Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T06:34:04.348Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders: A Global Perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 December 2017

Rowan Saloner
Affiliation:
The HIV Neurobehavioral Research Program (HNRP), Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego State University/University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California
Lucette A. Cysique*
Affiliation:
School of Medical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, The University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW Neuroscience Research Australia, Barker Street, Randwick, NSW Neuroscience Program and Peter Duncan Neurosciences Unit St. Vincent’s Hospital Centre for Applied Medical Research Centre, and departments of Neurology and HIV St. Vincent’s Hospital Sydney, NSW
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Lucette A. Cysique, Neuroscience Research Australia, 139 Barker Street Randwick, NSW. Ph: +61 2 9399 1880. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The present review on HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) provides a worldwide overview of studies that have investigated the rate and neuropsychological (NP) profile of HAND research since the inception of the 2007 HAND diagnostic nomenclature. In the first part, the review highlights some of the current controversies around HAND prevalence rates. In the second part, the review critically assesses some solutions to move the field forward. In the third part, we present the cross-sectional NP profile in non-Western HIV+ cohorts and in relation to Western cohorts’ findings. The adopted global perspective highlights the successful expansion of NP studies in HIV infection to culturally diverse low- to medium-income countries with high HIV burden. These studies have produced interestingly similar rates of HAND whether patients were naïve or treated and/or virally suppressed compared to the rich income countries where the NP research in NeuroHIV has originated. The perspective also demonstrates that globally, the group which is the most representative of the HIV epidemic, and thus at risk for HAND are persons with chronic HIV infection and survivors of past immunosuppression, while in relative terms, those who have been treated early with long-term viral suppression represent a minority. In the last part, we present a review of the naturalistic longitudinal NP global studies in HIV+cohorts, discuss the role of longitudinal design in solving issues around the question of asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment, and the question of biomarker discovery. Finally, we conclude by calling for greater methods and data harmonization at a global level. (JINS, 2017, 23, 860–869)

Type
Section 3 – Neuropsychiatric Disorders
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2017

INTRODUCTION

Thirty years after the earliest report of HIV-related neurocognitive complications (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Atkinson, Hesselink, Kennedy, Richman, Spector and McCutchan1987), and despite major changes in the clinical presentation of the disease since the introduction of combined antiretroviral therapy (cART), the neurological complications of HIV-infection remain significant, especially when we consider disease burden at a global level.

There is now a consensus that the severity of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) neuropsychological (NP) profile is milder based primarily on U.S. and Australian data (Cysique, Maruff, & Brew, Reference Cysique, Maruff and Brew2004; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Franklin, Ellis, McCutchan, Letendre, Leblanc and Grant2011), and more recently on global data as reviewed further below. Epidemiological figures show that HIV-associated dementia (HAD) is now relatively rare (2–4%) (McArthur, Reference McArthur2004). In contrast, milder forms of the disease (mild to moderate level of global neurocognitive impairment not severe enough to be characterized as dementia) remain fairly common (prevalence varies between 30% without AIDS and 50% in HIV+ adults with current/historical AIDS and in the context of no overt NP confounds) (Cysique et al., Reference Cysique, Heaton, Kamminga, Lane, Gates, Moore and Brew2014; Heaton, Clifford, et al., Reference Heaton, Clifford, Franklin, Woods, Ake, Vaida and Grant2010; Munoz-Moreno et al., Reference Munoz-Moreno, Perez-Alvarez, Munoz-Murillo, Prats, Garolera, Jurado and Clotet2014; Robertson et al., Reference Robertson, Bayon, Molina, McNamara, Resch, Munoz-Moreno and van Wyk2014; Robertson, Smurzynski, et al., Reference Robertson, Smurzynski, Parsons, Wu, Bosch, Wu and Ellis2007; Sacktor et al., Reference Sacktor, Skolasky, Seaberg, Munro, Becker, Martin and Miller2016; Tozzi et al., Reference Tozzi, Balestra, Lorenzini, Bellagamba, Galgani, Corpolongo and Narciso2005).

In stably virally suppressed cohorts or HIV-infected (HIV+) individuals treated early, the HAND prevalence rate varies between 20% and 30% (Bloch et al., Reference Bloch, Kamminga, Jayewardene, Bailey, Carberry, Vincent and Cysique2016; Crum-Cianflone et al., Reference Crum-Cianflone, Moore, Letendre, Poehlman Roediger, Eberly, Weintrob and Hale2013; De Francesco et al., Reference De Francesco, Underwood, Post, Vera, Williams, Boffito and Sabin2016; McDonnell et al., Reference McDonnell, Haddow, Daskalopoulou, Lampe, Speakman, Gilson and Rodger2014; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Grund, Cysique, Robertson, Brew, Collins and Price2015). There is, however, some debate concerning these results because two studies have found no significant difference between impairment rates in virally suppressed cases compared to their HIV- counterparts (Crum-Cianflone et al., Reference Crum-Cianflone, Moore, Letendre, Poehlman Roediger, Eberly, Weintrob and Hale2013; McDonnell et al., Reference McDonnell, Haddow, Daskalopoulou, Lampe, Speakman, Gilson and Rodger2014). Also, one outlier study found a HAND prevalence rate of >70% in virally suppressed HIV+ individuals with no NP confounds (Simioni et al., Reference Simioni, Cavassini, Annoni, Rimbault Abraham, Bourquin, Schiffer and Du Pasquier2010).

When considering this pool of studies in the United States, Western Europe, and Australia, we can unequivocally observe that the outlier studies are in the minority (whether they detected low and non-significantly different impairment rates compared to controls or high impairment rates in their HIV+ cohorts), yet their impact has been major. Inexplicably, some of these outlier studies have received more attention outside the NeuroHIV field as proof that HAND may not actually exist and that most of the impairment detected is first and foremost due to alcohol/drug use, uncontrolled neurological or psychiatric disorders, low education or being poor, female, and black. This interpretation, however, concentrates on the studies conducted in the Western centers. By taking a more global perspective, the review will highlight a more nuanced interpretation.

This global perspective will help the reader to form a critical overview of the current debate around prevalence rates and possible solutions. The global perspective also highlights one of the major achievements of the NeuroHIV research community: the successful expansion of NP studies in HIV infection to culturally diverse low- to medium-income countries with high HIV burden. This expansion has demonstrated that globally, the majority of persons with HIV infection are survivors of past immunosuppression, as opposed to early treated virally suppressed persons. Although this situation will change in the future, historically immune-suppressed persons are those who are currently aging, while many of their counterparts have died of AIDS with or without HAD. We are, therefore, currently studying a majority of survivors, who have withstood years of HIV-associated biological insults, with major potential impact on prevalence, incidence and severity and related biomarker research. Early treated patients are younger, but as they age, they could be representative of “less biologically resilient” individuals.

PREVALENCE CONTROVERSIES: ISSUES OF DIAGNOSTIC ACCURACY

Part of the validity of the Frascati HAND diagnostic criteria (Antinori et al., Reference Antinori, Arendt, Becker, Brew, Byrd, Cherner and Wojna2007) has been recently challenged (Gisslen, Price, & Nilsson, Reference Gisslen, Price and Nilsson2011; Nightingale et al., Reference Nightingale, Winston, Letendre, Michael, McArthur, Khoo and Solomon2014). While there is general agreement that NP testing is probably accurate in determining moderate to severe forms of HAND, researchers disagree over the criterion validity of NP testing in accurately identifying a clinically meaningful level of mild neurocognitive impairement, and in delineating asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment (ANI) from impairment resulting from confounding factors. Because of this, several studies have resorted to only including mild neurocognitive disorder (MND) and HAD cases, which can be useful at the level of primary care (McCombe, Vivithanaporn, Gill, & Power, Reference McCombe, Vivithanaporn, Gill and Power2013). But for biomarker discovery and prognostic value, the a priori exclusion of ANI is an issue, as this mild level of HAND is predictive of further cognitive decline (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Franklin, Deutsch, Woods, Vaida, Ellis and Heaton2014; Sacktor et al., Reference Sacktor, Skolasky, Seaberg, Munro, Becker, Martin and Miller2016).

