Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T10:12:12.181Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Clinical staging in severe mental disorder: evidence from neurocognition and neuroimaging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Ashleigh Lin
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
Renate L. E. P. Reniers
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
Stephen J. Wood*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
*
Professor Stephen Wood, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston B15 2TT, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

A new approach to understanding severe mental illnesses such as schizophrenia and affective disorders is to adopt a clinical staging model. Such a model defines the extent of the illness such that earlier and milder phenomena are distinguished from later, more impairing features. Part of the appeal of such a model is that it should have cross-diagnostic applications, but to date there has been no attempt to examine imaging or neurocognrtive evidence for staging in this way. We review these two domains of study with particular focus on major depression and bipolar affective disorder. Although there is some support for the staging model in affective disorders, conclusions are limited by the large variability in the clinical samples studied, especially with regard to the presence of psychotic symptoms. We suggest that future research needs to take a transdiagnostic and longitudinal approach.

Type
Special Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2013 

Clinical staging is a practical tool that has demonstrated utility in general medicine. It defines the extent of progression of disease at a particular point in time, and where a person's condition currently lies along a continuum of the course of illness. Reference McGorry, Hickie, Yung, Pantelis and Jackson1,Reference Wood, Yung, McGorry and Pantelis2 For example, stages of certain cancers are distinguished by the extent of local invasion of tumour, lymphatic involvement and metastatic spread. Thus, early and milder clinical phenomena are differentiated from later stages that have evidence of illness extension, progression and chronicity. From a practical perspective, clinical staging enables the clinician to select treatments relevant to the stage, with less invasive interventions being more effective in earlier stages than when delivered later in the illness course. Reference McGorry, Hickie, Yung, Pantelis and Jackson1 Again, the cancer analogy is useful here: minor surgery and local radiotherapy may be appropriate for early stages of breast cancer, whereas in later stages this would not be sufficient and more radical treatment such as mastectomy and chemotherapy might be indicated.

It has been proposed that the concept of a staging model can be applied to psychiatry. Reference McGorry, Hickie, Yung, Pantelis and Jackson1 In particular we put forward the hypothesis that severe mental disorders, such as schizophrenia, bipolar affective disorder and severe depression, develop from initial non-specific symptoms and syndromes (i.e. a pluripotential state) and from a background of specific and non-specific risk factors such as genes or early environment. From the initial non-specific clinical picture, worsening of symptoms and acquisition of new symptoms occur, together with progressive neurobiological changes and related neurobehavioural deficits, until clearly recognisable mental disorder appears. Further progression of symptoms and neurobiological abnormalities may occur after ‘threshold’ diagnosis. Thus, the natural history of major mental illness is postulated to consist of transition from being asymptomatic and not seeking help, through a stage of undifferentiated general symptoms such as mild anxiety, depressive and somatic symptoms, followed by the worsening of existing symptoms and acquisition of new ones (e.g. psychotic-like experiences, substance use) which may be associated with behavioural and functional decline. Further progression of illness may still occur, with development of chronic symptoms, relapses and ongoing impairment. Although the staging approach has much intellectual appeal, it is still a heuristic concept with extensive research work required to develop stage markers. Previously we have examined the evidence for clinical staging in schizophrenia, with a particular focus on neuroimaging and treatment data. Reference Wood, Yung, McGorry and Pantelis2 Here we extend this investigation to neurocognitive findings and to affective disorders.

Neurocognition

Neurocognitive impairments are a feature of severe mental illness, but it is unclear whether these impairments support a clinical staging model. In schizophrenia, cognitive impairment is large and documented across a range of cognitive domains, most notably verbal learning and memory, performance and full-scale IQ scores, sustained attention and cognitive flexibility. Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis3 Similarly, moderate impairments in a number of domains are documented in bipolar affective disorder, the largest occurring in verbal learning and memory and in executive function; Reference Quraishi and Frangou4Reference Bora, Yucel and Pantelis7 these are evident during euthymia and amplified when symptoms are experienced. Reference Bearden, Hoffman and Cannon8,Reference Kurtz and Gerraty9 Individuals with major depressive disorder also show neurocognitive impairment, although the affected domains are unclear; impairments have most consistently been demonstrated in verbal learning and memory, attention and executive function, Reference Zakzanis, Leach and Kaplan10Reference McClintock, Husain, Greer and Cullum12 although to a lesser extent than in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.

If neurocognition is to be a reliable indicator of clinical stage, then variability in performance should indicate illness severity, chronicity and progression. In schizophrenia the relationship between chronicity and impairment is not straightforward. The magnitude of neurocognitive impairment in the first psychotic episode Reference Mesholam-Gately, Giuliano, Goff, Faraone and Seidman13 is equivalent to that of samples with established illness, Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis3 suggesting that there is no further decline in neurocognitive ability after the onset of frank psychotic symptoms. This is supported by a lack of longitudinal evidence of progressive deterioration over illness course. Reference Szöke, Trandafir, Dupont, Méary, Schürhoff and Leboyer14 A subgroup of these individuals who develop ‘deficit’ schizophrenia, Reference Carpenter, Heinrichs and Wagman15 characterised by a chronic illness course, prominent negative symptoms, poor functional outcome and significantly reduced cognitive performance, Reference Fenton and McGlashan16,Reference Cohen, Saperstein, Gold, Kirkpatrick, Carpenter and Buchanan17 might show progressive impairment. However, it seems more likely that deficits are longstanding rather than associated with transition between clinical stages; cognitive deficit early in the illness course is predictive of poor functional outcome and negative symptoms many years later. Reference Milev, Ho, Arndt and Andreasen18,Reference Lin, Wood, Nelson, Brewer, Spiliotacopoulos and Bruxner19 In contrast, data from samples of people with bipolar disorder show evidence of a relationship between multiple episodes (both manic and depressive) and poorer neurocognitive performance, particularly for verbal learning and memory and for executive function. Reference Quraishi and Frangou4,Reference Savitz, Solms and Ramesar5 In these individuals longer illness duration is also associated with greater impairment, although not as robustly as number of affective episodes. Reference Bora, Yucel and Pantelis7,Reference Bearden, Hoffman and Cannon8 Similarly, meta-analytic evidence from individuals with major depression suggests that symptom severity is significantly associated with neurocognitive impairment in episodic memory, executive function and processing speed, but these associations explain less than 10% of the variance in performance. Reference McDermott and Ebmeier20 Other cross-sectional evidence for a relationship between other indices of severity (duration of illness, number of episodes and length of episodes) and cognitive deficits is variable and conclusions are difficult to draw. Reference McClintock, Husain, Greer and Cullum12

