Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T13:29:43.014Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are changes in ADHD course reflected in differences in IQ and executive functioning from childhood to young adulthood?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 November 2019

Jessica C. Agnew-Blais
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Guilherme V. Polanczyk
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of São Paulo Medical School, São Paulo, Brazil
Andrea Danese
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK National and Specialist Child Traumatic Stress and Anxiety Clinic, South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
Jasmin Wertz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
Terrie E. Moffitt
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC, USA
Louise Arseneault*
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Louise Arseneault, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with poorer cognitive functioning. We used a developmental, genetically-sensitive approach to examine intelligence quotient (IQ) from early childhood to young adulthood among those with different ADHD courses to investigate whether changes in ADHD were reflected in differences in IQ. We also examined executive functioning in childhood and young adulthood among different ADHD courses.

Methods

Study participants were part of the Environmental Risk (E-Risk) Longitudinal Twin Study, a population-based birth cohort of 2232 twins. We assessed ADHD in childhood (ages 5, 7, 10 and 12) and young adulthood (age 18). We examined ADHD course as reflected by remission, persistence and late-onset. IQ was evaluated at ages 5, 12 and 18, and executive functioning at ages 5 and 18.

Results

ADHD groups showed deficits in IQ across development compared to controls; those with persistent ADHD showed the greatest deficit, followed by remitted and late-onset. ADHD groups did not differ from controls in developmental trajectory of IQ, suggesting changes in ADHD were not reflected in IQ. All ADHD groups performed more poorly on executive functioning tasks at ages 5 and 18; persisters and remitters differed only on an inhibitory control task at age 18.

Conclusions

Differences in ADHD course – persistence, remission and late-onset – were not directly reflected in changes in IQ. Instead, having ADHD at any point across development was associated with lower average IQ and poorer executive functioning. Our finding that individuals with persistent ADHD have poorer response inhibition than those who remitted requires replication.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

Children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) exhibit, on average, lower intelligence quotient (IQ) than children without ADHD (Frazier et al., Reference Frazier, Demaree and Youngstrom2004). Similarly, adults with ADHD tend to have lower IQ than peers without, although this difference is less pronounced than in childhood (Hervey et al., Reference Hervey, Epstein and Curry2004; Seidman, Reference Seidman2006). While individuals with ADHD exhibit the full range of IQ scores and may also have high IQ, ADHD diagnosis and higher symptom levels are associated with lower IQ and poorer executive functioning on average in the population (Antshel et al., Reference Antshel, Faraone, Stallone, Nave, Kaufmann, Doyle, Fried, Seidman and Biederman2007). These associations are well-established cross-sectionally, however it is less clear the extent to which decreased cognitive functioning is an underlying, stable feature associated with ADHD, or a more transient manifestation of the disorder associated with concurrent expression of symptoms.

Research has found that those with ADHD in childhood tend to exhibit poorer cognitive functioning in adulthood (Seidman, Reference Seidman2006; van Lieshout et al., Reference Van Lieshout, Luman, Buitelaar, Rommelse and Oosterlaan2013; Lin and Gau, Reference Lin and Gau2018), however fewer studies compare cognitive functioning between individuals whose ADHD remits v. those for whom ADHD persists to investigate whether remission has an ameliorative effect on cognitive functioning (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Petty, Ball, Fried, Doyle, Cohen, Handerson and Faraone2009). Neuroimaging studies suggest that remission of ADHD has a measurable impact on brain structures: while children with ADHD show cortical thinning in brain areas important for attentional processes, remission is associated with a relative thickening of these cortical areas (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Malek, Watson, Greenstein, De Rossi and Sharp2013). Whether these brain changes are reflected in changes in cognitive functioning is unclear. If cognitive problems continue even when ADHD symptoms abate, this would suggest cognitive functioning is independent of overt manifestation of ADHD and unrelated to the remission process. However, if cognitive functioning improves as the disorder remits, this could indicate that aetiologic processes governing the remission of ADHD are linked with cognitive development.

Relatedly, a growing body of evidence indicates that for some individuals, ADHD may onset after childhood (Moffitt et al., Reference Moffitt, Houts, Asherson, Belsky, Corcoran, Hammerle, Harrington, Hogan, Meier, Polanczyk, Poulton, Ramrakha, Sugden, Williams, Rohde and Caspi2015; Agnew-Blais et al., Reference Agnew-Blais, Polanczyk, Danese, Wertz, Moffitt and Arseneault2016; Caye et al., Reference Caye, Rocha, Anselmi, Murray, Menezes, Barros, Gonçalves, Wehrmeister, Victora, Steinhausen, Jensen, Swanson, Kieling and Rohde2016; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Hammerton, Collishaw, Langley, Thapar, Dalsgaard, Stergiakouli, Tilling, Davey Smith, Maughan, O'donovan, Thapar and Riglin2018; Sibley et al., Reference Sibley, Rohde, Swanson, Hechtman, Molina, Mitchell, Arnold, Caye, Kennedy, Roy and Stehli2018). It is possible that late-onset ADHD is associated with emerging psychopathological processes that are reflected in worsening cognitive functioning. We took a longitudinal approach to examine whether late onset of ADHD is associated with a corresponding decline in IQ relative to unaffected peers. Examining IQ and executive functioning in this group not only helps clarify the nature of late-onset ADHD, but also provides an additional method for examining the association between cognition and ADHD symptoms, namely whether onset of symptoms is reflected in changes in IQ.

