliano, Nakayama and Mehner2016), while other investigated its consistency along ontogeny (Alfonso et al., Reference Alfonso, Peyrafort, Cousin and Bégout2020; Castanheira et al., Reference Castanheira, Herrera, Costas, Conceição and Martins2013; Edenbrow & Croft, Reference Edenbrow and Croft2011; Polverino et al., Reference Polverino, Cigliano, Nakayama and Mehner2016). Fernandes-Silva, Leite-Ferreira, Menezes and Luchiari (Reference Fernandes-Silva, Leite-Ferreira, Menezes and Luchiari2022) observed that zebrafish separated by the time of egg hatching (“early hatchers” vs. “late hatchers”) show consistent differences in the exploration-avoidance dimensions when tested at both 30 and 120 days post-fertilization, although individual consistency was not assessed. Alfonso et al. (Reference Alfonso, Peyrafort, Cousin and Bégout2020), on the other hand, showed consistent differences in exploration-avoidance between contexts but not over age in zebrafish.

From a mechanistic perspective, some attempts have been made to understand the neurobiological bases of exploration-aggression syndromes. Since both anxiety/avoidance and aggression are related to monoamines in both mammals and fish, with opponent effects of serotonin on avoidance and aggression (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Puty, Benzecry, Araújo, Lima, de Jesus Oliveira Batista, Renata de Matos Oliveira, Crespo-Lopez and Herculano2013; Olivier, Reference Olivier2004; Paul et al., Reference Paul, Johnson, Shekhar and Lowry2014), these neurotransmitters would naturally be the place to start research. Abbey-Lee, Kreshchenko, Fernandez Sala, Petkova and Løvlie (Reference Abbey-Lee, Kreshchenko, Fernandez Sala, Petkova and Løvlie2019) found that chronic treatment of sticklebacks with fluoxetine decreased the latency to enter a novel area, but did not affect aggressive display; treatment with ropinirole, a nonselective dopamine receptor agonist, decreased both the latency to explore the environment and aggressive displays. Moreover, the expression of stress-related receptor genes (NR3C1 and NR3C2) and a dopamine receptor gene (DRD1B) was predictors of individual differences in aggression and sociability (Abbey-Lee et al., Reference Abbey-Lee, Kreshchenko, Fernandez Sala, Petkova and Løvlie2019). Thus, at least in the experimental contexts proposed by Abbey-Lee et al. (Reference Abbey-Lee, Kreshchenko, Fernandez Sala, Petkova and Løvlie2019), dopaminergic signaling and glucocorticoid hormones appear to be more related than serotonin to the aggression-exploration syndrome.

A mutant strain of zebrafish, spiegeldanio, has been shown to carry a mutation in the fgfr1a gene, which encodes the fibroblast growth factor receptor 1a. Zebrafish from the spiegeldanio strain show increased aggressiveness (tested by the mirror-induced aggression test), as well as increased exploration (measured by the light/dark test, the novel object test, and the latency to fully explore a t-maze) (Norton et al., Reference Norton, Stumpenhorst, Faus-Kessler, Folchert, Rohner, Harris, Callebert and Bally-Cuif2011). Spiegeldanio also showed higher exploration than an F1 population derived from wild-caught zebrafish, but not in relation to animals from the AB line (Mustafa, Roman & Winberg, Reference Mustafa, Roman and Winberg2019). While spiegeldanio showed increased expression of the serotonin transporter slc6a4a in the superior raphe, treatment with fluoxetine did not rescue the exploration-aggression syndrome of these mutants. These animals also showed reduced brain histamine levels, and treatment with tacrine, a drug which blocks histamine metabolism, rescued the phenotype (Norton et al., Reference Norton, Stumpenhorst, Faus-Kessler, Folchert, Rohner, Harris, Callebert and Bally-Cuif2011), thus suggesting that histaminergic signaling is responsible for the aggression-exploration syndrome of spiegeldanio. Combined with the results from Abbey-Lee et al. (Reference Abbey-Lee, Kreshchenko, Fernandez Sala, Petkova and Løvlie2019), the data from spiegeldanio suggest that dopamine and histamine, but not serotonin, are involved in the exploration-aggression syndrome of fish. Nonetheless, it is important to understand that, in both cases, these syndromes have been described at the population level, but not at the individual level, and therefore, caution must be taken in interpreting these results.

2.3 Activity

A third dimension of fish personality that has consistently been described is activity. While activity levels certainly are a confounding factor to measure boldness and exploration (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011), sufficient consistency in individual differences in activity levels was found to suggest activity as a personality dimension in itself. Individual variation in activity levels is assessed in multiple tests as a control variable, but consistent individual differences are observed as well. Usually, activity levels are measured as distance traveled (swum) or swimming speed, but measures such as time budgets for specific activities have also been used (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011).

There is evidence for activity-boldness syndromes at the population level on various species. Using predator inspection as a surrogate for boldness, Moretz, Martins and Robison (Reference Moretz, Martins and Robison2007) showed positive correlations between activity levels and boldness across one wild-derived and two laboratory-derived populations. Dingemanse et al. (Reference Dingemanse, Wright, Kazem, Thomas, Hickling and Dawnay2007) analyzed twelve stickleback populations and found positive correlations between activity, exploration, and aggressiveness only in those populations that were raised in large ponds where piscivorous predators were present. An activity-sociality-exploration syndrome has also been described in sticklebacks, in which an individual’s propensity to stay near others was negatively related to swim speed across tests, and predicted spatial positioning and leadership within groups (Jolles, Boogert, Sridhar, Couzin & Manica, Reference Jolles, Boogert, Sridhar, Couzin and Manica2017). However, from a mechanistic point of view, studies on activity-boldness and activity-sociality syndromes are absent.

From an ecological perspective, activity levels are of interest because they are correlated with a general metabolic response (Biro & Stamps, Reference Biro and Stamps2010; Careau, Thomas, Humphries & Réale, Reference Careau, Thomas, Humphries and Réale2008; Nespolo & Franco, Reference Nespolo and Franco2007). As a result, correlations between activity levels and other personality dimensions can reflect time budget conflicts (e.g., time spent feeding vs. time occupying a refuge), or activity levels can directly reflect metabolism and therefore a constraint on the execution of other tasks (Sih, Bell, Johnson, et al., Reference Sih, Bell, Johnson and Ziemba2004). Thus, syndromes that involve activity are likely to also be related to metabolism. One theory to explain these correlations is the so-called “pace-of-life syndrome” theory (Réale et al., Reference Réale, Garant, Humphries, Bergeron, Careau and Montiglio2010). This hypothesis states that closely related species or populations occupying different ecological niches are likely to show population-level differences in behavioral traits, as well as in a range of physiological variables. The correlations between these behavioral and physiological traits suggest a co-evolution based on the particular life-history characteristics that are evoked by these niches. Binder et al. (Reference Binder, Wilson, Wilson, Suski, Godin and Cooke2016) showed that exploration-avoidance is associated with metabolism in the bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus), with individuals with higher exploration (assessed by the emergence test) showing higher maximum metabolic rates and metabolic scopes for activity, but not basal aerobic or anaerobic metabolism.