Evidence regarding the longitudinal course of neurocognitive impairment in affective disorders is limited by the lack of longitudinal studies. Reference Douglas and Porter11,Reference Lewandowski, Cohen and Ongur21 The longest follow-up of individuals with bipolar disorder showed that cognitive impairment persisted but did not deteriorate over a 3-year period. Reference Balanza-Martinez, Tabares-Seisdedos, Selva-Vera, Martinez-Aran, Torrent and Salazar-Fraile22 Longitudinal studies of neurocognition in major depression are rarely longer than 6 months, Reference Douglas and Porter11 making it impossible to ascertain how impairment progresses over the illness. Our understanding of the course of neurocognitive impairment in affective disorders is further complicated by the fact that state-related reductions in cognitive performance may persist over the short term, leading to the misclassification of such impairment as trait-related. Reference Savitz, Solms and Ramesar5,Reference Douglas and Porter11

Another problem in interpreting the neurocognitive performance of individuals with affective disorders is the effect of confounding factors, which are often not controlled for in analyses. Reference Douglas and Porter11,Reference Lewandowski, Cohen and Ongur21 These include the impact of medication, illness subtype, age, comorbid disorders and substance use, all of which may influence cognitive performance at the time of testing. In particular, a history of psychosis is rarely reported or controlled for, yet almost all of the published research has shown that current or past psychotic symptoms are associated with greater and more broad cognitive impairments in bipolar disorder and major depression. Reference Martinez-Aran, Vieta, Reinares, Colom, Torrent and Sanchez-Moreno23Reference Hill, Keshavan and Thase27 Individuals with affective disorders without psychotic features have been shown to perform at a level equivalent to healthy controls or show only minimal impairment. Reference Albus, Hubmann, Wahlheim, Sobizack, Franz and Mohr28Reference Rund, Sundet, Asbjornsen, Egeland, Landro and Lund30 The profile of individuals with non-psychotic depression is more consistent with frontostriatal dysfunction (i.e. reduced performance was most evident in attention and executive function), as opposed to the frontotemporal dysfunction associated with schizophrenia. Reference Hill, Keshavan and Thase27 In line with this, meta-analytic evidence demonstrates that the largest decrements in samples with affective psychosis are in psychomotor speed, sustained attention, verbal learning and memory and semantic fluency, similar to those observed in schizophrenia. Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis3,Reference Bora, Yucel and Pantelis31 Altogether, this suggests that psychotic rather than affective pathology is driving impairments; teasing out the effects of psychosis and other confounds is vital to our understanding of the pattern of neurocognitive impairment in affective disorder in regard to clinical staging.

The utility of neurocognition in clinical staging is enhanced if alterations in cognitive performance are evident early in the illness. Research has shown that individuals who later develop schizophrenia demonstrate poor academic performance and intellectual ability in childhood and adolescence. Reference Cannon, Caspi, Moffitt, Harrington, Taylor and Murray32Reference Reichenberg, Weiser, Rabinowitz, Caspi, Schmeidler and Mark37 Furthermore, it is now accepted that individuals at ultra-high risk of psychosis also perform worse than healthy controls across a range of neurocognitive domains. Within this group, those who make the transition to frank psychosis show greater impairment than those who do not develop psychosis, primarily in the verbal domain. The most often cited reductions include lower general vocabulary or verbal IQ score, Reference Eastvold, Heaton and Cadenhead38Reference Woodberry, Seidman, Giuliano, Verdi, Cook and McFarlane41 verbal learning and memory, Reference Eastvold, Heaton and Cadenhead38,Reference Pukrop, Ruhrmann, Schultze-Lutter, Bechdolf, Brockhaus-Dumke and Klosterkötter39,Reference Brewer, Francey, Wood, Jackson, Pantelis and Phillips42Reference Lencz, Smith, McLaughlin, Auther, Nakayama and Hovey44 verbal fluency Reference Pukrop, Ruhrmann, Schultze-Lutter, Bechdolf, Brockhaus-Dumke and Klosterkötter39,Reference Kim, Shin, Jang, Kim, Shim and Park43,Reference Becker, Nieman, Dingemans, van de Fliert, De Haan and Linszen45 and slower processing speed. Reference Pukrop, Ruhrmann, Schultze-Lutter, Bechdolf, Brockhaus-Dumke and Klosterkötter39,Reference Seidman, Giuliano, Meyer, Addington, Cadenhead and Cannon40,Reference Riecher-Rossler, Pflueger, Aston, Borgwardt, Brewer and Gschwandtner46 It remains unclear whether a decrement in cognition occurs from the prodromal stage to the first-episode stage of illness. Some cross-sectional studies have demonstrated that the magnitude of impairment in the ultra-high risk group who later develop psychosis is comparable to first-episode populations, at least in overall ability, Reference Eastvold, Heaton and Cadenhead38,Reference Lencz, Smith, McLaughlin, Auther, Nakayama and Hovey44 verbal IQ score Reference Woodberry, Seidman, Giuliano, Verdi, Cook and McFarlane41 and verbal memory. Reference Eastvold, Heaton and Cadenhead38,Reference Woodberry, Seidman, Giuliano, Verdi, Cook and McFarlane41 Only a few studies have followed ultra-high risk samples over the period of transition to psychosis, and these found little or no progressive impairment in neurocognitive ability. Reference Wood, Brewer, Koutsouradis, Phillips, Francey and Proffitt47Reference Becker, Nieman, Wiltink, Dingemans, van de Fliert and Velthorst49 (Further information available from the authors.)

Potential early neurocognitive impairment in bipolar disorder is less well understood. There is evidence of lowered performance in unaffected relatives of patients, particularly in verbal learning and memory and some executive functions, Reference Bora, Yucel and Pantelis7,Reference Balanza-Martínez, Rubio, Selva-Vera, Martinez-Aran, Sanchez-Moreno and Salazar-Fraile50 suggesting that cognitive reductions may be trait-related. However, findings to date show neurocognition to be an unreliable indicator of future bipolar disorder. Some studies have identified intellectual and language delays and lowered visual spatial reasoning and set-shifting in young people who later developed the illness, Reference Meyer, Carlson, Wiggs, Martinez, Ronsaville and Klimes-Dougan51Reference Tiihonen, Haukka, Henriksson, Cannon, Kieseppa and Laaksonen53 but others found no reduction relative to healthy controls. Reference Cannon, Caspi, Moffitt, Harrington, Taylor and Murray32,Reference Cannon, Moffitt, Caspi, Murray, Harrington and Poulton33,Reference Reichenberg, Weiser, Rabinowitz, Caspi, Schmeidler and Mark37,Reference Zammit, Allebeck, David, Dalman, Hemmingsson and Lundberg54 Once again, conclusions from these studies are limited by confounding psychotic symptoms; most studies did not differentiate the outcomes of bipolar disorder with and without psychosis.