There is empirical evidence that the Frascati criteria recommending at least a minus one standard deviation (−1 SD) on two cognitive domains, or 16% low NP performance threshold in an HIV- control sample optimally balances specificity and sensitivity for mild HAND detection (Taylor & Heaton, Reference Taylor and Heaton2001). Yet researchers who focus on this number at face value interpret that HAND rates are systematically over-estimated (Gisslen et al., Reference Gisslen, Price and Nilsson2011; Nightingale et al., Reference Nightingale, Winston, Letendre, Michael, McArthur, Khoo and Solomon2014). While some researchers contend that a -1 SD, as opposed to a proposed −1.5 or −2 SD, impairment threshold reflects the influence of confounding factors (e.g., psychiatric disorders that are common in HIV population) on rates of ANI, proponents of the −1 SD threshold point to data demonstrating relationships between objective markers of HIV brain involvement (e.g., neuropathology), and mild NP impairment at −1 SD (Cherner et al., 2002; Masliah et al., 1997; Moore et al., 2006).

Furthermore, there is substantial cumulative evidence that a cut-off at −1.5 SD or −2 SD will indisputably miss patients who have disease (Cherner et al., 2007; Grant et al., Reference Grant, Franklin, Deutsch, Woods, Vaida, Ellis and Heaton2014; Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Sheppard, Kordovski, Faytell, Avci and Woods2017). Importantly, low NP performance in an HIV- sample is not the exact equivalent of impaired NP performance in a clinical sample. A recent review (Gates & Cysique, Reference Gates and Cysique2016) has attempted to clarify this issue where it was demonstrated that the recommended −1 SD in two cognitive domains low NP performance threshold in an HIV- control sample yielded a 5% misclassification rate for HAND in the demographically comparable HIV+ sample, and not a 16% rate. This result is associated with two factors: higher correlations between NP tests measures as a function of greater impairment, and the selection of test batteries that focus on detecting HIV-related cognitive impairment but not general cognitive impairment.

Adding to the misunderstanding is the fact that some researchers have computed the Frascati criteria incorrectly when conducting comparisons between the Frascati criteria and potential new ones (Su et al., Reference Su, Schouten, Geurtsen, Wit, Stolte, Prins and Schmand2015). Computational errors may also explain why some studies found unusual discrepancies between the cognitive rating and the global deficit score methods (McDonnell et al., Reference McDonnell, Haddow, Daskalopoulou, Lampe, Speakman, Gilson and Rodger2014), despite the virtual equivalence between the two methods (Blackstone, Moore, Franklin, et al., Reference Blackstone, Moore, Franklin, Clifford, Collier, Marra and Heaton2012). Furthermore, in several instances, researchers assessing the robustness of the Frascati criteria in small test batteries have not modified—as required in this context (Ingraham & Aiken, Reference Ingraham and Aiken1996; Kamminga et al., Reference Kamminga, Bloch, Vincent, Carberry, Brew and Cysique2017) —the expected low performance rate in their HIV- control sample to meet the recommended 16% low-performance threshold. Failure to modify the expected threshold for impairment in small batteries by either using a stricter cut-off (Kamminga et al., Reference Kamminga, Bloch, Vincent, Carberry, Brew and Cysique2017), or/and requiring impairment in only one task (Ingraham & Aiken, Reference Ingraham and Aiken1996) will produce unexpectedly high rates of low performance in HIV- (McDonnell et al., Reference McDonnell, Haddow, Daskalopoulou, Lampe, Speakman, Gilson and Rodger2014). Finally, some reports have not used appropriate normative data (Simioni et al., Reference Simioni, Cavassini, Annoni, Rimbault Abraham, Bourquin, Schiffer and Du Pasquier2010) or have used control samples that diverged substantially from clinical samples in terms of some socio-demographics (McDonnell et al., Reference McDonnell, Haddow, Daskalopoulou, Lampe, Speakman, Gilson and Rodger2014).

PREVALENCE CONTROVERSIES: THE ROLE OF NP CONFOUNDS ON HAND PREVALENCE

The HIV epidemic is characterized by situations that make the detection of HAND in very specific populations or demographic categories challenging. For example, the study by Crum-Cianflone et al. (Reference Crum-Cianflone, Moore, Letendre, Poehlman Roediger, Eberly, Weintrob and Hale2013) evaluated NP functions in 200 HIV+ and 50 demographically comparable HIV- military beneficiaries. The HIV+ sample was relatively healthy (HIV duration=5 years; 64% on cART initiated; median CD4 of 333 cells/mm3). The overall impairment rate was found to be 19% in the HIV+ group and surprisingly 30% in the HIV- group. While the level of impairment in the HIV+ group is akin to another study in patients with high CD4 cell counts and cART initiation always above CD4 of 350 cells/mm3 (despite different methods it was 20%) (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Grund, Cysique, Robertson, Brew, Collins and Price2015), the rate of impairment in the HIV- sample was very unusual and akin to a clinical population with a history of traumatic brain injury (Rabinowitz et al., Reference Rabinowitz, Li, McCauley, Wilde, Barnes, Hanten and Levin2015), condition that is prevalent in the U.S. military (Helmick et al., Reference Helmick, Spells, Malik, Davies, Marion and Hinds2015).

The largest ever study of HIV- and HIV+ women (Maki et al., Reference Maki, Rubin, Valcour, Martin, Crystal, Young and Anastos2015) showed an overall small effect of HIV on cognition (demographically adjusted Cohen’s d<.20 ) in a cohort composed of HIV+ women where 30% had AIDS, 53% were virally undetectable and 35% were sub-optimally adherent. In relative terms, the effect of reading achievement, age, years of education, and racial/ethnicity category had a much larger explanatory value with respect to NP performance. However, the effect of HIV disease biomarkers (viral detection, lower CD4 and AIDS) on cognition was detected yet masked at the group level due to a dramatic combination of socio-economic burdens (poverty, childhood trauma, domestic violence) that negatively and independently impacted cognition in both HIV- and HIV+ women, particularly for learning, memory, and psychomotor speed (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Pyra, Cook, Weber, Cohen, Martin and Maki2016). Interestingly, the longitudinal follow-up of this cohort can be found in Rubin et al., (Reference Rubin, Maki, Springer, Benning, Anastos, Gustafson and Valcour2017). An editorial highlighted the main finding which was that cognitive decline was greater in HIV+ women whether virally suppressed or not compared to their HIV- counterparts and above and beyond the effect of confounds (Cysique & Becker, Reference Cysique and Becker2017).

THEMES TO CONSIDER IN THE POTENTIAL UPDATE OF THE FRASCATI CRITERIA

New indexes may be considered for improving the detection of clinically meaningful ANI/MND, but they will need further study to assess their validity. Investigators of the POPPY studies have proposed other statistical indexes. However, this work is only based on the CogState battery (De Francesco et al., Reference De Francesco, Underwood, Post, Vera, Williams, Boffito and Sabin2016). Any CogState cognitive domain grouping would require some validation as this battery is different from standard NP testing (Cysique, Maruff, Darby, & Brew, Reference Cysique, Maruff, Darby and Brew2006; Kamminga et al., Reference Kamminga, Bloch, Vincent, Carberry, Brew and Cysique2017), while a global score as opposed to cognitive domain ratings is best to quantify impairment when using this battery (Kamminga et al., Reference Kamminga, Bloch, Vincent, Carberry, Brew and Cysique2017). One proposed method (also used by Su et al., Reference Su, Schouten, Geurtsen, Wit, Stolte, Prins and Schmand2015) is the multivariate normative comparison (MNC), which has been developed to control for family-wise error in large NP test batteries (Huizenga, Agelink van Rentergem, Grasman, Muslimovic, & Schmand, Reference Huizenga, Agelink van Rentergem, Grasman, Muslimovic and Schmand2016). Recent improvement in the MNC method shows that individual cases need to be compared to normal age, education and gender matched cases, and not the entire normative sample (Agelink van Rentergem, Murre, & Huizenga, Reference Agelink van Rentergem, Murre and Huizenga2017). To the best of our knowledge, this requirement has not been conducted in NeuroHIV studies which used this method. Also, the MNC validity to detect mild cognitive impairment in non-HIV populations has not been tested. The other method propsed by POPPY investigators (Underwood, Leech, Winston, Sabin, & De Francesco, Reference Underwood, Leech, Winston, Sabin and De Francesco2017) is the Malahanobis distance (Crawford & Allan, Reference Crawford and Allan1994), which a priori takes into consideration the test correlation within a test battery while the Frascati criteria do not, and the Global Deficit Score [GDS, (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Woods, Gonzalez, Conover, Marcotte, Grant and Heaton2004)] does, but not to the same extent (Gates & Cysique, Reference Gates and Cysique2016). Similar to the GDS, however, the Malanobis distance needs to be thresholded, so the issue of how to accurately detect clinically meaningful forms of mild cognitive impairment remains.

