Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T07:17:32.542Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Topics and techniques in clinical supervision in psychotherapy training

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 July 2017

Florian Weck*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
Yvonne M. Kaufmann
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany Department of Clinical Psychology, Psychotherapy and Experimental Psychopathology, University of Mainz, Mainz, Germany
Michael Witthöft
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology, Psychotherapy and Experimental Psychopathology, University of Mainz, Mainz, Germany
*
*Author for correspondence: Dr Florian Weck, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Potsdam, Karl-Liebknecht-Straße 24–25, D-14476 Potsdam, Germany. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Clinical supervision is regarded as one of the most important components of psychotherapy training. In clinical practice, it has been found that the implementation of clinical supervision varies substantially and often differs from the recommendations made in the literature. The objective of the current study was to investigate the frequency of topics (e.g. ethical issues) and techniques (e.g. role play) in the clinical supervision of psychotherapy trainees in Germany. To this end, we considered supervisions in cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) and psychodynamic therapy (PT). A total of 791 psychotherapy trainees (533 CBT and 242 PT) were asked via the internet to provide information about their current supervision sessions. We found that clinical supervision in psychotherapy training addressed topics that are central for the effective treatment of supervised patients (i.e. therapeutic interventions, therapeutic alliance, maintaining factors, and therapeutic goals). However, the most frequently used intervention in clinical supervision in psychotherapy training was case discussion. Rarely were techniques used that allowed the supervisor to give the supervisee feedback based on the supervisee's demonstrated competencies. For example, 46% of the supervisors never used audiotapes or videotapes in the supervision. Differences between CBT and PT were rather small. Current practice regarding the techniques used in clinical supervision for psychotherapy trainees contradicts recommendations for active and feedback-oriented clinical supervision. Thus the potential of clinical supervision might not be fully used in clinical practice.

Type
Original Research
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2017 

Introduction

One empirically derived definition of clinical supervision is provided by Milne (Reference Milne2007, p. 440), who defines clinical supervision as ‘the formal provision by senior/qualified health practitioners of an intensive relationship-based education and training that is case-focused and which supports, directs and guides the work of colleagues (supervisees)’. The functions of clinical supervision are quality control, the maintenance and facilitation of supervisees’ competence and capability, and helping supervisees to work effectively. This definition and these functions imply that clinical supervision should play an important role in psychotherapy training. Thus it is not surprising that clinical supervision represents a necessary element in psychotherapy training in many countries (Strauß and Kohl, Reference Strauß and Kohl2009). Psychotherapy trainees evaluate clinical supervision as one of the most important and useful parts of their training (Nodop et al., Reference Nodop, Thiel and Strauß2010). The important role of clinical supervision is supported by empirical research indicating that clinical supervision has positive effects on supervisees’ therapeutic development and competencies (e.g. Lambert and Ogles, Reference Lambert, Ogles and Watkins1997; Wheeler and Richards, Reference Wheeler and Richards2007; Rakovshik and McManus, Reference Rakovshik and McManus2010; Bambling, Reference Bambling, Watkins and Milne2014).

The teaching process in clinical supervision can be heterogeneous, and clinical supervision may address various topics. For example, the psychotherapeutic alliance or the patient's homework may be discussed. Moreover, these topics may be addressed by different techniques (e.g. giving information, conducting a role play) to promote the supervisee's learning. Models that describe the development of therapeutic competence (Milne et al., Reference Milne, Claydon, Blackburn, James and Sheikh2001; Sharpless and Barber, Reference Sharpless and Barber2009) consider qualitatively different competence stages (e.g. novice, advance beginner, expert), which are associated with different learning needs. Therefore, clinical supervision should be adapted to the different learning stages of the supervisees and should therefore differ between accredited therapists and psychotherapy trainees in different stages of their training.

Recommendations regarding clinical supervision emphasize the importance of direct observation (live or videotaped) of the demonstrated competencies to provide accurate feedback (Falender and Shafranske, Reference Falender and Shafranske2014). Feedback might be especially important in clinical supervision because psychotherapists have difficulty adequately evaluating their own therapeutic competencies (e.g. Martino et al., Reference Martino, Ball, Nich, Frankforter and Carroll2009; Walfish et al., Reference Walfish, McAlister, O'Donnell and Lambert2012; Weck et al., Reference Weck, Richtberg, Jakob, Neng and Höfling2015). In particular, in psychotherapy training, the need for accurate self-evaluation of competencies is essential for the development of psychotherapeutic competencies. Therefore, it has been recommended that the supervisor's direct observation of the supervisee's therapy sessions and feedback regarding therapeutic work should be a central element of clinical supervision in psychotherapy training (O'Donovan et al., Reference O'Donovan, Halford and Walters2011).

The importance of the use of feedback, active learning approaches, and structuring techniques in clinical supervision was also confirmed by a systematic review (Milne et al., Reference Milne, Reiser, Aylott, Dunkerley, Fitzpatrick and Wharton2010). That review examined 24 studies in which clinical supervision demonstrated a positive impact on supervisees’ learning in cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT). The most common strategies of effective supervision were feedback (in nearly 80% of the studies), followed by the use of audiotapes or videotapes, multimodal methods of teaching (e.g. live demonstration, role play rehearsal), and prioritization (e.g. discussion of agenda).

