Background
Cannabis use can increase the risk of psychosis (Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Marconi, Carra, Fraietta, Trotta, Bonomo and Murray2015; Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Quattrone, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson and Quigley2019; Karpov, Lindgren, Kieseppa, Wegelius, & Suvisaari, Reference Karpov, Lindgren, Kieseppa, Wegelius and Suvisaari2021; Luzi, Morrison, Powell, di Forti, & Murray, Reference Luzi, Morrison, Powell, di Forti and Murray2008; Murray, Paparelli, Morrison, Marconi, & Di Forti, Reference Murray, Paparelli, Morrison, Marconi and Di Forti2013) and its prevalence is higher in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia (Bersani, Orlandi, Kotzalidis, & Pancheri, Reference Bersani, Orlandi, Kotzalidis and Pancheri2002; Hartz et al., Reference Hartz, Pato, Medeiros, Cavazos-Rehg, Sobell and Knowles2014), bipolar disorder, depressive and anxiety disorders, and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Koenfal, Gabrys, & Porath, Reference Koenfal, Gabrys and Porath2019; Lev-Ran, Le Foll, McKenzie, George, & Rehm, Reference Lev-Ran, Le Foll, McKenzie, George and Rehm2013) than in healthy controls.
Some cognitive and reasoning biases have been reported across the psychosis continuum including people with first episode psychosis (FEP), chronic psychosis, clinical and familial high risk for psychosis, and non-clinical samples with psychotic-like experiences (Henquet et al., Reference Henquet, van Os, Pries, Rauschenberg, Delespaul, Kenis and Guloksuz2022; Langdon, Still, Connors, Ward, & Catts, Reference Langdon, Still, Connors, Ward and Catts2014; Linney, Peters, & Ayton, Reference Linney, Peters and Ayton1998; Moritz, Van Quaquebeke, & Lincoln, Reference Moritz, Van Quaquebeke and Lincoln2012; Van Dael et al., Reference Van Dael, Versmissen, Janssen, Myin-Germeys, van Os and Krabbendam2006). These cognitive biases could increase psychosis risk by affecting the perception of others, decision-making about social situations, the meaning attributed to certain social stimuli, and real-life functioning (Green, Horan, & Lee, Reference Green, Horan and Lee2019; Mucci et al., Reference Mucci, Galderisi, Gibertoni, Rossi, Rocca and Bertolino2021).
Aberrant salience is the unusual or incorrect assignment of meaning to neutral stimuli, which can favor the development of attentional biases that may in turn lead to the perception that the environment is dangerous and to the development of paranoid ideation (Howes & Murray, Reference Howes and Murray2014; Kapur, Reference Kapur2003). Experimental illusion studies have focused on the paradigm of hearing voices in neutral random signals (white noise) in the absence of actual speech (speech illusion, SI) (Galdos et al., Reference Galdos, Simons, Fernandez-Rivas, Wichers, Peralta, Lataster and van Os2011).
Other cognitive biases have also consistently been reported in patients with schizophrenia (Hofer, Biedermann, Yalcin, & Fleischhacker, Reference Hofer, Biedermann, Yalcin and Fleischhacker2010; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Altshuler, Glahn, Miklowitz, Ochsner and Green2013), such as the jumping to conclusion (JTC) reasoning bias: under conditions of uncertainty, people with delusions use less information to arrive at a decision and express greater confidence in their judgment than controls (Dudley, Taylor, Wickham, & Hutton, Reference Dudley, Taylor, Wickham and Hutton2016; Garety & Freeman, Reference Garety and Freeman2013; Lincoln, Ziegler, Mehl, & Rief, Reference Lincoln, Ziegler, Mehl and Rief2010; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Mondelli, Stilo, Trotta, Sideli, Ajnakina and Di Forti2020; So, Siu, Wong, Chan, & Garety, Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016; Tripoli et al., Reference Tripoli, Quattrone, Ferraro, Gayer-Anderson, Rodriguez, La Cascia and Di Forti2021).
Also, deficits in facial recognition processing (discrimination, encoding, and recognition) have been found in people with schizophrenia (Archer, Hay, & Young, Reference Archer, Hay and Young1992; Shin et al., Reference Shin, Na, Ha, Kang, Yoo and Kwon2008; van 't Wout, Aleman, Kessels, Laroi, & Kahn, Reference van 't Wout, Aleman, Kessels, Laroi and Kahn2004; Walther et al., Reference Walther, Federspiel, Horn, Bianchi, Wiest, Wirth and Muller2009), with milder degrees of facial recognition impairments observed in non-affected first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia (Li, Chan, Zhao, Hong, & Gong, Reference Li, Chan, Zhao, Hong and Gong2010), thus suggesting that this could be a potential endophenotypic marker of psychosis (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Erzin, Delespaul, Kenis and Guloksuz2022a; Soria Bauser et al., Reference Soria Bauser, Thoma, Aizenberg, Brune, Juckel and Daum2012; Tripoli et al., Reference Tripoli, Quattrone, Ferraro, Gayer-Anderson, La Cascia, La Barbera and Murray2022).