The exclusion of neuropsychiatric symptoms from the Frascati criteria is problematic because it is well known that alteration to striato-frontal circuits affect emotional regulation and motivation (Cole, Castellon, et al., Reference Cole, Castellon, Perkins, Ureno, Robinet, Reinhard and Hinkin2007). Therefore, some neuropsychiatric symptoms prevalent in HIV+ persons, including apathy, irritability (Watkins & Treisman, Reference Watkins and Treisman2012), anxiety (Brandt et al., Reference Brandt, Zvolensky, Woods, Gonzalez, Safren and O’Cleirigh2017), possible treatment-resistant depression (Cysique, Dermody, Carr, Brew, & Teesson, Reference Cysique, Dermody, Carr, Brew and Teesson2016), could represent progressing HIV-related brain injury (Cysique & Brew, in press). Furthermore, in women and ethnically diverse cohorts, it appears that major depressive disorder is more often associated with HAND (Fellows, Byrd, Morgello, & Manhattan, Reference Fellows, Byrd and Morgello2013), and is more often predictive of cognitive decline (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Franklin, Deutsch, Letendre, Ellis, Casaletto and Grant2015) than in less diverse male cohorts (Cysique et al., Reference Cysique, Deutsch, Atkinson, Young, Marcotte, Dawson and Heaton2007). Such subtle divergences in the expression of a neurological and neuropsychiatric disease should be expected and not systematically interpreted as evidence that “ethnic minorities and women are a problem” or that their disease expression is not the “real” one. Historically, symptoms that were later accepted as part of the CDC AIDS definitions were not initially recognized in African-American women back in the 70s because their symptoms did not correspond to the typical presentation of AIDS in men (The ACT UP/New York Women and AIDS Book Group, 1990). Further illustrating this complexity, a recent study from Nigeria showed that higher plasma levels of HIV driving activation of circulating monocytes may be more explanatory of HAND in women than in men (Royal et al., Reference Royal, Cherner, Burdo, Umlauf, Letendre, Jumare and Blattner2016).

In a potentially new formulation of the Frascati criteria, the question of impact on everyday functioning would have to be considered carefully. The assessment of current everyday functioning primarily relies on patients’ self-insight (Chiao et al., Reference Chiao, Rosen, Nicolas, Wendelken, Alcantar, Rankin and Valcour2013). This is less of an issue in high-functioning persons with mild HAND, who show correlation between insight into their cognitive difficulties and their actual level of cognitive impairment (Cysique et al., Reference Cysique, Heaton, Kamminga, Lane, Gates, Moore and Brew2014); although depressed HIV+ patients typically report more cognitive symptoms than non-depressed patients, and sometimes without any evidence of objective cognitive deficits (Carter, Rourke, Murji, Shore, & Rourke, Reference Carter, Rourke, Murji, Shore and Rourke2003).

However, as soon as the impairment is moderate, especially in a fronto-striatal disease such as HAND (Thames et al., Reference Thames, Becker, Marcotte, Hines, Foley, Ramezani and Hinkin2011), insight can be impaired enough that the self-assessment of everyday functioning is likely to be inaccurate (Blackstone, Moore, Heaton, et al., Reference Blackstone, Moore, Heaton, Franklin, Woods, Clifford and Grant2012). Therefore, modifying the diagnostic Frascati criteria to require impact on everyday functioning as in the current DSM-5 criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) could have negative consequences by over-representing depressed, while those with lower self-insight and frontal symptoms would be systematically excluded. Recent advances have seen the development of several performance-based activities of daily living assessments which overcomes the issue of self-report (Blackstone, Moore, Heaton, et al., Reference Blackstone, Moore, Heaton, Franklin, Woods, Clifford and Grant2012; Sheppard, Woods, & Verduzco, Reference Sheppard, Woods and Verduzco2017), including some that are Internet based (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Iudicello, Morgan, Verduzco, Smith and Cushman2017). More work is needed to assess their construct validity as real-life indicators independent of cognitive functioning.

Further improvement in diagnostic validity will come not from any artificial reduction in the capacity of the criteria to detect baseline mild HAND, but by working toward the identification of biomarkers that will aid in determining if current HAND (including ANI) represent an active brain pathology or a static burnt out process (Brew, Reference Brew2004). This could be achieved by combining diagnostic criteria that determine if HIV+ patients have, within a clinically relevant period, stable, progressing, or incident cognitive decline (Gott, Gates, Dermody, Brew, & Cysique, Reference Gott, Gates, Dermody, Brew and Cysique2017; Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Sheppard, Kordovski, Faytell, Avci and Woods2017) against selected biomarkers. The Frascati criteria could be improved by determining guidelines for cognitive decline (Gates & Cysique, Reference Gates and Cysique2016; Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Sheppard, Kordovski, Faytell, Avci and Woods2017). These nomenclature changes would be similar to the current DSM-5 criteria for HAND, but with an improved formalization of cognitive impairment and decline (Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Sheppard, Kordovski, Faytell, Avci and Woods2017). Such an update would reduce the weight of any diagnosis on a cross-sectional assessment and confirm its value longitudinally, enhancing the criteria for biomarker discovery, therapeutic and clinical management, as well as selection in trials.

CROSS-SECTIONAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE: GLOBAL FINDINGS IN NON-WESTERN COHORTS IN RELATION TO WESTERN COHORTS

The past decade has witnessed a notable expansion of comprehensive NP studies to Sub-Saharan Africa (Buch et al., Reference Buch, Chivero, Hoare, Jumare, Nakasujja, Mudenda and Joseph2016), Asia (Joseph et al., Reference Joseph, Achim, Boivin, Brew, Clifford, Colosi and Wood2013; Ku et al., Reference Ku, Lee, Ahn, Song, Kim, Kim and Choi2014), Eastern Europe (Joseph et al., Reference Joseph, Achim, Boivin, Brew, Clifford, Colosi and Wood2013) and South America (Joseph et al., Reference Joseph, Achim, Boivin, Brew, Clifford, Colosi and Wood2013). These pools of research strongly demonstrate the feasibility of NP studies in culturally and economically diverse non-Western settings using standard NP tests adapted for language and cultural variations. This development represents a remarkable achievement on the part of the global NeuroHIV research community, especially when we consider the paucity of studies that existed in 2009 as reviewed by (Robertson, Liner, & Heaton, Reference Robertson, Liner and Heaton2009). Key to the success of these studies has been the clinical expertise of local researchers, the contribution of collaborative training strategies for local testing personnel (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Cysique, Jin, Shi, Yu, Letendre and Wu2008; Pumpradit et al., Reference Pumpradit, Ananworanich, Lolak, Shikuma, Paul, Siangphoe and Valcour2010), and the commitment of local researchers to the recognition of HAND as a significant health issue.

These studies variously assess NP functions in antiretroviral naïve patients, patients starting treatment, and patients who are stable on treatment where the vast majority had experienced immunosuppression. In untreated cohorts, and as expected, HAD prevalence rate is elevated [e.g., 25.4% in (Joska et al., Reference Joska, Westgarth-Taylor, Myer, Hoare, Thomas, Combrinck and Flisher2011); 39% in (Sacktor et al., Reference Sacktor, Nakasujja, Okonkwo, Skolasky, Robertson, Musisi and Katabira2013]), similar to pre-cART Western cohorts. The profile of HAD in untreated non-Western samples is identical to that of original reports in the United States, with a subcortical profile dominated by slowing of information processing, motor deficits, working memory/attention deficits, learning and retrieval memory impairment, verbal fluency deficits, and executive dysfunctions (Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Satishchandra, Gopukumar, Wilkie, Waldrop-Valverde, Ellis and Kumar2007; Robertson, Nakasujja, et al., Reference Robertson, Nakasujja, Wong, Musisi, Katabira, Parsons and Sacktor2007; Yepthomi et al., Reference Yepthomi, Paul, Vallabhaneni, Kumarasamy, Tate, Solomon and Flanigan2006). Although the effect of education on performance is often larger than in Western cohorts (Joska et al., Reference Joska, Westgarth-Taylor, Hoare, Thomas, Paul, Myer and Stein2012), this is probably due to a wider range of educational experience (Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Vaida, Riggs, Jin, Grant, Cysique and Heaton2011). The use of appropriate controls, fairly comprehensive NP test batteries, and the rigorous application of the Frascati criteria have ensured an excellent detection of HAND (Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Satishchandra, Gopukumar, Wilkie, Waldrop-Valverde, Ellis and Kumar2007; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Cysique, Jin, Shi, Yu, Letendre and Wu2008). However, tests requiring high levels of abstraction are probably best avoided among participants with very low levels of educations (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Cysique, Jin, Shi, Yu, Letendre and Wu2008), and reserved for participants with at least secondary school level (Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Iudicello, Shi, Letendre, Knight, Li and Heaton2014). Finally, similar to North-American cohorts, patients with AIDS (vs. non-AIDS) show increased levels of cognitive dysfunction (Kanmogne et al., Reference Kanmogne, Kuate, Cysique, Fonsah, Eta, Doh and Njamnshi2010).