While cognitive ability, as indexed by IQ, is often thought of as stable over the life course, high correlation from age to age does not rule out individual variation over time (Ramsden et al., Reference Ramsden, Richardson, Josse, Thomas, Ellis, Shakeshaft, Seghier and Price2011). A correlation coefficient as high as 0.80 means nearly 40% of variation could be explained by other factors. Adolescent years are a time of particular plasticity in cognitive development: research has found a third of teenagers showed considerable variation in IQ from age 14 to 17, and this change was reflected in differences in brain volumes (Ramsden et al., Reference Ramsden, Richardson, Josse, Thomas, Ellis, Shakeshaft, Seghier and Price2011). The environment may play a role in IQ change: one study found a decline in IQ among children in urban Detroit, while those in suburban communities showed no decline (Breslau et al., Reference Breslau, Chilcoat, Susser, Matte, Liang and Peterson2001). Environmental factors such as exposure to lead or other toxins can affect cognitive ability (Reuben et al., Reference Reuben, Caspi, Belsky, Broadbent, Harrington, Sugden, Houts, Ramrakha, Poulton and Moffitt2017). Psychopathology is also associated with changes in cognitive ability, for example declines in cognitive functioning associated with onset of psychotic disorders and their prodromes (Reichenberg et al., Reference Reichenberg, Caspi, Harrington, Houts, Keefe, Murray, Poulton and Moffitt2010).

We investigated the association between ADHD, IQ and executive functioning in a population-based cohort of twins to determine whether changes in the manifestation of ADHD across development – i.e. remission, persistence and late-onset – were reflected in changes in cognitive ability. First, using a longitudinal approach, we compared trajectories of IQ among different developmental patterns of ADHD to assess whether changes in ADHD diagnosis went hand-in-hand with changes in IQ, e.g. when ADHD remits, does IQ show a relative improvement? When ADHD onsets after childhood does IQ begin to lag relative to non-ADHD peers? In this way, we can examine whether the course of IQ development differed as a reflection of change in ADHD diagnosis. Second, as the Environmental Risk (E-Risk) Longitudinal Twin Study is a cohort of twins, we were able to investigate whether the association between ADHD and IQ across development was due to a family liability shared between twins or was unique to the twin affected by ADHD, thereby shedding light on the nature of the association between ADHD and IQ. Furthermore, ADHD has been conceptualised as fundamentally a disorder of impairments in executive functioning (Brown, Reference Brown2013). Therefore, third, we examined a range of tasks at ages 5 and 18 to determine whether executive functioning differed among groups with different ADHD courses from childhood to young adulthood.

Methods

Study cohort

Participants were members of the E-Risk Longitudinal Twin Study, a birth cohort of 2232 British children drawn from a larger birth register of twins born in England and Wales in 1994–95 (Trouton et al., Reference Trouton, Spinath and Plomin2002). Full details about the sample are reported elsewhere (Moffitt and E-Risk Study Team, Reference Moffitt2002). The E-Risk sample was constructed in 1999–2000 when 1116 families (93% of those eligible) with the same-sex 5-year-old twins participated in home-visit assessments administered by trained research workers. All research workers had university degrees in behavioural science and experience in psychology, anthropology or nursing. Each research worker completed a formal 15-day training programme on either the mother interview protocol or the child assessment protocol to attain certification to a rigorous reliability standard. The study sample comprised 56% monozygotic (MZ) and 44% dizygotic twin pairs; sex was evenly distributed within zygosity (49% male). Families were recruited to represent the UK population with newborns in the 1990s on the basis of residential location throughout England and Wales and mother's age; teenaged mothers with twins were over-selected to replace high-risk families who were selectively lost to the register through non-response. Older mothers having twins via assisted reproduction were under-selected to avoid an excess of well-educated older mothers. At follow-up, the study sample represented the full range and prevalence rates of socioeconomic levels in the UK (Odgers et al., Reference Odgers, Caspi, Russell, Sampson, Arseneault and Moffitt2012). Childhood SES reflected a composite of parental income, education and occupation.

Follow-up home visits were conducted when children were aged 7 (98% participation), 10 (96%), 12 (96%), and 18 years (93%). Home visits at ages 5–12 years included assessments with participants and their mother; we conducted full interviews with participants only at age 18 (n = 2066). There were no differences between those who did and did not take part at age 18 on socioeconomic status when the cohort was initially defined (χ2 = 0.86, p = 0.65), age-5 IQ (t = 0.98, p = 0.33), or rates of childhood ADHD (χ2 = 2.12, p = 0.71). The Joint South London and Maudsley and the Institute of Psychiatry Research Ethics Committee approved each phase of the study. Parents gave written informed consent and twins gave assent between 5 and 12 years and then written informed consent at age 18.

Childhood ADHD diagnoses

Mothers were interviewed by research workers and asked explicitly about each ADHD symptom during home visits. Children's symptoms were assessed with 18 items covering inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity derived from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-IV symptoms of ADHD and the Rutter Child Scales (e.g. ‘inattentive, easily distracted’, ‘impulsive, acts without thinking’, ‘very restless, has difficulty staying seated for long’). Symptoms were reported for the preceding 6 months and each symptom was scored as ‘not true’, ‘somewhat or sometimes true’ and ‘very true or often true’. Symptoms were counted as present if scored ‘very true or often true’. Teachers responded to mailed questionnaires and rated participants using the same items. Internal consistency reliabilities of parent and teacher reports were 0.90 and 0.93, respectively. Childhood ADHD diagnoses were based on DSM-IV criteria: participants had to have six or more symptoms of inattention or six or more symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity reported by mothers or teachers in the past 6 months, and the other informant must have endorsed at least two symptoms (thereby meeting the diagnostic criteria for the presence of symptoms in more than one setting). We considered participants to have a diagnosis of childhood ADHD if they met criteria at age 5, 7, 10 or 12. At age 5, 6.8% of participants (n = 131) met criteria for ADHD, 5.4% at age 7 (n = 102), 3.4% at age 10 (n = 65) and 3.4% at age 12 (n = 64).