Psychotherapy research that compares different supervision approaches also shows that feedback that is based on direct observations is particularly important to improve therapeutic competencies. Thus live supervision, which allows direct insight in the therapeutic process and immediate supervisory interventions, has been found to be superior to less process-oriented approaches (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Carpenter, Amrhein, Brooks, Levin, Schreiber, Travaglini, Hu and Nunes2012; Carmel et al., Reference Carmel, Villatte, Rosenthal, Chalker and Comtois2016; Weck et al., Reference Weck, Jakob, Neng, Höfling, Grikscheit and Bohus2016). In addition, training methods that provide video-based feedback regarding demonstrated therapeutic competencies have been shown to be effective in improving therapeutic competencies (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Yahne, Moyers, Martinez and Pirritano2004; Weck et al., Reference Weck, Kaufmann and Höfling2017).

Psychodynamically oriented supervisory models emphasize the similarity between psychotherapy and clinical supervision. Thus a competent psychotherapist is also considered a competent supervisor (Strauß et al., Reference Strauß, Wheeler and Nodop2010). For example, in psychodynamic treatments, structure has another importance than in CBT (Cutler et al., Reference Cutler, Goldyne, Markowitz, Devlin and Glick2004). Therefore, in contrast to CBT-oriented supervision, structuring and active supervision techniques are less relevant in clinical supervision than working out the insight and understanding of the psychodynamic process using psychodynamic methods.

Little is known about the exact type and frequency of the use of different supervision strategies in clinical practice. One exception is the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002), who investigated the strategies utilized in clinical supervision. In that study, 170 CBT psychotherapists in the United Kingdom (UK) [accredited by the British Association of Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy (BABCP)] described the practices involved in their supervision. Participants had to evaluate whether 16 specific topics (e.g. ethical issues) and eight specific techniques (e.g. homework tasks) were used ‘never’, ‘rarely’, ‘sometimes’ or ‘often’ in their supervision sessions. Based on the findings, the authors concluded that overall, clinical supervision was found to be less structured and active than psychotherapy. Strategies that enable the supervisee to receive feedback regarding his/her demonstrated competencies, such as the consideration of audiotapes or videotapes, were seldom used (approximately 52% of the trainees reported that audiotapes or videotapes were ‘never’ used). Role play was also seldom used (approximately 81% of the trainees reported that role plays were ‘rarely’ or ‘never’ used). Case discussions were the most frequently used supervision technique (approximately 90% of the trainees reported that case discussions were used ‘often’). It is remarkable that half of the therapists reported that strategies that allow supervisors to give feedback regarding demonstrated competencies and that were considered particularly important for the development of therapeutic competencies were not used.

Two recent investigations complement the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002) by considering the supervisors’ perspectives (Reiser and Milne, Reference Reiser and Milne2016; Gonsalvez et al., Reference Gonsalvez, Wahnon and Deane2017). In the study by Gonsalvez et al. (Reference Gonsalvez, Wahnon and Deane2017), 113 accredited supervisors of postgraduate psychology trainees in Australia were questioned regarding practices in clinical supervision. The study addressed core supervisor competences, i.e. goal-setting, providing formative feedback, and conducting summative assessments. Active supervision strategies were used more often than in the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002). For example, only 22% of the supervisors reported ‘never’ or ‘rarely’ using direct observations of the supervisee in sessions. In addition, the review of audiotaped or videotaped recordings of sessions was ‘never’ used in only 29% of the cases (in the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002), 52% of the respondents ‘never’ reviewed audio or video recordings). However, it is still of significant concern and against recommendations that only one-third of the cases never reviewed audio or videotapes.

In the study by Reiser and Milne (Reference Reiser and Milne2016), 110 supervisors and trainers in the UK (accredited by the BABCP) were asked which methods they used in clinical supervision. In accordance with the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002), case discussion was the most often used strategy in clinical supervision. However, supervisors reported using active strategies (e.g. role play, reviewing audio/video recordings) and structuring strategies (e.g. agenda setting) more frequently in clinical supervision than in the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002). For example, 100% of the supervisors used role play and 99% of the supervisors used audio or video recordings ‘sometimes’ or ‘frequently’. The difference in these findings might be explained by the different perspectives (i.e. supervisor versus supervisee), different samples (the supervisors in the study of Reiser and Milne occupied leadership roles within CBT supervision), or by a relevant and desirable change in the supervisory practice.

The study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002) examined supervisory practice in CBT in the UK more than a decade ago, raising the question of whether this supervisory practice has changed and whether this practice is different in other countries. The studies of Gonsalvez et al. (Reference Gonsalvez, Wahnon and Deane2017) in Australia and Reiser and Milne (Reference Reiser and Milne2016) in the UK showed that supervisory practice might have changed towards the use of more active and structuring methods; however, these studies are difficult to compare because their information is based on different perspectives (i.e. supervisor versus supervisee).