Several studies (Ghate et al., Reference Ghate, Mehendale, Meyer, Umlauf, Deutsch, Kamat and Marcotte2015; Joska et al., Reference Joska, Westgarth-Taylor, Hoare, Thomas, Paul, Myer and Stein2012; Robertson et al., Reference Robertson, Jiang, Kumwenda, Supparatpinyo, Evans, Campbell and Nair2012; Sacktor et al., Reference Sacktor, Nakasujja, Okonkwo, Skolasky, Robertson, Musisi and Katabira2013; Valcour et al., Reference Valcour, Shiramizu, Sithinamsuwan, Nidhinandana, Ratto-Kim, Ananworanich and Shikuma2009) demonstrated NP improvement after cART initiation. As a pool, these studies are more representative of an initial cART effect than Western studies because most patients were originally naïve participants, while in Western cohorts many were ART-experienced (Cysique & Brew, Reference Cysique and Brew2009). In studies using fairly comprehensive NP testing and a large number of patients with HAD, the domains that showed the most improvement were motor deficits, working memory/attention deficits, verbal fluency deficits, and executive dysfunctions, while improvement in learning/memory and psychomotor speed were either less pronounced or less consistent (Joska et al., Reference Joska, Westgarth-Taylor, Hoare, Thomas, Paul, Myer and Stein2012; Sacktor et al., Reference Sacktor, Nakasujja, Okonkwo, Skolasky, Robertson, Musisi and Katabira2013). In patients with mild to moderate or moderate levels of impairment (among which few were affected by HAD), most improvement occurred in learning, and to a lesser extent memory and motor functions (Ghate et al., Reference Ghate, Mehendale, Meyer, Umlauf, Deutsch, Kamat and Marcotte2015).

In long-term cART-treated cohorts including a majority of HIV+ patients who had initiated cART with a low CD4 count, a recent study in Zambia (Kabuba, Anitha Menon, Franklin, Heaton, & Hestad, Reference Kabuba, Anitha Menon, Franklin, Heaton and Hestad2017) detected 34.6% of HAND. This prevalence rate is not statistically different from North American studies, which have used the same NP methods [(Heaton, Clifford, et al., Reference Heaton, Clifford, Franklin, Woods, Ake, Vaida and Grant2010), including demographically corrected normative standard]. Also ANI is the most common HAND subtype (68%) showing that its a priori exclusion would be a major issue for the accurate assessment of HAND’s global burden. Moreover, the study demonstrated medium effect size impairment between controls and HIV+ participants in the domains of executive functioning, speed of information processing, working memory, learning, delayed recall, but not fine motor-coordination.

Another study of cART-treated patients in Botswana (Lawler et al., Reference Lawler, Jeremiah, Mosepele, Ratcliffe, Cherry, Seloilwe and Steenhoff2011) detected 37% with HAND. The study showed impairment in all domains tested (learning/memory; verbal fluency, executive functions, psychomotor speed) compared to demographically comparable controls. Of interest, fine motor-coordination was different at only p=.04 between groups. This relative insensitivity of the Grooved Pegboard to HIV effects has been found in several other African studies (Akolo et al., Reference Akolo, Royal, Cherner, Okwuasaba, Eyzaguirre, Adebiyi and Blattner2014; Clifford et al., Reference Clifford, Mitike, Mekonnen, Zhang, Zenebe, Melaku and Evans2007; Robertson, Nakasujja, et al., Reference Robertson, Nakasujja, Wong, Musisi, Katabira, Parsons and Sacktor2007). This could be consistent with the Western studies showing less motor-coordination deficits in cART compared to pre-cART cohorts (Cysique et al., Reference Cysique, Maruff and Brew2004; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Franklin, Ellis, McCutchan, Letendre, Leblanc and Grant2011), except that it has also been found in asymptomatic HIV+ patients in Nigeria that were not on treatment (Akolo et al., Reference Akolo, Royal, Cherner, Okwuasaba, Eyzaguirre, Adebiyi and Blattner2014).

Future studies dedicated to exploring this issue could perhaps use a more extensive fine-motor assessment, a thorough assessment of demographic factors that may affect manual dexterity (Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Vaida, Riggs, Jin, Grant, Cysique and Heaton2011), and control for any peripheral neuropathy(Fellows et al., Reference Fellows, Byrd, Elliott, Robinson-Papp, Mindt and Morgello2012). In this regard, focusing assessment on motor functions should also probably be avoided when possible in non-Western cohorts, as it seems to lead to lower than expected rates of HAND (Robertson et al., Reference Robertson, Kumwenda, Supparatpinyo, Jiang, Evans, Campbell and Brouwers2011; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Brew, Arayawichanont, Robertson, Samintharapanya, Kongsaengdao and Wesselingh2008) (<15%). Finally, a meta-analysis by (Habib et al., Reference Habib, Yakasai, Owolabi, Ibrahim, Habib, Gudaji and Nashabaru2013) for sub-Saharan countries which included studies using screening tests and more comprehensive NP testing found that among those on ART for ≥6 months, the estimate for neurocognitive impairment was 30.4%. This is mostly consistent with what is detected in Western cohorts.

In other parts of the world, and in cohorts composed of mostly virally suppressed HIV+ persons tested with comprehensive NP test batteries, impairment rates were found to be around the 25–35% benchmark (Ene et al., Reference Ene, Franklin, Burlacu, Luca, Blaglosov, Ellis and Marcotte2014; Heaton, Cysique, et al., Reference Heaton, Cysique, Jin, Shi, Yu, Letendre and Wu2010; Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, van Oosterhout, Ngwalo, Stewart, Benjamin, Robertson and Solomon2014; Ku et al., Reference Ku, Lee, Ahn, Song, Kim, Kim and Choi2014; Pumpradit et al., Reference Pumpradit, Ananworanich, Lolak, Shikuma, Paul, Siangphoe and Valcour2010). In these studies, as in Western cohorts, ANI again accounts for the majority of diagnosis, MND for approximately a quarter, and HAD is typically present in less than 5%. Slight variation in HAND prevalence rates may be accounted for by differences in past experience of immunosuppression/viral replication, timeliness of cART initiation, duration of stability of current cART, and some portion of confounds if those were not excluded or controlled for.

LONGITUDINAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE: GLOBAL NATURALISTIC STUDIES

This overview of naturalistic longitudinal studies focuses on observational studies and their definitions of cognitive decline, thus exclusive of drug trials. We included 10 naturalistic longitudinal studies of cognitive change with cognitive decline investigated as a primary outcome as reported by August 2017, inclusive of a majority of HIV+ participants on stable cART. Seven were conducted in the United States, two in Australia and one in China, highlighting the need for further expansion. Several principal findings can be extracted from those studies: (1) The definition of cognitive decline as an absorbing state (wherein once an individual is defined as having declined this status is permanent) as compared to studies with multiple follow-up points (wherein an individual’s status can fluctuate across the duration of the project) yields apparent differences in cognitive decline rates that have to be interpreted carefully. Cysique, Maruff, and Brew (Reference Cysique, Maruff and Brew2006) defined cognitive decline as a unique endpoint and found 30% of HIV+ participants were defined as decliners at 6–12 month follow-up and 5% at 15–27 months.