2.4 Aggressiveness

Aggressive behavior has long been a mainstay of ethology, given its ecological relevance in conspecific competition, territory defense, or offspring protection (Réale et al., Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007). While individual variation in aggressiveness has been observed in different situations and species, in the context of personality research, this trait is usually studied in correlation with other dimensions (i.e., a behavioral syndrome)(Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Réale et al., Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007; Toms et al., Reference Toms, Echevarria and Jouandot2010). This focus on these correlations does not mean, however, that individual differences in aggressiveness were not observed; for example, the seminal study of Huntingford (Reference Huntingford1976) described individual differences in stickleback aggression.

As is the case with most social behaviors, context-dependent plasticity is very common in the case of aggressiveness (Oliveira, Reference Oliveira2012). In a male subpopulation of tropical beau gregory damselfish Stegastes leucostictus, aggression levels are individually consistent only in lower-quality breeding sites, as individuals who are transferred to (artificial) higher-quality breeding sites lose this consistency: all individuals become highly aggressive in defending these sites (Snekser, Leese, Ganim & Itzkowitz, Reference Snekser, Leese, Ganim and Itzkowitz2009). This context-dependent social plasticity is crucial for the ecological and evolutionary consequences of personality dimensions and behavioral syndromes (Luttbeg & Sih, Reference Luttbeg and Sih2010), but has not commonly been addressed in the field of fish personality from the point of view of neuroscience.

Several syndromes have been proposed with aggression (Fig. 1). The aforementioned boldness-aggression syndromes were the first described across species (Bell, Reference Bell2005; Bell & Sih, Reference Bell and Sih2007; Huntingford, Reference Huntingford1976; Moretz et al., Reference Moretz, Martins and Robison2007), with an activity component sometimes being identified as well (Bell & Stamps, Reference Bell and Stamps2004; Dingemanse et al., Reference Dingemanse, Wright, Kazem, Thomas, Hickling and Dawnay2007). Other studies identified an aggression-exploration syndrome, despite calling these correlations an “aggression-boldness syndrome” (Norton et al., Reference Norton, Stumpenhorst, Faus-Kessler, Folchert, Rohner, Harris, Callebert and Bally-Cuif2011; Norton & Bally-Cuif, Reference Norton and Bally-Cuif2012). Metcalfe and Thorpe (Reference Metcalfe and Thorpe1992) found that, in Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar), earlier-feeding fry were dominant over their later-feeding siblings, leading to an increased probability of early-feeding fish migrating to sea 1 year earlier than their siblings. Similarly, in juvenile Atlantic salmon, a positive correlation between social status and standard metabolic rate was found, an effect that impacts the outcome of aggressive encounters (Metcalfe, Taylor & Thorpe., Reference Metcalfe, Taylor and Thorpe1995).

The aforementioned spiegeldanio zebrafish mutant sheds some light on the aggression-exploration syndrome (Mustafa, Roman, et al., Reference Mustafa, Roman and Winberg2019; Mustafa, Thörnqvist, Roman, & Winberg, Reference Mustafa, Thörnqvist, Roman and Winberg2019; Norton et al., Reference Norton, Stumpenhorst, Faus-Kessler, Folchert, Rohner, Harris, Callebert and Bally-Cuif2011; Norton & Bally-Cuif, Reference Norton and Bally-Cuif2012). As described above, spiegeldanio show increased aggressiveness in a mirror test (Mustafa, Thörnqvist, et al., Reference Mustafa, Thörnqvist, Roman and Winberg2019; Norton et al., Reference Norton, Stumpenhorst, Faus-Kessler, Folchert, Rohner, Harris, Callebert and Bally-Cuif2011), an effect that has been attributed to population-level differences in histaminergic signaling; this aggressiveness does not translate into more success in dyadic fights, however, as fgfr1a mutant fish did not have any advantage in fights for social dominance, and agonistic behavior of these mutants did not differ from that of AB fish during dyadic interactions (Mustafa, Thörnqvist, et al., Reference Mustafa, Thörnqvist, Roman and Winberg2019). Thus, the aggression-exploration syndrome of fgfr1a mutants is not associated with social plasticity, the ability to rapidly switch between behaviors in response to changing social conditions (Taborsky & Oliveira, Reference Taborsky and Oliveira2012). Interestingly, social plasticity has been associated with monoamines and nonapeptides, but not yet with histaminergic signaling (Maruska, Soares, Lima-Maximino, Henrique de Siqueira-Silva & Maximino, Reference Maruska, Soares, Lima-Maximino, Henrique de Siqueira-Silva and Maximino2019).

The neuroendocrine hypothalamus-pituitary-interrenal axis (the functional analog of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis of mammals) has been implicated in individual variation in aggressiveness. In sticklebacks, within-population variation in aggressiveness is negatively correlated with cortisol levels (Aubin-Horth et al., Reference Aubin-Horth, Deschênes and Cloutier2012). Interestingly, a boldness-aggression syndrome has been identified in this population, and both dimensions are positively correlated with brain expression of glucocorticoid receptors (Aubin-Horth et al., Reference Aubin-Horth, Deschênes and Cloutier2012). It is possible that the interplay between glucocorticoids and monoamines conjoins these syndromes.

2.5 Sociability

Sociability includes any interaction between two or more individuals, which can be positive (i. e. shoaling or cooperation) or negative (i. e. social avoidance or agonistic encounters). Studies using multidimensional statistics suggest that aggressiveness and sociability are separate personality dimensions in fish (Réale et al., Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007; Toms & Echevarria, Reference Toms and Echevarria2014). Social behavior varies between species, including large groups of fish forming a shoal to interactions between two individuals, between male and females aiming to breed or even between opponents fighting for resources (Maruska et al., Reference Maruska, Soares, Lima-Maximino, Henrique de Siqueira-Silva and Maximino2019). An important element in social behavior is communication; thus, sociality requires that individuals are exchanging information between them, not only sharing the same space.

Several adaptive functions of sociability can be pointed out, such as collective defense, collective searching for food patches, and mating (Taborsky & Oliveira, Reference Taborsky and Oliveira2012). Alliances established for territory and group defense are important features that guarantee survival and increase group safety in situations such as foraging in groups (Stenberg & Persson, Reference Stenberg and Persson2006) and caring for the young (Amundsen, Reference Amundsen2003; Bender, Heg-bachar, Oliveira, Canario & Taborsky, Reference Bender, Heg-bachar, Oliveira, Canario and Taborsky2008). Other benefits of social groups such as shoals may include vigilance and threat signaling by some individuals in favor of group survival (Clark & Dukas, Reference Clark and Dukas1994), dilution of risk (Ioannou, Bartumeus, Krause & Ruxton, Reference Ioannou, Bartumeus, Krause and Ruxton2011), and confusion effect or coordinated evasion that reduces predation (Krakauer, Reference Krakauer1995). Moreover, shoaling brings advantages in foraging when hunting in groups increases prey capture (Hintz & Lonzarich, Reference Hintz and Lonzarich2018). In this case, the size of the group plays a key role in success, as food will be shared with all individuals. Thus, a trade-off exists between the number of individuals foraging together and the competition for the resource (Rieucau, Fernö, Ioannou & Handegard, Reference Rieucau, Fernö, Ioannou and Handegard2015).