Despite consistent evidence supporting the presence of impairments across cognition and social cognition in schizophrenia (Green et al., Reference Green, Horan and Lee2019; Velthorst et al., Reference Velthorst, Fett, Reichenberg, Perlman, van Os, Bromet and Kotov2017), the effect of cannabis use on cognitive and social cognitive functioning in patients with FEP and their siblings is still controversial (Arnold, Allott, Farhall, Killackey, & Cotton, Reference Arnold, Allott, Farhall, Killackey and Cotton2015; Bruins, Pijnenborg, investigators, Visser, & Castelein, Reference Bruins, Pijnenborg, Visser and Castelein2021; Clausen et al., Reference Clausen, Hjorthoj, Thorup, Jeppesen, Petersen, Bertelsen and Nordentoft2014; Meijer et al., Reference Meijer, Dekker, Koeter, Quee, van Beveren and Meijer2012). Most studies report better or similar neuropsychological functioning in people with schizophrenia and FEP that use cannabis than in those who do not (Rabin, Zakzanis, & George, Reference Rabin, Zakzanis and George2011; Wobrock et al., Reference Wobrock, Falkai, Schneider-Axmann, Hasan, Galderisi and Davidson2013; Yucel et al., Reference Yucel, Bora, Lubman, Solowij, Brewer, Cotton and Pantelis2012). A recent study in the EU-GEI sample using the degraded facial affect recognition task reported better facial emotion recognition processing in cannabis users with schizophrenia, their siblings, and healthy controls, both for total scores and for specific emotion recognition (neutral, happy, fearful, and angry) relative to non-users (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Radhakrishnan, Pence, Erzin and Guloksuz2022b). However, other studies have observed poorer cognitive performance (D'Souza et al., Reference D'Souza, Abi-Saab, Madonick, Forselius-Bielen, Doersch, Braley and Krystal2005; Mata et al., Reference Mata, Rodriguez-Sanchez, Pelayo-Teran, Perez-Iglesias, Gonzalez-Blanch, Ramirez-Bonilla and Crespo-Facorro2008) or no differences in some cognitive tasks in people with schizophrenia using cannabis (Ahuir et al., Reference Ahuir, Crosas, Estrada, Zabala, Perez-Munoz, Gonzalez-Fernandez and Labad2021; Jockers-Scherubl et al., Reference Jockers-Scherubl, Wolf, Radzei, Schlattmann, Rentzsch, Gomez-Carrillo de Castro and Kuhl2007; Sevy et al., Reference Sevy, Burdick, Visweswaraiah, Abdelmessih, Lukin, Yechiam and Bechara2007).
As social skills may be necessary for patients to acquire and use some substances, especially in context where access is more restricted, some researchers suggest that cannabis use in people with FEP could correlate with better social abilities, more drug taking opportunities, and better neurocognitive functioning (Arnold et al., Reference Arnold, Allott, Farhall, Killackey and Cotton2015; Menendez-Miranda et al., Reference Menendez-Miranda, Garcia-Alvarez, Garcia-Portilla, Gonzalez-Blanco, Saiz and Bobes2019; Rodriguez-Sanchez et al., Reference Rodriguez-Sanchez, Ayesa-Arriola, Mata, Moreno-Calle, Perez-Iglesias, Gonzalez-Blanch and Crespo-Facorro2010; Yucel et al., Reference Yucel, Bora, Lubman, Solowij, Brewer, Cotton and Pantelis2012). Indeed, previous research in the EU-GEI sample suggests that better premorbid social and cognitive functioning could contribute to the likelihood of beginning to use cannabis before psychosis onset in patients with FEP (Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, La Cascia, Quattrone, Sideli, Matranga, Capuccio and Di Forti2020).
In this study, we sought to examine for the first time the relationship between cannabis use and speech illusions, facial recognition processing deficits, and jumping to conclusions in patients with FEP, their non-psychotic siblings, and controls. These biases may underlie processes in social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia related with positive symptoms of schizophrenia such as persecutory delusions and auditory hallucinations. Following previous studies, we hypothesized that lifetime cannabis use would correlate with lower odds of cognitive biases relative to lifetime abstinence. As secondary objectives, we aimed to explore the association of frequency of cannabis use and cannabis potency with the odds of cognitive biases in cannabis users.
Methods
Study design
Members of a large, international, multisite, observational study, The European Network of National Schizophrenia Networks Studying Gene-Environment Interactions (EU-GEI), recruited participants between May 2010 and April 2015 from 17 catchment areas in 6 countries (Brazil, France, Italy, the Netherlands, Spain, and the United Kingdom). Study goals include analyzing the effects of genetic, environmental, and clinical variables and their interaction on the development, severity, and outcome of schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders. The EU-GEI team recruited a subset of people with FEP for a concurrent case-control study and controls from the same catchment areas. Additionally, they recruited patients' siblings to focus on the role of gene-environment interaction of the vulnerability and severity of psychosis in a family-based setting. Detailed study procedures are available in (Gayer-Anderson et al., Reference Gayer-Anderson, Jongsma, Di Forti, Quattrone, Velthorst, de Haan and Morgan2020; Jongsma et al., Reference Jongsma, Gayer-Anderson, Lasalvia, Quattrone, Mule and Szoke2018).
Participants
The EU-GEI study sample included 1130 participants with FEP who attended mental health services in the catchment areas. Inclusion criteria for patients were: (1) diagnosis of non-organic psychotic disorder, (2) 18–64 years of age, (3) resident within one of the study catchment areas at the time of their first presentation. Exclusion criteria included: (1) presence of psychotic symptoms due to acute intoxication (ICD10: F1X.5) or organic psychosis (ICD10: F09), (2) previous contact with mental health services because of psychotic symptoms outside of the study period.