In contrast, the large CNS HIV Antiretroviral Therapy Effects Research (CHARTER) study which defined cognitive decline as an absorbing state found an overall 23% cognitive decline rate over the 3-year study follow-up (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Franklin, Deutsch, Letendre, Ellis, Casaletto and Grant2015). A subsequent project by Cysique et al. (Reference Cysique, Letendre, Ake, Jin, Franklin, Gupta and Heaton2010) also used an absorbing rate definition and found an overall decline over the 27-month study period of 27%. (2) These three studies used standard global change scores (standard in the sense that scores are corrected for practice effect and regression toward the mean) and found fairly similar rates of cognitive decline ranging between ~30% across different cohorts context/nationalities. (3)

Some studies focused on incident HAND. One study (Sheppard et al., Reference Sheppard, Woods, Bondi, Gilbert, Massman and Doyle2015) found that HIV infection confers a nearly fivefold risk for developing a neurocognitive disorder over approximately 1 year. The sample was composed of a majority of participants on cART, of which 26% had detectable viral load. Of note, the authors applied a custom-made group practice effect correction based on the sub-sample of patients who had stable performance. They detected that 15.7% of the cohort had incident HAND. Another study (Gott et al., Reference Gott, Gates, Dermody, Brew and Cysique2017) in a sample of fully virally suppressed patients detected that incident HAND occurred in 7%, progressing HAND in 10%, while 35% has stable HAND over a 18-month period. The study used standard global change scores and combined this information with baseline HAND status. (4)

Studies that assessed deterioration within the various stages of HAND (thus not correcting for practice effect and regression toward the mean) typically found lower rates of cognitive decline. The Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study (MACS) found that across four years of follow-up, 10% of HIV+ individuals progressed to a worse stage of HAND stage (Sacktor et al., Reference Sacktor, Skolasky, Seaberg, Munro, Becker, Martin and Miller2016). Another study (Brouillette et al., Reference Brouillette, Yuen, Fellows, Cysique, Heaton and Mayo2016) reassessed the CHARTER sample using a relatively new group trajectory method but without practice effect corrections and found 15% cognitive decline.

Finally, in a more recent study, Vassallo et al. (Reference Vassallo, Fabre, Durant, Lebrun-Frenay, Joly, Ticchioni and Pradier2017) found 32% of cognitive decline in a group of patients of which approximately a quarter had detectable viral loads. While they used no correction for practice effect, they chose a fairly stringent criterion for degree decline (-2 SD) but only in one cognitive domain, potentially explaining the >30% decline rate. (5) Studies that used a restricted number of tests showed inconsistent results, suggesting that as with cross-sectional studies, a minimum of five cognitive domains should be assessed (Antinori et al., Reference Antinori, Arendt, Becker, Brew, Byrd, Cherner and Wojna2007). For example, when focusing on verbal memory, one study found worse performance over time in HIV+ individuals compared to HIV-individuals at 1 year follow-up (Seider et al., Reference Seider, Luo, Gongvatana, Devlin, de la Monte, Chasman and Cohen2014) and worse in the oldest participants, while another which focused only on two tasks of psychomotor speed and mental flexibility and a large selected group of long-term asymptomatic HIV+ adults reported stable cognitive performance across eight years (Cole, Margolick, et al., Reference Cole, Margolick, Cox, Li, Selnes, Martin and Miller2007). These results from the MACS cohort have never been replicated, probably because particularly healthy individuals were selected and many without any evidence of HAND in the first place. Overall, this overview demonstrates that standardization of the definition of cognitive decline is urgently needed, especially as the HIV population is aging with an increased prevalence of age-related comorbidities (Guaraldi et al., Reference Guaraldi, Orlando, Zona, Menozzi, Carli, Garlassi and Palella2011), and risk for brain premature aging (Brew & Cysique, Reference Brew and Cysique2017; Goodkin et al., Reference Goodkin, Miller, Cox, Reynolds, Becker, Martin and Sacktor2017).

CONCLUSIONS

In conclusion, the review purposefully demonstrates that the expansion of standard NP testing to non-Western centers has been done with a high level of success. When taken globally, it is clear that HAND remains an important concern for HIV+ persons which may worsen as they age. To build on this positive momentum for NeuroHIV research, NP methods and data would need to be better harmonized to better circumscribe HAND and its evolution across the life-span, as well as assist in treatment and intervention development.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Associate Professor Melinda Cooper for reviewing the English language of the manuscript. Lucette Cysique also thanks her mentors who by sharing and discussing many ideas on the questions of HIV, neuropsychology, and the brain have provided her with a unique multi-disciplinary perspective in NeuroHIV research: Prof. Robert K. Heaton; Prof. Buce J. Brew, Prof. Caroline Rae and Prof. Paul Maruff. Conflicts of Interest and Source of Funding: L.A.C. received honoraria from Abbvie Ltd, CogState Ltd, ViiV healthcare, partial salary support in 2012 from Mercks Sharp Dome and CogState Ltd. L.A.C. is funded by a National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia Career Development Fellowship #APP1045400 and has unrelated research support from Abbvie Ltd, ViiV healthcare, the Australian National Association of People Living with HIV/AIDS (NAPWA) and Gilead Sciences. CogState did not pay her for the current review or any other commercial entity. R.S. is supported by an Institutional Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (NRSA) T32 grant funded by the NIAAA within the National Institutes of Health (Award T32 AA013525).