In Germany, clinical supervision is a compulsory part of state-approved psychotherapy training. Psychotherapy training includes different training modules, one of which includes 150 hours of supervision. Thus clinical supervision is of high importance in German psychotherapy training, which is associated with high financial costs within psychotherapy training (Weck, Reference Weck2013). In Germany, CBT and psychodynamic therapy (PT, i.e. psychodynamic psychotherapy and psychoanalysis) are part of state-approved patient care. However, there is no formal state-approved training programme for clinical supervisors in Germany. The requirements for licensure as a supervisor in psychotherapy training are one's own clinical practice (five years after accreditation as a psychotherapist) and activities as a lecturer in psychotherapy training (for at least three years). Therefore, it is unclear how familiar supervisors in Germany are with the current recommendations for supervisory practice.

The aim of the present study was to investigate topics and techniques of clinical supervision currently practised in state-approved psychotherapy training in Germany. The current study is therefore a replication of the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002) in Germany. Moreover, the current study extends the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002) because we considered psychotherapy trainees and supervision in CBT as well as in PT. Because of the different supervisory models in CBT and PT, strong differences between supervisory practices in these different therapeutic orientations can be expected. Therefore, we hypothesized a stronger focus on intervention-related topics in CBT supervisions and a stronger focus on the relationship between the patient and therapist in PT supervisions (hypothesis 1). Furthermore, we hypothesized that PT supervisions would be less structured and less active than CBT supervisions (hypothesis 2). Moreover, we expected that the supervisees’ progress in psychotherapy training (i.e. received supervision sessions, duration of psychotherapy training) would influence the frequency of supervisory topics and techniques (hypothesis 3).

Method

Setting and design

The current study investigated clinical supervision for psychotherapy trainees in Germany. In Germany, health insurance funds pay for psychotherapeutic treatment. Thus CBT and PT (i.e. psychodynamic psychotherapy and psychoanalysis) are state approved. The law regulates the licensure and psychotherapy training of these approaches. The requirement for psychotherapy training is a master's degree in psychology, medicine, or educational science. The training programme lasts at least three years, including a minimum of 4200 hours of different content [e.g. workshops (minimum of 600 hours), supervision (minimum of 120 hours), and psychotherapeutic treatment of patients (minimum of 600 hours)], and concludes with an oral and written state examination. The supervision of treated patients is thus an important and necessary part of psychotherapy training. At least every fourth therapy session must be supervised by a licensed supervisor.

The current study was conducted between November 2015 and January 2016. Participants were recruited in postgraduate psychotherapy training institutions (a total of 211 institutions in Germany). We contacted the institutions by email and asked them to forward a link to the online questionnaire to their trainees. The inclusion criteria of the current study were as follows: (a) participating in psychotherapy training, (b) currently treating at least one patient, (c) receiving clinical supervision, and (d) informed consent. Figure 1 shows the participants’ flowchart.

Fig. 1. Participant flowchart

Because the psychoanalyses group (n = 72) was relatively small and because of the conceptual closeness to psychodynamic psychotherapy (n = 163), both groups were pooled into one group with the trainees who were trained in psychodynamic psychotherapy and psychoanalysis (n = 7). Thus the pooled group of PT trainees included 242 trainees.Footnote 1

Participants

A total of 791 participants fulfilled our inclusion criteria and were considered for the data analysis (533 CBT and 242 PT). The characteristics of the participants are given in Table 1. Note that a pedagogue is an individual who has studied pedagogy (methods of teaching) at a university. We found significant differences between trainees of different therapeutic orientations. Trainees of PT were an average of 4.6 years older (F (1,773) = 90.24; p < .001), had received an average of 14.1 more supervision sessions (F (1,773) = 6.64; p = .01) and had an average of 1.04 years more psychotherapy training (F (1,773) = 40.96; p < .001) than trainees of CBT. No differences were found regarding sex (χ² (1) = 1.76; p = .20) between trainees of PT and CBT.

Table 1. Characteristics of the participants (n = 791)

Survey questionnaire

The survey questionnaire of the current study was based on a previous survey on clinical supervision (Townend et al., Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002) and included four sections. The first section assessed sociodemographic data (i.e. sex, age, university education, and psychotherapeutic orientation).

The second section assessed details regarding psychotherapy training, psychotherapeutic activities, and clinical supervision (i.e. start of psychotherapy training, whether psychotherapy training was finished, number of weekly therapy sessions, number of different supervisors, and number of received supervisory sessions).

The third section assessed the frequency of 16 topics in clinical supervision: (1) the therapeutic alliance, (2) therapeutic goals and agreements, (3) patient motivation and suitability, (4) maintaining factors, (5) case reports regarding symptoms and progress, (6) treatment plan, (7) selection and implementation of interventions, (8) patient homework, (9) evaluation of the treatment or the treatment progress, (10) ethical issues, (11) risk evaluation regarding the patient (e.g. suicidality) or evaluation of risks for the therapist due to the patient, (12) diagnostic issues, (13) therapy success, (14) therapist competencies, (15) therapist's own personal problems, and (16) satisfaction with the supervision. Supervisees were asked how often those topics were part of their supervision sessions. The response format for the 16 items regarding the supervision topics was a four-point scale (1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, and 4 = often).