Other instances of social behavior, such as social recognition (Silveira, Silva, Ferreira & Luchiari, Reference Silveira, Silva, Ferreira and Luchiari2020) and cooperation (Pimentel et al., Reference Pimentel, Carvalho, Lima, Lima-Maximino, Soares and Maximino2019, Reference Pimentel, Lima-Maximino, Soares and Maximino2021), have been observed in fish. Nonetheless, most of the research on sociability as a personality dimension focuses on shoaling. In sticklebacks, bolder individuals engage in fewer social interactions than shy individuals, but create more diverse social networks (Pike et al., Reference Pike, Madhumita, Lindström and Royle2008). In guppies (Poecilia reticulata), shoaling tendency and boldness (both measured in the laboratory) predict the strength of a social network assessed in the field, with bolder individuals showing weaker social ties than shy individuals (Croft et al., Reference Croft, Krause and Darden2009). As is the case with guppies and sticklebacks, boldness-sociability syndromes were also described in zebrafish at the population level, with a negative correlation between these dimensions (Moretz et al., Reference Moretz, Martins and Robison2007).

Many different mechanisms have been studied in relation to sociability in general. Sexual hormones represent interesting starting points, as steroids have been shown to impact sociability, aggressiveness, boldness, and exploration by themselves (Bender et al., Reference Bender, Heg-bachar, Oliveira, Canario and Taborsky2008; Diotel et al., Reference Diotel, do Rego, Anglade, Vaillant, Pellegrini, Vaudry and Kah2011; Dzieweczynski, Eklund & Rowland, Reference Dzieweczynski, Eklund and Rowland2006; Ogawa, Pfaff & Parhar, Reference Ogawa, Pfaff and Parhar2021). Nonapeptides (isotocin-like and vasotocin-like) have also been implicated in those dimensions separately (Kawabata, Hiraki, Takeuchi & Okubo, Reference Kawabata, Hiraki, Takeuchi and Okubo2012; Larson, O’Malley & Melloni, Reference Larson, O’Malley and Melloni2006; Rose & Moore, Reference Rose and Moore2002; Santangelo & Bass, Reference Santangelo and Bass2010). Monoamines have also been implicated in both sociability and stress (Soares et al., Reference Soares, Gerlai and Maximino2018). Nonetheless, these mechanisms have not yet been investigated in depth as mechanisms for the correlations between these dimensions (i.e., behavioral syndromes).

3. The use of fish personality in anxiety research

The relationship between personality and psychopathology has been observed extensively in the history of the field. Theoretical models of personality (e.g., RST or Clark’s negative affect-positive affect-disinhibition model) have been developed that incorporate explicit references to psychopathology while simultaneously describing individual differences that can be understood as a continuum between “normal” and “pathological” behavior. While most of the work in fish personality has focused on describing either proximal mechanisms or evolutionary causes of individual differences, the amount of work in the field, as well as the continued appeal to use fish organisms in neuroscience research, can contribute to understanding the relationships between personality, brain mechanisms, and psychopathology. In what follows, we discuss how the five-dimensional model of boldness-exploration-activity-aggressiveness-sociality can be compared to models such as RST (and the difficulties of doing so).

Fish are increasingly being used as model organisms in behavioral neuroscience and experimental psychopathology. A full review of the methods and models that have been proposed using fish (especially D. rerio) fall beyond the scope of this article (but see Fontana et al., Reference Fontana, Francescon, Rosemberg, Norton, Kalueff and Parker2019; Abreu et al., Reference de Abreu, Genario, Giacomini, Demin, Lakstygal, Amstislavskaya, Fontana, Parker and Kalueff2020; War, Surendra, Paul, Sharma, Suresh & Manjunatha, Reference War, Surendra, Paul, Sharma, Suresh and Manjunatha2022, for recent reviews). Nonetheless, extensive work has been done on behavioral models of anxiety-like behavior in zebrafish (War et al., Reference War, Surendra, Paul, Sharma, Suresh and Manjunatha2022), including the now widely used novel tank test and the light-dark test (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Oliveira, Rosemberg, Batista, Herculano, Oliveira and Blaser2012). While potential points of contact have been proposed between research on zebrafish anxiety-like behavior and personality (e.g., Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Oliveira, Rosemberg, Batista, Herculano, Oliveira and Blaser2012), the full potential has not yet been reached.

The usefulness of fish models in personality neuroscience lies in producing an eco-ethological and evolutionary framework for personality research in general. As discussed in the present work, a lot of research using fish in this field focused on adaptive mechanisms for the evolution of personality differences across populations, as well as on the contexts which sustain these differences in currently existing populations (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Réale et al., Reference Réale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007; Weiss & Adams, Reference Weiss, Adams, Carere and Maestripieri2013). However, one difficulty remains: how does the five-dimension model of boldness/exploration/aggressiveness/sociability/activity map to models which are currently favored in non-fish research?

To understand the origin of this issue, one must return to the meta-theoretical considerations discussed at the beginning of this article. In order to provide an evolutionary framework to understand individual variation, population-level differences, and behavioral syndromes in fish, early researchers began with a bottom-up approach, describing exhaustively behavioral differences across contexts in different species. These descriptions eventually coalesced into a common vocabulary that was adopted by investigators that were researching other taxa, including mammals (Whitham & Washburn, Reference Whitham, Washburn, Vonk, Weiss and Kuczaj2017). This common vocabulary informed experiments and field observations, effectively initiating a top-down approach: researchers were now no longer cataloging behavioral variation, but doing so with the framework of the five-dimension model in mind. This led to observations on variations in a wide range of species and higher taxa, but distanced animal personality research from other models that are usually applied to common model organisms in neuroscience (mainly rodents and primates).

One of these theories is RST, which has been developed from Jeffrey Gray’s (Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2003) neuropsychology of anxiety (Corr, Reference Corr2002, Reference Corr2004; Corr & Perkins, Reference Corr and Perkins2006). RST views significant affective events as either positive or negative, postulating three interacting systems that process these events and control behavioral responses to them: the fight-flight-freeze system (FFFS), which mediates reactions to all aversive stimuli, with the associated emotion of fear; the behavioral approach system (BAS), which mediates reactions to all appetitive stimuli, with the associated emotion of anticipatory pleasure; and the behavioral inhibition system, responsible to solve conflicts between approach (BAS) and avoidance (FFFS). Thus, the three systems are associated with different emotions and also represent separate reinforcement sensitivities. These modules are associated with hierarchical brain systems which – at least in the case of the FFFS and the BAS – are highly conserved across vertebrates, including fish (do Carmo Silva, Lima-Maximino & Maximino, Reference do Carmo Silva, Lima-Maximino and Maximino2018; O’Connell & Hofmann, Reference O’Connell and Hofmann2012). Individual differences in the overall functioning of these systems are associated with fear-proneness and avoidance (FFFS); optimism, reward orientation, and impulsivity (BAS); and a combination of worry-proneness and anxiety (BIS) (Corr & Perkins, Reference Corr and Perkins2006). Thus, normal variation in personality would mean variation in the sensitivity in either of the separate modules and/or general modulatory (e.g., monoamines) influences on the overall system (Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr and McNaughton2012). It is important to understand that the general personality consequences of individual variation in the sensitivities of these systems are not the result of activity in a single system, but rather the joint sensitivities of the systems (Corr, Reference Corr2004; Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2003).

