Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T12:18:46.146Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Validation of the Domain-Specific Risk-Taking Scale in Chinese college students

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Xiaoxiao Hu*
Affiliation:
George Mason University
*
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Using college student samples, two studies were conducted to validate the Chinese version of the Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) Scale. The results replicated important findings reported by Weber et al. (2002) in the Chinese culture. Risk-taking and risk perception were domain-specific, whereas perceived-risk attitudes were relatively stable across domains, supporting the risk-return model of risk taking. Results of both exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) showed that the ethical, recreational, health/safety, and gambling domains were preserved in the Chinese version of DOSPERT and that the items from social and investment domains formed one factor. This result may be explained by Weber and Hsee's (1998) cushion hypothesis. Other possible reasons for this cross-cultural difference in the factor structure were also discussed.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2012] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

Significant past research has been conducted to understand people’s differences in their risk-taking behaviors, which are often referred as differences in risk attitude. According to the expected utility theory (Reference Von Neumann. and Morgenstern.Von Neumann, & Morgenstern, 1947) and prospect theory (Reference Kahneman. and Tversky.Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Reference Tversky. and Kahneman.Tversky & Kahneman, 1992), risk attitude is explained by the shape of a utility function. In this model, risk attitude—an individual’s location on the continuum from risk aversion to risk seeking—is assumed to be a personality trait (Reference Howell. and Mulligan.Weber, Blais, & Betz, 2002).

Based on this single trait view of risk attitude, researchers have employed various methods to measure it. Some studies require participants to choose between lotteries and then compare participants’ observed choices to choices predicted by the expected value (e.g., Reference Holt. and Laury.Holt & Laury, 2002; Mellers, Schwartz, & Weber, 1997). Using the probability equivalence method, Kogan and Wallach (1964) developed the Choice Dilemma Questionnaire, asking respondents for probability equivalents in twelve choice dilemmas, which are combined into a single score that represents risk attitude. Nonetheless, according to Reference Bentler. and Bonnett.Blais and Weber (2006), these studies were questionable due to two major problems. Firstly, people may be classified as risk seeking or risk averse depending on the method employed to measure risk attitude (Reference Slovic.Slovic, 1964). Secondly, even the same method may categorize an individual into different groups across different situations and domains (Reference Schoemaker.Schoemaker, 1990; Reference MacCrimmon. and Wehrung.MacCrimmon & Wehrung, 1990). These problems suggest that those measurements do not possess a satisfactory predictive validity across a range of situations (Reference Bromiley., Curley. and J. F..Bromiley & Curley, 1992).

Given this obvious deficiency of many risk attitude measurements, recently some researchers have claimed that risk attitude should be conceptualized in the risk-return framework of risky choice and treated in a domain-specific manner (Reference Bell.Bell, 1995; Reference Sarin. and Weber.Sarin & Weber, 1993). Risk-return models assume risk taking to reflect a tradeoff between perceived risk (fear) and expected return (hope) (Reference Weber., N. J. and P. B.Weber, 2001):

Risk Taking Preference (X) = a (Expected Benefit (X)) + b (Perceived Risk (X)) + c

where a represents the marginal effect of expected benefit of X on the risk taking preference, b represents the marginal effect of perceived risk of X on the risk taking preference, and c is the error term.

Researchers have found systematic situational and group differences in perceived risk and expected benefit of risky behaviors (Reference Bontempo., Bottom. and Weber.Bontempo, Bottom, & Weber, 1997; Reference Weber.Weber, 1988; Reference Johnson., Wilke. and Weber.Johnson, Wilke, & Weber, 2004). Nevertheless, people’s perceived-risk attitude (coefficient b)—remains relatively stable across situations and groups (Reference Weber.Weber, 1998, 2001). Therefore, differences in perceived risk and expected return of risky choices seem to play a major part in the observed domain-specificity of risk attitude (Reference Weber., N. J. and P. B.Weber, 2001).