We also recruited siblings of FEP participants, aged 18 years or older (N = 265) via communications with the patient and/or his or her reference clinician. We excluded siblings if they presented current or past psychotic disorders (including a psychosis diagnosis within the time frame of the study) or if they had received treatment with antipsychotic medication.
We recruited volunteers from the same catchment areas and same age range as patients for the control sample by using a mixture of random and quota sampling to maximize the representativeness of samples in each catchment area. A total of 1497 controls agreed to participate in the study. We excluded controls if they had received psychotic disorder diagnoses, including during the study, or had undergone treatment with antipsychotic medication. More information about the recruitment procedures is available elsewhere (Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Quattrone, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson and Quigley2019; Gayer-Anderson et al., Reference Gayer-Anderson, Jongsma, Di Forti, Quattrone, Velthorst, de Haan and Morgan2020; van Os et al., Reference van Os, Rutten, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul, Viechtbauer, van Zelst and Mirjanic2014).
For the purposes of this study, we included participants that had complete data on cannabis use and complete assessments of each of the three cognitive biases. This yielded a final sample size of 543 participants with FEP, 203 siblings, and 1168 controls (see online Supplementary Figure 1).
Local ethical committees of all sites approved the study at all the study sites. All participants gave written informed consent before entering the study. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.
Measures
Demographic and clinical measures
We used the modified version of the Medical Research Council (MRC) socio-demographic scale to collect socio-demographic data (Mallett, Leff, Bhugra, Pang, & Zhao, Reference Mallett, Leff, Bhugra, Pang and Zhao2002). Diagnoses were operationalized through the 90-item computerized Operational CRITeria (OPCRIT) system for psychosis (McGuffin, Farmer, & Harvey, Reference McGuffin, Farmer and Harvey1991; Williams, Farmer, Ackenheil, Kaufmann, & McGuffin, Reference Williams, Farmer, Ackenheil, Kaufmann and McGuffin1996) in the group of patients. To estimate full scale-IQ scores (including Digit Symbol Substitution, Arithmetic, BlockDesign, and Information subtests) we used an abbreviated and adapted version of the WAIS (Velthorst et al., Reference Velthorst, Levine, Henquet, de Haan, van Os, Myin-Germeys and Reichenberg2013). We measured clinical symptoms with the CAPE scale (Konings, Bak, Hanssen, van Os, & Krabbendam, Reference Konings, Bak, Hanssen, van Os and Krabbendam2006; Mossaheb et al., Reference Mossaheb, Becker, Schaefer, Klier, Schloegelhofer, Papageorgiou and Amminger2012), a questionnaire designed to rate self-reports of positive, negative, and depressive psychotic experiences. Duration of untreated psychosis (in weeks) was estimated with the Notting-ham Onset Schedule (Singh et al., Reference Singh, Cooper, Fisher, Tarrant, Lloyd, Banjo and Jones2005).
Measures of cannabis use
Information about cannabis use was collected with the Cannabis Experience Questionnaire (CEQ) further modified for the EUGEI study (CEQEU−GEI) (Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Quattrone, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson and Quigley2019). The CEQ was modified to (1) include questions to assess dependence for cannabis use and other drugs, and (2) to describe use and changes in cannabis use over specific age periods. We asked participants whether they had ever used cannabis in their lifetime and, if so, we asked about the frequency, type (high or low potency), and duration of use based on the current pattern of use, or, when there was no current use of cannabis, based on the pattern of use that described best the overall pattern of cannabis use. We subdivided cannabis use frequency into three levels: daily use, occasional use (ranging from only once or twice in a lifetime to more than once a week), and never use (absence of lifetime cannabis use) (Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Quattrone, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson and Quigley2019; Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, La Cascia, Quattrone, Sideli, Matranga, Capuccio and Di Forti2020).
We used data on the concentration of Δ⁹-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in the different types of cannabis available across Europe from in the European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction 2016 report to create a measure of cannabis potency. A cutoff of THC = 10% was used to define the potency variable [high (> = 10%) v. low (<10%) potency] based on the mean THC concentration expected in the different types of cannabis available across the sites (for further information, please see online Supplementary Methods and Di Forti et al. (Reference Di Forti, Quattrone, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson and Quigley2019)).
Cognitive bias assessment
To explore speech illusions and aberrance salience we used the White Noise Task. The task consists of a random presentation of 75 audio fragments, 25 of which include just white noise, 25 that contain white noise and barely audible speech, and 25 that include white noise mixed with clearly audible speech, with positive, negative, or neutral affective content. Participants listened to the sounds binaurally through headphones. The length of the task was approximately 15 min. After each fragment, we asked participants to press specific buttons on a keyboard which reflected what they heard: (1) speech with positive content; (2) speech with negative content; (3) speech with neutral content; (4) absence of speech, and (5) presence of speech but uncertain to choose between positive, negative, or neutral emotional valence. Speech illusion was defined as a white noise fragment in which any speech was heard (option 1, 2, 3, or 5 in just white noise fragments). Only 25 out of 75 fragments contained white noises, so the maximum score for speech illusion was 25. A dichotomous variable was then calculated based on a cut-off of two or more speech illusions independently of the emotional valence attributed by the participant (Catalan et al., Reference Catalan, Simons, Bustamante, Drukker, Madrazo, de Artaza and Gonzalez-Torres2014).