The fourth section assessed the frequency of 10 techniques in clinical supervision: (1) direct observation (live supervision), (2) consideration of audiotapes or videotapes, (3) role play, (4) agenda setting for the supervisory session, (5) personal goal setting for the supervisee, (6) case discussion, (7) considering the supervisee's own cognitive processes and their relevance for the therapeutic behaviour, (8) therapist homework tasks, (9) information brokering (e.g. regarding interventions or diagnosis), and (10) recommending literature (e.g. manuals, worksheets, diagnostic instruments). Supervisees were asked how often those techniques were used in their supervision sessions. The response format of the 10 items regarding supervision techniques was a four-point scale (1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, and 4 = often).

The internal consistency (Cronbach's α) of the 26 items assessing supervisory topics and techniques was .81. The Cronbach's α subscale for the assessment of topics (16 items) was .76, and for the subscale for the assessment of techniques (10 items) was .65.

Statistical analyses

Analyses of variance (ANOVA) were used to compare the dimensional variables. Thereby, CBT and PT were considered group factors and the dimensional variables (e.g. age) were considered dependent variables. Differences between CBT and PT in terms of the categorical variables (e.g. sex) were analysed using chi-squared tests. Differences in the frequency of topics and techniques in clinical supervision between groups (CBT versus PT) were analysed using two multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA). In the first MANOVA, the 15 items that measured topics were defined as the dependent variables and the supervisory group (CBT and PT) was the independent variable (relevant for hypothesis 1). In the second MANOVA, the 10 items that measured techniques were defined as the dependent variables and the supervisory group (CBT and PT) was the independent variable (relevant for hypothesis 2). Because of the differences in the group sample sizes, between-group effect sizes were determined by Hedges’ g. A Hedges’ g of .40 is considered a small effect, a Hedges’ g between .50 and .80 is considered a moderate effect, and a Hedges’ g larger than 0.80 is considered a large effect size (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). Correlations, which were relevant for hypothesis 3, were analysed using the Pearson correlation coefficient.

Results

Frequency of specific topics

Figure 2 and Table 2 present the frequency of different topics in clinical supervision. The most frequently discussed topics were ‘interventions’ (item 7), ‘therapeutic alliance’ (item 1), ‘maintaining factors’ (item 4), and ‘therapeutic goals’ (item 2). The less frequently discussed topics were ‘satisfaction with the supervision’, ‘therapist personal problems’, and ‘ethical issues’.

Fig. 2. Frequency of the different topics in clinical supervision of psychotherapy trainees (n = 791)

Table 2. Mean scores and standard deviations regarding specific topics in supervision

Items were ordered according to their frequency in cognitive behavioural supervisions, beginning with the most frequent topic. *Scale range: 1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often; n.s. = not significant.

To address the first hypothesis, we compared the frequency of supervisory topics in CBT and PT. In the MANOVA, significant differences between CBT and PT approaches were found for supervisory topics (V = 0.40; F (1,773) = 31.30; p < .001). For the different topics, most effect sizes for between-group differences were of small or moderate magnitude (see Table 2). Only for ‘therapeutic alliance’ (item 1; g = –0.88) and ‘patient homework’ (item 8; g = 1.24) were large effect sizes found. The ‘therapeutic alliance’ was discussed more frequently in PT-oriented than in CBT-oriented supervisions, and ‘patient homework’ was discussed more frequently in CBT-oriented than in PT-oriented supervisions. In contrast to the third hypothesis, the frequency of specific topics was not correlated or was only marginally correlated with the number of received supervision sessions or the duration of psychotherapy training (all r values < .20).

Frequency of techniques used

Figure 3 and Table 3 present the frequency of different techniques used in supervision. The most frequent technique was ‘case discussion’ (item 6), followed by ‘information brokering’ (item 9). Several techniques were seldom used, such as ‘direct observation’ (item 1), ‘therapist homework tasks’ (item 8), ‘role play’ (item 3), and ‘audio/videotapes’ (item 2).

Fig. 3. Frequency of the different techniques in clinical supervision of psychotherapy trainees (n = 791)

Table 3. Mean scores and standard deviations regarding specific techniques in supervision

Items were ordered according to their frequency in cognitive behavioural supervisions, beginning with the most frequent technique. *Scale range: 1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often; n.s. = not significant.

To address the second hypothesis, we compared the frequency of supervisory topics in CBT and PT. Supervision in CBT and PT approaches differed significantly in the MANOVA for supervisory techniques (V = 0.31; F (1,773) = 34.08; p < .001). None of the effect sizes for the between-group differences was of large magnitude. The largest effect sizes were found for the use of ‘audio/videotapes’ (item 2; g = 0.69) and for ‘agenda setting’ (item 4; g = 0.64). ‘Audio/videotapes’ were used more frequently in CBT-oriented than in PT-oriented supervisions, and an agenda was implemented more often in CBT-oriented than in PT-oriented supervisions. In contrast to the third hypothesis, the frequency of specific techniques was not correlated or was only marginally correlated with the number of received supervision sessions or the duration of psychotherapy training (all r values < .20).