References

REFERENCES

Agelink van Rentergem, J.A., Murre, J.M., & Huizenga, H.M. (2017). Multivariate normative comparisons using an aggregated database. PLoS One, 12(3), e0173218.Google Scholar
Akolo, C., Royal, W. III, Cherner, M., Okwuasaba, K., Eyzaguirre, L., Adebiyi, R., & Blattner, W.A. (2014). Neurocognitive impairment associated with predominantly early stage HIV infection in Abuja, Nigeria. Journal of Neurovirology, 20(4), 380387.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Antinori, A., Arendt, G., Becker, J.T., Brew, B.J., Byrd, D.A., Cherner, M., & Wojna, V.E. (2007). Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology, 69, 17891799. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8b CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackstone, K., Moore, D.J., Franklin, D.R., Clifford, D.B., Collier, A.C., Marra, C.M., & Heaton, R.K. (2012). Defining neurocognitive impairment in HIV: Deficit scores versus clinical ratings. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26(6), 894908.Google Scholar
Blackstone, K., Moore, D.J., Heaton, R.K., Franklin, D.R. Jr., Woods, S.P., Clifford, D.B., & Grant, I. (2012). Diagnosing symptomatic HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders: Self-report versus performance-based assessment of everyday functioning. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18(1), 7988.Google Scholar
Bloch, M., Kamminga, J., Jayewardene, A., Bailey, M., Carberry, A., Vincent, T., & Cysique, L.A. (2016). A screening strategy for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders that accurately identifies patients requiring neurological review. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 63(5), 687693.Google Scholar
Brandt, C., Zvolensky, M.J., Woods, S.P., Gonzalez, A., Safren, S.A., & O’Cleirigh, C.M. (2017). Anxiety symptoms and disorders among adults living with HIV and AIDS: A critical review and integrative synthesis of the empirical literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 51, 164184.Google Scholar
Brew, B.J. (2004). Evidence for a change in AIDS dementia complex in the era of highly active antiretroviral therapy and the possibility of new forms of AIDS dementia complex. Aids, 18(Suppl. 1), S75S78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brew, B.J., & Cysique, L. (2017). Does HIV prematurely age the brain? Lancet HIV, 4(9), e380e381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brouillette, M.J., Yuen, T., Fellows, L.K., Cysique, L.A., Heaton, R.K., & Mayo, N.E. (2016). Identifying neurocognitive decline at 36 months among HIV-positive participants in the CHARTER cohort using group-based trajectory analysis. PLoS One, 11(5), e0155766.Google Scholar
Buch, S., Chivero, E.T., Hoare, J., Jumare, J., Nakasujja, N., Mudenda, V., & Joseph, J. (2016). Proceedings from the NIMH symposium on “NeuroAIDS in Africa: Neurological and neuropsychiatric complications of HIV”. Journal of Neurovirology, 22(5), 699702.Google Scholar
Carey, C.L., Woods, S.P., Gonzalez, R., Conover, E., Marcotte, T.D., Grant, I., & Heaton, R.K. (2004). Predictive validity of global deficit scores in detecting neuropsychological impairment in HIV infection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 307319. doi: 10.1080/13803390490510031 Google Scholar
Carter, S.L., Rourke, S.B., Murji, S., Shore, D., & Rourke, B.P. (2003). Cognitive complaints, depression, medical symptoms, and their association with neuropsychological functioning in HIV infection: A structural equation model analysis. Neuropsychology, 17(3), 410419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cherner, M., Cysique, L., Heaton, R. K., Marcotte, T. D., Ellis, R. J., Masliah, E., & Grant, I. (2007). Neuropathologic confirmation of definitional criteria for human immunodeficiency virus-associated neurocognitive disorders. Journal of Neurovirology, 13, 2328. doi: 10.1080/13550280601089175 Google Scholar
Cherner, M., Masliah, E., Ellis, R. J., Marcotte, T. D., Moore, D. J., Grant, I., & Heaton, R.K. (2002). Neurocognitive dysfunction predicts postmortem findings of HIV encephalitis. Neurology, 59(10), 15631567.Google Scholar
Chiao, S., Rosen, H.J., Nicolas, K., Wendelken, L.A., Alcantar, O., Rankin, K.P., & Valcour, V. (2013). Deficits in self-awareness impact the diagnosis of asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment in HIV. AIDS Research and Human Retroviruses, 29(6), 949956.Google Scholar
Clifford, D.B., Mitike, M.T., Mekonnen, Y., Zhang, J., Zenebe, G., Melaku, Z., & Evans, S. (2007). Neurological evaluation of untreated human immunodeficiency virus infected adults in Ethiopia. Journal of Neurovirology, 13(1), 6772.Google Scholar
Cole, M.A., Castellon, S.A., Perkins, A.C., Ureno, O.S., Robinet, M.B., Reinhard, M.J., & Hinkin, C.H. (2007). Relationship between psychiatric status and frontal-subcortical systems in HIV-infected individuals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(3), 549554.Google Scholar
Cole, M.A., Margolick, J.B., Cox, C., Li, X., Selnes, O.A., Martin, E.M., & Miller, E.N. (2007). Longitudinally preserved psychomotor performance in long-term asymptomatic HIV-infected individuals. Neurology, 69(24), 22132220.Google Scholar
Crawford, J.R., & Allan, K.M. (1994). The Mahalanobis Distance index of WAIS-R subtest scatter: Psychometric properties in a healthy UK sample. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33(Pt 1), 6569.Google Scholar
Crum-Cianflone, N.F., Moore, D.J., Letendre, S., Poehlman Roediger, M., Eberly, L., Weintrob, A., & Hale, B.R. (2013). Low prevalence of neurocognitive impairment in early diagnosed and managed HIV-infected persons. Neurology, 80(4), 371379.Google Scholar
Cysique, L.A., & Becker, J.T. (2017). HIV-related cognitive impairment and decline can be detected in virally suppressed women above and beyond the effects of confounds. Neurology. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cysique, L.A., & Brew, B.J. (2009). Neuropsychological functioning and antiretroviral treatment in HIV/AIDS: A review. Neuropsychology Review, 19, 169185. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9092-3 Google Scholar
Cysique, L.A., & Brew, B.J. (in press). Comorbid depression and apathy in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder in the era of chronic HIV infection. In V.I. Reus & D. Lindqvist (Eds.), The Psychopharmacology of HIV-related dementia and HAND. Amsterdam: Elsevier/Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cysique, L.A., Dermody, N., Carr, A., Brew, B.J., & Teesson, M. (2016). The role of depression chronicity and recurrence on neurocognitive dysfunctions in HIV-infected adults. Journal of Neurovirology, 22(1), 5665.Google Scholar
Cysique, L.A., Deutsch, R., Atkinson, J.H., Young, C., Marcotte, T.D., Dawson, L., & Heaton, R.K. (2007). Incident major depression does not affect neuropsychological functioning in HIV-infected men. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 111. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707070026 Google Scholar
Cysique, L.A., Heaton, R.K., Kamminga, J., Lane, T., Gates, T.M., Moore, D.M., & Brew, B.J. (2014). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder in Australia: A case of a high-functioning and optimally treated cohort and implications for international neuroHIV research. Journal of Neurovirology, 20(3), 258268.Google Scholar
Cysique, L.A., Letendre, S.L., Ake, C., Jin, H., Franklin, D.R., Gupta, S., & Heaton, R.K. (2010). Incidence and nature of cognitive decline over 1 year among HIV-infected former plasma donors in China. Aids, 24(7), 983990.Google Scholar
Cysique, L.A., Maruff, P., & Brew, B.J. (2004). Prevalence and pattern of neuropsychological impairment in human immunodeficiency virus-infected/acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (HIV/AIDS) patients across pre- and post-highly active antiretroviral therapy eras: A combined study of two cohorts. Journal of Neurovirology, 10, 350357. doi: 10.1080/13550280490521078 Google Scholar
Cysique, L.A., Maruff, P., & Brew, B.J. (2006). Variable benefit in neuropsychological function in HIV-infected HAART-treated patients. Neurology, 66, 14471450. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000210477.63851.d3 Google Scholar
Cysique, L.A., Maruff, P., Darby, D., & Brew, B.J. (2006). The assessment of cognitive function in advanced HIV-1 infection and AIDS dementia complex using a new computerised cognitive test battery. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 185194. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.011 Google Scholar
De Francesco, D., Underwood, J., Post, F.A., Vera, J.H., Williams, I., Boffito, M., & Sabin, C.A. (2016). Defining cognitive impairment in people-living-with-HIV: The POPPY study. BMC Infectious Diseases, 16(1), 617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ene, L., Franklin, D.R., Burlacu, R., Luca, A.E., Blaglosov, A.G., Ellis, R.J., & Marcotte, T.D. (2014). Neurocognitive functioning in a Romanian cohort of young adults with parenterally-acquired HIV-infection during childhood. Journal of Neurovirology, 20(5), 496504.Google Scholar
Fellows, R.P., Byrd, D.A., Elliott, K., Robinson-Papp, J., Mindt, M.R., & Morgello, S. (2012). Distal sensory polyneuropathy is associated with neuropsychological test performance among persons with HIV. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18(5), 898907. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000707 Google Scholar