We used the Benton Facial Recognition Test (BFRT) (Benton, Reference Benton1994; Benton and Van Allen, Reference Benton and Van Allen1968) to assess facial recognition processing. The BFRT is a face discrimination test in which participants are required to match a target face to either one face keeping the same viewpoint and lighting conditions (6 items) or three or six faces presented simultaneously that vary in viewpoints and lighting (16 items). The total score is calculated based on the total correct answers given (maximum score is 54). Deficits in facial recognition processing were defined as scores of 20 or fewer correct answers in the BFRT (Duchaine & Nakayama, Reference Duchaine and Nakayama2006).
The Drawing to Decisions (DTD) index (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Waller, Emsley, Jolley, Kuipers, Bebbington and Freeman2015; So et al., Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016) was used to assess Jumping To Conclusion (JTC) bias. We obtained this index from the probabilistic reasoning (beads) task, with 60:40 task ratios. Two jars of beads were shown to participants in equal but opposite ratios (85 red and 15 blue and vice versa). Both jars were hidden, and researchers told participants that individual beads were drawn consecutively from one jar. The beads were actually presented in a prespecified sequence. Participants were required to either decide from which jar the beads had come or postpone the decision (up to a maximum of 20 beads). The key outcome variable employed as an index of the JTC bias was the number of ‘Draws-To-Decision’ (DTD); the lower the DTD, the greater the JTC bias. For the purposes of this study JTC bias was defined as a DTD index of 2 or less in the beads task (Klein & Pinkham, Reference Klein and Pinkham2018), as participants are considered to have an extreme JTC bias when a decision is made after presentation of two or fewer items (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Freeman, Jolley, Dunn, Bebbington, Fowler and Dudley2005).
Statistical analysis
We calculated means and standard deviations for continuous variables and frequency and percentages for categorical variables. For the comparisons in cognitive biases and frequency of cannabis use between FEP patients, siblings, and controls, we used Chi-square tests. We used ANOVA analyses to compare quantitative socio-demographic variables between the three groups. We used the Bonferroni test for post-hoc pairwise comparisons.
Considering the uneven distribution of the cognitive biases, we decided on a dichotomous analysis and used cutoffs based on previous studies using customary definitions of cognitive biases from the literature. To examine the influence of group and frequency of cannabis use on cognitive biases variables we conducted three sets of stratified logistic regression model analyses, as we were interested in analyzing the effect of cannabis use on the three cognitive biases separately. We adjusted our analyses for demographic variables: sex, age, years in education, ethnicity, employment (yes/no), migrant status (yes/no), and intelligence quotient (IQ). Variables were included in two models; Model 1 included clinical group (FEP, sibling, or control), demographic variables (sex, age, years of education, employment, and migration), and IQ; Model 2 added the frequency of lifetime cannabis use (never/occasional/daily) and the interaction between the frequency and the clinical group. We performed secondary analyses to examine the effects of frequency of use (occasional v. daily) and potency of cannabis (high v. low) in the group of cannabis users only.
We conducted supplementary analyses in the FEP group to additionally adjust Model 2 for the (i) severity of negative psychotic symptomatology (CAPE Negative) and (ii) Duration of Untreated Psychosis (DUP).
The level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 25.
Results
Demographic and clinical characteristics
Socio-demographic characteristics of the sample are described in Table 1. We observed significant differences between the three clinical groups in sex, age, country, ethnicity, years in education, estimated IQ, and frequency of cannabis use. The FEP sample not analyzed in this study (n = 587; see online Supplementary Figure 1) showed significantly lower IQ, included lower rates of white and mixed ethnic groups and higher rates of black and north African ethnic groups and migrants, and higher rates of high-potency cannabis use relative to the FEP sample analyzed in the study (n = 543). Relative to the sibling and control samples not analyzed in the study, the sibling sample not analyzed in the study was significantly older and had fewer years in education and the control sample not analyzed in the study was older and included higher rates of white ethnic group. We did not find any significant differences in any other demographic or cannabis use variables on cognitive bias variables between the samples analyzed and not analyzed in this study.
CAPE, Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences scale; DUP, Duration of Untreated Psychosis; FEP, First-Episode Psychosis; IQ, Intelligence Quotient.
a Outlier cases for DUP were not included in the analysis (n = 14).
Cannabis use
Participants with FEP were less likely to have never used cannabis (v. occasional and daily use) than siblings and controls. Thus, 38.9% of FEPs, 59.6% of siblings, and 52.3% of controls had never used cannabis (χ2 (4) = 176.6 < 0.001). Patients were also more likely to have used cannabis daily (29.3%) relative to controls (6.9%) and siblings (5.9%); (χ2 (2) = 176.6; p < 0.001). Occasional use of cannabis was more frequent in the control (40.8%) than in the FEP group (31.9%); (χ2 (4) = 176.6 < 0.001) (see Table 1). In the subgroup of cannabis users, high potency (64%) was more frequent than low potency use (36%) in siblings (χ2 (2) = 8.248; p < 0.016), with no significant differences in the FEP or control groups.