Age-18 ADHD diagnosis

We ascertained ADHD diagnosis at age 18 based on private structured interviews administered by trained interviewers with participants regarding 18 symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity–impulsivity reflecting DSM-5 ADHD symptoms (Agnew-Blais et al., Reference Agnew-Blais, Polanczyk, Danese, Wertz, Moffitt and Arseneault2016). ADHD diagnoses were made based on DSM-5 criteria: participants had to endorse five or more inattentive and/or five or more hyperactivity–impulsivity symptoms to be diagnosed. We also required that symptoms interfere with individual's life at ‘home, or with family and friends’ and at ‘school or work’, thereby meeting impairment and pervasiveness criteria. The requirement of symptom onset prior to age 12 was met if parents or teachers reported more than two ADHD symptoms at any childhood assessment. A total of 8.1% of participants (n = 166) met criteria for ADHD at age 18.

Remitted, persistent and late-onset ADHD groups

We identified three groups of individuals with ADHD across childhood and young adulthood: 9.5% of participants (n = 193) showed remitted ADHD (met diagnostic criteria in childhood but not at age 18), 2.6% (n = 54) persistent ADHD (met diagnostic criteria in childhood and age 18) and 5.5% (n = 112) ‘late-onset’ ADHD (did not meet diagnostic criteria in childhood but did at age 18) (Agnew-Blais et al., Reference Agnew-Blais, Polanczyk, Danese, Wertz, Moffitt and Arseneault2016). A total of 82.4% of participants (n = 1681) did not meet criteria for ADHD in childhood or adulthood.

IQ and executive functioning assessment

IQ was assessed at ages 5, 12 and 18. At age 5, assessment was based on a short form of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised (WPPSI-R) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1990), prorated with the Sattler approach using Vocabulary and Block Design subtests (Sattler, Reference Sattler1992). At age 12, IQ was assessed with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2003) and at age 18 the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008); total IQ was estimated using the same subtests – Information and Matrix Reasoning – which were pro-rated to obtain total IQ. IQ across ages was moderately correlated: age-5 IQ with age-12 IQ at r = 0.55 and with age-18 at r = 0.49, and age-12 IQ with age-18 IQ at r = 0.70. ADHD symptoms were negatively correlated with IQ at each age: r = −0.21 at age 5, r = −0.24 at age 12, and r = −0.15 at age 18 (all p < 0.001).

Executive functioning was assessed at age 5 and was measured as a composite mean score of the Mazes Task (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1990), Day–Night Task (Gerstadt et al., Reference Gerstadt, Hong and Diamond1994) and Sentence Working Memory Task (Case et al., Reference Case, Kurland and Goldberg1982). At age 18, it was assessed with the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) (Sahakian and Owen, Reference Sahakian and Owen1992), a computerised test battery of neuropsychological functioning that study participants completed on an iPad. Tests included Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVP); A-prime taps into sustained attention, and false alarm assesses response inhibition (due to the distribution of the data, this was dichotomised as more than two false alarms v. zero or one). Tests also included spatial working memory (SWM), which assesses problem-solving and the ability to hold information about spatial location in active memory, and Spatial Span (SSP), a non-verbal equivalent of the Digit Span working memory task.

Statistical analyses

We compared IQ at ages 5, 12 and 18 among those with remitted, persistent and late-onset ADHD to those who never had ADHD. We used logistic regression to compare groups adjusting for sex and childhood SES. To understand whether changes in ADHD diagnosis across development – remission, persistence and late-onset – were associated with changes in IQ over time, we modelled IQ trajectories with multilevel regression analyses using the ‘mixed’ command in STATA (STATA, 2015). All models included random intercepts and slopes, and fixed effects for sex, childhood SES, and ADHD group. A significant ADHD group effect indicates a difference in level of IQ across development; a significant ADHD group by age interaction indicates a different slope, or trajectory, of IQ development between ADHD group and controls. We restricted analyses to individuals with information on childhood and young adult ADHD, and IQ for at least two of the three assessments (N = 2038).

To determine whether lower IQ among those with ADHD was accounted for by family-level factors or was unique to the twin affected by ADHD, we modelled IQ over time using multilevel regression analyses and estimated a family-wide effect, representing variation accounted for by factors common to both twins in a concordant pair, and a unique effect, representing variation specific to the ADHD twin in a discordant pair. We conducted analyses first with all twins, then with MZ twins only, to test whether any unique effect remained over and above genetic relatedness between MZ twins. We tested for consistency of these effects across time by including an interaction term between age and between- and within-twin effects.

Regarding executive functioning, we compared those with remitted, persistent and late-onset ADHD to those who never had ADHD on executive functioning composite score at age 5 and on CANTAB tasks of working memory and response inhibition at age 18 using logistic regressions adjusting for sex and childhood SES. We calculated effect sizes comparing each ADHD group to non-ADHD controls and ADHD groups to one another, adjusting only for sex and SES for the composite score at age 5, and then further adjusting for age-5 executive functioning and age-18 IQ for CANTAB task scores. All analyses adjusted for the non-independence of twin observations using the Sandwich variance estimator in Stata (STATA, 2015).

In sensitivity analyses we further explored whether those with ADHD limited to early childhood (at ages 5 and/or 7 only, N = 96) may have a different trajectory of IQ, as earlier recovery could be reflected in an accelerated rate of catch-up in IQ in this early childhood limited group.