Attempts have been made to correlate aspects of RST with other psychobiological theories of personality, including Eysenck’s (Reference Eysenck1967) model: Eysenck’s Extraversion and Neuroticism dimensions would be derivative factors of punishment and reward sensitivities, with Extraversion reflecting the balance between both sensitivities and Neuroticism their joint strengths (Corr, Reference Corr2004). Empirical analyses of the relationship between RST and the five-factor model (“Big Five”) were also made in human subjects, suggesting that Sensitivity to Punishment is positively associated with Neuroticism and Agreeableness and negatively associated with Extraversion, Openness, and Conscientiousness; in contrast, Sensitivity to Reward (SR) was positively associated with Extraversion and Neuroticism, and negatively associated with Agreeableness and Conscientiousness (Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Kimbrel, Hundt, Cobb, Nelson-Gray and Lootens2007).

Likewise, we have previously attempted to map the five-dimension model of fish personality onto RST (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Oliveira, Rosemberg, Batista, Herculano, Oliveira and Blaser2012). We proposed that the shyness-boldness and exploration-avoidance axes could be understood as orthogonal to each other and resultant from individual variation in the joint sensitivities of reward and punishment. Thus, individuals higher on exploration and/or boldness would be more sensitive to rewards, while individuals in the opposite ends of the dimensions (higher on avoidance and/or shyness) would be more sensitive to punishment. Specifically, we predicted that, since the FFFS is more related to fear-proneness and avoidance, it would be more directly involved with shyness, while the BIS would be more involved with avoidance. Importantly, both dimensions are also influenced by reward sensitivity and, therefore, by the BAS.

It is currently not clear how other dimensions of fish personality could map on the other components of RST. One of the possible reasons is that RST proposes general systems that are well-suited to explain and model psychobiological processes underlying anxiety, but are too nonspecific to understand other important factors. For example, the BAS represents a general energizing effect that is not confounded with general arousal, and therefore unlikely to represent the activity dimension fully; moreover, while social decision-making certainly involves evaluating stimulus salience – therefore including and interacting with brain circuits involved in reward processing (O’Connell & Hofmann, Reference O’Connell and Hofmann2011, Reference O’Connell and Hofmann2012) – there are specific regions of the social brain network that are responsive to social stimuli, and thus, the BAS could not fully represent the sociability dimension as well. It is likely that aggressiveness, sociability, and activity are indirectly related to the RST systems, but other aspects of personality need to be invoked to fully translate RST and the five-dimensional model.

Indeed, as Smillie, Pickering and Jackson (Reference Smillie, Pickering and Jackson2006) suggested, “[e]xplanation of personality is a compelling by-product of RST, but a by-product nonetheless” (p. 321). RST was primarily concerned not with “anxiety” and “impulsivity” as descriptive personality dimensions, but as spectra of disorders and dispositions. Depue and Collins (Reference Depue and Collins1999) identify Gray’s (Reference Gray1981) critique of Eysenck’s arousal-activation model of personality as the beginning of RST as a personality theory that investigated the relationship between personality traits and “basic” processes of motivation and emotion.

Trying to harmonize RST and the five-dimensional model of fish personality is tempting because RST was developed strongly on rodent data, and therefore could “bridge” the translation of both human and fish data. Nonetheless, it is now clear that RST was never intended as a “complete” model of personality. To the best of our knowledge, no attempts have been made to map fish data and the Big Five model; it could be argued that shyness-boldness could conceptually map to Neuroticism, exploration-avoidance could conceptually map to Agreeableness, activity to Conscientiousness, aggressiveness to Extraversion, and sociability to Openness/Intellect. But it could be similarly argued that sociability should map to Extraversion, for example. The difficulty, again, lies at the meta-theoretical level and can be solved partially by addressing that level in conjunction with further empirical research.

One of the difficulties in translating most of the theoretical models of personality developed to understand human data (or, at best, mammalian data) is that direct analogies of behavior between species might not be straightforward or applicable (Trofimova et al., Reference Trofimova, Bajaj, Bashkatov, Blair, Brandt, Chan and Pickering2022). In part, this is due to over-reliance on looking for links between behavioral traits (i.e., “dimensions” in the sense used in this paper) and morphophysiological traits, especially in the rodent literature (McNaughton & Corr, Reference McNaughton and Corr2022). The studies reviewed in the present paper provide an evolutionary and comparative context for that and also tend to focus on the relationship between variation in a single behavioral dimension and variation in one (or a handful) biomarker or physiological trait. Some progress has been made in the direction of analyzing how biological variation is related to variation in the correlation between dimensions (a systems/domain interplay approach; Kalueff, Ren-Patterson, LaPorte & Murphy, Reference Kalueff, Ren-Patterson, LaPorte and Murphy2008), and, although many methodological and meta-theoretical issues remain, the current context – while not directly and easily translated to mammalian personality – offers great promise as comparative research. De Young (Reference De Young2010), for example, suggested that the traits in the Big Five model can be hierarchically organized in meta-traits of stability and plasticity and that these meta-traits are related to serotonergic and dopaminergic neurotransmission, respectively. While this is still speculative, this could represent a way of simultaneously looking for behavioral variation and neural basis while maintaining the meta-theoretical concerns at close. As can be seen in Table 1, for example, serotonin, dopamine, and histamine appear to participate in almost all dimensions, which can have several explanations: receptor-specific functions of these neurotransmitters, a participation of the neurotransmitter in the pleiotropic modulation of different personality dimensions (and therefore of their correlation at the individual level), or lack of specificity are possible hypotheses.

4. Conclusion

The present article reviewed research on fish personality dimensions, presenting a five-dimensional model and some of the caveats and limitations of the field as a whole. Using fish in personality research can contribute to understanding neurobehavioral correlates of personality; the extensive use of these animals to understand the evolutionary basis of personality holds great promise in providing a comparative, ecological, and evolutionary perspective for personality neuroscience. There are many meta-theoretical issues to be solved in the field, including how one defines personality; how different dimensions are measured; what is the unit of analysis; and how to best approach the identification of dimensions (i.e., through a bottom-up or a top-down approach, or a serial combination of both). These issues underline the importance of coherent initiatives in understanding and defining personality and impact the ability of the field to produce mechanistic research. For example, the interesting work on spiegeldanio (Norton et al., Reference Norton, Stumpenhorst, Faus-Kessler, Folchert, Rohner, Harris, Callebert and Bally-Cuif2011) reveals promising mechanisms, but differences were observed at the population level, not the individual level, and, while the authors claim to have observed a behavioral syndrome, the correlation between behaviors within populations was not assessed. As a result, the role of histaminergic signaling in an aggression-exploration syndrome remains an interesting theoretical possibility that needs to be further assessed using the tools of personality research.