Association of clinical group and frequency of cannabis use with cognitive biases
FEP patients presented a higher proportion of all cognitive biases than siblings and controls. Across the whole sample, cognitive biases in the three tests were significantly more frequent in participants that had never used cannabis relative to those with occasional or daily use. Occasional users showed cognitive biases less frequently than daily users (all p < 0.001) (Table 2 and online Supplementary Figure 1).
BFR, Benton facial recognition; DTD, drawing to decision.
Table 3 shows the logistic regression models of speech illusions (SI). In model 1, clinical group did not show a significant association with the presence of SI. In model 2, we found that frequency of cannabis use was a predictive factor in the model, with no significant effects of clinical group or the interaction between clinical group and frequency of use. Specifically, occasional use of cannabis correlated with 0.64-fold decreased odds (OR = 0.646; 95% CI 0.457–0.9113), and daily use of cannabis with 0.60-fold decreased odds (OR = 0.605; 95% CI 0.368–0.997) of having SI relative to never use of cannabis. The resulting final model explained 16.5% of the variance of the presence of SI (R 2 = 0.165, p < 0.001). Secondary analyses conducted in cannabis users showed a significant effect of low-potency cannabis on SI (OR = 1.829; 95%CI 1.297–2.578). Frequency of cannabis use (occasional v. daily) was no longer a predictive factor in the model and the effects decreased substantially. We found no significant interaction between clinical group and the frequency of cannabis use or potency (see online Supplementary Table 1).
Note. All the models are adjusted for sex, age, ethnicity, years in education, employment, migration, and estimated intelligence quotient (IQ). Age was significant in Model 1 (p < 0.001); ethnicity and education were significant in Models 1 and 2 (p < 0.001) (not shown in the Table).
Model 1 = Group + sex, age, ethnicity, years in education, employment, migrant; Model 2 = Model 1 + frequency of cannabis use + interaction group × frequency of cannabis use (p > 0.05 for all interactions studied, data not shown in the Table). Significant results are highlighted in bold.
Presence of aberrance salience was considered as presence of speech illusion (speech illusion/yes) using a cutoff of two or more speech illusions.
R 2=Nagelkerke's r 2; Hosmer–Lemeshow test p > 0.05 for Models 1 and 2.
CI, confidence interval; FEP, first-episode psychosis; OR, odds ratio.
Table 4 shows the logistic regression models for the BFR. In model 1, the FEP group showed significantly higher odds of facial recognition processing (FRP) deficits compared with controls (OR = 1.467;95%CI 1.122–1.918), with no significant differences with their siblings. After controlling for frequency of cannabis in model 2, the difference in patients and controls remained significant, with comparable effects (OR = 1.642; 95%CI 1.123–2.402), while frequency of cannabis use was not a significant predictor in the model. The interaction effect between clinical group and frequency of cannabis use in the model was also not significant. The final model accounted for 12.1% of the explained variance (R 2 = 0.121, p < 0.001). Secondary analyses including cannabis users only showed a comparable effect of clinical group (OR = 1.588; 95%CI 1.104–2.284, a significant effect of potency (OR = 1.393; 95%CI 1.031–1.882), and no significant effects of frequency of cannabis use or the interaction between clinical group and frequency of use or potency in the models (see online Supplementary Table 2).
Note. All the models are adjusted for sex, age, ethnicity, years in education, employment, migration, and estimated intelligence quotient (IQ). Age was significant in Model 1 (p < 0.001); ethnicity and education were significant in Models 1 and 2 (p < 0.001); IQ was significant in Model 1 (p < 0.001) and 2 (p = 0.002) (not shown in Table).
Model 1 = Group + sex, age, ethnicity, education, employment, migrant; Model 2 = Model 1 + Frequency of cannabis use + Interaction group x frequency of cannabis use (p > 0.05 in all interactions studied, data not shown in the Table). Significant results are highlighted in bold.
Deficits in FRP deficits were defined as scores of 20 or less in the BFR test (BFR low).
R 2 = Nagelkerke's r 2; Hosmer–Lemeshow test p > 0.05 for Models 1 and 2.
BFR, Benton facial recognition; CI, confidence interval; FEP, first-episode psychosis; OR, odds ratio.
Table 5 shows the logistic regression analyses for the DTD. We did not find a significant effect of clinical group on JTC bias in Model 1 or 2. After including frequency of use in Model 2, participants who used cannabis occasionally or daily showed significantly lower odds of JTC bias compared with abstainers in the whole sample (OR = 0.602; 95%CI 0.460–0.787) and (OR = 0.625; 95% CI 0.422–0.925), respectively. There was no significant interaction between clinical group and frequency of cannabis use in the model. This model explained 19.3% of the variance (R2 = 0.193, p < 0.001;).
Note. All the models are adjusted for sex, age, ethnicity, years in education, employment, migration, and estimated intelligence quotient (IQ). Age was significant in Model 1 (p < 0.001); ethnicity, education, and IQ were significant in Models 1 and 2 (p < 0.001) (not shown in the Table).
Model 1 = group + sex, age, ethnicity, education, employment, migrant; Model 2 = Model 1 + frequency of cannabis use + Interaction group × frequency of cannabis use (p > 0.05 for all the interactions studied, data not shown). Significant results are highlighted in bold.
Jumping to conclusions bias was defined as a DTD index of 2 or less draws in the beads task.
R 2=Nagelkerke's r 2; Hosmer–Lemeshow test p > 0.05 for Models 1 and 2.
CI, confidence interval; DTD, drawing to decision; FEP, first-episode psychosis; OR, odds ratio.
Secondary analyses in the subgroup of cannabis users showed low-potency as a significant predictor of DTD (OR = 1.661; 95%CI 1.271–2.171), with no significant effect of frequency of use (occasional v. daily) or the interaction between clinical group and frequency of use or potency in the model (see online Supplementary Table 3).
Supplementary analyses in the FEP sample adjusted for the duration of untreated psychosis or the score on the negative CAPE subscale found comparable effects for the frequency of cannabis use variables and similar predictive capacity to those from the main models (see online Supplementary Tables 4–6).
Conclusions
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the odds of speech illusions, facial recognition deficits, and jumping to conclusions biases in a large international sample of patients with FEP, their siblings, and healthy controls. We found that FEP patients showed greater likelihood of facial recognition processing (FRP) deficits than siblings and controls. Contrary to previous findings in schizophrenia (Hoffman, Reference Hoffman1999; Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Woods, Hawkins, Pittman, Tohen, Preda and McGlashan2007; Holt et al., Reference Holt, Titone, Long, Goff, Cather, Rauch and Kuperberg2006; Kapur, Reference Kapur2003), we did not find significantly higher odds of speech illusions (SI) and JTC in FEP patients relative to siblings and controls, in this large international study.
Together with theory of mind, facial processing is critical for meaningful social interactions and for guiding social behavior and deficits in FRP may underlie some of the social cognition deficits in patients with psychosis (Comparelli et al., Reference Comparelli, De Carolis, Corigliano, Di Pietro, Trovini, Granese and Girardi2014; Green et al., Reference Green, Bearden, Cannon, Fiske, Hellemann, Horan and Nuechterlein2012; Mucci et al., Reference Mucci, Galderisi, Gibertoni, Rossi, Rocca and Bertolino2021; Velthorst et al., Reference Velthorst, Fett, Reichenberg, Perlman, van Os, Bromet and Kotov2017). In this study, we found more frequent FRP deficits in individuals with FEP than in their siblings and controls, thus suggesting that FRP deficits could be a specific marker related to psychosis but not genetically shared with siblings. This is consistent with previous studies reporting increased likelihood of cognitive biases related to facial processing deficits in individuals with psychosis relative to their siblings or controls (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Radhakrishnan, Pence, Erzin and Guloksuz2022b; Shin et al., Reference Shin, Na, Ha, Kang, Yoo and Kwon2008; Tripoli et al., Reference Tripoli, Quattrone, Ferraro, Gayer-Anderson, La Cascia, La Barbera and Murray2022; van 't Wout et al., Reference van 't Wout, Aleman, Kessels, Laroi and Kahn2004) and with previous evidence in the EU-GEI sample. For example, Fusar-Poli et al. (Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Erzin, Delespaul, Kenis and Guloksuz2022a, Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Radhakrishnan, Pence, Erzin and Guloksuz2022b) found that deficits in facial emotion recognition (measured by total Degraded Facial Affect Recognition task scores) were greater in individuals with schizophrenia than in siblings, who showed greater deficits than healthy controls. In our study, we did not find differences between siblings and controls regarding FRP deficits. Our divergent findings may be due to differing tasks or a more diagnostically heterogenous sample of FEP and their siblings in our study (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Radhakrishnan, Pence, Erzin and Guloksuz2022b).
We did not find an increased likelihood of aberrant salience in FEP patients than in siblings or healthy controls. Previous studies found that proneness to aberrant salience correlated with familial vulnerability for psychosis, and was more likely found in delusional schizophrenia patients than in non-delusional and healthy subjects or their siblings, thus suggesting a familial liability to psychosis associated with aberrant salience (Catalan et al., Reference Catalan, Simons, Bustamante, Drukker, Madrazo, de Artaza and Gonzalez-Torres2014; Galdos et al., Reference Galdos, Simons, Fernandez-Rivas, Wichers, Peralta, Lataster and van Os2011; Hoffman, Reference Hoffman1999; Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Woods, Hawkins, Pittman, Tohen, Preda and McGlashan2007; Holt et al., Reference Holt, Titone, Long, Goff, Cather, Rauch and Kuperberg2006; Schepers et al., Reference Schepers, Lousberg, Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis and van Os2019). Also, previous studies focusing on FEP patients showed higher rates of speech illusions than the control group, contrary to our findings, and speech illusions correlated with positive symptomatology (Catalan et al., Reference Catalan, Simons, Bustamante, Drukker, Madrazo, de Artaza and Gonzalez-Torres2014). Our results may differ in part because most of the studies mentioned did not include FEP-only samples or studied smaller samples with more unspecific tasks to measure these cognitive processes.