Results

Differences in IQ amongst groups with different ADHD courses

Findings indicated that IQ does not mirror changes in ADHD course across development. Mean total IQ at ages 5, 12 and 18 was lowest in those with persistent ADHD followed by the remitted and late-onset groups, compared with controls (Table 1). IQ in the remitted group remained nearly half a standard deviation lower than controls by age 18. However, those who remitted had higher IQ scores than those who persisted: the remitted group had higher total and performance IQ at age 5, and higher verbal IQ at ages 12 and 18 compared to the persistent group. At each assessment, effect sizes comparing the remitted group to controls were smaller than those for the persistent group. The late-onset ADHD group's total, performance and verbal IQ were lower than controls at ages 5, 12 and 18, but were also higher than those with persistent ADHD.

Table 1. Total, performance and verbal IQ in childhood, adolescence and young adulthood among individuals who never had ADHD, and persistent, remitted and late-onset ADHD groups

IQ, intelligence quotient; d, Cohen's d; s.d., standard deviation.

Age-5 total IQ was estimated using the Sattler approach with WPPSI-R Vocabulary and Block Design subtests; age-12 and age-18 total IQ was estimated using the Sattler approach with the WISC-R (age-12) and WAIS-IV (age-18) Information and Matrix Reasoning subtests.

Total IQ scores were standardised on the full sample to mean = 100, standard deviation (s.d.) = 15, and performance and verbal IQ scores to mean = 10, s.d. = 3.

ES and p values adjusted for sex and childhood SES.

~p < 0.10, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Longitudinal trajectories of IQ amongst ADHD groups

Examining trajectories of IQ supports the finding that levels of cognitive ability in ADHD are stable from early childhood to young adulthood, even among those for whom the disorder remitted or emerged after childhood (Fig. 1). Persistent, remitted and late-onset ADHD groups all showed lower levels of IQ compared to controls, as evidenced by a significant effect of ADHD group in longitudinal models (Table 2). However, we did not find significant group by age interactions, indicating no differences in total IQ trajectory between ADHD groups and controls. Furthermore, deficits in IQ among ADHD groups were of similar magnitudes across childhood into young adulthood. Compared with persisters, the remitted ADHD group had higher total IQ across development, but their developmental trajectories did not differ. The late-onset group had intermediate IQ level between the other ADHD groups and controls, and again there was no difference in developmental trajectory of IQ. Overall, this pattern was similar for performance and verbal IQ. However, we observed an age by group interaction for performance IQ comparing persistent ADHD to non-ADHD controls, indicating the persistent group had a faster rate of development from age 5 to age 12 compared to controls. This finding suggests the persistent ADHD group may be catching up more rapidly from their larger deficit in early childhood.

Fig. 1. Mean total, performance and verbal IQ at ages 5, 12 and 18 amongst ADHD groups and controls.

Table 2. ADHD group and group-by-age interaction effects on total, performance and verbal IQ at ages 5, 12 and 18 years comparing persistent, remitted and late-onset ADHD groups to those without ADHD

Bold values are statistically significant.

Association between IQ and ADHD controlling for familial factors

Parsing whether lower IQ among those with ADHD was entirely accounted for by family-level (between-twin pair) factors, v. factors unique to twins affected by ADHD (within-twin pair), led to two notable findings. First, ADHD was associated with about 10 points lower IQ across childhood and into young adulthood owing to family-wide factors (Table 3). Within-twin pair factors also accounted for lower IQ, although of smaller magnitude. In other words, when comparing families with twins concordant for ADHD with families in which both twins didn't have ADHD, the pair with ADHD had, on average, 10 points lower IQ than the pair without; when comparing one twin with ADHD to their co-twin without, the twin with ADHD had about 1.6 points lower IQ across development. Second, when restricting analyses to MZ twins, the difference in IQ across development comparing the twin with ADHD to their co-twin without remained statistically significant, indicating the association was not entirely due to genetic factors. We found a significant interaction between the within-twin factor and age, indicating that among discordant MZ twins, IQ differed more as twins aged.

Table 3. Family-wide and unique effects of ADHD on IQ from age 5 to 18

CI, confidence interval; MZ, monozygotic.

Family-wide indicates between-twin pair difference; unique, within-twin pair difference.

Total number of discordant twin pairs = 211; number of MZ discordant twin pairs = 91.

~p < 0.10, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Executive functioning amongst ADHD groups

At age 5, all ADHD groups had lower composite executive functioning scores than controls. However, this measure did not significantly distinguish amongst the different ADHD groups (Table 4). Executive functioning at age 18 did not appear to be sensitive to changes in ADHD diagnosis into young adulthood, as all ADHD groups performed more poorly compared with controls. Again, overall these tasks did not distinguish between the different ADHD groups. One exception was the remitted group had fewer RVP false alarms than the persistent group. Effect sizes comparing persistent and remitted groups to controls were generally of similar magnitude, while the late-onset group showed smaller effect sizes (Fig. 2). Further adjusting these comparisons for age-5 executive functioning only slightly reduced the magnitude of effect sizes, while adjusting for age-18 IQ more substantially reduced effect sizes. However, the finding that those with persistent ADHD made more RVP false alarms than those with remitted ADHD was robust to these adjustments and remained significant.

Fig. 2. Effect sizes comparing CANTAB executive functioning task scores among those with persistent, remitted and late-onset ADHD to controls without ADHD, first adjusted for sex and childhood social class only, followed by further adjustment for age-5 executive functioning composite score, and then by age-18 total IQ. All ES adjusted for sex and childhood SES. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Table 4. Executive functioning at age 5 and age 18 among never ADHD and persistent, remitted and late-onset ADHD groups

ES, effect size; s.d., standard deviation; d, Cohen's d; RVP, rapid visual processing; SWM, spatial working memory; SSP, spatial span.