While the field certainly needs to progress further, with researchers that are seeking proximal mechanisms needing to have a deeper understanding and contact with the ethological-ecological literature, and vice-versa, personality neuroscientists in general can also benefit from fish research. This is especially true given the extensive ethological research that attempted to define the ecological and evolutionary causes of individual differences. The apparent incompatibility between the five-dimensional model and other models of non-human personality may seem like an obstacle, but researchers from both the rodent/primate field and fish researchers can collaborate to harmonize datasets, explicitly stating the meta-theoretical issues that are usually left implicit. Thus, work is needed both within the field of fish personality and in concert with researchers working with rodents and primates to better understand continuities and discontinuities in vertebrate personality neuroscience.

Financial support

Part of the research reported in this manuscript was funded by the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq), the Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (CAPES), and the Serrapilheira Institute, Brazil (Grant number serra-1709–20 842) to ACL. CM was the recipient of a CNPq research productivity grant (grant number 302998/2019-5). The funding agencies had no role in the research strategy, data collection, discussions, or manuscript preparation.

Competing interests

None.

References

Abbey-Lee, R. N., Kreshchenko, A., Fernandez Sala, X., Petkova, I., & Løvlie, H. (2019). Effects of monoamine manipulations on the personality and gene expression of three-spined sticklebacks. Journal of Experimental Biology, 222(20), jeb211888. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.211888 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alfonso, S., Peyrafort, M., Cousin, X., & Bégout, M.-L. (2020). Zebrafish Danio rerio shows behavioural cross-context consistency at larval and juvenile stages but no consistency between stages. Journal of Fish Biology, 96, 14111421. https://doi.org/10.1111/jfb.14310 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allport, F. H., & Allport, G. W. (1921). Personality traits: Their classification and measurement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 16, 640. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0069790 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Álvarez, D., & Bell, A. M. (2007). Sticklebacks from streams are more bold than sticklebacks from ponds. Behavioural Processes, 76, 215217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2007.05.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amundsen, T. (2003). Fishes as models in studies of sexual selection and parental care. Journal of Fish Biology, 63, 1752. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.2003.00219.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aubin-Horth, N., Deschênes, M., & Cloutier, S. (2012). Natural variation in the molecular stress network correlates with a behavioural syndrome. Hormones & Behavior, 61, 140146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.11.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, A. M. (2005). Behavioural differences between individuals and two populations of stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 18, 464473. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00817.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, A. M. (2007). Animal personalities. Nature, 447, 539540. https://doi.org/10.1038/447539a CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, A. M., Backström, T., Huntingford, F. A., Pottinger, T. G., & Winberg, S. (2007). Variable neuroendocrine responses to ecologically-relevant challenges in sticklebacks. Physiology & Behavior, 91, 1525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, A. M., Henderson, L., & Huntingford, F. A. (2010). Behavioral and respiratory responses to stressors in multiple populations of three-spined sticklebacks that differ in predation pressure. Journal of Comparative Physiology. B, Biochemical, Systemic, and Environmental Physiology, 180, 211220. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00360-009-0395-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, A. M., & Sih, A. (2007). Exposure to predation generates personality in threespined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Ecology Letters, 10, 828834. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2007.01081.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, A. M., & Stamps, J. A. (2004). Development of behavioural differences between individuals and populations of sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Animal Behaviour, 68, 13391348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bender, N., Heg-bachar, Z., Oliveira, R. F., Canario, A. V. M., & Taborsky, M. (2008). Hormonal control of brood care and social status in a cichlid fish with brood care helpers. Steroids, 94, 349358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.02.002 Google Scholar
Binder, T. R., Wilson, A. D. M., Wilson, S. M., Suski, C. D., Godin, J.-G. J., & Cooke, S. J. (2016). Is there a pace-of-life syndrome linking boldness and metabolic capacity for locomotion in bluegill sunfish? Animal Behaviour, 121, 175183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.09.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biro, P. A., & Stamps, J. A. (2010). Do consistent individual differences in metabolic rate promote consistent individual differences in behavior? Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 25, 653659. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2010.08.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, C., & Braithwaite, V. A. (2004). Size matters: A test of boldness in eight populations of the poeciliid Brachyraphis episcopi. Animal Behaviour, 68, 13251329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.04.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, C., Burgess, F., & Braithwaite, V. A. (2007). Heritable and experiential effects on boldness in a tropical poeciliid. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 62, 237243. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-007-0458-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, C., Jones, F., & Braithwaite, V. (2005). In situ examination of boldness–shyness traits in the tropical poeciliid, Brachyraphis episcopi. Animal Behaviour, 70, 10031009. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.12.022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, C., Jones, F., & Braithwaite, V. A. (2007). Correlation between boldness and body mass in natural populations of the poeciliid Brachyrhaphis episcopi. Journal of Fish Biology, 71, 15901601. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.2007.01627.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Careau, V., Thomas, D., Humphries, M. M., & Réale, D. (2008). Energy metabolism and animal personality. Oikos, 641653. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0030-1299.2008.16513.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, A. J., Feeney, W. E., Marshall, H. H., Cowlishaw, G., & Heinsohn, R. (2013). Animal personality: What are behavioural ecologists measuring? Biological Reviews, 88, 465475. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castanheira, M. F., Cerqueira, M., Millot, S., Gonçalves, R. A., Oliveira, C. C. V., Conceição, L. E. C., & Martins, C. I. M. (2016). Are personality traits consistent in fish?—The influence of social context. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 178, 96101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.02.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castanheira, M. F., Herrera, M., Costas, B., Conceição, L. E. C., & Martins, C. I. M. (2013). Can we predict personality in fish? Searching for consistency over time and across contexts. PLOS ONE, 8, e62037. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cervone, D., Shadel, W. G., & Jencius, S. (2001). Social-cognitive theory of personality assessment. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 3351. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, C. W., & Dukas, R. (1994). Balancing foraging and antipredator demands: An advantage of sociality. American Naturalist, 144, 542548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coleman, K., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Shyness and boldness in pumpkinseed sunfish: Individual differences are context-specific. Animal Behaviour, 56, 927936. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1998.0852 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conrad, J. L., Weinersmith, K. L., Brodin, T., Saltz, J. B., & Sih, A. (2011). Behavioural syndromes in fishes: A review with implications for ecology and fisheries management. Journal of Fish Biology, 78, 395435. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.2010.02874.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corr, P. J. (2002). J. A. Gray’s reinforcement sensitivity theory: Tests of the joint subsystems hypothesis of anxiety and impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 511532. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-88690100170-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corr, P. J. (2004). Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 317332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corr, P. J., & McNaughton, N. (2012). Neuroscience and approach/avoidance personality traits: A two stage (valuation–motivation) approach. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 23392354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corr, P. J., & Perkins, A. M. (2006). The role of theory in the psychophysiology of personality: From Ivan Pavlov to Jeffrey Gray. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 62, 367376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.01.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, D. P., Krause, J., Darden, S. K. et al. (2009). Behavioural trait assortment in a social network: patterns and implications. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63, 14951503. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-009-0802-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Abreu, M. S., Genario, R., Giacomini, A. C. V. V., Demin, K. A., Lakstygal, A. M., Amstislavskaya, T. G., Fontana, B. D., Parker, M. O., & Kalueff, A. V. (2020). Zebrafish as a model of neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroscience, 445, 311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.08.034 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Young, C. G. (2010). Personality neuroscience and the biology of traits. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 11651180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 22, 491569. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002046 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dingemanse, N. J., Wright, J., Kazem, A. J. N., Thomas, D. K., Hickling, R., & Dawnay, N. (2007). Behavioural syndromes differ predictably between 12 populations of three-spined stickleback. The Journal of Animal Ecology, 76, 11281138. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01284.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diotel, N., do Rego, J.-L., Anglade, I., Vaillant, C., Pellegrini, E., Vaudry, H., & Kah, O. (2011). The brain of teleost fish, a source, and a target of sexual steroids. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5, 115. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2011.00137 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
do Carmo Silva, R. X., Lima-Maximino, M. G., & Maximino, C. (2018). The aversive brain system of teleosts: Implications for neuroscience and biological psychiatry. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 95, 123135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dzieweczynski, T. L., Eklund, A. C., & Rowland, W. J. (2006). Male 11-ketotestosterone levels change as a result of being watched in Siamese fighting fish. Steroids, 147, 184189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygcen.2005.12.023 Google ScholarPubMed
Edenbrow, M., & Croft, D. P. (2011). Behavioural types and life history strategies during ontogeny in the mangrove killifish, Kryptolebias marmoratus. Animal Behaviour, 82, 731741. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Piscataway, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Fernandes-Silva, B. W., Leite-Ferreira, M. E., Menezes, F. P., & Luchiari, A. C. (2022). Covariation among behavioral traits and hatching time in zebrafish. Behavioural Processes, 194, 104546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2021.104546 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, M. C. O., Sih, A., & Chivers, D. P. (2009). The paradox of risk allocation: A review and prospectus. Animal Behaviour, 78, 579585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.034 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrari, S., Horri, K., Allal, F., Vergnet, A., Benhaim, D., Vandeputte, M., Chatain, B., & Bégout, M.-L. (2016). Heritability of boldness and hypoxia avoidance in European Seabass, Dicentrarchus labrax . PLOS ONE, 11, e0168506. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168506 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filby, A. L., Paull, G. C., Hickmore, T. F., & Tyler, C. R. (2010). Unravelling the neurophysiological basis of aggression in a fish model. BMC Genomics, 11, 498. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2164-11-498 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fontana, B. D., Francescon, F., Rosemberg, D. B., Norton, W. H. J., Kalueff, A. V., & Parker, M. O. (2019). Zebrafish models for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 100, 918. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graeff, F. G. (2004). Serotonin, the periaqueductal gray and panic. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 239259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.12.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for personality, pp. 246–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2003). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198522713.001.0001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hintz, W. D., & Lonzarich, D. G. (2018). Maximizing foraging success: The roles of group size, predation risk, competition, and ontogeny. Ecosphere, 9, e02456. https://doi.org/10.1002/ecs2.2456 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, R. N. (1997). Intrinsic exploration in animals: Motives and measurement. Behavioural Processes, 41, 213226. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-63579700055-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huntingford, F. A. (1976). A comparison of the reaction of sticklebacks in different reproductive conditions towards conspecifics and predators. Animal Behaviour, 24, 694697. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-34727680083-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibarra-Zatarain, Z., Rey, S., Boglino, A., Fatsini, E., & Duncan, N. (2020). Senegalese sole (Solea senegalensis) coping styles are consistent over time: Behavioural and physiological responses during ontogenesis. Physiology & Behavior, 217, 112803. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112803 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ioannou, C. C., Bartumeus, F., Krause, J., & Ruxton, G. D. (2011). Unified effects of aggregation reveal larger prey groups take longer to find. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 29852990. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.0003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolles, J. W., Boogert, N. J., Sridhar, V. H., Couzin, I. D., & Manica, A. (2017). Consistent individual differences drive collective behavior and group functioning of schooling fish. Current Biology, 27, 28622868.e7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, M. I., & Müller, C. (2021). What is an animal personality? Biology & Philosophy, 36, 125. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-020-09776-w CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalueff, A. V., Echevarria, D. J., Homechaudhuri, S., Stewart, A. M., Collier, A. D., Kaluyeva, A. A., … Song, C. (2016). Zebrafish neurobehavioral phenomics for aquatic neuropharmacology and toxicology research. Aquatic Toxicology, 170, 297309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquatox.2015.08.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalueff, A. V., Ren-Patterson, R. F., LaPorte, J. L., & Murphy, D. L. (2008). Domain interplay concept in animal models of neuropsychiatric disorders: A new strategy for high-throughput neurophenotyping research. Behavioural Brain Research, 188, 243249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.11.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawabata, Y., Hiraki, T., Takeuchi, A., & Okubo, K. (2012). Sex differences in the expression of vasotocin/isotocin, gonadotropin-releasing hormone, and tyrosine and tryptophan hydroxylase family genes in the medaka brain. Neuroscience, 218, 6577. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.05.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, A. J., Fürtbauer, I., Mamuneas, D., James, C., & Manica, A. (2013). Sex-differences and temporal consistency in stickleback fish boldness. PLoS ONE, 8, e81116. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0081116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, S. M., De Boer, S. F., Van Der Vegt, B. J., Van Reenen, C. G., Hopster, H., De Jong, I. C., Ruis, M. A. W., & Blokhuis, H. J. (1999). Coping styles in animals: Current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 925935. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-76349900026-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Køppe, S. (2012). A moderate eclecticism: Ontological and epistemological issues. Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science, 46, 119. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-011-9175-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krakauer, D. C. (1995). Groups confuse predators by exploiting perceptual bottlenecks: A connectionist model of the confusion effect. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 36, 421429. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00177338 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kysil, E. V., Meshalkina, D. A., Frick, E. E., Echevarria, D. J., Rosemberg, D. B., Maximino, C., … Kalueff, A. V. (2017). Comparative analyses of zebrafish anxiety-like behavior using conflict-based novelty tests. Zebrafish, 14, 197208. https://doi.org/10.1089/zeb.2016.1415 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, E. T., O’Malley, D. M., & Melloni, R. H. (2006). Aggression and vasotocin are associated with dominant–subordinate relationships in zebrafish. Behavioural Brain Research, 167, 94102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2005.08.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leblond, C., & Reebs, S. G. (2006). Individual leadership and boldness in shoals of golden shiners (Notemigonus crysoleucas). Behaviour, 143, 12631280. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853906778691603 Google Scholar
Lee, J. S., & Bereijikian, B. A. (2008). Stability of behavioral syndromes but plasticity in individual behavior: Consequences for rockfish stock enhancement. Environmental Biology of Fishes, 82, 179186. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10641-007-9288-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lima-Maximino, M., Pyterson, M. P., Silva, R. X. do C., Gomes, G. C. V., Rocha, S. P., Herculano, A. M., … Maximino, C. (2020). Phasic and tonic serotonin modulate alarm reactions and post-exposure behavior in zebrafish. Journal of Neurochemistry, 153, 495509. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnc.14978 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luttbeg, B., & Sih, A. (2010). Risk, resources and state-dependent adaptive behavioural syndromes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Part B, 365, 39773990. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0207 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKay, J. R. D., & Haskell, M. J. (2015). Consistent individual behavioral variation: The difference between temperament, personality and behavioral syndromes. Animals, 5, Art. 3. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani5030366 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martins, E. P., & Bhat, A. (2014). Population-level personalities in zebrafish: Aggression-boldness across but not within populations. Behavioral Ecology, 25, 368373. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maruska, K., Soares, M. C., Lima-Maximino, M., Henrique de Siqueira-Silva, D., & Maximino, C. (2019). Social plasticity in the fish brain: Neuroscientific and ethological aspects. Brain Research, 1711, 156172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2019.01.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maximino, C., de Brito, T. M., da Silva Batista, A. W., Herculano, A. M., Morato, S., & Gouveia, A. (2010). Measuring anxiety in zebrafish: A critical review. Behavioural Brain Research, 214, 157171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.031 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maximino, C., Oliveira, D. L. de, Rosemberg, D. B., Batista, E. de J. O., Herculano, A. M., Oliveira, K. R. M., … Blaser, R. (2012). A comparison of the light/dark and novel tank tests in zebrafish. Behaviour, 149, 10991123. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003029 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maximino, C., Puty, B., Benzecry, R., Araújo, J., Lima, M. G., de Jesus Oliveira Batista, E., Renata de Matos Oliveira, K., Crespo-Lopez, M. E., & Herculano, A. M. (2013). Role of serotonin in zebrafish (Danio rerio) anxiety: Relationship with serotonin levels and effect of buspirone, WAY 100635, SB 224289, fluoxetine and para-chlorophenylalanine (pCPA) in two behavioral models. Neuropharmacology, 71, 8397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazué, G. P. F., Dechaume-Moncharmont, F.-X., & Godin, J.-G. J. (2015). Boldness–exploration behavioral syndrome: Interfamily variability and repeatability of personality traits in the young of the convict cichlid (Amatitlania siquia). Behavioral Ecology, 26, 900908. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arv030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNaughton, N., & Corr, P. J. (2022). The non-human perspective on the neurobiology of temperament, personality, and psychopathology: What’s next? Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 43, 255262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.11.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metcalfe, N. B., Taylor, A. C., & Thorpe, J. E. (1995). Metabolic rate, social status and life-history strategies in Atlantic salmon. Animal Behaviour, 49, 431436. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1995.0056 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metcalfe, N. B., & Thorpe, J. E. (1992). Early predictors of life-history events: The link between first feeding date, dominance and seaward migration in Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar L. Journal of Fish Biology, 41, 9399. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.1992.tb03871.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, J. T., Kimbrel, N. A., Hundt, N. E., Cobb, A. R., Nelson-Gray, R. O., & Lootens, C. M. (2007). An analysis of reinforcement sensitivity theory and the five-factor model. European Journal of Personality, 21, 869887. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.644 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, K. C. (1955). The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory drive. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 48, 254260. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043788 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, K. C., & Monkman, J. A. (1955). The relation between fear and exploratory behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 48, 132136. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048596 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moretz, J. A., Martins, E. P., & Robison, B. D. (2007). Behavioral syndromes and the evolution of correlated behavior in zebrafish. Behavioral Ecology, 18, 556562. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arm011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mustafa, A., Roman, E., & Winberg, S. (2019). Boldness in male and female zebrafish (Danio rerio) is dependent on strain and test. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 248. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00248 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mustafa, A., Thörnqvist, P.-O., Roman, E., & Winberg, S. (2019). The aggressive spiegeldanio, carrying a mutation in the fgfr1a gene, has no advantage in dyadic fights with zebrafish of the AB strain. Behavioural Brain Research, 370, 111942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.111942 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nespolo, R. F., & Franco, M. (2007). Whole-animal metabolic rate is a repeatable trait: A meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Biology, 210, 20002005. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.02780 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nomakuchi, S., Park, P. J., & Bell, M. A. (2009). Correlation between exploration activity and use of social information in three-spined sticklebacks. Behavioral Ecology, 20, 340345. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arp001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norton, W. H. J., & Bally-Cuif, L. (2012). Unravelling the proximate causes of the aggression-boldness behavioural syndrome in zebrafish. Behaviour, 149, 10631079. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norton, W. H. J., Stumpenhorst, K., Faus-Kessler, T., Folchert, A., Rohner, N., Harris, M. P., Callebert, J., & Bally-Cuif, L. (2011). Modulation of Fgfr1a signaling in Zebrafish reveals a genetic basis for the aggression–boldness syndrome. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1379613807. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2892-11.2011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connell, L. A., & Hofmann, H. A. (2011). The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: A comparative synthesis. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 519, 35993639. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22735 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connell, L. A., & Hofmann, H. A. (2012). Evolution of a vertebrate social decision-making network. Science, 336, 11541157. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1218889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogawa, S., Pfaff, D. W., & Parhar, I. S. (2021). Fish as a model in social neuroscience: Conservation and diversity in the social brain network. Biological Reviews, 96, 9991020. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12689 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliveira, R. F. (2012). Social plasticity in fish: Integrating mechanisms and function. Journal of Fish Biology, 81, 21272150. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.2012.03477.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olivier, B. (2004). Serotonin and aggression. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1036, 382392. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1330.022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Øverli, Ø., Sørensen, C., Pulman, K. G. T., Pottinger, T. G., Korzan, W., Summers, C. H., & Nilsson, G. E. (2007). Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles: Relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 31, 396412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, E. D., Johnson, P. L., Shekhar, A., & Lowry, C. A. (2014). The Deakin/Graeff hypothesis: Focus on serotonergic inhibition of panic. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 46, 379396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, E. D., & Lowry, C. A. (2013). Functional topography of serotonergic systems supports the Deakin/Graeff hypothesis of anxiety and affective disorders. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 27, 10901106. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881113490328 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pervin, L. A. (1994). A critical analysis of current trait theory. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 103113. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0502_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philip, J., Dellinger, M., & Benhaïm, D. (2022). Among-individual variation of risk-taking behaviour in group and solitary context is uncorrelated but independently repeatable in a juvenile Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus) aquaculture strain. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 249, 105596. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2022.105596 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pike, T. W., Madhumita, S., Lindström, J., & Royle, N. J. (2008). Behavioural phenotype affects social interactions in an animal network. Proceedings of the Royal Society B., 275, 25152520. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0744 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pimentel, A. F. N., Carvalho, T. dos S., Lima, F., Lima-Maximino, M., Soares, M. C., & Maximino, C. (2019). Conditional approach as cooperation in predator inspection: A role for serotonin? Behavioural Brain Research, 365, 164169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.03.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pimentel, A. F. N., Lima-Maximino, M. G., Soares, M. C., & Maximino, C. (2021). Zebrafish cooperate while inspecting predators: Experimental evidence for conditional approach. Animal Behaviour, 177, 5968. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.04.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polverino, G., Cigliano, C., Nakayama, S., & Mehner, T. (2016). Emergence and development of personality over the ontogeny of fish in absence of environmental stress factors. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 70, 20272037. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-016-2206-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Réale, D., Garant, D., Humphries, M. M., Bergeron, P., Careau, V., & Montiglio, P.