High-risk clinics do not exist for bipolar disorder in the same way that they do for schizophrenia, making it difficult to characterise neurocognitive ability immediately prior to illness onset. Only one study has assessed cognitive performance in the prodromal period of bipolar disorder by investigating the onset of this disorder in patients at ultra-high risk of psychosis. Reference Olvet, Stearns, McLaughlin, Auther, Correll and Cornblatt55 The authors found no difference in premorbid IQ, current IQ or global ability between individuals who developed bipolar disorder and those who did not develop either bipolar disorder or schizophrenia. It is similarly unclear which impairments might be present immediately after the onset of bipolar disorder, especially since a first episode of mania could have been preceded by a period of depression. Albus et al found that individuals with a first episode of mania without psychosis demonstrated cognitive performance that was equivalent to healthy controls. Reference Albus, Hubmann, Wahlheim, Sobizack, Franz and Mohr28 Two other studies of first-episode mania have shown impairment in verbal fluency, perceptual–motor ability, set-shifting and psychomotor speed/attention; Reference Nehra, Chakrabarti and Pradhan56,Reference Gruber, Rosso and Yurgelun-Todd57 however, neither study reported the prevalence of psychotic symptoms in the samples.

The evidence also suggests that early neurocognition is not useful as an indicator of later major depression. Studies of offspring of women with depression have identified a specific reduction in verbal ability Reference Seidman, Giuliano, Smith, Stone, Glatt and Meyer58 or no decrement at all, Reference Klimes-Dougan, Ronsaville, Wiggs and Martinez59,Reference Micco, Henin, Biederman, Rosenbaum, Petty and Rindlaub60 relative to healthy controls. Interestingly, in offspring of women with depression there was an association between current depressive symptoms and performance on some indices of executive function and processing speed, Reference Micco, Henin, Biederman, Rosenbaum, Petty and Rindlaub60 suggesting that reductions in these domains might be related to current symptoms or occur as part of the disease process rather than being markers of vulnerability. On the other hand, evidence from a large population study demonstrated that children who later developed major depression showed poorer performance on tasks of psychomotor speed and attention at age 13 years but no reduction in other domains, Reference Cannon, Moffitt, Caspi, Murray, Harrington and Poulton33 nor deviations in motor or language development. Reference Cannon, Caspi, Moffitt, Harrington, Taylor and Murray32 Considering the evidence to date it is unclear whether cognition is impaired before the diagnosis of depressive disorder.

It is important to note that not all individuals with these severe mental illnesses show cognitive impairment. A quarter to a third of individuals with schizophrenia demonstrate ‘normal’ neurocognitive performance within the average range. Reference Rund, Sundet, Asbjornsen, Egeland, Landro and Lund30,Reference Palmer, Heaton, Paulsen, Kuck, Braff and Harris61 Furthermore, it has been estimated that only 28% of people with major depression, Reference Rund, Sundet, Asbjornsen, Egeland, Landro and Lund30,Reference Iverson, Brooks, Langenecker and Young62 and 38–41% of those with bipolar disorder, Reference Iverson, Brooks, Langenecker and Young62,Reference Martino, Strejilevich, Scapola, Igoa, Marengo and Ais63 have neurocognitive impairment. It is possible that people with psychotic and affective disorders who present with neurocognitive impairments represent a different underlying disease process. Support for the concept of different disease processes comes from demographic and clinical differences between those with impaired and intact neurocognitive performance in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Reference Palmer, Heaton, Paulsen, Kuck, Braff and Harris61,Reference Martino, Strejilevich, Scapola, Igoa, Marengo and Ais63 Clinical staging offers the potential to overcome artificial diagnostic boundaries by incorporating cognitive performance into stage definitions, although its usefulness is dependent on how well it can be used to predict illness progression and treatment response.

In summary, although there is some evidence that neurocognitive impairment might fit within a clinical staging framework, there are too many confounds at present for it to be incorporated into the model. Critically, these conclusions are limited by the large variability of clinical populations with affective disorders, particularly where data from individuals with and without psychotic symptoms are combined. Further, the lack of longitudinal data examining progression over time in affective disorders and comparing individuals with earlyv. late-stage disorder reduces our ability to draw strong inferences.

Neuroimaging

In addition to neurocognitive functioning, a model of clinical staging should differentiate the neurobiological correlates of the disorder's distinct stages. Neurobiological changes associated with mental disorders do not necessarily develop in parallel with behavioural symptoms or correlate with behaviour, Reference Fornito, Yoon, Zalesky, Bullmore and Carter64 highlighting the importance of investigating both. We recently reviewed the imaging literature for psychotic disorders and showed that whereas some neurobiological changes are already present before the illness onset, others arise as it progresses and tend to be more pronounced with severity of illness. Reference Wood, Yung, McGorry and Pantelis2,Reference Oertel-Knochel, Bittner, Knochel, Prvulovic and Hampel65 Similar differential patterns of biomarkers have been suggested for early v. late stages of major depression and bipolar disorder, Reference Hetrick, Parker, Hickie, Purcell, Yung and McGorry66Reference Fernandes, Gama, Maria Cereser, Yatham, Fries and Colpo69 suggesting it might also be possible to find neuroimaging markers of specific illness stages in affective disorders.

Enhanced vulnerability to psychosis is associated with grey-matter volume reductions in prefrontal, limbic and temporo-parietal regions of the brain, Reference Fusar-Poli, Borgwardt, Crescini, Deste, Kempton and Lawrie70 whereas those who later develop first-episode psychosis have more specific reductions in the inferior frontal, superior temporal and parietal regions. Reference Fusar-Poli, Borgwardt, Crescini, Deste, Kempton and Lawrie70,Reference Dazzan, Soulsby, Mechelli, Wood, Velakoulis and Phillips71 Not surprisingly, therefore, individuals with schizophrenia commonly present with reductions in grey matter in the frontotemporal regions. Reference Fornito, Yucel, Patti, Wood and Pantelis72,Reference Ellison-Wright, Glahn, Laird, Thelen and Bullmore73 Consistent with the predictions of clinical staging, these changes become more extensive through first-episode and chronic illness. Reference Chan, Di, McAlonan and Gong74Reference Velakoulis, Wood, Wong, McGorry, Yung and Phillips77 A similar staging pattern has been observed for cortical thickness, Reference Jung, Kim, Jang, Choi, Jung and Park78 and for structural abnormalities affecting white matter. Reference Olabi, Ellison-Wright, McIntosh, Wood, Bullmore and Lawrie79