Discussion

The current study investigated topics and techniques in clinical supervision in a large sample of psychotherapy trainees in Germany. The findings showed that supervision focused on topics that are central for the effective treatment of patients (i.e. therapeutic interventions, therapeutic alliance, maintaining factors, therapeutic goals). However, we found that active strategies (e.g. role play), structuring strategies (e.g. agenda setting), and strategies that allow for feedback regarding supervisees’ competencies (e.g. via the use of audiotapes or videotapes) were seldom used in clinical supervision. Case discussion was the predominant strategy in clinical supervision of psychotherapy trainees. In contrast to hypotheses 1 and 2, we have found rather small differences between CBT-oriented and PT-oriented supervisions. In contrast to hypothesis 3, we found that supervisees’ characteristics are less relevant for the reported supervisory topics and techniques.

Overall, our results are comparable to the findings of Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002) who investigated accredited psychotherapists in the UK. Clinical supervision was less active and less structured than recommended in the literature. For example, 46% of the supervisees reported that they never used audiotapes or videotapes. This phenomenon might make clinical supervision less effective than it could otherwise be. Empirical research has demonstrated that therapists have difficulty adequately evaluating their competencies and that video-based or observation-based feedback was superior for improving therapeutic competencies. One reason for this discrepancy between recommendations and clinical practice might be that direct observation is associated with fears of negative evaluation by the supervisor. These fears are related to non-disclosure (Jakob et al., Reference Jakob, Weck, Höfling, Richtberg and Bohus2014; Sweeney and Creaner, Reference Sweeney and Creaner2014), which is easier to realize within case discussions than within strategies based on direct observation. Therefore, supervisees may avoid more direct supervision strategies (e.g. use of videotapes, direct observation). Another reason might be that strategies that are more direct are also more time consuming. For example, live supervision, which allows for direct observation of therapeutic competencies and direct feedback, is associated with larger organizational costs (Jakob et al., Reference Jakob, Weck, Schornick, Krause and Bohus2015). Furthermore, there might be a lack of knowledge regarding the appropriate implementation of more active or direct strategies, such as videotapes, in supervision sessions. Thus supervisors and supervisees may seldom use these techniques. Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002) described this issue critically more than 15 years ago as follows: ‘What this means is that supervision is mainly about what supervisees say they are doing in practice and not what may actually be taking place’ (p. 497). In contrast to the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002), we did not investigate accredited psychotherapists but psychotherapy trainees. In that context, the lack of active strategies can be evaluated as even more problematic (O'Donovan et al., Reference O'Donovan, Halford and Walters2011).

The current study and the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002) differ from more recent studies, which found that active and structuring strategies were used more frequently (Reiser and Milne, Reference Reiser and Milne2016; Gonsalvez et al., Reference Gonsalvez, Wahnon and Deane2017). However, those more recent studies also differed significantly regarding the frequency of specific supervisory methods. In the study by Gonsalvez et al. (Reference Gonsalvez, Wahnon and Deane2017), 46% of the supervisors reported reviewing audio/videotapes ‘never’ or ‘rarely’, whereas in the study by Reiser and Milne (Reference Reiser and Milne2016), only 1% of the supervisors reported reviewing audio/videotapes ‘never’ or ‘rarely’. Differences between the current study and the more recent studies might be due to different perspectives because the more recent studies surveyed supervisors and not supervisees. It is possible that the information given by supervisors and supervisees is incongruent. A further explanation might be that supervisory practices in the UK have changed and that supervisory practices in Germany are comparable to those in the UK 15 years ago. Therefore, future studies should include both the perspective of the supervisors and the supervisees. In addition, observational studies would allow a more objective evaluation of supervisory practices.

In addition to the study by Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002), CBT as well as PT trainees were considered in the current study. In contrast to our hypotheses 1 and 2, we found that differences between CBT and PT regarding topics and techniques in clinical supervision were not present, or were mostly of small to moderate magnitude. This result is surprising because theoretical models of supervision would suggest a different approach in CBT and PT supervisions. One explanation for this finding might be that the recommendations regarding CBT supervision (i.e. structured and active) are not implemented satisfactorily. For example, in our study, case discussion was the dominant technique in CBT and PT and more active techniques (e.g. role play), which are particularly preferred in CBT, were seldom used. Because of this lack of specificity in CBT supervision, differences between CBT and PT might be marginal for most areas.

Strong differences between CBT-oriented and PT-oriented supervisions were only found for the frequency of addressing the therapeutic alliance (g = –0.88) and patient homework (g = 1.24). This partial result is in line with our first hypothesis, but in contrast with other topics (e.g. interventions), where the differences between CBT and PT were only marginal. Homework tasks are not confined to one therapeutic orientation but are particularly relevant in CBT (Kazantzis and Dattilio, Reference Kazantzis and Dattilio2010). Therefore, it is in accordance with theoretical models stating that homework tasks are more often a topic in CBT-oriented supervisions. The therapeutic alliance, in contrast, is of primary importance in PT, but is also considered an important therapeutic process in CBT and is significantly related to therapy success (Horvath et al., Reference Horvath, Del Re, Flückiger and Symonds2011). Therefore, it might be an important difference that the therapeutic alliance is more frequently a topic in PT than in CBT. The empirical findings suggest that the quality of the therapeutic alliance in CBT is as good as in PT (e.g. Raue et al., Reference Raue, Putterman, Goldfried and Wolitzky1995). However, we cannot rule out the possibility that those differences influence the effectiveness of the given supervision.