In this risk return framework, Reference Weber., Blais. and Betz.Weber et al. (2002) developed the Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) Scale in order to assess both conventional risk attitudes (apparent risk taking; i.e., risk taking preference in the formula above) and perceived-risk attitudes in six domains, namely, social, recreational, gambling, investment, health/safety, and ethical decisions. The conventional risk attitudes is defined as people’s stated level of risk taking, and perceived-risk attitude is defined as the willingness to engage in a risky behavior as a function of its perceived risk (Reference Howell. and Mulligan.Weber et al., 2002). Therefore, the DOSPERT Scale measures both individuals’ propensity to engage in risk activities and their perceived risk and expected return of these activities, making it possible to study the relationship between apparent risk taking and risk perception (and thus perceived-risk attitude). Using this scale, Reference Weber., Blais. and Betz.Weber et al. (2002) demonstrated that participants’ degree of risk taking differed across domains and these differences were primarily associated with differences in perceived risk and expected return of risky behaviors rather than differences in perceived-risk attitude. These results provided strong support to the risk-return model of risky choice.

Researchers have provided strong evidence for the DOSPERT Scale’s reliability and validity (e.g., Reference Weber., Blais. and Betz.Weber et al., 2002; Reference Johnson., Wilke. and Weber.Johnson et al., 2004). Reference Weber., Blais. and Betz.Weber et al. (2002) reported its initial psychometric properties, including moderate reliability estimates, satisfactory construct validity and convergent/discriminant validity with respect to constructs such as sensation seeking, intolerance for ambiguity, and social desirability.

Geographically, the DOSPERT has been validated in many other cultures such as German, Dutch, Italian, and Spanish (e.g., Reference Johnson., Wilke. and Weber.Johnson et al., 2004). Nonetheless, to date, the scale has not been tested in eastern cultures. As mentioned above, the items and the six content domains in the DOSPERT were originally developed and validated in the U.S. Since substantial cultural differences exist between western societies and eastern societies, there is a need to examine its psychometric properties in an eastern culture.

Therefore, in order to further examine the cross-cultural validity of the DOSPERT, the current paper validated the scale in Chinese culture using university students in China. In this process, we looked for similarities and differences in risk taking as a function of culture and then discuss possible explanations for observed differences.

Specifically, we conducted two studies to address these issues. For simplicity sake, we will refer to our translated and revised version of the scale as DOSPERT-C. In Study 1, we attempted to identify the factor structure of the DOSPERT-C using an exploratory factor-analytic procedure. Moreover, we examined the reliability of the DOSPERT-C. We also performed a regression to examine the relationship between risk perception and risk behavior. As mentioned above, the risk-return framework assumes the involvement of various determinants, namely, perceived risk, expected benefit, and perceived-risk attitude. However, because of logistical constraints, expected benefit could not be collected in the present study. Thus, this study’s emphasis is on conventional risk attitudes, the perception of risk, and perceived-risk attitude. In Study 2, in order to provide further empirical support for the factor structure of the DOSPERT-C, we conducted confimatory factor analysis. In addition, we provided evidence for convergent validity for the DOSPERT-C.

2 Study 1

2.1 Method

2.1.1 Participants and procedure

Participants were 216 (125 females, 57.9%) students at Peking University, China. The average age of the sample was 20.96 years (SD = 0.66, range = 18 to 24). Each participant completed the scales and provided demographic information.

2.1.2 Measures

The domain-specific risk-taking (DOSPERT) scale of Weber et al. (2002) consisted of 40 items. Eight items of the scale represent risky behaviors from each of five content domains: financial, health/safety, recreational, ethics, and social. Those eight financial items further split into two groups with four items each, related to investing and gambling, which were identified as independent and separate risk-raking domains by Reference Weber., Blais. and Betz.Weber et al. (2002). The risk-taking scale of the DOSPERT assesses participants’ likelihood of engaging in these risk actions on a five-point scale, ranging from “Very unlikely” to “Very likely”. The risk perception scale asks participants to rate their perception of risk entailed by each risky behavior on a five-point scale, ranging from “Not at all risky” to “Extremely risky”. The DOSPERT-C was developed using back-translation methodology. As in the English version of the scale, items were not presented by domains but were randomly interspersed. Items appeared in different orders for the two scales. The presentation order of the two scales was counterbalanced across participants. Worth noting here is that no order effect was found in the current study.