Progressive brain changes and increased pathological signs related to severity of illness have also been observed in affective disorders. In major depression reduced thickness of the posterior cingulate cortex has been observed in people with non-remitted disorder compared with those in remission, and decreased perfusion in frontal regions and the anterior cingulate cortex has been shown in the non-remission group compared with healthy controls. Reference Jarnum, Eskildsen, Steffensen, Lundbye-Christensen, Simonsen and Thomsen80 There are cautious suggestions that reductions in cerebral and cerebellar grey matter volume, Reference Pillay, Yurgelun-Todd, Bonello, Lafer, Fava and Renshaw81 as well as basal ganglia volume, Reference Pillay, Renshaw, Bonello, Lafer, Fava and Yurgelun-Todd82 are related to severity of illness. Furthermore, basal ganglia volume reductions have been linked to illness duration and the number of prior depressive episodes. Reference Lacerda, Nicoletti, Brambilla, Sassi, Mallinger and Frank83 With increased duration of illness, individuals with major depression have shown reduced glutamate and increased choline concentrations in ventromedial prefrontal regions, Reference Portella, de Diego-Adelino, Gomez-Anson, Morgan-Ferrando, Vives and Puigdemont84 and (more inconsistently) a reduction in hippocampal volume. Reference Sheline, Wang, Gado, Csernansky and Vannier85Reference Kempton, Salvador, Munafo, Geddes, Simmons and Frangou88 In bipolar disorder the number of episodes of illness has been related to enlargement of the lateral ventricles, Reference Strakowski, DelBello, Zimmerman, Getz, Mills and Ret89,Reference Brambilla, Harenski, Nicoletti, Mallinger, Frank and Kupfer90 and decreased cerebellar vermal volume. Reference DelBello, Strakowski, Zimmerman, Hawkins and Sax91 Compared with healthy individuals, grey matter density of the hippocampus, fusiform gyrus and cerebellum of individuals with bipolar disorder has been shown to reduce at an accelerated rate. Reference Moorhead, McKirdy, Sussmann, Hall, Lawrie and Johnstone92

Many structural abnormalities such as ventricular enlargement have been repeatedly associated with both schizophrenia and affective disorders, albeit with greater enlargements in schizophrenia. Reference Savitz, Solms and Ramesar5,Reference Bearden, Hoffman and Cannon8,Reference Kempton, Salvador, Munafo, Geddes, Simmons and Frangou88,Reference Arnone, Cavanagh, Gerber, Lawrie, Ebmeier and McIntosh93,Reference Elkis, Friedman and Wise94 Such non-specific changes perhaps reflect the presence of psychotic symptoms in the affective disorder group, Reference Bearden, Hoffman and Cannon8 and/or similarities relating to clinical stage. Diagnostic differences do exist, however. Smaller hippocampal and amygdala volumes have been observed in individuals with schizophrenia compared with bipolar disorder. Reference Altshuler, Bartzokis, Grieder, Curran, Jimenez and Leight95 Further distinctions on the basis of grey matter deficits have been made, Reference McDonald, Bullmore, Sham, Chitnis, Suckling and MacCabe96 and functional differences in medial frontal and visual cortex, as well as differential disruptions in white matter tracts associated with the occipital and frontal lobes, Reference Sui, Pearlson, Caprihan, Adali, Kiehl and Liu97 have been shown. Whereas volumetric reductions in brain tissue, in particular temporal lobe grey matter, are more consistently found in schizophrenia than in bipolar disorder, white matter hyperintensities are more common in affective disorders. Reference Bearden, Hoffman and Cannon8 Individuals with bipolar disorder additionally show enlargement of basal ganglia and amygdala, whereas those with major depressive disorder are characterised by volume reduction in these regions as well as in the hippocampus. Reference Kempton, Salvador, Munafo, Geddes, Simmons and Frangou88,Reference Strakowski, Adler and DelBello98,Reference Videbech and Ravnkilde99 Affective disorders are furthermore distinguished by increased corpus callosum cross-sectional area in major depression compared with bipolar disorder. Reference Kempton, Salvador, Munafo, Geddes, Simmons and Frangou88 These neurobiological differences may provide useful diagnostic markers in relation to the different stages of the individual disorders.

The reports of white matter pathology indicate that severe mental illness may not simply be a result of structural lesions to the brain, Reference Savitz, Solms and Ramesar5 but rather of abnormal connectivity between regions. Reference Pettersson-Yeo, Allen, Benetti, McGuire and Mechelli100 As early as 1998 Friston suggested that schizophrenia was caused by dysfunctional interaction in the dynamics of associated brain regions rather than by dysfunctional specialisation within a region. Reference Friston101 Certainly in psychotic disorders, stage of illness affects structural and functional networks differently. Reference Wood, Yung, McGorry and Pantelis2 Consistent across stages of illness are findings of reduced (or in some cases increased) connectivity in frontal lobe and frontotemporal interactions, but as illness progresses these patterns become more widespread across the brain and are observed with higher frequencies. Reference Pettersson-Yeo, Allen, Benetti, McGuire and Mechelli100 Progression between stages could represent weakened strength of connections or even a total loss of connections in a network with a consequential imbalance between local and global connections. Indeed, connection patterns could show alterations, resulting in a loss of function such as working memory impairment, Reference Kang, Sponheim, Chafee and Macdonald102 or phenomena such as positive symptoms. Reference Rotarska-Jagiela, van de Ven, Oertel-Knochel, Uhlhaas, Vogeley and Linden103 Evidence for dysconnectivity in grey and white matter across all stages of this disorder, and even before onset of illness, is building. Reference Fornito, Yoon, Zalesky, Bullmore and Carter64,Reference Pettersson-Yeo, Allen, Benetti, McGuire and Mechelli100,Reference Patel, Mahon, Wellington, Zhang, Chaplin and Szeszko104Reference Woodward, Rogers and Heckers106 Suggestions of dysconnectivity in affective disorders are also rapidly emerging, Reference Savitz, Solms and Ramesar5,Reference Hasler and Northoff107Reference Zhang, Wang, Wu, Kuang, Huang and He109 with distinct patterns for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder being identified. Reference Ongur, Lundy, Greenhouse, Shinn, Menon and Cohen110 Factors such as genetics, insults during brain development and neurotransmitter imbalance are thought to influence the process of dysconnectivity. Reference Cole, Anticevic, Repovs and Barch111

For neurobiological changes to consolidate their position in a model of clinical staging, changes caused by the illness need to be distinguished from epiphenomena. Factors such as life stress and substance use have been related to progression in severity of illness, Reference Wood, Yung, McGorry and Pantelis2,Reference Kapczinski, Dias, Kauer-Sant'Anna, Frey, Grassi-Oliveira and Colom112Reference Foti, Kotov, Guey and Bromet114 and individual differences in dysconnectivity have been shown to relate to individual differences in symptom presentation. Reference Cole, Anticevic, Repovs and Barch111 Antidepressant medication has been found to decrease resting-state functional connectivity, Reference McCabe and Mishor115 and the effects of medication on brain structure, volume and functioning also require further investigation. Reference Wood, Yung, McGorry and Pantelis2,Reference Fusar-Poli, Borgwardt, Crescini, Deste, Kempton and Lawrie70,Reference Pettersson-Yeo, Allen, Benetti, McGuire and Mechelli100,Reference Savitz and Drevets113,Reference Ho, Andreasen, Ziebell, Pierson and Magnotta116,Reference Phillips, Travis, Fagiolini and Kupfer117 In addition, the influence of adolescent development, Reference Wood, Yung, McGorry and Pantelis2,Reference Brambilla, Harenski, Nicoletti, Mallinger, Frank and Kupfer90,Reference Pettersson-Yeo, Allen, Benetti, McGuire and Mechelli100,Reference Savitz and Drevets113,Reference Yatham, Kapczinski, Andreazza, Trevor Young, Lam and Kauer-Sant'anna118,Reference Chakos, Lieberman, Bilder, Borenstein, Lerner and Bogerts119 gender, Reference Pettersson-Yeo, Allen, Benetti, McGuire and Mechelli100 and comorbidity, Reference Wood, Yung, McGorry and Pantelis2,Reference Sheline86,Reference Owen, O'Donovan, Thapar and Craddock120,Reference Altamura, Serati, Albano, Paoli, Glick and Dell'Osso121 should be considered. Accounting for these factors provides important challenges for the immediate future.