For some techniques, we found moderate differences between CBT and PT. We found that PT-oriented supervisions were less structured (e.g. agenda setting, personal goal setting) than CBT-oriented supervisions. This partial result is also in line with hypotheses 2 because structure is seen as more relevant in CBT than in psychodynamic treatments (Cutler et al., Reference Cutler, Goldyne, Markowitz, Devlin and Glick2004). However, the fact that we seldom found the use of structuring strategies in CBT-oriented supervisions contradicts recommendations regarding the effective design of clinical supervision in CBT (Cummings et al., Reference Cummings, Ballantyne and Scallion2015).

In terms of our third hypothesis, we found no evidence that different supervision strategies were used in relation to the training progress of the trainees. This result is surprising when we consider the development models for psychotherapy competencies (Milne et al., Reference Milne, Claydon, Blackburn, James and Sheikh2001; Sharpless and Barber, Reference Sharpless and Barber2009) because these models imply that trainees have different learning needs in relation to their level of competence. In particular, in psychotherapy training, a stronger adapted supervisory practice would be desirable.

Limitations

Our assessment method was limited to supervisees’ retrospective reports and is potentially confounded by memory biases. Therefore, direct observation of supervision sessions would be preferable. Our recruiting method (due to the postgraduate psychotherapy training institutions) did not allow us to determine how many psychotherapy trainees received the link to our study; we can only determine how many participants started the survey. For this sample, our response rate (60% included in the data analysis) was comparable to the survey of Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002), which had a response rate of 61%, and was larger than that of the study by Reiser and Milne (Reference Reiser and Milne2016; response rate: 44%) and the study by Gonsalvez et al. (Reference Gonsalvez, Wahnon and Deane2017; response rate: 18%). The response rate in our study can be seen as very high when we consider that 91% of the participants who started the survey completed the sociodemographic data section. Moreover, the assessment was conducted via the internet, which might influence participants’ responses. However, empirical findings show that internet findings are consistent with findings from traditional assessment methods (Gosling et al., Reference Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava and John2004).

Furthermore, while the reliability of the whole measure (Cronbach's α = .81) and the subscale supervisory topics (Cronbach's α = .76) were satisfactory, the reliability of the subscale supervisory techniques (Cronbach's α = .65) was questionable. One explanation for the low reliability of the subscale supervisory techniques might be that the scale assessed relatively heterogeneous supervisory techniques. Thus the intercorrelations of the items and consequently the internal consistency of the scale were low. Therefore, the test–retest reliability might be a better estimator of the reliability; however, test–retest reliability was not evaluated in the current study. The study by Reiser and Milne (Reference Reiser and Milne2016) reported the reliability of the measurement, but reliability was not reported by the Townend et al. (Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002) or Gonsalvez et al. (Reference Gonsalvez, Wahnon and Deane2017) studies. In the study by Reiser and Milne (Reference Reiser and Milne2016), the internal consistencies of the measures were satisfactory. However, a very homogenous sample of supervisors was interviewed, which might explain the more homogenous use of supervisory methods and the higher internal consistency of the measurement.

Another limitation is that supervisees did not differ only with regard to their therapeutic orientations. Trainees of PT orientations were older, had received more supervision sessions and had been in psychotherapy training longer than trainees of CBT. These characteristics might have influenced the frequency of topics and techniques in clinical supervision. However, in our analyses, we found no correlations or only small correlations between those characteristics and the frequency of topics and techniques in clinical supervision. Therefore, we can assume that the differences are attributable to the therapeutic orientations rather than to those characteristics.

Furthermore, the current study was limited to clinical supervision in CBT and PT. These therapeutic approaches are the most common and important in Germany. However, other therapeutic approaches, such as interpersonal psychotherapy or systemic therapy, are also relevant and should be investigated in future studies. Clinical supervision in additional countries should also be considered.

In addition to the Likert scales used in the current study, qualitative data regarding clinical supervision would also be interesting (see Townend et al., Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002). For example, supervisees could be questioned about the topics and techniques they identified during the supervision sessions. It would be interesting to determine whether the supervisees would name additional topics and techniques. Therefore, further studies should also consider open questions and qualitative analyses in supervision research.

Conclusions

Consistent with findings from more than a decade ago in the UK (Townend et al., Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002) and in contrast to more recent studies (e.g. Reiser and Milne, Reference Reiser and Milne2016), the current study shows that supervision practices in psychotherapy training differ substantially from experts’ recommendations and empirical evidence. Clinical supervision is a training element that can go beyond other training elements (e.g. workshops) by providing direct feedback regarding trainees’ competencies. In line with this consideration, directors of psychotherapy training institutes see supervision as the most appropriate part of training to identify deficits in trainees’ competencies (Nodop and Strauß, Reference Nodop and Strauß2013). However, this potential of clinical supervision is currently not fully used in clinical practice. To date in Germany, only practical experience as a psychotherapist and trainer are required to become a supervisor in psychotherapy training. Thus it can be questioned whether this procedure is sufficient. In contrast, it could be argued that a professional and state-approved supervisory training programme that considers current recommendations for supervisory practice is necessary.

Main points

  1. (1) Literature recommendations and empirical research on clinical supervision emphasize the importance of providing accurate feedback (e.g. by using videotapes) to ensure effective supervision.

  2. (2) Within the context of psychotherapy training, we found that clinical supervision seldom uses active strategies, structuring strategies or strategies that allow for direct feedback regarding supervisees’ competencies (e.g. the use of audiotapes or videotapes).

  3. (3) Differences regarding topics and techniques in clinical supervision between different therapeutic orientations (cognitive behavioural therapy and psychodynamic psychotherapy) were only marginal.

  4. (4) To ensure supervisees’ optimal learning, methods that allow the direct evaluation of supervisees’ competencies more should be considered (e.g. live supervisions or the use of videotapes).

Ethical statements

The authors assert that all procedures that contributed to this work comply with the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as set out by the APA. Because this study was considered a low-risk study and did not include patients, invasive procedures, or deception, our institution does not require official approval by an ethics committee.

Conflicts of interest

Florian Weck, Yvonne M. Kaufmann and Michael Witthöft have no conflicts of interest with respect to this publication.

Learning objectives

  1. (1) Accurate and direct feedback is important in clinical supervision to ensure optimal learning for psychotherapy trainees.

  2. (2) There is a large discrepancy between recommendations regarding clinical supervision and supervisory practice in psychotherapy training.

  3. (3) Differences in clinical supervision between cognitive behavioural therapy and psychodynamic therapy are rather small in psychotherapy training.

  4. (4) Supervision strategies that address competencies demonstrated by the supervisees (e.g. review of videotapes) should be used more frequently in psychotherapy training.

Footnotes

1 In the statistical analyses, we found only minimal differences between psychodynamic psychotherapy and psychoanalyses regarding topics and techniques in supervision. Regarding topics, ‘therapist competencies’ (item 14) was significantly more often addressed in psychoanalyses-oriented supervisions. Regarding techniques, ‘agenda setting’ (item 4) was more often used in psychodynamic-oriented supervisions and ‘therapist cognitive processes’ were more often used in psychoanalyses-oriented supervisions. However, the effect sizes were only of small magnitude.

References

Recommended follow-up reading

Muse, K, McManus, F (2013). A systematic review of methods for assessing competence in cognitive-behavioural therapy. Clinical Psychology Review 33, 484499.Google Scholar
Rakovshik, SG, McManus, F (2010). Establishing evidence-based training in cognitive behavioral therapy: a review of current empirical findings and theoretical guidance. Clinical Psychology Review 30, 496516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Bambling, M (2014). Creating positive outcomes in clinical supervision. In The Wiley International Handbook of Clinical Supervision (eds Watkins, CE, Milne, DL), pp. 445457. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Carmel, A, Villatte, JL, Rosenthal, MZ, Chalker, S, Comtois, KA (2016). Applying technological approaches to clinical supervision in dialectical behavior therapy: a randomized feasibility trial of the bug-in-the-eye (BITE) model. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice 23, 221229.Google Scholar
Cohen, J (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cummings, JA, Ballantyne, EC, Scallion, LM (2015). Essential processes for cognitive behavioral clinical supervision: agenda setting, problem-solving, and formative feedback. Psychotherapy 52, 158163.Google Scholar
Cutler, JL, Goldyne, A, Markowitz, JC, Devlin, MJ, Glick, RA (2004). Comparing cognitive behavior therapy, interpersonal psychotherapy, and psychodynamic psychotherapy. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 15671573.Google Scholar
Falender, CA, Shafranske, EP (2014). Clinical supervision: the state of the art. Journal of Clinical Psychology 70, 10301041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonsalvez, CJ, Wahnon, T, Deane, FP (2017). Goal-setting, feedback, and assessment practices reported by Australian clinical supervisors. Australian Psychologist 52, 2130.Google Scholar
Gosling, SD, Vazire, S, Srivastava, S, John, OP (2004). Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about Internet questionnaires. American Psychologist 59, 93104.Google Scholar
Horvath, AO, Del Re, AC, Flückiger, C, Symonds, D (2011). Alliance in individual psychotherapy. Psychotherapy 48, 916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jakob, M, Weck, F, Höfling, V, Richtberg, S, Bohus, M (2014). Non-disclosure during psychotherapy supervision: validation of the German version of the supervisory questionnaire (SQ). Psychotherapy Research 24, 4251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakob, M, Weck, F, Schornick, M, Krause, T, Bohus, M (2015). Wenn der Supervisor zuschaut. Eine qualitative Analyse der Akzeptanz von Live-Supervision. [When the supervisor is watching. Qualitative analysis of the acceptance of live supervision]. Psychotherapeut 60, 210215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazantzis, N, Dattilio, FM (2010). Definitions of homework, types of homework, and ratings of the importance of homework among psychologists with cognitive behavior therapy and psychoanalytic theoretical orientations. Journal of Clinical Psychology 66, 758773.Google Scholar
Lambert, MJ, Ogles, BM (1997). The effectiveness of psychotherapy supervision. In Handbook of Psychotherapy Supervision (ed Watkins, CE), pp. 421446. New York: Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Martino, S, Ball, S, Nich, C, Frankforter, TL, Carroll, KM (2009). Correspondence of motivational enhancement treatment integrity ratings among therapists, supervisors, and observers. Psychotherapy Research 19, 181193.Google Scholar
Miller, WR, Yahne, CE, Moyers, TB, Martinez, J, Pirritano, M (2004). A randomized trial of methods to help clinicians learn motivational interviewing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 72, 10501062.Google Scholar
Milne, D (2007). An empirical definition of clinical supervision. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 46, 437447.Google Scholar
Milne, D, Claydon, T, Blackburn, I-M, James, I, Sheikh, A (2001). Rationale for a new measure of competence in therapy. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 29, 2133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milne, D, Reiser, R, Aylott, H, Dunkerley, C, Fitzpatrick, H, Wharton, S (2010). The systematic review as an empirical approach to improving CBT supervision. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy 3, 278294.Google Scholar
Nodop, S, Strauß, B (2013). Mangelnde Eignung bei angehenden Psychotherapeuten. Psychotherapeut 58, 446454.Google Scholar
Nodop, S, Thiel, K, Strauß, B (2010). Supervision in der psychotherapeutischen Ausbildung in Deutschland. Psychotherapeut 55, 485495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Donovan, A, Halford, WK, Walters, B (2011). Towards best practice supervision of clinical psychology trainees. Australian Psychologist 46, 101112.Google Scholar
Rakovshik, SG, McManus, F (2010). Establishing evidence-based training in cognitive behavioral therapy: a review of current empirical findings and theoretical guidance. Clinical Psychology Review 30, 496516.Google Scholar
Raue, P, Putterman, J, Goldfried, M, Wolitzky, D (1995). Effect of rater orientation on the evaluation of therapeutic alliance. Psychotherapy Research 5, 337342.Google Scholar
Reiser, RP, Milne, DL (2016). A survey of CBT supervision in the UK: methods, satisfaction and training, as viewed by a selected sample of CBT supervision leaders. the Cogntive Behaviour Therapist 9, e20. doi: 10.1017/S1754470X15000689 Google Scholar
Sharpless, BA, Barber, JP (2009). A conceptual and empirical review of the meaning, measurement, development, and teaching of intervention competence in clinical psychology. Clinical Psychology Review 29, 4756.Google Scholar
Smith, JL, Carpenter, KM, Amrhein, PC, Brooks, AC, Levin, D, Schreiber, EA, Travaglini, LA, Hu, MC, Nunes, EV (2012). Training substance abuse clinicians in motivational interviewing using live supervision via teleconferencing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 80, 450464.Google Scholar
Strauß, B, Kohl, S (2009). Entwicklung der Psychotherapie und der Psychotherapieausbildung in europäischen Ländern. Psychotherapeut 54, 457463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauß, B, Wheeler, S, Nodop, S (2010). Klinische Supervision. Überblick über den Stand der Forschung [Clinical supervison. Review of the state of research]. Psychotherapeut 55, 455464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweeney, J, Creaner, M (2014). What's not being said? Recollections of non-disclosure in clinical supervision while in training. British Journal of Guidance and Counselling 42, 211224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townend, M, Iannetta, L, Freeston, MH (2002). Clinical supervision in practice: a survey of UK cognitive behavioural psychotherapists accredited by the BABCP. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 30, 485500.Google Scholar
Walfish, S, McAlister, B, O'Donnell, P, Lambert, MJ (2012). An investigation of self-assessment bias in mental health providers. Psychological Reports 110, 639644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weck, F (2013). Psychotherapeutische Kompetenzen: Theorien, Erfassung und Förderung. [Psychotherapy competencies: theory, assessment, and training]. Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weck, F, Jakob, M, Neng, JMB, Höfling, V, Grikscheit, F, Bohus, M (2016) The effects of bug-in-the-eye supervision on therapeutic alliance and therapist competence in cognitive-behavioral therapy: a randomized controlled trial. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 23, 386396.Google Scholar
Weck, F, Kaufmann, YM, Höfling, V (2017). Competence feedback improves CBT competence in trainee therapists: a randomized controlled pilot study. Psychotherapy Research 27, 501509.Google Scholar
Weck, F, Richtberg, S, Jakob, M, Neng, JM, Höfling, V (2015). Therapist competence and therapeutic alliance are important in the treatment of health anxiety (hypochondriasis). Psychiatry Research 228, 5358.Google Scholar
Wheeler, S, Richards, K (2007). The impact of clinical supervision on counsellors and therapists, their practice and their clients. A systematic review of the literature. Counselling and Psychotherapy Research 7, 5465.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Participant flowchart

Figure 1

Table 1. Characteristics of the participants (n = 791)

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Frequency of the different topics in clinical supervision of psychotherapy trainees (n = 791)

Figure 3

Table 2. Mean scores and standard deviations regarding specific topics in supervision

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Frequency of the different techniques in clinical supervision of psychotherapy trainees (n = 791)

Figure 5

Table 3. Mean scores and standard deviations regarding specific techniques in supervision

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.