2.2 Results and discussion

2.2.1 Item discrimination

In the assessment of item discrimination, the Discrimination Index(D) is computed by subtracting the mean score of participants in the lower group (27%) from the mean value of those in the upper group (27%) and dividing it by the maximum possible discrimination. A value of 0.19 or below indicates that the item is subject to improvement (Reference Hopkins., Kenneth., Julian. and Hopkins.Hopkins et al., 1990). Items with D < 0.19 include: item 4 (buying an illegal drug for your own use), item 8 (consuming five or more servings of alcohol in a single evening), item 20 (illegally copying a piece of software), item 25 (shoplifting a small item (e.g. a lipstick or a pen), and item 27 (engaging in unprotected sex). These five items are also those ones participants regarded as problematic according to their feedback and thus may not be applicable to Chinese university students. For instance, university students in China, especially in large cities, have very little chance to encounter illegal drugs which are very strictly banned by the government. For another example, software piracy is not well prevented in China and illegally copies of software can be always seen around university students. Due to the apparent limitations of these five items, they were eliminated for the DOSPERT-C; this reduced the number of items from 40 to 35.

2.2.2 Factor analysis

A principal components analysis (PCA) using varimax rotation was conducted on the 35 items of the risk-taking scale. 10 factors were identified based on the eigenvalues ≥ 1.0 criterion and the scree plot identified either four or five factors that could be extracted. Each of the factor solutions from 4 to 6 factor solutions were examined for simple structure and interpretability. Among the factor solutions, the five-factor solution was most interpretable. As shown in Table 1, investment and social items loaded on factor 1. The remaining four factors corresponded well to the gambling, recreational, ethical, health/safety risk, respectively. Thus, the factor structure of the DOSPERT-C is similar to that of the DOSPERT except that the investment and social items of the original DOSPERT were combined in the DOSPERT-C.

Table 1 Factor loadings of the 35 items of Risk-taking scale. Loadings greater than or equal to 0.30 are shown in bold.

The same PCA procedure was performed on the 35 items of risk-perception scale and the factor structure of this scale is similar to the risk-taking scale reported above. The factor loading of each item is shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Factor loadings of the 35 items of the Risk-perception Scale. Loadings greater than or equal to 0.30 are shown in bold.

Since investment and social items loaded on one factor, the DOSPERT-C corresponded well to a five-factor model. Henceforth we will refer to the new combined domain as social-investment and will examine the characteristics of this domain together with other the four domains below.

2.2.3 Reliability

Table 3 shows coefficient alphas and average item-total correlations for the risk-taking and risk-perception scales, separately for each of the five subscales. Although internal consistencies were acceptable with Cronbach alphas for the subscales ranging from 0.63 to 0.80, with the majority clustering around 0.75, they were somewhat lower than those reported by Reference Weber., Blais. and Betz.Weber et al. (2002). For both scales, the gambling subscale was most reliable according to Cronbach’s alphas and the health/safety subscale was least reliable. For item-total correlations, gambling subscale had the highest mean values for both scales while social-investment subscale had the lowest values.

Table 3 Cronbach’s alphas and mean item-subscale-total correlation (and ranges of correlations) for Risk-taking and Risk-perception subscales

2.3 Subscale correlations

Correlations among the five subscales of both scales are reported in Table 4 and Table 5. Consistent with Reference Weber., Blais. and Betz.Weber et al. (2002), the generally moderate correlations, coupled with the EFA results, indicate that both risk behavior and risk perception are domain-specific.