Current models of clinical staging do not make explicit whether an individual can move down a stage, i.e. whether a remission of symptoms is equivalent to moving from stage 2 to stage 1. However, certain functions can (at least partially) be recovered. Reference Payne and Lomber122,Reference Castren and Rantamaki123 Furthermore, brain volume abnormalities seem to be potentially reversible (in first-episode psychosis), Reference Schaufelberger, Lappin, Duran, Rosa, Uchida and Santos124 or at least to lessen with continued development in childhood-onset schizophrenia. Reference Gogtay125 This suggests regression in severity of illness to some extent, which should be reflected in the staging model.

Neurobiological evidence for staging in severe mental illness is still limited. Different methods adopted by the various studies make it difficult to compare findings and stress the need for future research to adopt a transdiagnostic perspective. Studies investigating disorders with overlapping features will not only be able to highlight shared neurobiological features but may provide evidence for distinct neurobiological markers important for treatment and prognosis. As adolescence is the critical period for onset of severe mental illness, studies should focus on brain networks that develop during this period. Furthermore, multimodal longitudinal studies will be crucial in monitoring transition between stages and associated neurobiological changes. Reference Wood, Yung, McGorry and Pantelis2

Future research

Clinical staging is a promising model for improving our understanding of the way in which severe mental illnesses develop and helping clinicians choose the most appropriate treatment. Both neurocognition and neuroimaging evidence provide tentative support for the application of a staging model to schizophrenia and affective disorders. The paradox here is that we are using the current diagnostic categories to investigate the validity of a model that explicitly attempts to negate the current categorical system. Future work needs to take a transdiagnostic and longitudinal view, covering both neurocognition and neuroimaging in order to overcome current issues.

