Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T18:23:45.888Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Don't Give Up on the Self Too Quickly

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 December 2016

Scott N. Taylor*
Affiliation:
Management Division, Babson College
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Scott N. Taylor, Management Division, Babson College, Tomasso Hall, Number 229, 231 Forest Street, Babson Park, MA 02457. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Bracken, Rose, and Church (2016) call for less self- versus other research. They say that self-ratings versus non-self-ratings research (often referred to as self–other agreement research, or SOA) is not particularly helpful and that self-ratings’ primary purpose is to serve as a “reflection point for individuals receiving feedback” (Bracken, Rose, & Church, p. 785). It is true that SOA research has played a dominant role with 360° feedback for many years (for a review, see Fleenor, Smither, Atwater, Braddy, & Sturm, 2010), but the value we can draw from the self through 360° feedback remains terrain largely unexplored.

Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology 2016 

Bracken, Rose, and Church (Reference Bracken, Rose and Church2016) call for less self- versus other research. They say that self-ratings versus non-self-ratings research (often referred to as self–other agreement research, or SOA) is not particularly helpful and that self-ratings’ primary purpose is to serve as a “reflection point for individuals receiving feedback” (Bracken, Rose, & Church, p. 785). It is true that SOA research has played a dominant role with 360° feedback for many years (for a review, see Fleenor, Smither, Atwater, Braddy, & Sturm, Reference Fleenor, Smither, Atwater, Braddy and Sturm2010), but the value we can draw from the self through 360° feedback remains terrain largely unexplored.

Scholar and practitioner interest in SOA is understandable because SOA has been shown to predict important outcomes. For example, researchers and practitioners have frequently used SOA to predict managerial success and effectiveness (e.g., Fletcher, Reference Fletcher1999), leader behavior and performance (e.g., Atwater & Yammarino, Reference Atwater and Yammarino1992), and interpersonal effectiveness (e.g., Brutus, Fleenor, & Tisak, Reference Brutus, Fleenor and Tisak1999) and to distinguish between high and average performing managers (e.g., Church, Reference Church1997).

On the other hand, it is baffling to see how researchers and practitioners have become so enamored with SOA. For decades we have had compelling evidence that self-assessment, self-rating, and self-evaluation in general tend to be inflated, biased, and unreliable regarding a person's ability to assess his or her own behavior, skills, or personality traits (e.g., Dunning, Reference Dunning2005; Leary, Reference Leary2004; Podsakoff & Organ, Reference Podsakoff and Organ1986). Sedikides (Reference Sedikides1993) found that among the three major motivations for self-evaluation (i.e., self-assessment, self-enhancement, and self-verification), the self-enhancement motive was shown to be the most powerful determinant of the self-evaluation process. As Taylor (Reference Taylor2014) points out, these biases call into question the accuracy and validity of using self-ratings at all given their simplistic treatment of the complex self.

Still, in reviewing the research of what SOA has predicted at the individual level, we find yet another benefit of SOA. It is not uncommon to find SOA referred to as a measure of self-awareness (e.g., Church, Reference Church1997; Fletcher & Baldry, Reference Fletcher and Baldry2000; Wohlers & London, Reference Wohlers and London1989). The custom has been to assume that higher self-awareness equates to a smaller gap between self-ratings and others’ ratings. With the use of 360° feedback's SOA capability, we presumably now have an easier, less intrusive way to measure such an important leader and manager capability in self-awareness.

These two long-standing benefits from 360° feedback—obtaining ratings from the self as a measure of self-awareness and SOA research—provide the springboard for future 360° feedback research and practice. But to enter possible new frontiers, we must not give up on the self too quickly.

Classical theories of individual self-awareness collectively define self-awareness as more than just how a person sees him- or herself (captured to some degree with self-ratings). Instead, self-awareness is defined as having two primary components: (a) how people see themselves and the process by which people make assessments about themselves and (b) the ability to detect how they are perceived by others. Accordingly, this two-component definition assumes that individuals must have an understanding of themselves, but they must also have an understanding of and appreciation for others’ perceptions of them (Baumeister, Reference Baumeister2005; Hall, Reference Hall, Day, Zacarro and Halpin2004).

