Whereas a considerable amount is known about individual-level risk factors for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), which include trauma exposure and indices of vulnerability such as social and educational disadvantage, Reference Brewin, Andrews and Valentine1–Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss3 there has been little research into country-level predictors. Initial concerns that PTSD was a specifically Western formulation of response to trauma have been allayed by cross-national research indicating that although there is some cultural patterning of symptoms the condition occurs around the world. Reference Yehuda4 Despite this evidence for cross-cultural validity, however, there are relatively large unexplained variations in PTSD rates across countries, with lifetime prevalence in general populations ranging from zero to more than 6%. Reference Kessler and Ustun5 Our objective was to test whether national differences in lifetime PTSD prevalence can be explained by countries' rates of exposure to trauma and their vulnerability, both singly and in interaction. The interaction has been suggested, for instance, by Cutter: ‘Vulnerability is the likelihood that an individual or group will be exposed to and adversely affected by a hazard. It is the interaction of the hazards of place (risk and mitigation) with the social profile of communities’. Reference Cutter6 The specific vulnerability of nations to major disturbances such as disasters has recently been captured in a comprehensive combination of cultural and socioeconomic country features. Reference Welle, Birkmann, Rhyner, Witting and Wolfertz7 There is consistent evidence that within countries more disadvantaged groups have higher prevalence levels of PTSD in response to trauma exposure. Reference Bonanno, Brewin, Kaniasty and La Greca8,Reference Hobfoll, Palmieri, Robert, Canetti-Nisim, Hall and Galea9 These approaches suggest that more vulnerable countries should have higher prevalence rates, and that trauma exposure interacts with group vulnerability to increase PTSD prevalence.
Method
Country-level data on prevalence of lifetime PTSD and exposure to trauma
In order to ensure quality and standardise measurement of trauma and PTSD we selected studies using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). 10 The CIDI is a widely used structured diagnostic interview, validated cross-culturally and designed to be used by trained lay interviewers. 10,Reference Kessler and Ustun11 It was the main measure of the World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Trauma exposure is measured using detailed lists of events including, among others, combat or war experience, natural disaster, physical or sexual assault, physical abuse as a child, motor vehicle accident, unexpected death or life-threatening illness of a loved one and witnessing a potentially traumatic event. Reference Scott, Koenen, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Angermeyer and Benjet12,Reference Kessler, Rose, Koenen, Karam, Stang and Stein13
We searched Medline, EMBASE, PsycINFO and PILOTS for prevalence studies on lifetime PTSD and exposure to trauma, conducted in representative samples with a CIDI-based assessment of PTSD according to the DSM-IV criteria. The literature databases were searched in the second half of January 2015 using the following combination of search terms: (a) trauma-related (for example ‘PTSD’, ‘posttraumatic stress’, ‘post-traumatic stress’, ‘traumatic’, ‘trauma’) and (b) prevalence in all fields, together with (c) ‘lifetime’ and (d) ‘Clinical International Diagnostic Interview’ or ‘CIDI’ in title and abstract. We did not apply restrictions regarding language, publication type or date of publication. Reference lists were inspected to identify other potentially relevant studies. Studies focusing solely on 12-month PTSD prevalence, using older DSM versions or not using the CIDI, were excluded. Where we found more than one data-set for any country meeting the inclusion criteria we selected the most recent one.