Age-5 executive functioning composite score based on mean score of the Mazes Task (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1990), Day–Night Task (Gerstadt et al., Reference Gerstadt, Hong and Diamond1994) and Sentence Working Memory Task (Case et al., Reference Case, Kurland and Goldberg1982).

ES and p values adjusted for sex and childhood SES.

~p < 0.10, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Sensitivity analyses

Similar to the main analyses, changes in ADHD diagnosis were not reflected in changes in IQ amongst the group with ADHD limited to early childhood (online Supplementary table and figure). Similar to other ADHD groups, this early-childhood limited ADHD group exhibited a stable deficit in IQ across development.

Discussion

We did not find that changes in the outward manifestation of ADHD – namely the disorder's remission, persistence and late-onset from early childhood to young adulthood – were reflected in changes in IQ and executive functioning. Rather, the associations between ADHD and IQ were stable across development and independent of whether ADHD was evident at a particular developmental stage. Moreover, the association between ADHD and IQ across development was not entirely accounted for by genetic factors. Executive functioning in young adulthood did not appear to be particularly sensitive to changes in ADHD, with the possible exception of inhibitory control.

Changes in ADHD across development not mirrored in IQ changes

While neuroimaging research suggests remission of ADHD has an impact on brain structures (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Malek, Watson, Greenstein, De Rossi and Sharp2013), we did not observe that remission of ADHD by young adulthood was reflected by changes in IQ. Despite improvements in the manifestation of ADHD among those who remitted, this group exhibited a stable deficit in IQ compared with controls. While those with remitted ADHD fared better than those who persisted, longitudinal analyses showed that higher IQ among children whose ADHD remitted by age 18 was already apparent in early childhood. Among studies that have examined IQ among persisters and remitters in adolescence/young adulthood, several found no differences in IQ (Barkley and Fischer, Reference Barkley and Fischer2011; Mick et al., Reference Mick, Byrne, Fried, Monuteaux, Faraone and Biederman2011). One study in a clinical cohort found those with persistent ADHD had lower IQ in young adulthood compared to those who remitted, but this was also the case at childhood baseline (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Rijdijk, Mcloughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2016). Longitudinal trajectory analyses from a clinical cohort found remitted and persistent groups had lower IQ from middle childhood to young adulthood, but found no group difference in IQ between persisters and remitters, and no group by age interaction between ADHD groups and controls (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Petty, Ball, Fried, Doyle, Cohen, Handerson and Faraone2009). This stable deficit in IQ among those with remitted ADHD could contribute to the poorer functional outcomes experienced by individuals in this group as they reach adulthood (Agnew-Blais et al., Reference Agnew-Blais, Polanczyk, Danese, Wertz, Moffit and Arseneault2018).

Late-onset ADHD associated with lower IQ across development

Late-onset ADHD remains an area of active research, with controversy around levels of childhood psychopathology experienced by those with late-onset ADHD, and the possibility that some ADHD symptoms in this group could be explained by other disorders such as depression or anxiety (Asherson and Agnew-Blais, Reference Asherson and Agnew-Blais2019). Given the lack of research and remaining open questions about late-onset ADHD, it is important to examine cognitive functioning in this group to better understand the nature of late-onset ADHD. If late-onset ADHD represents an emerging psychopathology, perhaps this comes along with an increasing lag in IQ behind peers as ADHD emerges. Our findings, however, do not support this hypothesis. Despite the late-onset group exhibiting lower IQ at age 18 compared with controls, it was not that the emergence of ADHD was associated with deleterious effects on IQ, but rather those who developed late-onset ADHD had lower IQ already at age 5. While slightly lower IQ may be a vulnerability factor for late-onset ADHD, this group had significantly higher IQ than those with childhood ADHD. These findings are similar to those found in the ALSPAC study, which reported that IQ of the late-onset group fell slightly below those with low ADHD symptoms across development, but also above those with persistent ADHD at ages 8 and 16 (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Hammerton, Collishaw, Langley, Thapar, Dalsgaard, Stergiakouli, Tilling, Davey Smith, Maughan, O'donovan, Thapar and Riglin2018). While much remains to be understood about late-onset ADHD, in the current study we find that those who meet ADHD criteria at age 18 but not in childhood show slight IQ and executive functioning deficits compared to non-ADHD controls; these deficits are present already in early childhood.

Associations between ADHD and IQ across development not entirely accounted for by shared genetics

Our findings that ADHD and IQ share significant genetic aetiology is consistent with molecular genetic research showing higher ADHD genetic risk is associated with lower IQ and poorer working memory in childhood (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Hamshere, Stergiakouli, O'donovan and Thapar2015). However, while we found genetic risk explained a large portion of the association between IQ and ADHD across development (i.e. within twin pair differences were much smaller than between-pair), it did not entirely explain the longitudinal association of ADHD with lower IQ. We found differences in IQ between discordant MZ twins actually increased as the twins grew into young adulthood, which warrants further investigation. While MZ twins share nearly 100% of their DNA, they can differ, for example, in number of copy number variants and epigenetic profiles (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Sudre, Sharp, Donovan, Chandrasekharappa, Hansen, Elnitski and Shaw2018). These differences could lead to ADHD in one twin only, as well as to a different trajectory of IQ in the affected twin compared to their unaffected co-twin.