Fellows, R.P., Byrd, D.A., Morgello, S., & Manhattan HIV Brain Bank. (2013). Major depressive disorder, cognitive symptoms, and neuropsychological performance among ethnically diverse HIV+ men and women. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(2), 216225. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712001245 Google Scholar
Gates, T.M., & Cysique, L.A. (2016). The chronicity of HIV infection should drive the research strategy of neuroHIV treatment studies: A critical review. CNS Drugs, 30(1), 5369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghate, M., Mehendale, S., Meyer, R., Umlauf, A., Deutsch, R., Kamat, R., & Marcotte, T.D. (2015). The effects of antiretroviral treatment initiation on cognition in HIV-infected individuals with advanced disease in Pune, India. Journal of Neurovirology, 21(4), 391398.Google Scholar
Gisslen, M., Price, R.W., & Nilsson, S. (2011). The definition of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders: Are we overestimating the real prevalence? BMC Infectious Diseases, 11(356), 356.Google Scholar
Goodkin, K., Miller, E.N., Cox, C., Reynolds, S., Becker, J.T., Martin, E., & Sacktor, N.C. (2017). Effect of ageing on neurocognitive function by stage of HIV infection: Evidence from the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study. Lancet HIV, 4(9), e411e422.Google Scholar
Gott, C., Gates, T., Dermody, N., Brew, B.J., & Cysique, L.A. (2017). Cognitive change trajectories in virally suppressed HIV-infected individuals indicate high prevalence of disease activity. PLoS One, 12(3), e0171887.Google Scholar
Grant, I., Atkinson, J.H., Hesselink, J.R., Kennedy, C.J., Richman, D.D., Spector, S.A., & McCutchan, J.A. (1987). Evidence for early central nervous system involvement in the acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS) and other human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) infections. Studies with neuropsychologic testing and magnetic resonance imaging. Annals of Internal Medicine, 107, 828836.Google Scholar
Grant, I., Franklin, D.R. Jr., Deutsch, R., Woods, S.P., Vaida, F., Ellis, R.J., & Heaton, R.K. (2014). Asymptomatic HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment increases risk for symptomatic decline. Neurology, 82(23), 20552062.Google Scholar
Guaraldi, G., Orlando, G., Zona, S., Menozzi, M., Carli, F., Garlassi, E., & Palella, F. (2011). Premature age-related comorbidities among HIV-infected persons compared with the general population. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 53(11), 11201126.Google Scholar
Gupta, J.D., Satishchandra, P., Gopukumar, K., Wilkie, F., Waldrop-Valverde, D., Ellis, R., & Kumar, M. (2007). Neuropsychological deficits in human immunodeficiency virus type 1 clade C-seropositive adults from South India. Journal of Neurovirology, 13(3), 195202.Google Scholar
Gupta, S., Iudicello, J.E., Shi, C., Letendre, S., Knight, A., Li, J., & Heaton, R.K. (2014). Absence of neurocognitive impairment in a large Chinese sample of HCV-infected injection drug users receiving methadone treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 137, 2935.Google Scholar
Gupta, S., Vaida, F., Riggs, K., Jin, H., Grant, I., Cysique, L., & Heaton, R.K. (2011). Neuropsychological performance in mainland china: The effect of urban/rural residence and self-reported daily academic skill use. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 163173. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001384 Google Scholar
Habib, A.G., Yakasai, A.M., Owolabi, L.F., Ibrahim, A., Habib, Z.G., Gudaji, M., & Nashabaru, I. (2013). Neurocognitive impairment in HIV-1-infected adults in Sub-Saharan Africa: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Infectious Diseases, 17(10), e820e831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R.K., Clifford, D.B., Franklin, D.R. Jr., Woods, S.P., Ake, C., Vaida, F., & Grant, I. (2010). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders persist in the era of potent antiretroviral therapy: CHARTER Study. Neurology, 75, 20872096. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318200d727 Google Scholar
Heaton, R.K., Cysique, L.A., Jin, H., Shi, C., Yu, X., Letendre, S., & Wu, Z. (2008). Neurobehavioral effects of human immunodeficiency virus infection among former plasma donors in rural China. Journal of Neurovirology, 14, 536549. doi: 10.1080/13550280802378880 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R.K., Cysique, L.A., Jin, H., Shi, C., Yu, X., Letendre, S., & Wu, Z. (2010). Neurobehavioral effects of human immunodeficiency virus infection among former plasma donors in rural China. Journal of Neurovirology, 16(2), 185188.Google Scholar
Heaton, R.K., Franklin, D.R., Ellis, R.J., McCutchan, J.A., Letendre, S.L., Leblanc, S., & Grant, I. (2011). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders before and during the era of combination antiretroviral therapy: Differences in rates, nature, and predictors. Journal of Neurovirology, 17, 316. doi: 10.1007/s13365-010-0006-1 Google Scholar
Heaton, R.K., Franklin, D.R. Jr., Deutsch, R., Letendre, S., Ellis, R.J., Casaletto, K., & Grant, I. (2015). Neurocognitive change in the era of HIV combination antiretroviral therapy: The longitudinal CHARTER study. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 60(3), 473480.Google Scholar
Helmick, K.M., Spells, C.A., Malik, S.Z., Davies, C.A., Marion, D.W., & Hinds, S.R. (2015). Traumatic brain injury in the US military: Epidemiology and key clinical and research programs. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 9(3), 358366.Google Scholar
Huizenga, H.M., Agelink van Rentergem, J.A., Grasman, R.P., Muslimovic, D., & Schmand, B. (2016). Normative comparisons for large neuropsychological test batteries: User-friendly and sensitive solutions to minimize familywise false positives. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 38(6), 611629.Google Scholar
Ingraham, L.J., & Aiken, C.B. (1996). An empirical approach to determining criteria for abnormality in test batteries with multiple measures. Neuropsychology, 10(1), 120124. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.10.1.120 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joseph, J., Achim, C.L., Boivin, M.J., Brew, B.J., Clifford, D.B., Colosi, D.A., & Wood, C. (2013). Global NeuroAIDS roundtable. Journal of Neurovirology, 19(1), 19.Google Scholar
Joska, J.A., Westgarth-Taylor, J., Hoare, J., Thomas, K.G., Paul, R., Myer, L., & Stein, D.J. (2012). Neuropsychological outcomes in adults commencing highly active anti-retroviral treatment in South Africa: A prospective study. BMC Infectious Diseases, 12(39), 39.Google Scholar
Joska, J.A., Westgarth-Taylor, J., Myer, L., Hoare, J., Thomas, K.G., Combrinck, M., & Flisher, A.J. (2011). Characterization of HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders among individuals starting antiretroviral therapy in South Africa. AIDS and Behavior, 15(6), 11971203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kabuba, N., Anitha Menon, J., Franklin, D.R. Jr., Heaton, R.K., & Hestad, K.A. (2017). Use of Western Neuropsychological Test Battery in Detecting HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders (HAND) in Zambia. AIDS and Behavior, 21, 17171727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamminga, J., Bloch, M., Vincent, T., Carberry, A., Brew, B.J., & Cysique, L.A. (2017). Determining optimal impairment rating methodology for a new HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder screening procedure. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 4, 115.Google Scholar
Kanmogne, G.D., Kuate, C.T., Cysique, L.A., Fonsah, J.Y., Eta, S., Doh, R., & Njamnshi, A.K. (2010). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders in sub-Saharan Africa: A pilot study in Cameroon. BMC Neurology, 10, 60. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-10-60 Google Scholar
Kelly, C.M., van Oosterhout, J.J., Ngwalo, C., Stewart, R.C., Benjamin, L., Robertson, K.R., & Solomon, T. (2014). HIV associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) in Malawian adults and effect on adherence to combination anti-retroviral therapy: A cross sectional study. PLoS One, 9(6), e98962.Google Scholar
Ku, N.S., Lee, Y., Ahn, J.Y., Song, J.E., Kim, M.H., Kim, S.B., & Choi, J.Y. (2014). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder in HIV-infected Koreans: The Korean NeuroAIDS Project. HIV Medicine, 15(8), 470477.Google Scholar
Lawler, K., Jeremiah, K., Mosepele, M., Ratcliffe, S.J., Cherry, C., Seloilwe, E., & Steenhoff, A.P. (2011). Neurobehavioral effects in HIV-positive individuals receiving highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART) in Gaborone, Botswana. PLoS One, 6(2), e17233.Google Scholar
Maki, P.M., Rubin, L.H., Valcour, V., Martin, E., Crystal, H., Young, M., & Anastos, K. (2015). Cognitive function in women with HIV: Findings from the Women’s Interagency HIV Study. Neurology, 84(3), 231240.Google Scholar
Masliah, E., Heaton, R. K., Marcotte, T. D., Ellis, R. J., Wiley, C. A., Mallory, M., & Grant, I. (1997). Dendritic injury is a pathological substrate for human immunodeficiency virus-related cognitive disorders. HNRC Group. The HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center. Annals of Neurology, 42(6), 963972.Google Scholar
McArthur, J.C. (2004). HIV dementia: An evolving disease. Journal of Neuroimmunology, 157, 310. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2004.08.042 Google Scholar
McCombe, J.A., Vivithanaporn, P., Gill, M.J., & Power, C. (2013). Predictors of symptomatic HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders in universal health care. HIV Medicine, 14(2), 99107.Google Scholar
McDonnell, J., Haddow, L., Daskalopoulou, M., Lampe, F., Speakman, A., Gilson, R., & Rodger, A. (2014). Minimal cognitive impairment in UK HIV-positive men who have sex with men: Effect of case definitions and comparison with the general population and HIV-negative men. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes (1999), 67(2), 120127. doi: 10.1097/QAI.0000000000000273 Google Scholar
Moore, D. J., Masliah, E., Rippeth, J. D., Gonzalez, R., Carey, C. L., Cherner, M., & Grant, I. (2006). Cortical and subcortical neurodegeneration is associated with HIV neurocognitive impairment. Aids, 20(6), 879887.Google Scholar
Munoz-Moreno, J.A., Perez-Alvarez, N., Munoz-Murillo, A., Prats, A., Garolera, M., Jurado, M.A., & Clotet, B. (2014). Classification models for neurocognitive impairment in HIV infection based on demographic and clinical variables. PLoS One, 9(9), e107625.Google Scholar
Nightingale, S., Winston, A., Letendre, S., Michael, B.D., McArthur, J.C., Khoo, S., & Solomon, T. (2014). Controversies in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Lancet Neurology, 13(11), 11391151.Google Scholar
Pumpradit, W., Ananworanich, J., Lolak, S., Shikuma, C., Paul, R., Siangphoe, U., & Valcour, V. (2010). Neurocognitive impairment and psychiatric comorbidity in well-controlled human immunodeficiency virus-infected Thais from the 2NN Cohort Study. Journal of Neurovirology, 16(1), 7682.Google Scholar
Rabinowitz, A.R., Li, X., McCauley, S.R., Wilde, E.A., Barnes, A., Hanten, G., & Levin, H.S. (2015). Prevalence and Predictors of poor recovery from mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 32(19), 14881496.Google Scholar
Robertson, K., Bayon, C., Molina, J.M., McNamara, P., Resch, C., Munoz-Moreno, J.A., & van Wyk, J. (2014). Screening for neurocognitive impairment, depression, and anxiety in HIV-infected patients in Western Europe and Canada. AIDS Care, 26(12), 15551561.Google Scholar
Robertson, K., Jiang, H., Kumwenda, J., Supparatpinyo, K., Evans, S., Campbell, T.B., & Nair, A. (2012). Improved neuropsychological and neurological functioning across three antiretroviral regimens in diverse resource-limited settings: AIDS Clinical Trials Group study a5199, the International Neurological Study. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 55(6), 868876.Google Scholar
Robertson, K., Kumwenda, J., Supparatpinyo, K., Jiang, J.H., Evans, S., Campbell, T.B., & Brouwers, P. (2011). A multinational study of neurological performance in antiretroviral therapy-naive HIV-1-infected persons in diverse resource-constrained settings. Journal of Neurovirology, 17(5), 438447.Google Scholar
Robertson, K., Liner, J., & Heaton, R. (2009). Neuropsychological assessment of HIV-infected populations in international settings. Neuropsychology Review, 19(2), 232249.Google Scholar
Robertson, K.R., Nakasujja, N., Wong, M., Musisi, S., Katabira, E., Parsons, T.D., & Sacktor, N. (2007). Pattern of neuropsychological performance among HIV positive patients in Uganda. BMC Neurology, 7(8), 8.Google Scholar
Robertson, K.R., Smurzynski, M., Parsons, T.D., Wu, K., Bosch, R.J., Wu, J., & Ellis, R.J. (2007). The prevalence and incidence of neurocognitive impairment in the HAART era. Aids, 21, 19151921. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32828e4e27 Google Scholar
Royal, W. III, Cherner, M., Burdo, T.H., Umlauf, A., Letendre, S.L., Jumare, J., & Blattner, W.A. (2016). Associations between cognition, gender and monocyte activation among HIV infected individuals in Nigeria. PLoS One, 11(2), e0147182.Google Scholar
Rubin, L.H., Maki, P.M., Springer, G., Benning, L., Anastos, K., Gustafson, D., & Valcour, V.G. (2017). Cognitive trajectories over four years among HIV-infected women with optimal viral suppression. Neurology. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004491Google Scholar
Rubin, L.H., Pyra, M., Cook, J.A., Weber, K.M., Cohen, M.H., Martin, E., & Maki, P.M. (2016). Post-traumatic stress is associated with verbal learning, memory, and psychomotor speed in HIV-infected and HIV-uninfected women. Journal of Neurovirology, 22(2), 159169.Google Scholar
Sacktor, N., Nakasujja, N., Okonkwo, O., Skolasky, R.L., Robertson, K., Musisi, S., & Katabira, E. (2013). Longitudinal neuropsychological test performance among HIV seropositive individuals in Uganda. Journal of Neurovirology, 19(1), 4856.Google Scholar
Sacktor, N., Skolasky, R.L., Seaberg, E., Munro, C., Becker, J.T., Martin, E., & Miller, E. (2016). Prevalence of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders in the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study. Neurology, 86(4), 334340.Google Scholar
Seider, T.R., Luo, X., Gongvatana, A., Devlin, K.N., de la Monte, S.M., Chasman, J.D., & Cohen, R.A. (2014). Verbal Memory Declines More Rapidly with Age in HIV Infected versus Uninfected Adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 36(4), 356367. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.892061 Google Scholar
Sheppard, D.P., Woods, S.P., Bondi, M.W., Gilbert, P.E., Massman, P.J., Doyle, K.L., & HIV Neurobehavioral Research Program Group. (2015). Does older age confer an increased risk of incident neurocognitive disorders among persons living with HIV disease? The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 29(5), 656677. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2015.1077995 Google Scholar
Sheppard, D.P., Woods, S.P., & Verduzco, M. (2017). Construct validity of the UCSD performance-based skills assessment-brief version (UPSA-B) in HIV disease. Applied Neuropsychology. Adult, 25, 112.Google Scholar
Simioni, S., Cavassini, M., Annoni, J.M., Rimbault Abraham, A., Bourquin, I., Schiffer, V., & Du Pasquier, R.A. (2010). Cognitive dysfunction in HIV patients despite long-standing suppression of viremia. Aids, 24, 12431250. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e3283354a7b Google Scholar
Su, T., Schouten, J., Geurtsen, G.J., Wit, F.W., Stolte, I.G., Prins, M., & Schmand, B.A. (2015). Multivariate normative comparison, a novel method for more reliably detecting cognitive impairment in HIV infection. Aids, 29(5), 547557.Google Scholar
Taylor, M.J., & Heaton, R.K. (2001). Sensitivity and specificity of WAIS-III/WMS-III demographically corrected factor scores in neuropsychological assessment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(7), 867874.Google Scholar
Thames, A.D., Becker, B.W., Marcotte, T.D., Hines, L.J., Foley, J.M., Ramezani, A., & Hinkin, C.H. (2011). Depression, cognition, and self-appraisal of functional abilities in HIV: An examination of subjective appraisal versus objective performance. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25(2), 224243.Google Scholar
The ACT UP/New York Women and AIDS Book Group. (1990). Women, AIDS & Activism. Boston, MA: South End Press.Google Scholar
Tierney, S.M., Sheppard, D.P., Kordovski, V.M., Faytell, M.P., Avci, G., & Woods, S.P. (2017). A comparison of the sensitivity, stability, and reliability of three diagnostic schemes for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Journal of Neurovirology, 23(3), 404421.Google Scholar
Tozzi, V., Balestra, P., Lorenzini, P., Bellagamba, R., Galgani, S., Corpolongo, A., & Narciso, P. (2005). Prevalence and risk factors for human immunodeficiency virus-associated neurocognitive impairment, 1996 to 2002: Results from an urban observational cohort. Journal of Neurovirology, 11(3), 265273.Google Scholar
Underwood, J., Leech, R., Winston, A., Sabin, C., & De Francesco, D. (2017). Medicalising normality? Using a simulated dataset to assess the performance of different diagnostic criteria of cognitive impairment. HIV Medicine, 18, British HIV Association Conference, Poster 83.Google Scholar
Valcour, V.G., Shiramizu, B.T., Sithinamsuwan, P., Nidhinandana, S., Ratto-Kim, S., Ananworanich, J., & Shikuma, C.M. (2009). HIV DNA and cognition in a Thai longitudinal HAART initiation cohort: The SEARCH 001 Cohort Study. Neurology, 72(11), 992998.Google Scholar
Vassallo, M., Fabre, R., Durant, J., Lebrun-Frenay, C., Joly, H., Ticchioni, M., & Pradier, C. (2017). A decreasing CD4/CD8 ratio over time and lower CSF-penetrating antiretroviral regimens are associated with a higher risk of neurocognitive deterioration, independently of viral replication. Journal of Neurovirology, 23, 216225.Google Scholar
Watkins, C.C., & Treisman, G.J. (2012). Neuropsychiatric complications of aging with HIV. Journal of Neurovirology, 18(4), 277290.Google Scholar
Woods, S.P., Iudicello, J.E., Morgan, E.E., Verduzco, M., Smith, T.V., & Cushman, C. (2017). Household everyday functioning in the internet age: Online shopping and banking skills are affected in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 23(7), 605615.Google Scholar
Wright, E., Brew, B., Arayawichanont, A., Robertson, K., Samintharapanya, K., Kongsaengdao, S., & Wesselingh, S. (2008). Neurologic disorders are prevalent in HIV-positive outpatients in the Asia-Pacific region. Neurology, 71(1), 5056.Google Scholar
Wright, E.J., Grund, B., Cysique, L.A., Robertson, K.R., Brew, B.J., Collins, G., & Price, R.W. (2015). Factors associated with neurocognitive test performance at baseline: A substudy of the INSIGHT Strategic Timing of AntiRetroviral Treatment (START) trial. HIV Medicine, 16(Suppl 1), 97108.Google Scholar
Yepthomi, T., Paul, R., Vallabhaneni, S., Kumarasamy, N., Tate, D.F., Solomon, S., & Flanigan, T. (2006). Neurocognitive consequences of HIV in southern India: A preliminary study of clade C virus. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(3), 424430.Google Scholar