Surprisingly, 360° feedback practitioners and researchers alike have largely ignored the second component of self-awareness, in spite of its importance to being socially aware and to leadership (Taylor, Reference Taylor2010). Since the 1950s, researchers may have assumed that the two components of self-awareness were the same when operationalized. Specifically, studies from Reeder, Donohue, and Biblarz (Reference Reeder, Donohue and Biblarz1960); Quarantelli and Cooper (Reference Quarantelli and Cooper1966); Shrauger and Schoeneman (Reference Shrauger and Schoeneman1979); and many others had participants predict how people viewed them and then compared those predicted ratings with the actual ratings of others. Results primarily showed that peoples’ self-ratings and how they predicted others would see them were nearly identical but not very close to how others actually saw them. Interestingly, these studies were not assessing behaviors commonly assessed by 360° feedback but instead were largely focused on personality traits and mannerisms of young college students.

There are important reasons why we should care about the second component of self-awareness as it relates to 360° feedback. First, self-ratings of behavior appear to not be the same as predicted ratings of behavior (e.g., Sturm, Taylor, Atwater, & Braddy, Reference Sturm, Taylor, Atwater and Braddy2014; Taylor & Hood, Reference Taylor and Hood2011). This is likely because self-ratings access the first component of self-awareness, and predicted ratings access the second component. In other words, predicted ratings are accessing a different part of the self than self-ratings. By predicted ratings, I mean asking participants of 360° feedback to predict as accurately as possible how others rated them (i.e., their manager, peers, and/or direct reports). This can be done by asking participants to select a rater group they are most interested in predicting, by asking participants to predict an assigned rater group (e.g., their manager), or by randomly assigning a rater group for participants to predict. Participants then retake the 360° survey and try to predict how the assigned group or person rated them.

Although still early as a new frontier in 360° feedback research, one study has shown that prediction–other ratings agreement with self- and direct reports’ ratings was a stronger predictor of performance than SOA (Taylor, Wang, & Zhan, Reference Taylor, Wang and Zhan2012). With the collection of predicted ratings, 360° feedback can become an important tool to better understanding the second component of self-awareness.

From a practical perspective, I have found that most 360° feedback participants like the challenge of trying to guess as accurately as possible how they were rated by others and like to receive a feedback report that allows them to compare how they see themselves (i.e., self-ratings), how others see them (i.e., others’ ratings), and how they thought others would see them (predicted ratings). I have found that it is important to limit the number of rater groups participants are asked to predict. In our research, we have limited this to two randomly assigned rater groups or to just the participants’ manager or direct reports (depending on the research study). In practice, I have mostly allowed the participants to select the rater group they are most interested in predicting. I have found most participants choose to predict either their manager's or their direct reports’ ratings.

Second, there is a more important reason we should not be quick to cast aside exploring predicted ratings that come from the self versus ratings that come from others using 360° feedback. Recent research shows that men and women predict the ratings of others differently (e.g., Sturm et al., Reference Sturm, Taylor, Atwater and Braddy2014; Taylor & Hood, Reference Taylor and Hood2011). This is important in today's world, where we see greater numbers of women assuming managerial and leadership positions and yet facing obstacles along their way, such as being underpaid compared with men and having an inability to reach the upper echelons of leadership compared with men (e.g., women on boards of governance). Prediction–other agreement research shows that women leaders predict others’ ratings of them to be lower than are men's predictions, which has important implications when it comes to leadership. Such implications can include decreases in self-efficacy/confidence, lack of advancement, decreased assertiveness, lower performance, gender inequality, or negative emotional responses (Taylor, Sturm, Atwater, & Braddy, Reference Taylor, Sturm, Atwater and Braddy2016). With the collection of predicted ratings, 360° feedback can become an important tool both in research and in practice to further our understanding of important gender differences in organizations.

Third, Taylor and Bright (Reference Taylor and Bright2011) propose that there is a benefit in asking 360° feedback participants to predict how others rate them because predicted ratings may foster greater openness on the part of the participant. Predicted ratings may defuse some of the defensiveness triggered by the frequent discrepancy between self-ratings and the ratings of others. Too often, 360° feedback participants feel like they must create a narrative to explain or even justify why they do not see themselves the same way others see them. The collection of predicted ratings can broaden the 360° feedback development discussion by moving beyond self-versus-other defensiveness to questions like, “How aware am I as to how others experience me?” For example, for those whose self-ratings are higher than the ratings of others and who are yet unaware of the negative implications their overrating is having on others, predicted ratings are especially beneficial in moving the conversation away from defending self-ratings to exploring why these participants were not able to predict how others rated them.