Executive functioning not sensitive to changes in ADHD course

Consistent with a growing body of research (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Petty, Ball, Fried, Doyle, Cohen, Handerson and Faraone2009), we did not find that those with persistent ADHD had poorer executive functioning than those with remitted ADHD. While executive dysfunction is considered a core feature of ADHD, similar to IQ it did not vary with changes in ADHD diagnosis. Rather, having ADHD at any point in development was associated with poorer executive functioning both at ages 5 and 18. We did not find evidence for differences in working memory tasks between persisters and remitters, in contrast to some (Halperin et al., Reference Halperin, Trampush, Miller, Marks and Newcorn2008; Coghill et al., Reference Coghill, Hayward, Rhodes, Grimmer and Matthews2014), but not all (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Petty, Ball, Fried, Doyle, Cohen, Handerson and Faraone2009; Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Rijdijk, Mcloughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2016) previous studies.

We did, however, find evidence for better inhibitory control among those with remitted ADHD, as shown by fewer false alarms on the RVP task, suggesting those whose ADHD persisted may be more likely to ‘act without thinking’. This difference was robust to adjustment for age-5 executive functioning and age-18 IQ. Studies have found that individuals with ADHD perform more poorly on tests of inhibitory control, for example in Stop-Signal and Go/No-Go paradigms, and neuroimaging studies identified differential patterns of functioning in the frontal lobes during inhibitory tasks among ADHD participants and controls (Mulligan et al., Reference Mulligan, Knopik, Sweet, Fischer, Seidenberg and Rao2011). Our findings suggest that inhibitory control could be of particular salience to ADHD remission. Similar results have been found by some clinical cohorts in which persistence was associated with more false alarms and fewer correct hits (Halperin et al., Reference Halperin, Trampush, Miller, Marks and Newcorn2008) and more commissions on inhibitory control tasks (Fischer et al., Reference Fischer, Barkley, Smallish and Fletcher2005), or in which reductions in symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity were associated with decreased Continuous Performance Task commissions over time (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Loya and Hinshaw2013). This is consistent with functional magnetic resonance imaging findings in which the persistent ADHD group showed reduced inferior frontal cortex activity specifically during failed inhibition v. remitted and never ADHD groups, who did not differ from one another (Szekely et al., Reference Szekely, Sudre, Sharp, Leibenluft and Shaw2017). It is therefore possible that inhibitory control taps into a process associated with remission of ADHD. However, not all studies have replicated this finding (Vaughn et al., Reference Vaughn, Epstein, Rausch, Altaye, Langberg, Newcorn and Wigal2011), or have found that overall IQ explained differences in response inhibition (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Chen and Gau2014). Additionally, whether those who would remit already had better inhibitory control in early childhood remains unclear.

Limitations

This study has several strengths, including repeated assessments of IQ and ADHD spanning early childhood to young adulthood in a population-based cohort of twins. Nevertheless, some limitations must be considered. First, age-18 ADHD diagnoses were based on self-report. However, prior work in this cohort found co-informant reports of ADHD symptoms at age 18 corroborated self-reports, as those with self-reported late-onset ADHD have significantly more co-informant-rated ADHD symptoms than those without ADHD (Agnew-Blais et al., Reference Agnew-Blais, Polanczyk, Danese, Wertz, Moffitt and Arseneault2016). Second, our sample comprised twins and results may not generalise to singletons. Nevertheless, the prevalence of ADHD at each age in our cohort is well within ranges estimated in other samples. Third, because we assessed executive functioning at only at ages 5 and 18, we could not investigate longitudinal trajectories among the different ADHD groups. Fourth, we did not assess participants for performance on other measures associated with ADHD, such as other measures of executive functioning (e.g. Stockings of Cambridge) (Lin and Gau, Reference Lin and Gau2017), response time variability, delay aversion or biometric assessments such as electroencephalogram (Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Kitsune, Cheung, Bradeis, Banaschewski, Asherson, Mcloughlin and Kuntsi2016), which could be more sensitive to changes in ADHD. Fifth, we examined the association of changes in ADHD diagnosis from childhood to young adulthood (ADHD remission, persistence and late-onset) with IQ and executive functioning, rather than focusing on changes in number of ADHD symptoms over time. By concentrating on changes in ADHD diagnosis our analyses emphasise clinically important change in level of ADHD symptoms, as well as aspects of ADHD including pervasiveness of symptoms and impairment. Finally, our findings do not reveal the directionality of the underlying causal link between ADHD and IQ: it could be improvements in IQ and executive functioning affect ADHD, ADHD affects IQ, or underlying brain developments account for changes in both.

Our study emphasises the importance of taking a developmental approach to investigating ADHD and associated IQ. Lack of change in IQ in groups whose ADHD changed across development suggests that rather than lower IQ being an epiphenomenon that emerges as a by-product of ADHD symptoms, lower IQ is a stable trait associated with the presence of ADHD at any point in development. That early childhood IQ was associated with a poorer course of ADHD – both for persistence among children with ADHD and for risk of late-onset ADHD among children without – could have implications for directing additional resources to children more likely to have a more pernicious course of the disorder. Continued cognitive problems among those whose ADHD remitted may lead to poorer functioning in young adulthood, suggesting remission from the full disorder should not be considered the end to potential intervention and clinical attention. Research should continue to explore the genetic underpinnings of the association between ADHD and IQ across development, as well as potential environmental factors that might contribute to both ADHD and lower IQ. It is important that future studies follow participants past young adulthood into mid- and later-life to better understand the longer-term associations between ADHD and cognitive functioning.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719003015.

Acknowledgements

The E-Risk Study is funded by the UK Medical Research Council (UKMRC grant G1002190). Additional support was provided by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (grant HD077482) and by the Jacobs Foundation. Jessica Agnew-Blais is an MRC Skills Development Fellow. Louise Arseneault is the Mental Health Leadership Fellow for the UK Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC). The authors are grateful to the Avielle Foundation, CACI, Inc., and to members of the E-Risk team for their dedication, hard work and insights. We are grateful to the study mothers and fathers, the twins and the twins' teachers for their participation. Our thanks to Avshalom Caspi, Ph.D. (Duke University and King's College London) and Sir Michael Rutter, MD, Ph.D. (King's College London) for their involvement in establishing the E-Risk cohort. No compensation was received for this involvement.

References

Agnew-Blais, JC, Polanczyk, GV, Danese, A, Wertz, J, Moffitt, TE and Arseneault, L (2016) Persistence, remission and emergence of ADHD in young adulthood: results from a longitudinal, prospective population-based cohort. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 713720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Agnew-Blais, JC, Polanczyk, GV, Danese, A, Wertz, J, Moffit, TE and Arseneault, L (2018) Young adult mental health and functional outcomes among individuals with remitted, persistent and late-onset ADHD in an 18-year prospective cohort of twins. British Journal of Psychiatry 213, 526534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antshel, KM, Faraone, SV, Stallone, K, Nave, A, Kaufmann, FA, Doyle, A, Fried, R, Seidman, L and Biederman, J (2007) Is attention deficit hyperactivity disorder a valid diagnosis in the presence of high IQ? Results from the MGH Longitudinal Family Studies of ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48, 687694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asherson, P and Agnew-Blais, JC (2019) Annual research review: does late-onset attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder exist? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 60, 333352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, RA and Fischer, M (2011) Predicting impairment in major life activities and occupational functioning in hyperactive children as adults: self-reported executive function (EF) deficits versus EF tests. Developmental Neuropsychology 36, 137161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biederman, J, Petty, CR, Ball, SW, Fried, R, Doyle, AE, Cohen, D, Handerson, C and Faraone, SV (2009) Are cognitive deficits in ADHD related to the course of the disorder? A prospective controlled follow-up study of grown up boys with persistent and remitting course. Psychiatry Research 170, 177182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N, Chilcoat, HD, Susser, ES, Matte, T, Liang, KY and Peterson, EL (2001) Stability and change in children's intelligence quotient scores: a comparison of two socioeconomically disparate communities. American Journal of Epidemiology 154, 711717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, TE (2013) A New Understanding of ADHD in Children and Adults. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Case, R, Kurland, DM and Goldberg, J (1982) Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 33, 386404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caye, A, Rocha, TBM, Anselmi, L, Murray, J, Menezes, AMB, Barros, F, Gonçalves, H, Wehrmeister, F, Victora, C, Steinhausen, HC, Jensen, CM, Swanson, JM, Kieling, C and Rohde, LA (2016) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trajectories from childhood to young adulthood: evidence from a birth cohort supporting a late-onset syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 705712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, YC, Sudre, G, Sharp, W, Donovan, F, Chandrasekharappa, SC, Hansen, N, Elnitski, L and Shaw, P (2018) Neuroanatomic, epigenetic and genetic differences in monozygotic twins discordant for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry 23, 683690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, CH, Rijdijk, F, Mcloughlin, G, Brandeis, D, Banaschewski, T, Asherson, P and Kuntsi, J (2016) Cognitive and neurophysiological markers of ADHD persistence and remission. British Journal of Psychiatry 208, 548555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coghill, DR, Hayward, D, Rhodes, SM, Grimmer, C and Matthews, K (2014) A longitudinal examination of neuropsychological and clinical functioning in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): improvements in executive functioning do not explain clinical improvement. Psychological Medicine 44, 10871099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, M, Hammerton, G, Collishaw, S, Langley, K, Thapar, A, Dalsgaard, S, Stergiakouli, E, Tilling, K, Davey Smith, G, Maughan, B, O'donovan, M, Thapar, A and Riglin, L (2018) Investigating late-onset ADHD: a population cohort investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 59, 11051113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, M, Barkley, RA, Smallish, L and Fletcher, K (2005) Executive functioning in hyperactive children as young adults: attention, inhibition, response perseveration, and the impact of comorbidity. Developmental Neuropsychology 27, 107133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frazier, TW, Demaree, HA and Youngstrom, EA (2004) Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology 18, 543555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerstadt, CL, Hong, YJ and Diamond, A (1994) The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3.5–7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition 53, 129153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halperin, JM, Trampush, JW, Miller, CJ, Marks, DJ and Newcorn, JH (2008) Neuropsychological outcome in adolescents/young adults with childhood ADHD: profiles of persisters, remitters and controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49, 958966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hervey, AS, Epstein, JN and Curry, JF (2004) Neuropsychology of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 18, 485503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, YJ and Gau, SF (2017) Comparison of neuropsychological functioning between adults with early- and late-onset DSM-5 ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders doi:10.1177/1087054717730609.Google ScholarPubMed