Contrary to our JTC findings, previous literature reports that patients with psychosis frequently use less information to arrive at a decision than controls and show increased risk of JTC bias (Ahuir et al., Reference Ahuir, Crosas, Estrada, Zabala, Perez-Munoz, Gonzalez-Fernandez and Labad2021; Dudley et al., Reference Dudley, Taylor, Wickham and Hutton2016; Henquet et al., Reference Henquet, van Os, Pries, Rauschenberg, Delespaul, Kenis and Guloksuz2022; Hofer et al., Reference Hofer, Biedermann, Yalcin and Fleischhacker2010; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Altshuler, Glahn, Miklowitz, Ochsner and Green2013; Ross, McKay, Coltheart, & Langdon, Reference Ross, McKay, Coltheart and Langdon2015). Similarly, previous studies based on the EU-GEI WP6 sample including schizophrenia patients, their siblings and healthy controls, showed increased odds of JTC bias in both patients and siblings relative to controls, thus suggesting an association between JTC bias and familial risk for psychosis (Henquet et al., Reference Henquet, van Os, Pries, Rauschenberg, Delespaul, Kenis and Guloksuz2022). Our different results may be because some of these studies were conducted in chronic schizophrenia samples or did not include FEP patients only. Indeed, a previous study observed a higher prevalence of JTC bias in individuals with schizophrenia than in individuals with recent-onset psychosis. Inconsistent findings could also result from previous studies not including IQ as a potential confounding variable in the analyses. In fact, the FEP group showed increased odds of JCT bias relative to controls in our sample in the analyses not adjusted by IQ, with no significant effect of the clinical group after controlling for this variable.
Our overall results confirmed our hypothesis that lifetime abstention from cannabis correlated with the presence of SI and JTC bias compared to daily or occasional cannabis use, with a comparable effect across FEP patients, siblings, and controls. As social skills could enable FEP patients to acquire and maintain a drug habit, or facilitate the use of drugs in social environments (Menendez-Miranda et al., Reference Menendez-Miranda, Garcia-Alvarez, Garcia-Portilla, Gonzalez-Blanco, Saiz and Bobes2019), cannabis use in people with FEP could be indicative of better or preserved social abilities, more drug taking opportunities, better neurocognitive functioning, and therefore, fewer and less significant cognitive biases (Arnold et al., Reference Arnold, Allott, Farhall, Killackey and Cotton2015; Rodriguez-Sanchez et al., Reference Rodriguez-Sanchez, Ayesa-Arriola, Mata, Moreno-Calle, Perez-Iglesias, Gonzalez-Blanch and Crespo-Facorro2010; Yucel et al., Reference Yucel, Bora, Lubman, Solowij, Brewer, Cotton and Pantelis2012). In this respect, a previous analysis of the EU-GEI sample observed better social adjustment prior to FEP in occasional and daily cannabis users relative to non-users. This difference was not observed in the control group (Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, La Cascia, Quattrone, Sideli, Matranga, Capuccio and Di Forti2020). Previous literature on patients with psychosis has also reported premorbid differences between cannabis users and non-users, with cannabis users potentially developing psychosis at an earlier age through an alternative pathway with less cognitive vulnerability (both neurocognitive and social cognitive) (Myles, Myles, & Large, Reference Myles, Myles and Large2016; Schnakenberg Martin et al., Reference Schnakenberg Martin, Bonfils, Davis, Smith, Schuder and Lysaker2016).
Taken together, we conclude that FEP cannabis users could constitute a group of less cognitively impaired individuals (Arnold et al., Reference Arnold, Allott, Farhall, Killackey and Cotton2015) in whom cannabis use would be an important contributor to psychosis risk (Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, La Cascia, Quattrone, Sideli, Matranga, Capuccio and Di Forti2020; Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, Murray, Di Forti, Quattrone, Tripoli, Sideli and La Cascia2019; Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, Russo, O'Connor, Wiffen, Falcone, Sideli and Di Forti2013). In fact, we found no significant differences in cognitive biases between occasional and daily use in cannabis users, supporting the idea that FEP cannabis users and nonusers may represent distinct populations with differing premorbid load and risk factors, rather than there being a global direct protective effect of cannabis use on social cognition. Exploring if these associations exist both before and after the FEP warrants future longitudinal studies.
Among lifetime cannabis users, the use of low-potency cannabis was significantly associated with increased odds of SI, FRP, and JTC relative to high-potency cannabis use, with comparable effects across FEP patients, their siblings, and controls. Potent cannabis varieties, with high concentrations of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (Δ9-THC), correlate with the most harm to mental health (Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Marconi, Carra, Fraietta, Trotta, Bonomo and Murray2015; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, van der Pol, Kuijpers, Wisselink, Das, Rigter and Lynskey2018) and recent studies based on the EU-GEI sample have shown how FEP patients with a history of daily use of high-potency cannabis present with more positive symptoms, compared with those who never used cannabis or used low potency variants (Quattrone et al., Reference Quattrone, Ferraro, Tripoli, La Cascia, Quigley, Quattrone and Di Forti2021). However, the relationship between the potency of cannabis and neurocognition is still unclear, and growing evidence suggests that two of the main cannabinoids, THC and CBD, display opposing neural, cognitive, and behavioral effects (Iseger & Bossong, Reference Iseger and Bossong2015). Moreover, a study of cannabis users implicated THC in impaired facial emotional recognition, while CBD improved facial emotional recognition and attenuated THC-induced impairment (Hindocha et al., Reference Hindocha, Freeman, Schafer, Gardener, Das, Morgan and Curran2015). Our results show that FEP patients who use high potency variants of cannabis show less cognitive bias than those who use low potency cannabis, contrary to previous findings. One possible explanation could be related with the gateway hypothesis; FEP patients who smoke low potency variants of cannabis and show social cognitive impairment may not progress to later consumption of higher potency variants of cannabis.
This study has several limitations. First, cross-sectional studies limit analyses to a descriptive level and we could not assess causality. We also lacked a direct measure of social skills. Future studies using methodologies that allow for a more in-depth analysis and using a prospective design could clarify the longitudinal association between cannabis use and the risk of cognitive biases, as well as ascertain the specific role of social skills in this association. Second, cannabis use was not corroborated with the collection of biological samples. Instead, we used self-reporting tools, with a risk of recall bias and we did not conduct specific memory assessments. Notwithstanding, previous studies reported that the use of interviews to collect information about drug use in the adult population is generally reliable and valid (Curran et al., Reference Curran, Hindocha, Morgan, Shaban, Das and Freeman2019; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Morgan, Hindocha, Schafer, Das and Curran2014; Van Dorn, Desmarais, Scott Young, Sellers, & Swartz, Reference Van Dorn, Desmarais, Scott Young, Sellers and Swartz2012). Third, although we analyzed a large sample of participants with FEP, their siblings, and healthy controls, our findings may not be generalizable to all FEP and sibling populations. Exclusion of participants from the analysis due to lacking information on cannabis use or cognitive bias variables could influence the representativeness of our findings. Fourth, in keeping with other EU-GEI publications we classified cannabis use frequency in three categories (never use, occasional use, daily use). However, the heterogeneity in the “occasional use” category may have influenced our results. Finally, despite potential effect of antipsychotic treatments and other interventions on cognitive functioning in first-episode psychosis (Allot et al., Reference Allott, Yuen, Baldwin, O’Donoghue, Fornito, Chopra and Wood2023), we did not consider pharmacological treatments and other cognitive or psychosocial interventions in our analyses. Future studies should assess the effect of these factors on the association between cannabis use and cognitive biases.
In conclusion, FRP deficits were more prevalent in FEP patients than in their siblings or control participants, with no significant differences between the three groups in other cognitive biases such as speech illusions or JTC bias in the adjusted analyses. Cognitive biases were more frequent in cannabis abstainers and in participants who used low potency of cannabis relative to daily and occasional users and high potency participants respectively. Our findings suggest that FEP patients who have never used cannabis on a regular basis would be more likely to present cognitive biases. Considering the detrimental effects of cannabis on psychosis risk even beyond genetic predisposition (Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, Quattrone, La Barbera, La Cascia, Morgan, Kirkbride and Murray2023), future studies should elucidate this association and its potential clinical implications further.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724001715.
Acknowledgements
We thank the patients and their families for their participation in this study.
Funding statement
The European Network of National Schizophrenia Networks Studying Gene-Environment Interactions (EU-GEI) Project was supported by grant agreement HEALTH-F2-2010-241909 (Project EU-GEI) from the European Community's Seventh Framework Programme. This work was partially supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation, Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII) (PI19/01024, PI20/00721, PI22/01824, PI23/00625), CIBER -Consorcio Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red- (CB/07/09/0023), co-financed by the European Union and ERDF Funds from the European Commission, ‘A way of making Europe’, financed by the European Union - NextGenerationEU (PMP21/00051), Madrid Regional Government (S2022/BMD-7216 AGES 3-CM), European Union Structural Funds, EU Seventh Framework Program, H2020 Program under the Innovative Medicines Initiative 2 Joint Undertaking (grant agreement No.101034377, Project PRISM-2), and Horizon Europe (HORIZON-HLTH-2021-STAYHLTH-01-02, grant agreement No 101057182, project Youth-GEMs), the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number 1U01MH124639-01 (Project ProNET) and Award Number 5P50MH115846-03 (project FEP-CAUSAL), Fundación Familia Alonso, and Fundación Alicia Koplowitz. MRC is a Ramon y Cajal Research Fellow (RYC-2017-23144), Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities and was supported by a NARSAD independent investigator grant (no. 24628) from the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation. MRC has received grant support from Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (PI18/00753, PI21/00701) and the Alicia Koplowitz Foundation. CDC holds a Juan Rodés Grant from Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (JR19/00024). JBK is supported by the National Institute of Health Research (NIHR) University College London Hospitals (UCLH) Biomedical Research Centre (BRC).
Competing interests
Celso Arango has been a consultant to or has received honoraria or grants from Acadia, Angelini, Gedeon Richter, Janssen Cilag, Lundbeck, Minerva, Otsuka, Roche, Sage, Servier, Shire, Schering Plough, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Sunovion and Takeda. Miquel Bernardo has been a consultant for, received grant/research support and honoraria from, and been on the speakers/advisory board of ABBiotics, Adamed, Angelini, Casen Recordati, Janssen-Cilag, Menarini, Rovi and Takeda. Covadonga M. Díaz-Caneja has received honoraria from Angelini and support to attend conferences from Janssen and Angelini. Peter B. Jones declares grant support from NIHR (0616-20003) and consulted for MSD. Carmen Moreno has received honoraria as a consultant and/or advisor and/or for lectures from Angelini, Esteve, Exeltis Janssen, Lundbeck, Neuraxpharm, Nuvelution, Otsuka, Pfizer, Servier and Sunovion outside the submitted work.