-O. (2010). Personality and the emergence of the pace-of-life syndrome concept at the population level. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365, 40514063. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0208 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Réale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T., & Dingemanse, N. J. (2007). Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews, 82, 291318. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rieucau, G., Fernö, A., Ioannou, C. C., & Handegard, N. O. (2015). Towards of a firmer explanation of large shoal formation, maintenance and collective reactions in marine fish. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries, 25, 2137. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11160-014-9367-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, J. D., & Moore, F. L. (2002). Behavioral neuroendocrinology of vasotocin and vasopressin and the sensorimotor processing hypothesis. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 23, 317341. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0091-30220200004-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy, T., & Bhat, A. (2018a). Population, sex and body size: Determinants of behavioural variations and behavioural correlations among wild zebrafish Danio rerio. Royal Society Open Science, 5, 170978. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.170978 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy, T., & Bhat, A. (2018b). Repeatability in boldness and aggression among wild zebrafish (Danio rerio) from two differing predation and flow regimes. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 132, 349360. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000150 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, P. A. (1973). Relationships between exploratory behaviour and fear: A review. British Journal of Psychology, 64, 417433. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01369.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez-Tójar, A., Moiron, M., & Niemelä, P. T. (2022). Terminology use in animal personality research: A self-report questionnaire and a systematic review. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 289, 20212259. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.2259 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santangelo, N., & Bass, A. H. (2010). Individual behavioral and neuronal phenotypes for arginine vasotocin mediated courtship and aggression in a territorial teleost. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 75, 282291. https://doi.org/10.1159/000316867 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuett, W., Tregenza, T., & Dall, S. R. X. (2010). Sexual selection and animal personality. Biological Reviews, 85, 217246. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2009.00101.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sih, A., Bell, A. M., & Johnson, J. C. (2004). Behavioral syndromes: An ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 19, 372378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2004.04.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sih, A., Bell, A. M., Johnson, J. C., & Ziemba, R. E. (2004). Behavioral syndromes: An integrative overview. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 79, 241277. https://doi.org/10.1086/422893 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silveira, M. M., Silva, P. F., Ferreira, R. G., & Luchiari, A. C. (2020). Fighting off the intruder: Context-dependent territory defence in the damselfish Stegastes fuscus. Environmental Biology of Fishes, 103, 10911104. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10641-020-01011-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smillie, L. D., Pickering, A. D., & Jackson, C. J. (2006). The new reinforcement sensitivity theory: Implications for personality measurement. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 320335. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snekser, J. L., Leese, J., Ganim, A., & Itzkowitz, M. (2009). Caribbean damselfish with varying territory quality: Correlated behaviors but not a syndrome. Behavioral Ecology, 20, 124130. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn123 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soares, M. C., Gerlai, R., & Maximino, C. (2018). The integration of sociality, monoamines and stress neuroendocrinology in fish models: applications in the neurosciences. Journal of Fish Biology, 93: 170191. https://doi.org/10.1111/jfb.13757 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stamps, J., & Groothuis, T. G. G. (2010). The development of animal personality: Relevance, concepts and perspectives. Biological Reviews, 85, 301325. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2009.00103.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stamps, J. A., & Groothuis, T. G. G. (2010). Developmental perspectives on personality: Implications for ecological and evolutionary studies of individual differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365, 40294041. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0218 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stenberg, M., & Persson, A. (2006). Patch use behaviour in benthic fish depends on their long-term growth prospects. Oikos, 112, 332341. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0030-1299.2006.13459.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taborsky, B., & Oliveira, R. F. (2012). Social competence: An evolutionary approach. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 27, 679688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2012.09.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toms, C. N., & Echevarria, D. J. (2014). Back to basics: Searching for a comprehensive framework for exploring individual differences in zebrafish (Danio rerio) Behavior. Zebrafish, 11, 325340. https://doi.org/10.1089/zeb.2013.0952 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toms, C. N., Echevarria, D. J., & Jouandot, D. J. (2010). A methodological review of personality-related studies in fish: Focus on the shy-bold axis of behavior. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 125. https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2010.23.01.08 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trofimova, I., Bajaj, S., Bashkatov, S. A., Blair, J., Brandt, A., Chan, R. C. K., … Pickering, A. D. (2022). What is next for the neurobiology of temperament, personality and psychopathology? Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 45, 101143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101143 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uher, J. (2008). Three methodological core issues of comparative personality research. European Journal of Personality, 22, 475496. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.688 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uher, J. (2011). Individual behavioral phenotypes: An integrative meta-theoretical framework. Why “behavioral syndromes” are not analogs of “personality”. Developmental Psychobiology, 53, 521548. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20544 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uher, J. (2013). Personality psychology: Lexical approaches, assessment methods, and trait concepts reveal only half of the story—why it is time for a paradigm shift. Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science, 47, 155. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-013-9230-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uher, J. (2015). Conceiving “personality”: Psychologist’s challenges and basic fundamentals of the transdisciplinary philosophy-of-science paradigm for research on individuals. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 49, 398458. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-014-9283-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uher, J., Werner, C. S., & Gosselt, K. (2013). From observations of individual behaviour to social representations of personality: Developmental pathways, attribution biases, and limitations of questionnaire methods. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 647667. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.03.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
War, R., Surendra, V., Paul, S., Sharma, U. R., Suresh, J., & Manjunatha, P. M. (2022). Zebrafish as an emerging alternative tool for studying anxiety disorders. Journal of Advanced Scientific Research, 13, 1320. https://doi.org/10.55218/JASR.2022131103 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, A. (2018). Personality traits: A view from the animal kingdom. Journal of Personality, 86, 1222. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12310 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, A., & Adams, M. J. (2013). Differential behavioral ecology: The structure, life history, and evolution of primate personality. In Carere, E. C. & Maestripieri, D. (Orgs.), Animal personalities: Behavior, physiology and evolution: University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226922065.003.0005 Google Scholar
Whitham, W., & Washburn, D. A. (2017). A history of animal personality research. In Vonk, E. J., Weiss, A., & Kuczaj, S. A. (Orgs.), Personality in nonhuman animals (pp. 316): Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-59300-5_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Five-dimensional model of fish personality and observed or inferred correlations between dimensions. Blue arrows denote positive correlations, while red arrows denote negative correlations. The “classical” approach in fish personality research is to look for mechanisms in single dimensions, while a “system” approach involves looking for mechanisms for the interplay between dimensions (i.e., for behavioral syndromes). Abbreviations: ExpAv: Exploration-avoidance; Bold: Shyness-boldness; Act: Activity; Soc: Sociability; Agr: Aggressiveness.

Figure 1

Table 1. Summary of behavioral tests commonly used to measure each dimension of fish personality. Note that some tests can measure more than one dimension