Footnotes

These authors contributed equally to the work.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 McGorry, PD Hickie, IB Yung, AR Pantelis, C Jackson, HJ Clinical staging of psychiatric disorders: a heuristic framework for choosing earlier, safer and more effective interventions. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2006; 40: 616–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Wood, SJ Yung, AR McGorry, PD Pantelis, C Neuroimaging and treatment evidence for clinical staging in psychotic disorders: from the at-risk mental state to chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70: 619–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Heinrichs, RW Zakzanis, KK. Neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 1998; 12: 426–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Quraishi, S Frangou, S. Neuropsychology of bipolar disorder: a review. J Affect Disord 2002; 72: 209–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Savitz, J Solms, M Ramesar, R Neuropsychological dysfunction in bipolar affective disorder: a critical opinion. Bipolar Disord 2005; 7: 216–35.Google Scholar
6 Robinson, O Sahakian, B. Recurrence in major depressive disorder: a neurocognitive perspective. Psychol Med 2008; 38: 315–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Bora, E Yucel, M Pantelis, C Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. J Affect Disord 2009; 113: 120.Google Scholar
8 Bearden, CE Hoffman, KM Cannon, TD The neuropsychology and neuroanatomy of bipolar affective disorder: a critical review. Bipolar Disord 2001; 3: 106–50.Google Scholar
9 Kurtz, MM Gerraty, RT. A meta-analytic investigation of neurocognitive deficits in bipolar illness: profile and effects of clinical state. Neuropsychology 2009; 23: 551–62.Google Scholar
10 Zakzanis, KK Leach, L Kaplan, E On the nature and pattern of neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1998; 11: 111–9.Google Scholar
11 Douglas, KM Porter, RJ. Longitudinal assessment of neuropsychological function in major depression. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2009; 43: 1105–17.Google Scholar
12 McClintock, SM Husain, MM Greer, TL Cullum, CM Association between depression severity and neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder: a review and synthesis. Neuropsychology 2010; 24: 934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13 Mesholam-Gately, RI Giuliano, AJ Goff, D Faraone, SV Seidman, LJ Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 2009; 23: 315–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14 Szöke, A Trandafir, A Dupont, M-E Méary, A Schürhoff, F Leboyer, M Longitudinal studies of cognition in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2008; 192: 248–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Carpenter, WT Heinrichs, DW Wagman, AMI Deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: the concept. Am J Psychiatry 1988; 145: 578–83.Google ScholarPubMed
16 Fenton, WS McGlashan, TH Antecedents, symptom progression, and long-term outcome of deficit syndrome schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151: 351–6.Google Scholar
17 Cohen, AS Saperstein, AM Gold, JM Kirkpatrick, B Carpenter, WT Buchanan, RW Neuropsychology of the deficit syndrome: new data and meta-analysis of findings to date. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33: 1201–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18 Milev, P Ho, BC Arndt, S Andreasen, NC Predictive values of neurocognition and negative symptoms on functional outcome in schizophrenia: a longitudinal first-episode study with 7-year follow-up. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162: 495506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19 Lin, A Wood, SJ Nelson, B Brewer, WJ Spiliotacopoulos, D Bruxner, A et al Neurocognitive predictors of functional outcome two to 13 years after identification as ultra-high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res 2011; 132: 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20 McDermott, LM Ebmeier, KP A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. J Affect Disord 2009; 119: 18.Google Scholar
21 Lewandowski, K Cohen, B Ongur, D Evolution of neuropsychological dysfunction during the course of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychol Med 2011; 41: 225–41.Google Scholar
22 Balanza-Martinez, V Tabares-Seisdedos, R Selva-Vera, G Martinez-Aran, A Torrent, C Salazar-Fraile, J et al Persistent cognitive dysfunctions in bipolar I disorder and schizophrenic patients: a 3-year follow-up study. Psychother Psychosom 2005; 74: 113–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23 Martinez-Aran, A Vieta, E Reinares, M Colom, F Torrent, C Sanchez-Moreno, J et al Cognitive function across manic or hypomanic, depressed, and euthymic states in bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161: 262–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24 Torrent, C Martínez-Aran, A Daban, C Sánchez-Moreno, J Comes, M Goikolea, JM et al Cognitive impairment in bipolar II disorder. Br J Psychiatry 2006; 189: 254–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25 Glahn, DC Bearden, CE Barguil, M Barrett, J Reichenberg, A Bowden, CL et al The neurocognitive signature of psychotic bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62: 910–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26 Fleming, SK Blasey, C Schatzberg, AF Neuropsychological correlates of psychotic features in major depressive disorders: a review and meta-analysis. J Psychiatr Res 2004; 38: 2735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27 Hill, SK Keshavan, MS Thase, ME Sweeney JA. Neuropsychological dysfunction in antipsychotic-naive first-episode unipolar psychotic depression. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161: 9961003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28 Albus, M Hubmann, W Wahlheim, C Sobizack, N Franz, U Mohr, F Contrasts in neuropsychological test profile between patients with first episode schizophrenia and first episode affective disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1996; 94: 8793.Google Scholar
29 Simonsen, C Sundet, K Vaskinn, A Birkenaes, AB Engh, JA Færden, A et al Neurocognitive dysfunction in bipolar and schizophrenia spectrum disorders depends on history of psychosis rather than diagnostic group. Schizophr Bull 2011; 37: 7383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30 Rund, BR Sundet, K Asbjornsen, A Egeland, J Landro, NI Lund, A et al Neuropsychological test profiles in schizophrenia and non-psychotic depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2006; 113: 350–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31 Bora, E Yucel, M Pantelis, C Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and affective psychoses: meta-analytic study. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 475–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32 Cannon, M Caspi, A Moffitt, TE Harrington, H Taylor, A Murray, RM et al Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59: 449–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33 Cannon, M Moffitt, TE Caspi, A Murray, RM Harrington, H Poulton, R Neuropsychological performance at the age of 13 years and adult schizophreniform disorder. Prospective birth cohort study. Br J Psychiatry 2006; 189: 463–4.Google Scholar
34 David, AS Malmberg, A Brandt, L Allebeck, P Lewis, G IQ and risk for schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study. Psychol Med 1997; 27: 1311–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35 Davidson, M Reichenberg, A Rabinowitz, J Weiser, M Kaplan, Z Mark, M Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156: 1328–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36 MacCabe, JH Lambe, MP Cnattingius, S Torrang, A Bjork, C Sham, PC et al Scholastic achievement at age 16 and risk of schizophrenia and other psychoses: a national cohort study. Psychol Med 2008; 38: 1133–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37 Reichenberg, A Weiser, M Rabinowitz, J Caspi, A Schmeidler, J Mark, M et al A population-based cohort study of premorbid intellectual, language, and behavioral functioning in patients with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and nonpsychotic bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159: 2027–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38 Eastvold, AD Heaton, RK Cadenhead, KS Neurocognitive deficits in the (putative) prodrome and first episode of psychosis. Schizophr Res 2007; 93: 266–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39 Pukrop, R Ruhrmann, S Schultze-Lutter, F Bechdolf, A Brockhaus-Dumke, A Klosterkötter, J Neurocognitive indicators for a conversion to psychosis: comparison of patients in a potentially initial prodromal state who did or did not convert to a psychosis. Schizophr Res 2007; 92: 116–25.Google Scholar
40 Seidman, LJ Giuliano, AJ Meyer, EC Addington, J Cadenhead, KS Cannon, TD et al Neuropsychology of the prodrome to psychosis in the NAPLS Consortium: relationship to family history and conversion to psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67: 578–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41 Woodberry, KA Seidman, LJ Giuliano, AJ Verdi, MB Cook, WL McFarlane, WR Neuropsychological profiles in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis: relationship to psychosis and intelligence. Schizophr Res 2010; 123: 188–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42 Brewer, W Francey, S Wood, S Jackson, H Pantelis, C Phillips, L et al Memory impairments identified in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis who later develop first-episode psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162: 71–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43 Kim, HS Shin, NY Jang, JH Kim, E Shim, G Park, HY et al Social cognition and neurocognition as predictors of conversion to psychosis in individuals at ultra high risk. Schizophr Res 2011; 130: 170–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44 Lencz, T Smith, CW McLaughlin, D Auther, A Nakayama, E Hovey, L et al Generalized and specific neurocognitive deficits in prodromal schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59: 863–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45 Becker, HE Nieman, DH Dingemans, PM van de Fliert, JR De Haan, L Linszen, DH Verbal fluency as a possible predictor for psychosis. Eur Psychiatry 2010; 25: 105–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46 Riecher-Rossler, A Pflueger, MO Aston, J Borgwardt, SJ Brewer, WJ Gschwandtner, U et al Efficacy of using cognitive status in predicting psychosis: a 7-year follow-up. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 66: 1023–30.Google Scholar