Table 4 Pearson correlations among subscales and with total score for Risk-taking scale

* p < .05

** p < .01.

Table 5 Pearson correlations among subscales and with total score for Risk-perception scale

* p < .05

** p < .01.

2.3.1 Relationship between risk perception and risk behavior

In order to test the relationship between risk perception and apparent risk taking reported by Weber et al. (2002), we regressed risk behavior on risk perception across respondents. As presented in Table 6, the largest proportion of explained variance is 0.41 in the recreational domain and the smallest one is 0.16 in social-investment domain. The perceived risk coefficient shows the impact of perceived risk on apparent risk behavior.

3 Study 2

Study 2 was designed to confirm the factor structure found in study 1 using another sample and to examine the convergent validity of our instruments. In addition to the risk-taking scale revised in Study 1, participants also completed the Chinese version of Zuckerman’s sensation seeking scales (Reference Wang., Wu., Peng., Lu., Yu. and Wang.Wang et al., 2000) and the Chinese version of Budner’s scale for intolerance of ambiguity (Zhu, 2006). As explained by Reference Weber., Blais. and Betz.Weber et al. (2002), these scales measure attributes of people’s response to risk that may well surpass domain-specific aspects of their behavior. Therefore, we predict significant correlations between these scales and most subscales of risk-taking scale.

3.1 Method

Participants in study 2 were 383 (200 females, 52.2%) students at Peking University. The average age of the sample was 21.13 years (SD = 0.78, range = 18–25). They filled out the 35-item risk-taking scale revised in Study 1. Items for this scale were presented in a random order. Participants also provided responses to the validation scales described above as well as demographic information.

3.2 Results and discussion

3.2.1 Factor analysis

In order to confirm the factor structure found in study 1, we performed confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) on the 35 items of the risk-taking scale. In the CFA, the scale of the latent factor was set by fixing the variance of the latent factor equal to 1.

As presented in Table 8, a number of indexes were used to determine the goodness of fit. The comparative fit index (CFI) may range from 0 to 1 and values equal to or greater than 0.90 indicate a good fit to the data (Reference Bentler. and Bonnett.Bentler & Bonett, 1980; Reference Kline.Kline, 1998). Similarly, scores 0.90 or above are desired with the non-normed fit index (NNFI) and the incremental fit index (IFI). Finally, a value of 0.08 or less for the root mean square error approximation (RMSEA) reflects a model with an adequate fit to the data, while values greater than 0.10 suggest strongly that the model fit is unsatisfactory (Reference Browne. and Cudeck.Browne & Cudeck, 1989, 1993).

The model in the study fulfilled all these criteria except the NNFI, which was very close to the criterion (0.90). Therefore, the fit statistics for this model generally indicated a good fit to the data and supported the factor structure found in study 1.

3.2.2 Validity

The correlations between risk-taking subscales and sensation seeking/intolerance of ambiguity are presented in Table 7. As predicted, sensation seeking correlated significantly with the risk-taking subscales in all five domains. In particular, physical risks (i.e., recreational and health/safety) have the highest correlations with sensation seeking, which makes conceptual sense. Intolerance of ambiguity correlated significantly with all risk-taking subscales except health/safety subscale.

Table 6 Coefficients and R 2 of regression of Risk-taking scale mean on Risk-perception scale mean by domain

Notes.

** p < .01

* p < .05

3.2.3 Gender differences

As shown in Table 9, male and female respondents differed significantly in ethical and health/safety domains. Men were more likely to engage in risky behaviors than were women in ethical and health/safety domains as well as total scores.

4 General discussion

Our results replicate many important findings reported by Reference Weber., Blais. and Betz.Weber et al. (2002) in Chinese culture. Both apparent risk taking and perceived risk differed across domains. As shown in Table 4 and Table 5, risk behaviors and risk perceptions in one content domain had small relationships with risk behaviors and risk perceptions in another domain, documenting the appropriateness of using domain-specific scales. Those differences in apparent risk taking seems to be associated, to a great extent, with differences of the perceived risk (Table 6), rather than perceived risk attitude (the coefficient in the risk-return regression), which did not vary greatly across domains.

The paper contributed a Chinese version of the original scale. The DOSPERT-C exhibits acceptable psychometric properties and it proves to be a useful instrument for Chinese university students. Still, there is room for improvement. First of all, based on the EFA, a few items had multi-loadings or did not load on the expected factor. Secondly, Cronbach’s alphas for the health/safety domain were below 0.70, indicating that items in this domain may need further improvement. Thirdly, after deleting five items from the original scale and combining the items from social and investment domains, the number of items in each domain in the DOSPERT-C differs from others. Therefore, we might consider add new items to the DOSPERT-C in the future to order to increase its reliability and validity. In any event, this paper provided a useful scale that could help those scholars to assess conventional risk attitude as well as perceived-risk attitude in five domains in Chinese populations.

A very interesting finding of current studies is the difference in the factor structure between the DOSPERT and the DOSPERT-C. The main difference in the factor structure is that social items and investment items loaded on a single factor in the DOSPERT-C. We offered several possible reasons to explain what may lead to this difference.

Table 7 Pearson correlations between Risk-taking subscales and Sensation Seeking/Intolerance of Ambiguity

Notes.

** p < .01

* p < .05.

Table 8 Fit Indices for the factor structure found in Study 1.

Table 9 Means (and standard deviations) of risk-taking ratings by gender.

It is possible that university students in China have less chance to gamble than university students in America. There are few horse races in Beijing and betting large sums of money on sports events is illegal. Therefore, gambling may be relatively distant to the daily lives of Chinese students in comparison with investment. In addition, gambling is often considered as an inappropriate behavior for university students in China but may not be in America. This can also be the reason why gambling items and investment items are separate in the DOSPERT-C.

It is also possible that students in China regard investment risk and social risk as more closely connected than American students. This explanation is consistent with Reference Weber. and Hsee.Weber and Hsee’s (1998) cushion hypothesis. In this hypothesis, they claimed that in collectivistic cultures like China, in-group members would help out any group member who faced a great financial loss after a risky choice. In contrast, people are expected to personally bear more of the consequences of their risky decisions in individualist cultures like America (Reference Weber. and Hsee.Weber & Hsee, 1998). Therefore, collectivism acts as a cushion against possible losses for the members of a collective culture. If people in China are more likely to receive substantive financial help from others when they are in need, lost in social connections may mean lost in monetary help when needed. In this case, investment risk and social risk should be more closely related with each other for Chinese people. It will be interesting to design further studies to identify which one of these explanations is the main reason for the differences found in factor structure or to find other reasons that lead to the differences.

Recent research on risk-taking has identified interesting cross-cultural differences between China and the U.S. For instance, Reference Gong., Krantz. and Weber.Gong, Krantz, and Weber (2012) showed that, compared with American people, Chinese people were generally more concerned with the uncertainty and immediacy of future gains. Future research connecting this result with the current finding would be of great value. For example, an interesting research questions may be: do social connections reduce Chinese people’s insecurity of future gains?

Male respondents to the DOSPERT were more risk-taking in all domains except the social domain than female participants. In contrast, gender differences were found only in ethical and health/safety domains when using the DOSPERT-C. Given the item adjustment in the DOSPERT-C, we could not offer very solid explanation in terms of why such differences exist. However, China’s rising gender equality over the past a few years (Reference Howell. and Mulligan.Howell & Mulligan, 2005) may be an underlying reason for the fewer gender differences found in China. More work should be done to examine the gender differences in different domains of risk-taking using Chinese participants.

Finally, the test-retest reliability is an important index of a measure’s stability over time. That we did not collect data to assess such is a limitation for our studies and we encourage additional work to test the test-retest reliability for this scale in the future.

Footnotes

Project “Validation of the Domain-Specific Risk-Taking Scale in Chinese College Students” was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (Fund number: 71172024). The scale will be available at http://www.sjdm.org/dmidi/.