Country vulnerability
In the annual World Risk Report, produced by Alliance Development Works, the UN University and the University of Bonn, a broad collection of data-sets are brought together and combined into a vulnerability index, reflecting a variety of social and economic country features. Reference Welle, Birkmann, Rhyner, Witting and Wolfertz7 In the 2013 report, the vulnerability of 173 countries was summarised using 23 indicators, divided into three components, and measured using worldwide and publicly accessible data. Reference Welle, Birkmann, Rhyner, Witting and Wolfertz7 Susceptibility describes a country's structural characteristics and framework conditions that can sustain harm. For example, indicators involve malnutrition, access to sanitation, income equality and gross domestic product per capita. Lack of coping capacity refers to the ability of a country to minimise negative impacts of events and includes indicators such as number of physicians and hospital beds per 1000 inhabitants and the Corruption Perceptions Index. Lack of adaptive capacities refers to conditions supporting long-term, structural change. Example indicators include the adult literacy rate, combined gross school enrolment, forest management and public and private health expenditure. More background information on the index, its composition and analysis can be found in the World Risk Report of 2013. Reference Welle, Birkmann, Rhyner, Witting and Wolfertz7
Analysis
We calculated correlation coefficients and tested four linear regression models with lifetime PTSD as dependent variable. Models with one predictor, exposure or vulnerability, were followed by a model with both predictors and a final model to test whether the relationship between exposure and PTSD was moderated by a country's level of vulnerability. We verified that associations were not affected by survey response rates. All analyses were performed in IBM SPSS Statistics, version 20.
Results
We found 24 studies meeting our inclusion criteria (86 687 respondents). Exposure to trauma could be determined for 16 countries (53 038 respondents). Information about each study is displayed in online Table DS1. All studies were published between 2005 and 2014, based on surveys administered between 2001 and 2007, with an average response rate of 70.9%. Most of them were conducted using CIDI version 3.0 (87.5%). The highest PTSD prevalence rates were found in Canada, The Netherlands and Australia, the lowest in Nigeria, China, and Romania (Fig. 1). Exposure to trauma was the highest in The Netherlands, Colombia and the USA and the lowest in Romania, Spain and Italy. Besides lifetime PTSD and exposure to trauma, online Table DS1 also contains the country vulnerability score. The most vulnerable countries were Nigeria, Iraq and Colombia and the least vulnerable were The Netherlands, Germany and Belgium.
Table 1 shows distributional and correlational information for the study variables. Lifetime PTSD in the various country samples was correlated positively with exposure to trauma and negatively with vulnerability. Exposure and vulnerability were not related.
Distributional information | Correlations | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Study, n | Mean | Median | IQR | Minimum-Maximum | % lifetime PTSD | % exposure to trauma | |
% lifetime PTSD | 24 | 3.21 | 2.30 | 3.18 | 0.00–9.20 | – | |
% exposed to trauma | 16 | 67.14 | 70.30 | 18.43 | 41.50–80.70 | 0.60* | – |
Vulnerability score | 24 | 39.34 | 36.63 | 15.50 | 28.39–68.99 | −0.49* | 0.05 |
IQR, interquartile range, PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.
* P <0.05.
The results of the regression analyses are detailed in Table 2 and Fig. 2. Exposure to trauma was a significant positive predictor for PTSD, accounting for approximately one-third of the variance (model 1, 16 countries). In contrast, country vulnerability was a significant negative predictor, explaining roughly a quarter of the variance (model 2, 24 countries). When both variables were included simultaneously in model 3 (16 countries), the effects of each remained significant and the explained variance increased to around 60%. Model 4 (16 countries) showed that, in addition to the main effects, the relation between trauma exposure and lifetime PTSD was significantly moderated by vulnerability such that high exposure was only associated with high PTSD prevalence when country vulnerability was low. About 75% of the variance in PTSD was explained in the final model.
Model 1 (exposure) |
Model 2 (vulnerability) |
Model 3 (exposure and vulnerability) |
Model 4 (exposure, vulnerability and interaction) |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
Coefficients (95% CI) | ||||
Intercept | −5.470 (−12.470 to 1.530) | 7.715** (4.075 to 11.354) | 0.669 (−6.487 to 7.825) | −39.068* (−68.610 to −9.526) |
Exposure | 0.136* (0.033 to 0.238) | – | 0.142** (0.059 to 0.225) | 0.701** (0.288 to 1.114) |
Vulnerability | – | −0.115* (−0.205 to −0.026) | −0.181* (−0.311 to −0.051) | 0.903* (0.106 to 1.701) |
Exposure × vulnerability | – | – | – | −0.015* (−0.026 to −0.004) |
Predictor importance, % | ||||
Exposure | 100 | – | 60 | 48 |
Vulnerability | – | 100 | 40 | 21 |
Exposure × vulnerability | – | – | – | 31 |
n | 16 | 24 | 16 | 16 |
R 2 (adjusted R 2), % | 36 (32) | 24 (21) | 62 (57) | 78 (73) |
*P<0.05, **P<0.01.
Survey response rates were not correlated with PTSD or exposure rates, but strongly correlated with vulnerability (r = 0.71, P<0.01); more vulnerable countries had higher response rates. Inclusion of response rates in the regression models did not affect the results.
The average trauma exposure and vulnerability rates of the 16 countries are 67.14 and 36.23, respectively. Based on these averages countries can be divided into four groups (Fig. 3). As the pattern found in model 4 indicates (Fig. 2), Australia, Canada, The Netherlands, New Zealand and the USA have a fairly high average lifetime PTSD of 7.34%. These five countries were characterised by higher rates of exposure and lower vulnerability levels.
The other 11 countries clustered together around a lower PTSD average, but their exposure and vulnerability profiles were not homogeneous. Belgium, Germany, Italy, Japan and Spain were characterised by lower exposure and vulnerability with a PTSD average of 1.96%. A third group of five countries had an equivalent PTSD average of 2.1% and were characterised by higher exposure and vulnerability: Colombia, Israel, Lebanon, Mexico and South Africa. With its fairly low exposure and high vulnerability profile Romania did not belong with any of the other countries.
In the scatterplots based on models 1 and 2 (Fig. 2), we see that exposure and vulnerability as single predictors under-estimated lifetime PTSD in ‘high exposure–low vulnerability’ populations. A prediction based on exposure alone overestimated lifetime PTSD in case of ‘high exposure–high vulnerability’. The prediction based on vulnerability appeared to overestimate lifetime PTSD in ‘low exposure–low vulnerability’ populations. The model with both predictors yielded a more accurate estimate, although Romania's predicted negative lifetime PTSD rate was unrealistic. The interaction term corrected this in the final model.
Discussion
The current study replicated the individual-level positive relation between exposure to trauma and PTSD prevalence at the country level but identified a ‘vulnerability paradox’: whereas higher vulnerability is associated with increased PTSD at individual Reference Brewin, Andrews and Valentine1 and group Reference Bonanno, Brewin, Kaniasty and La Greca8 levels, it shows the opposite association at a country level. The average lifetime PTSD in ‘low exposure–low vulnerability’ and ‘high exposure–high vulnerability’ populations are similar. Average PTSD prevalence in ‘high exposure–low vulnerability’ countries is more than three times as high.
Before we further explore the relevance of these findings it is important to emphasise the need for caution in interpreting the pattern found and to encourage replication. Beyond doubt, comparing population studies from different countries is methodologically challenging as language issues, and demographic and cultural properties can affect the validity and comparability of measurements that reflect an isolated moment in time. For example, the vulnerability effect could be biased by language: four of the five ‘high exposure–low vulnerability’ countries are English-speaking. Steel et al identified high lifetime rates of mental illness in English-speaking countries. Reference Steel, Marnane, Iranpour, Chey, Jackson and Patel14 However, we found other population studies from non-English-speaking countries that, although they did not meet our inclusion criteria, reveal PTSD prevalence and exposure rates conforming to the pattern identified in our analyses. Three studies (not using the CIDI) fall within the ‘high exposure–low vulnerability’ group and reported similarly high rates of PTSD: Portugal (PTSD 7.9%, exposure 75.7%, vulnerability 34.8%), Reference De Albuquercue, Soares, Martins De Jesus and Alves15 Sweden (PTSD 5.6%, exposure 80.8%, vulnerability 28.4%) Reference Frans, Rimmö, Åberg and Fredrikson16 and Denmark (adolescents: PTSD 7.7%, exposure 78%, vulnerability 28.5%). Reference Elklit and Frandsen17
Apart from the language issue we checked whether alternative available data-sets corroborated the findings. An earlier general population survey found that the lifetime PTSD prevalence in The Netherlands was 4.0, Reference De Graaf, Ormel, Ten Have, Burger, Buist-Bouwman, Kessler and Ustun18 with an exposure rate of 61.9. Reference Scott, Koenen, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Angermeyer and Benjet12 Both lifetime PTSD and exposure were lower than those reported by De Vries & Olff, Reference De Vries and Olff19 whose study was included in our analysis. However, when repeating the analysis using this earlier population sample, the effects remained unaltered and significant. We followed the same procedure with earlier survey data from the USA that pointed to a slightly different lifetime PTSD prevalence score of 7.8% in the general population (DSM-III). Reference Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes and Nelson20 Using the older percentage did not result in a different outcome either.
Additionally, we explored the presence of the paradox in the context of mood disorders (for example depression). Based on the WMH survey data Kessler and colleagues reported the lifetime prevalence of mood disorders in 17 countries. Reference Kessler, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Chatterji, Lee and Ormel21 We combined these prevalence rates with the country vulnerability scores from the 2013 World Risk Report. The negative correlation between mood disorder prevalence and vulnerability points to a similar paradox (r = −0.58, P<0.05, n = 17). The correlation is even higher than the correlation between PTSD and vulnerability (Table 1). As a next step we tested the group averages based on exposure and vulnerability scores. The average mood disorder prevalence in ‘high exposure–low vulnerability’ countries (The Netherlands, New Zealand and USA, 19.9%) is higher than in the ‘high exposure–high vulnerability’ (Colombia, Israel, Lebanon, Mexico and South Africa, 11.38%) and ‘low exposure–low vulnerability’ (Belgium, Germany, Italy, Japan and Spain, 10.42%) groups (ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test: P<0.01, n = 13).
All in all, our findings cannot readily be accounted for by explanations applied to observed differences in the cross-national prevalence of mental disorders, such as limitations in Western-based diagnostic assumptions or differences in the age structure of populations (N.B. life expectancy in more vulnerable countries is lower). Reference Steel, Marnane, Iranpour, Chey, Jackson and Patel14 The possible role of cultural and psychological factors should be taken seriously. Burri & Maercker, for instance, succeeded in explaining substantial levels of variance in cross-national PTSD in 12 European countries (not using the CIDI) after including cultural value orientations. Reference Burri and Maercker22 Moreover, based on an analysis of data from 60 countries, it has recently been suggested that over two-thirds of the variance in national vulnerability can be explained by cultural factors: less vulnerable countries are inhabited by more individualistic cultures with a more equal power balance, less uncertainty avoidance, a more long-term orientation, higher indulgence and less restraint. Reference Dückers, Frerks and Birkmann23 Although these factors may have an impact on responses to diagnostic instruments such as the CIDI, it is conceivable that such cultures may be lacking aspects of social capital such as community engagement and support that could help victims of trauma repair their resources and rebuild their lives. Reference Durant24 This contrasts with the expectation that less vulnerable countries should be better equipped to anticipate a higher burden of disease, and should be more favourably placed to overcome barriers on the path to equitable care. Reference Dückers, Witteveen, Bisson and Olff25
Another explanation for a higher conditional PTSD prevalence in low vulnerability countries might be that the relative impact of a traumatic event on long-term goals is greater, because there is more expectation of achieving such goals. This account is consistent with classic research linking greater status striving and aspiration–achievement discrepancies to mental illness. Reference Kleiner and Parker26 Our findings may also be related to theories that PTSD represents an overturning of basic assumptions about self-worth, and about the meaningfulness, predictability and benevolence of the world. Reference Janoff-Bulman27 Countries high in vulnerability may foster conditions that minimise comforting illusions and reduce the contradictions brought about when cherished assumptions are invalidated by traumatic events.
We believe that more detailed investigation of the vulnerability paradox and its possible theoretical interpretations may not only throw light on the nature of PTSD but also proffer important clues about the nature of resilience to trauma that could be harnessed for general benefit. Specifically, it raises the possibility that vulnerability as measured through deprivation may be an index of greater resilience rather than lesser resilience as is commonly assumed.
Strengths and weaknesses
It is one of the strengths of this study that researchers everywhere in the world can access the national surveys as well as the vulnerability data. We applied a stepwise approach to make the changes in predicted PTSD in each successive step transparent. Since the main predictors – exposure and vulnerability – are unrelated, confounding is unlikely. We also demonstrated that the results were not specific to a single set of surveys or to the PTSD diagnosis alone, and ruled out various other potential confounding factors such as English v. non-English speaking status and differential response rates. The analysis was nevertheless based on only 24 countries with information on lifetime PTSD and a subsample of 16 countries with available exposure data. This enjoins caution in drawing inferences from the data, especially because the average vulnerability of the 16 countries (36.23) is lower than the worldwide average of the 173 countries in the 2013 World Risk Report (48.56, range 27.30–75.41). The limited sample makes an extensive assessment of particular vulnerability characteristics or other country features problematic.
Another issue to keep in mind in interpreting the findings is that one CIDI-based measure of overall trauma exposure was used. This can be seen as a strength, yet in reality countries vary in their types of exposure, and exposure types vary substantially in the likelihood that they will lead to PTSD. Therefore, two countries that have the same level of exposure but different types of exposure underlying the total percentage exposed measure might have a different risk of PTSD. With only five publications included in our study containing information on different types of exposure to trauma rigorous comparisons were not possible.
When it comes to the PTSD and exposure data, several advantages of the CIDI have been addressed. At the same time, the absence of PTSD in Nigeria is puzzling and it is worthwhile considering the explanations for a possible underrepresentation provided by Gureje et al. Reference Gureje, Lasebikan, Kola and Makanjuola28 One of their explanations is that in a setting where mental illness is still highly stigmatised, symptoms of such illness might be embarrassing and so more likely to be denied. A second explanation could be that respondents might not feel comfortable disclosing their symptoms to a lay interviewer and thus keep important information to themselves. Reference Gureje, Lasebikan, Kola and Makanjuola28 Stigma and reservations might, and this could apply to any and all the countries, form a stronger explanation in more vulnerable countries that are, again, more collectivistic and with a less equal power balance. Reference Dückers, Frerks and Birkmann23 It would be interesting to combine country vulnerability with population data on stigma in relation to mental health problems and to explore this association (for example, the Stigma in Global Context – Mental Health Study; see Pescosolido et al Reference Pescosolido, Medina, Martin and Long29 ).
Although the data-set is based on thousands of respondents, we were limited to using aggregated individual scores at country level, and could not work with the original data-sets. Unfortunately, we were unable to examine individual-, group- and country-level characteristics simultaneously with more advanced analysis techniques incorporating a multilevel approach, while taking factors such as gender, age, socioeconomic or marital status into account. It could be informative to bring the original data-sets together and further assess the variance in PTSD at different levels in relation to other health issues, types of exposure and risk and protective factors.
Finally, some limitations of the world vulnerability index must be mentioned. An array of data-sets from different sources are used to bring together social and economic dimensions and natural hazard analysis in the vulnerability index. The data-sets used are not designed for this purpose; they are incorporated simply because they are available. Reference Heesen, Lorenz, Nagenborg, Wenzel and Voss30 That said, indicators have been assigned to three constructs with a good reliability coefficient and the index has been thoroughly tested. 31 Although the index is a helpful source to understand disaster risk internationally, the statistical work on it is still in progress and there is scope for a follow-up analysis covering more relevant data. The correlation between the vulnerability scores in the 2012 Reference Welle, Birkmann, Rhyner, Witting and Wolfertz32 and the 2013 report is almost perfect (r = 0.998, n = 173), suggesting that the vulnerability index is stable. This might be important given the gap between the years of data collection for the trauma prevalence rates and the later created vulnerability construct. Changes over time in country vulnerability might affect its relationship to the prevalence of lifetime PTSD.
In conclusion, we tested multiple models to explain the prevalence of lifetime PTSD in different countries. The analysis suggests that the effect of exposure on PTSD is moderated by the level of vulnerability. Paradoxically, in the context of high trauma exposure, the populations of less vulnerable countries with more resources and better healthcare have higher chances of developing PTSD in their lifetime.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.