47 Wood, SJ Brewer, WJ Koutsouradis, P Phillips, LJ Francey, SM Proffitt, TM et al Cognitive decline following psychosis onset. Data from the PACE clinic. Br J Psychiatry 2007; 191: s527.Google Scholar
48 Hawkins, KA Keefe, R Christensen, B Addington, J Woods, SW Callahan, J et al Neuropsychological course in the prodrome and first epsiode of psychosis: findings from the PRIME North American Double Blind Treatment Study. Schizophr Res 2008; 105: 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
49 Becker, HE Nieman, DH Wiltink, S Dingemans, PM van de Fliert, JR Velthorst, E et al Neurocognitive functioning before and after the first psychotic episode: does psychosis result in cognitive deterioration? Psychol Med 2010; 40: 1599–606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50 Balanza-Martínez, V Rubio, C Selva-Vera, G Martinez-Aran, A Sanchez-Moreno, J Salazar-Fraile, J et al Neurocognitive endophenotypes (endophenocognitypes) from studies of relatives of bipolar disorder subjects: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008; 32: 1426–38.Google Scholar
51 Meyer, SE Carlson, GA Wiggs, EA Martinez, PE Ronsaville, DS Klimes-Dougan, B et al A prospective study of the association among impaired executive functioning, childhood attentional problems, and the development of bipolar disorder. Dev Psychopathol 2004; 16: 461–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52 Sigurdsson, E Fombonne, E Sayal, K Checkley, S Neurodevelopmental antecedents of early-onset bipolar affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry 1999; 174: 121–7.Google Scholar
53 Tiihonen, J Haukka, J Henriksson, M Cannon, M Kieseppa, T Laaksonen, I et al Premorbid intellectual functioning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: results from a cohort study of male conscripts. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162: 1904–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54 Zammit, S Allebeck, P David, AS Dalman, C Hemmingsson, T Lundberg, I et al A longitudinal study of premorbid IQ score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61: 354–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55 Olvet, DM Stearns, WH McLaughlin, D Auther, AM Correll, CU Cornblatt, BA Comparing clinical and neurocognitive features of the schizophrenia prodrome to the bipolar prodrome. Schizophr Res 2010; 123: 5963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
56 Nehra, R Chakrabarti, S Pradhan, BK Khehra N. Comparison of cognitive functions between first-and multi-episode bipolar affective disorders. J Affect Disord 2006; 93: 185–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57 Gruber, SA Rosso, IM Yurgelun-Todd, D. Neuropsychological performance predicts clinical recovery in bipolar patients. J Affect Disord 2008; 105: 253–60.Google Scholar
58 Seidman, LJ Giuliano, AJ Smith, CW Stone, WS Glatt, SJ Meyer, E et al Neuropsychological functioning in adolescents and young adults at genetic risk for schizophrenia and affective psychoses: results from the Harvard and Hillside Adolescent High Risk Studies. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32: 507–24.Google Scholar
59 Klimes-Dougan, B Ronsaville, D Wiggs, EA Martinez, PE Neuropsychological functioning in adolescent children of mothers with a history of bipolar or major depressive disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 60: 957–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60 Micco, JA Henin, A Biederman, J Rosenbaum, JF Petty, C Rindlaub, LA et al Executive functioning in offspring at risk for depression and anxiety. Depress Anxiety 2009; 26: 780–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
61 Palmer, B Heaton, R Paulsen, JS Kuck, J Braff, D Harris, MJ Is it possible to be schizophrenic yet neuropsychologically normal? Neuropsychology 1997; 11: 437–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62 Iverson, GL Brooks, BL Langenecker, SA Young, AH Identifying a cognitive impairment subgroup in adults with mood disorders. J Affect Disord 2011; 132: 360–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
63 Martino, DJ Strejilevich, SA Scapola, M Igoa, A Marengo, E Ais, ED et al Heterogeneity in cognitive functioning among patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 2008; 109: 149–56.Google Scholar
64 Fornito, A Yoon, J Zalesky, A Bullmore, ET Carter, CS General and specific functional connectivity disturbances in first-episode schizophrenia during cognitive control performance. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70: 6472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65 Oertel-Knochel, V Bittner, RA Knochel, C Prvulovic, D Hampel, H Discovery and development of integrative biological markers for schizophrenia. Progr Neurobiol 2011; 95: 686702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66 Hetrick, S Parker, A Hickie, I Purcell, R Yung, A McGorry, P Early identification and intervention in depressive disorders: towards a clinical staging model. Psychother Psychosom 2008; 77: 263–70.Google Scholar
67 Kapczinski, F Dias, VV Kauer-Sant'Anna, M Brietzke, E Vazquez, GH Vieta, E et al The potential use of biomarkers as an adjunctive tool for staging bipolar disorder. Progr Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2009; 33: 1366–71.Google Scholar
68 Kauer-Sant'Anna, M Kapczinski, F Andreazza, AC Bond, DJ Lam, RW Young, LT et al Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and inflammatory markers in patients with early- vs. late-stage bipolar disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2009; 12: 447–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
69 Fernandes, BS Gama, CS Maria Cereser, K Yatham, LN Fries, GR Colpo, G et al Brain-derived neurotrophic factor as a state-marker of mood episodes in bipolar disorders: a systematic review and meta-regression analysis. J Psychiatr Res 2011; 45: 9951004.Google Scholar
70 Fusar-Poli, P Borgwardt, S Crescini, A Deste, G Kempton, MJ Lawrie, S et al Neuroanatomy of vulnerability to psychosis: a voxel-based meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011; 35: 1175–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71 Dazzan, P Soulsby, B Mechelli, A Wood, SJ Velakoulis, D Phillips, LJ et al Volumetric abnormalities predating the onset of schizophrenia and affective psychoses: an MRI study in subjects at ultrahigh risk of psychosis. Schizophr Bull 2012: 38: 1083–91.Google Scholar
72 Fornito, A Yucel, M Patti, J Wood, SJ Pantelis, C Mapping grey matter reductions in schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Schizophr Res 2009; 108: 104–13.Google Scholar
73 Ellison-Wright, I Glahn, DC Laird, AR Thelen, SM Bullmore, E The anatomy of first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165: 1015–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
74 Chan, RC Di, X McAlonan, GM Gong, QY Brain anatomical abnormalities in high-risk individuals, first-episode, and chronic schizophrenia: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of illness progression. Schizophr Bull 2011; 37: 177–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
75 Premkumar, P Kumari, V Corr, PJ Sharma, T Frontal lobe volumes in schizophrenia: effects of stage and duration of illness. J Psychiatr Res 2006; 40: 627–37.Google Scholar
76 Mitelman, SA Nikiforova, YK Canfield, EL Hazlett, EA Brickman, AM Shihabuddin, L et al A longitudinal study of the corpus callosum in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2009; 114: 144–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
77 Velakoulis, D Wood, SJ Wong, MT McGorry, PD Yung, A Phillips, L et al Hippocampal and amygdala volumes according to psychosis stage and diagnosis: a magnetic resonance imaging study of chronic schizophrenia, first-episode psychosis, and ultra-high-risk individuals. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63: 139–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
78 Jung, WH Kim, JS Jang, JH Choi, JS Jung, MH Park, JY et al Cortical thickness reduction in individuals at ultra-high-risk for psychosis. Schizophr Bull 2011; 37: 839–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
79 Olabi, B Ellison-Wright, I McIntosh, AM Wood, SJ Bullmore, E Lawrie, SM Are there progressive brain changes in schizophrenia? A meta-analysis of structural magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70: 8896.Google Scholar
80 Jarnum, H Eskildsen, SF Steffensen, EG Lundbye-Christensen, S Simonsen, CW Thomsen, IS et al Longitudinal MRI study of cortical thickness, perfusion, and metabolite levels in major depressive disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2011; 124: 435–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
81 Pillay, SS Yurgelun-Todd, DA Bonello, CM Lafer, B Fava, M Renshaw, PF A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of cerebral and cerebellar gray matter volume in primary unipolar major depression: relationship to treatment response and clinical severity. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 42: 7984.Google Scholar
82 Pillay, SS Renshaw, PF Bonello, CM Lafer, BC Fava, M Yurgelun-Todd, D A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of caudate and lenticular nucleus gray matter volume in primary unipolar major depression: relationship to treatment response and clinical severity. Psychiatr Res 1998; 84: 6174.Google Scholar
83 Lacerda, AL Nicoletti, MA Brambilla, P Sassi, RB Mallinger, AG Frank, E et al Anatomical MRI study of basal ganglia in major depressive disorder. Psychiatr Res 2003; 124: 129–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
84 Portella, MJ de Diego-Adelino, J Gomez-Anson, B Morgan-Ferrando, R Vives, Y Puigdemont, D et al Ventromedial prefrontal spectroscopic abnormalities over the course of depression: a comparison among first episode, remitted recurrent and chronic patients. J Psychiatr Res 2011; 45: 427–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
85 Sheline, YI Wang, PW Gado, MH Csernansky, JG Vannier, MW Hippocampal atrophy in recurrent major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996; 93: 3908–13.Google Scholar
86 Sheline, YI. 3D MRI studies of neuroanatomic changes in unipolar major depression: the role of stress and medical comorbidity. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48: 791800.Google Scholar
87 MacQueen, GM Campbell, S McEwen, BS Macdonald, K Amano, S Joffe, RT et al Course of illness, hippocampal function, and hippocampal volume in major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003; 100: 1387–92.Google Scholar
88 Kempton, MJ Salvador, Z Munafo, MR Geddes, JR Simmons, A Frangou, S et al Structural neuroimaging studies in major depressive disorder: meta-analysis and comparison with bipolar disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68: 675–90.Google Scholar
89 Strakowski, SM DelBello, MP Zimmerman, ME Getz, GE Mills, NP Ret, J et al Ventricular and periventricular structural volumes in first- versus multipleepisode bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159: 1841–7.Google Scholar
90 Brambilla, P Harenski, K Nicoletti, M Mallinger, AG Frank, E Kupfer, DJ et al MRI study of posterior fossa structures and brain ventricles in bipolar patients. J Psychiatr Res 2001; 35: 313–22.Google Scholar
91 DelBello, MP Strakowski, SM Zimmerman, ME Hawkins, JM Sax, KW MRI analysis of the cerebellum in bipolar disorder: a pilot study. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999; 21: 63–8.Google Scholar
92 Moorhead, TW McKirdy, J Sussmann, JE Hall, J Lawrie, SM Johnstone, EC et al Progressive gray matter loss in patients with bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62: 894900.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
93 Arnone, D Cavanagh, J Gerber, D Lawrie, SM Ebmeier, KP McIntosh, AM Magnetic resonance imaging studies in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 194201.Google Scholar
94 Elkis, H Friedman, L Wise, A Meltzer HY. Meta-analyses of studies of ventricular enlargement and cortical sulcal prominence in mood disorders. Comparisons with controls or patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52: 735–46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
95 Altshuler, LL Bartzokis, G Grieder, T Curran, J Jimenez, T Leight, K et al An MRI study of temporal lobe structures in men with bipolar disorder or schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48: 147–62.Google Scholar
96 McDonald, C Bullmore, E Sham, P Chitnis, X Suckling, J MacCabe, J et al Regional volume deviations of brain structure in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. Computational morphometry study. Br J Psychiatry 2005; 186: 369–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
97 Sui, J Pearlson, G Caprihan, A Adali, T Kiehl, KA Liu, J et al Discriminating schizophrenia and bipolar disorder by fusing fMRI and DTI in a multimodal CCA+ joint ICA model. Neuroimage 2011; 57: 839–55.Google Scholar
98 Strakowski, SM Adler, CM DelBello, MP Volumetric MRI studies of mood disorders: do they distinguish unipolar and bipolar disorder? Bipolar Disord 2002; 4: 80–8.Google Scholar
99 Videbech, P Ravnkilde, B. Hippocampal volume and depression: a metaanalysis of MRI studies. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161: 1957–66.Google Scholar
100 Pettersson-Yeo, W Allen, P Benetti, S McGuire, P Mechelli, A Dysconnectivity in schizophrenia: where are we now? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011; 35: 1110–24.Google Scholar
101 Friston, KJ. The disconnection hypothesis. Schizophr Res 1998; 30: 115–25.Google Scholar
102 Kang, SS Sponheim, SR Chafee, MV Macdonald, AW Disrupted functional connectivity for controlled visual processing as a basis for impaired spatial working memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49: 2836–47.Google Scholar
103 Rotarska-Jagiela, A van de Ven, V Oertel-Knochel, V Uhlhaas, PJ Vogeley, K Linden, DE Resting-state functional network correlates of psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2010; 117: 2130.Google Scholar
104 Patel, S Mahon, K Wellington, R Zhang, J Chaplin, W Szeszko, PR A meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging studies of the corpus callosum in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2011; 129: 149–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
105 Whitfield-Gabrieli, S Thermenos, HW Milanovic, S Tsuang, MT Faraone, SV McCarley, RW et al Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of persons with schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009; 106: 1279–84.Google Scholar
106 Woodward, ND Rogers, B Heckers, S Functional resting-state networks are differentially affected in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2011; 130: 8693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
107 Hasler, G Northoff, G. Discovering imaging endophenotypes for major depression. Molec Psychiatry 2011; 16: 604–19.Google Scholar
108 Hamilton, JP Furman, DJ Chang, C Thomason, ME Dennis, E Gotlib, IH Default-mode and task-positive network activity in major depressive disorder: implications for adaptive and maladaptive rumination. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70: 327–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
109 Zhang, J Wang, J Wu, Q Kuang, W Huang, X He, Y et al Disrupted brain connectivity networks in drug-naive, first-episode major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70: 334–42.Google Scholar
110 Ongur, D Lundy, M Greenhouse, I Shinn, AK Menon, V Cohen, BM et al Default mode network abnormalities in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2010; 183: 5968.Google Scholar
111 Cole, MW Anticevic, A Repovs, G Barch, D Variable global dysconnectivity and individual differences in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70: 4350.Google Scholar
112 Kapczinski, F Dias, VV Kauer-Sant'Anna, M Frey, BN Grassi-Oliveira, R Colom, F et al Clinical implications of a staging model for bipolar disorders. Exp Rev Neurotherap 2009; 9: 957–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
113 Savitz, J Drevets, WC. Bipolar and major depressive disorder: neuroimaging the developmental-degenerative divide. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2009; 33: 699771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
114 Foti, DJ Kotov, R Guey, LT Bromet, EJ Cannabis use and the course of schizophrenia: 10-year follow-up after first hospitalization. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167: 987–93.Google Scholar
115 McCabe, C Mishor, Z Antidepressant medications reduce subcorticalcortical resting-state functional connectivity in healthy volunteers. Neuroimage 2011; 57: 1317–23.Google Scholar
116 Ho, BC Andreasen, NC Ziebell, S Pierson, R Magnotta, V Long-term antipsychotic treatment and brain volumes: a longitudinal study of first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68: 128–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
117 Phillips, ML Travis, MJ Fagiolini, A Kupfer, DJ Medication effects in neuroimaging studies of bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165: 313–20.Google Scholar
118 Yatham, LN Kapczinski, F Andreazza, AC Trevor Young, L Lam, RW Kauer-Sant'anna, M Accelerated age-related decrease in brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels in bipolar disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2009; 12: 137–9.Google Scholar
119 Chakos, MH Lieberman, JA Bilder, RM Borenstein, M Lerner, G Bogerts, B et al Increase in caudate nuclei volumes of first-episode schizophrenic patients taking antipsychotic drugs. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151: 1430–6.Google Scholar
120 Owen, MJ O'Donovan, MC Thapar, A Craddock, N Neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 198: 173–5.Google Scholar
121 Altamura, AC Serati, M Albano, A Paoli, RA Glick, ID Dell'Osso, B An epidemiologic and clinical overview of medical and psychopathological comorbidities in major psychoses. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2011; 261: 489507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
122 Payne, BR Lomber, SG. Reconstructing functional systems after lesions of cerebral cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 2001; 2: 911–9.Google Scholar
123 Castren, E Rantamaki, T. The role of BDNF and its receptors in depression and antidepressant drug action: reactivation of developmental plasticity. Dev Neurobiol 2010; 70: 289–97.Google Scholar
124 Schaufelberger, MS Lappin, JM Duran, FL Rosa, PG Uchida, RR Santos, LC et al Lack of progression of brain abnormalities in first-episode psychosis: a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychol Med 2011; 41: 1677–89.Google Scholar
125 Gogtay, N. Cortical brain development in schizophrenia: insights from neuroimaging studies in childhood-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2008; 34: 30–6.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.