References

Bell., D. E. (1995). Risk, return, and utility. Management Science, 41, 2330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler., P. M., & Bonnett., D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blais., A., & Weber., E. U. (2006). A domain-specific risk-taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgment and Decision Making, 1, 3347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bontempo., R. N., Bottom., W. P., & Weber., E. U. (1997). Crosscultural differences in risk perception: A model-based approach. Risk Analysis, 17, 479488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bromiley., P., & Curley., S. (1992). Individual differences in risk taking. In. J. F.., Yates (Ed.), Risktaking behavior (pp. 5690). New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Browne., M. W. & Cudeck., R. (1989). Single sample cross validation indices for covariance structures. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 24, 445455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browne., M. W., & Cudeck., R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A., Bollen & J. S., Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 445455). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Gong., M., Krantz., D., & Weber., E. (2012). Explanations for Differences in Temporal Discounting of Future Financial and Environmental Gains but not Losses Between Chinese and Americans. Working paper, CRED, Columbia University.Google Scholar
Hopkins., , Kenneth., D., Julian., C. S., & Hopkins., B. R. (1990). Educational and Psychological Measurement and Evaluation. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Holt., C., & Laury., S. (2002). Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects. American Economic Review, 92, 16441655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell., J. & Mulligan., D. (2005). Gender and civil society. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kahneman., D., & Tversky., A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline., R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. Guilford, New York.Google Scholar
Johnson., J. G., Wilke., A., & Weber., E. U. (2004). Beyond a trait view of risk-taking: A domainspecific scale measuring risk perceptions, expected benefits, and perceived-risk attitude in Germanspeaking populations. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 35, 153172.Google Scholar
MacCrimmon., K. R., & Wehrung., D. A. (1990). Characteristics of risk taking executives. Management Science, 36, 422435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarin., R. K., & Weber., M. (1993). Risk-value models. European Journal of Operations Research, 70, 135149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic., P. (1964). Assessment of risk taking behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 61, 330333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoemaker., P. J. H. (1990). Are risk preferences related across payoff domains and response modes? Management Science, 36, 14511463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky., A., & Kahneman., D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5, 297323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Von Neumann., J., & Morgenstern., O. (1947). Theory of games and economic behavior, 2nd ed. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wang., W., Wu., Y., Peng., Z., Lu., S., Yu., L., Wang., G., et al. (2000). Test of sensation seeking in a Chinese sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 169179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber., E. U. (2001). Personality and risk taking. In N. J., Smelser & P. B., Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 1127411276). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber., E. U. (1988). A descriptive measure of risk. Acta Psychologica, 69, 185203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber., E. U., Blais., A.R., & Betz., E. (2002). A Domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15, 263290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber., E. U., & Hsee., C. K. (1998). Cross-cultural differences in risk perception but cross-cultural similarities in attitudes towards risk. Management Science, 44, 12051217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Factor loadings of the 35 items of Risk-taking scale. Loadings greater than or equal to 0.30 are shown in bold.

Figure 1

Table 2 Factor loadings of the 35 items of the Risk-perception Scale. Loadings greater than or equal to 0.30 are shown in bold.

Figure 2

Table 3 Cronbach’s alphas and mean item-subscale-total correlation (and ranges of correlations) for Risk-taking and Risk-perception subscales

Figure 3

Table 4 Pearson correlations among subscales and with total score for Risk-taking scale

Figure 4

Table 5 Pearson correlations among subscales and with total score for Risk-perception scale

Figure 5

Table 6 Coefficients and R2 of regression of Risk-taking scale mean on Risk-perception scale mean by domain

Figure 6

Table 7 Pearson correlations between Risk-taking subscales and Sensation Seeking/Intolerance of Ambiguity

Figure 7

Table 8 Fit Indices for the factor structure found in Study 1.

Figure 8

Table 9 Means (and standard deviations) of risk-taking ratings by gender.