Lin, YJ and Gau, SF (2018) Developmental changes of neuropsychological functioning in individuals with and without childhood ADHD from early adolescence to young adulthood: a 7-year follow-up study. Psychological Medicine 49, 940951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, YJ, Chen, WJ and Gau, SF (2014) Neuropsychological functions among adolescents with persistent, subsyndromal and remitted attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine 44, 17651777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J, Hamshere, ML, Stergiakouli, E, O'donovan, MC and Thapar, A (2015) Neurocognitive abilities in the general population and composite genetic risk scores for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology Psychiatry 56, 648656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michelini, G, Kitsune, GL, Cheung, CHM, Bradeis, D, Banaschewski, T, Asherson, P, Mcloughlin, G and Kuntsi, J (2016) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder remission is linked to better neurophysiological error detection and attention-vigilance processes. Biological Psychiatry 80, 923932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mick, E, Byrne, D, Fried, R, Monuteaux, M, Faraone, SV and Biederman, J (2011) Predictors of ADHD persistence in girls at 5-year follow-up. Journal of Attention Disorders 15, 183192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, M, Loya, F and Hinshaw, SP (2013) Executive functions in girls with and without childhood ADHD: developmental trajectories and associations with symptom change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 54, 10051015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, TE and E-Risk Study Team (2002) Teen-aged mothers in contemporary Britain. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 43, 727742.Google ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, TE, Houts, R, Asherson, P, Belsky, DW, Corcoran, DL, Hammerle, M, Harrington, H, Hogan, S, Meier, MH, Polanczyk, GV, Poulton, R, Ramrakha, S, Sugden, K, Williams, B, Rohde, LA and Caspi, A (2015) Is adult ADHD a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence from a four-decade longitudinal cohort study. American Journal of Psychiatry 172, 976977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulligan, RC, Knopik, VS, Sweet, LH, Fischer, M, Seidenberg, M and Rao, SM (2011) Neural correlates of inhibitory control in adult ADHD: evidence from the Milwaukee longitudinal sample. Psychiatry Research 194, 119129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odgers, C, Caspi, A, Russell, M, Sampson, R, Arseneault, L and Moffitt, TE (2012) Supportive parenting mediates neighborhood socioeconomic disparities in children's antisocial behavior from ages 5 to 12. Developmental Psychopathology 24, 705721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsden, S, Richardson, FM, Josse, G, Thomas, MSC, Ellis, C, Shakeshaft, C, Seghier, ML and Price, CJ (2011) Verbal and non-verbal intelligence changes in the teenage brain. Nature 479, 113116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichenberg, A, Caspi, A, Harrington, H, Houts, R, Keefe, RS, Murray, RM, Poulton, R and Moffitt, TE (2010) Static and dynamic cognitive deficits in childhood preceding adult schizophrenia: a 30-year study. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 160169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuben, A, Caspi, A, Belsky, DW, Broadbent, J, Harrington, H, Sugden, K, Houts, RM, Ramrakha, S, Poulton, R and Moffitt, TE (2017) Association of childhood blood lead levels with cognitive function and socioeconomic status at age 38 years and with IQ change and socioeconomic mobility between childhood and adulthood. Journal of the American Medical Association 317, 12441251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sahakian, B and Owen, A (1992) Computerized assessment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB: discussion paper. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 85, 399.Google ScholarPubMed
Sattler, J (1992) Assessment of Children: WISC-III and WPPSI-R Supplement. San Diego, CA: Jerome M Sattler, Publisher, Inc.Google Scholar
Seidman, LJ (2006) Neuropsychological functioning in people with ADHD across the lifespan. Clinical Psychology Review 26, 466485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, P, Malek, M, Watson, B, Greenstein, D, De Rossi, D and Sharp, W (2013) Trajectories of cerebral cortical development in childhood and adolescence and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 74, 599606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sibley, MH, Rohde, LA, Swanson, JM, Hechtman, LT, Molina, BSG, Mitchell, JT, Arnold, LE, Caye, A, Kennedy, TM, Roy, A and Stehli, A and MTA group (2018) Late-onset ADHD reconsidered with comprehensive repeated assessments between ages 10 and 25. American Journal of Psychiatry 175, 140149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stata (2015) STATA. 14 ed. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.Google Scholar
Szekely, E, Sudre, GP, Sharp, W, Leibenluft, E and Shaw, PW (2017) Defining the neural substrate of the adult outcome of childhood ADHD: a multimodal neuroimaging study of response inhibition. American Journal of Psychiatry 174, 867876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trouton, A, Spinath, F and Plomin, R (2002) Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): a multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems in childhood. Twin Research 5, 444448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Lieshout, M, Luman, M, Buitelaar, J, Rommelse, NNJ and Oosterlaan, J (2013) Does neurocognitive functioning predict future or persistence of ADHD? A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review 33, 539560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, AJ, Epstein, JN, Rausch, J, Altaye, M, Langberg, J, Newcorn, JH and Wigal, T (2011) Relation between outcomes on a continuous performance test and ADHD symptoms over time. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 39, 853864.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D (1990) Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Revised. London, England: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (2003) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th Edn. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (2008) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th Edn. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Total, performance and verbal IQ in childhood, adolescence and young adulthood among individuals who never had ADHD, and persistent, remitted and late-onset ADHD groups

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Mean total, performance and verbal IQ at ages 5, 12 and 18 amongst ADHD groups and controls.

Figure 2

Table 2. ADHD group and group-by-age interaction effects on total, performance and verbal IQ at ages 5, 12 and 18 years comparing persistent, remitted and late-onset ADHD groups to those without ADHD

Figure 3

Table 3. Family-wide and unique effects of ADHD on IQ from age 5 to 18

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Effect sizes comparing CANTAB executive functioning task scores among those with persistent, remitted and late-onset ADHD to controls without ADHD, first adjusted for sex and childhood social class only, followed by further adjustment for age-5 executive functioning composite score, and then by age-18 total IQ. All ES adjusted for sex and childhood SES. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Figure 5

Table 4. Executive functioning at age 5 and age 18 among never ADHD and persistent, remitted and late-onset ADHD groups

Supplementary material: File

Agnew-Blais et al. supplementary material

Agnew-Blais et al. supplementary material

Download Agnew-Blais et al. supplementary material(File)
File 94.4 KB