As tempting as it may be to move away from more self- versus other research and decrease the focus on the self when it comes to 360° feedback, to date, we have not taken advantage of the complex self. We have used a limited definition when defining self-awareness with 360° feedback. It is time we broaden our definition to include the second component of self-awareness and begin exploring the value of predicted ratings. Doing so will help us explore new directions in 360° feedback research and practice with important implications for both.

References

Atwater, L. E., & Yammarino, F. J. (1992). Does self–other agreement on leadership perceptions moderate the validity of leadership and performance predictions? Personnel Psychology, 45, 141164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2005). The cultural animal: Human nature, meaning, and social life. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracken, D. W., Rose, D. S., & Church, A. H. (2016). The evolution and devolution of 360° feedback. Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice, 9 (4), 761794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brutus, S., Fleenor, J. W., & Tisak, J. (1999). Exploring the link between rating congruence and managerial effectiveness. Canadian Journal of Administrative Sciences, 16, 308322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Church, A. H. (1997). Managerial self-awareness in high-performing individuals in organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 281292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunning, D. (2005). Self-insight: Roadblocks and detours on the path to knowing thyself. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fleenor, J. W., Smither, J. W., Atwater, L. E., Braddy, P. W., & Sturm, R. E. (2010). Self–other rating agreement in leadership: A review. Leadership Quarterly, 21, 10051034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, C. (1999). The implications of research on gender differences in self-assessment and 360 degree appraisal. Human Resource Management Journal, 9 (1), 3946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, C., & Baldry, C. (2000). A study of individual differences and self-awareness in the context of multi-source feedback. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 73, 303319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, D. T. (2004). Self-awareness, identity, and leader development. In Day, D. V., Zacarro, S. J., & Halpin, S. M. (Eds.), Leader development for transforming organizations: Growing leaders for tomorrow (pp. 153176). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R. (2004). The curse of the self: Self-awareness, egotism, and the quality of human life. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podsakoff, P. M., & Organ, D. W. (1986). Self-reports in organizational research: Problems and prospects. Journal of Management, 12, 531544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quarantelli, E. L., & Cooper, J. (1966). Self-conceptions and others: A further test of Meadian hypotheses. Sociological Quarterly, 7, 281297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reeder, L. G., Donohue, G. A., & Biblarz, A. (1960). Conceptions of self and others. American Journal of Sociology, 66, 153159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sedikides, C. (1993). Assessment, enhancement, and verification determinants of the self-evaluation process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 317338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shrauger, J. S., & Schoeneman, T. J. (1979). Symbolic interactionist view of self-concept: Through the looking glass darkly. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 549573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sturm, R. E., Taylor, S. N., Atwater, L. E., & Braddy, P. W. (2014). Leader self-awareness: An examination and implications for women leaders. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 35, 657677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. N. (2010). Redefining leader self-awareness by integrating the second component of self-awareness. Journal of Leadership Studies, 3 (4), 5768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. N. (2014). Student self-assessment and multisource feedback assessment: Exploring benefits, limitations, and remedies. Journal of Management Education, 38 (3), 360384.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. N., & Bright, D. S. (2011). Exploring conditions for openness in multisource feedback assessment. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 47, 432460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. N., & Hood, J. (2011). It may not be what you think: Gender differences in predicting emotional and social competence. Human Relations, 64 (5), 627652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. N., Sturm, R. E., Atwater, L. E., & Braddy, P. W. (2016). Underestimating one's leadership impact: Are women leaders more susceptible? Organizational Dynamics, 45, 132138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. N., Wang, M., & Zhan, Y. (2012). Going beyond self–other rating agreement: Comparing two components of self-awareness using multisource feedback assessment. Journal of Leadership Studies 6 (2), 631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wohlers, A. J., & London, M. (1989). Ratings of managerial characteristics: Evaluation difficultly, co-worker agreement, and self-awareness. Personnel Psychology, 42, 235261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar