Introduction
Physical activity (PA) and cognitive performance (CP) are strongly linked and have been shown to decline with ageing (Cheval et al., Reference Cheval, Sieber, Guessous, Orsholits, Courvoisier, Kliegel, Stringhini, Swinnen, Burton-Jeangros, Cullati and Boisgontier2018b, Reference Cheval, Orsholits, Sieber, Courvoisier, Cullati and Boisgontier2020c; Sebastiani et al., Reference Sebastiani, Andersen, Sweigart, Du, Cosentino, Thyagarajan, Christensen, Schupf and Perls2020). Multiple observational and interventional studies have demonstrated that a higher level of PA is associated with better CP in old age (Colcombe and Kramer, Reference Colcombe and Kramer2003; Angevaren et al., Reference Angevaren, Aufdemkampe, Verhaar, Aleman and Vanhees2008; Hamer et al., Reference Hamer, Terrera and Demakakos2018; Cheval et al., Reference Cheval, Darrous, Choi, Klimentidis, Raichlen, Alexander, Cullati, Kutalik and Boisgontier2020b) However, most of these studies focused on the association between PA levels and CP. Yet, disregarding the intra-individual evolution of PA over time may have biased the observed association with CP (Mok et al., Reference Mok, Khaw, Luben, Wareham and Brage2019). Because PA is a complex behaviour that evolves over time (Mok et al., Reference Mok, Khaw, Luben, Wareham and Brage2019), examining the associations between life-course PA trajectories and CP is warranted.
Recent studies have attempted to address this gap by considering the evolution of PA over time (Wannamethee et al., Reference Wannamethee, Shaper and Walker1998; Gregg et al., Reference Gregg, Cauley, Stone, Thompson, Bauer, Cummings and Ensrud2003; Stessman et al., Reference Stessman, Hammerman-Rozenberg, Cohen, Ein-Mor and Jacobs2009; Almeida et al., Reference Almeida, Khan, Hankey, Yeap, Golledge and Flicker2014; Jefferis et al., Reference Jefferis, Whincup, Lennon, Papacosta and Goya Wannamethee2014; Elhakeem et al., Reference Elhakeem, Murray, Cooper, Kuh, Whincup and Hardy2018; Laddu et al., Reference Laddu, Parimi, Cauley, Cawthon, Ensrud, Orwoll, Stefanick and Langsetmo2018; Saint-Maurice et al., Reference Saint-Maurice, Coughlan, Kelly, Keadle, Cook, Carlson, Fulton and Matthews2019; Aggio et al., Reference Aggio, Papachristou, Papacosta, Lennon, Ash, Whincup, Wannamethee and Jefferis2020; Sanchez-Sanchez et al., Reference Sanchez-Sanchez, Izquierdo, Carnicero-Carreño, García-García and Rodríguez-Mañas2020). However, most of those studies were based on conventional methods to identify groups with different PA trajectories, which typically use clinical or empirical cut-points (Wannamethee et al., Reference Wannamethee, Shaper and Walker1998; Gregg et al., Reference Gregg, Cauley, Stone, Thompson, Bauer, Cummings and Ensrud2003; Stessman et al., Reference Stessman, Hammerman-Rozenberg, Cohen, Ein-Mor and Jacobs2009; Almeida et al., Reference Almeida, Khan, Hankey, Yeap, Golledge and Flicker2014; Jefferis et al., Reference Jefferis, Whincup, Lennon, Papacosta and Goya Wannamethee2014; Elhakeem et al., Reference Elhakeem, Murray, Cooper, Kuh, Whincup and Hardy2018). To reduce the reliance on these cut-points, data-driven approaches such as growth mixture modelling have been proposed (Nagin and Odgers, Reference Nagin and Odgers2010). These models do not require a priori trajectory classifications as the trajectories emerge from the data (Aggio et al., Reference Aggio, Papachristou, Papacosta, Lennon, Ash, Whincup, Wannamethee and Jefferis2020; Formánek et al., Reference Formánek, Csajbók, Wolfová, Kučera, Tom, Aarsland and Cermakova2020). Using this approach (Barnett et al., Reference Barnett, Gauvin, Craig and Katzmarzyk2008; Laddu et al., Reference Laddu, Cawthon, Parimi, Hoffman, Orwoll, Miljkovic and Stefanick2017), studies have shown PA trajectories in older adults were associated with multiple health outcomes including disability, cardiovascular diseases and all-cause mortality (Laddu et al., Reference Laddu, Parimi, Cauley, Cawthon, Ensrud, Orwoll, Stefanick and Langsetmo2018, Reference Laddu, Parimi, Stone, Lapidus, Hoffman, Stefanick and Langsetmo2020; Saint-Maurice et al., Reference Saint-Maurice, Coughlan, Kelly, Keadle, Cook, Carlson, Fulton and Matthews2019; Aggio et al., Reference Aggio, Papachristou, Papacosta, Lennon, Ash, Whincup, Wannamethee and Jefferis2020; Sanchez-Sanchez et al., Reference Sanchez-Sanchez, Izquierdo, Carnicero-Carreño, García-García and Rodríguez-Mañas2020). However, the associations of PA trajectories with CP and is unknown.
In this study, we aimed to investigate the associations of PA trajectories with CP level and its decline in adults 50 years of age or older. We hypothesised that unfavourable profiles (e.g., individuals with decreasing PA over time) would be associated with lower levels and steeper decline of CP compared to more favourable profiles (e.g., individuals maintaining high PA levels).
Methods
Design and population
Data were drawn from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE), an ongoing population-based study of health, social network and economic conditions of community-dwelling middle-aged and older individuals, living in 28 European countries and Israel (N = 139 556). SHARE was described in detail elsewhere (Börsch-Supan et al., Reference Börsch-Supan, Brandt, Hunkler, Kneip, Korbmacher, Malter, Schaan, Stuck and Zuber2013). Briefly, participants were sampled based on probability selection methods. Individuals eligible for the study were people aged 50 years or older and their partners, irrespective of age. Data were collected using computer-assisted personal interviewing (CAPI) in participants' homes. The study was initiated in 2004 and followed by six subsequent waves with approximately 2-year intervals and wave 7 being completed in 2017. Wave 3 was devoted to data collection related to childhood histories (SHARELIFE). This wave did not contain any data related to PA and CP and was therefore not used in the current study. This study was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. SHARE has been approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Mannheim (waves 1–4) and the Ethics Council of the Max Plank Society (waves 4–7). All participants provided written informed consent. Data were pseudo-anonymised and participants were informed about the storage and use of the data and their right to withdraw consent.
In the present study, we included a total of 38 729 participants who fulfilled the following criteria: (1) age = 50 years or older at baseline, (2) measures of PA in at least three waves, (3) measures of cognition in at least two waves and (4) no report of diagnosed dementia (Fig. 1).
Measures
Physical activity
PA was assessed as part of CAPI in waves 1, 2, 4, 5, 6 and 7 using two questions. ‘How often do you engage in vigorous physical activity, such as sports, heavy housework, or a job that involves physical labour?’ and ‘How often do you engage in activities that require a low or moderate level of energy such as gardening, cleaning the car, or doing a walk?’ Participants answered on a four-point scale (0 = Hardly ever, or never; 1 = One to three times a month; 2 = Once a week; 3 = More than once a week). An overall score of PA ranging from 0 to 6 was created by summing up the scores on the two questions, with higher scores reflecting greater PA.
Cognitive tests
CP was assessed in waves 1, 2, 4, 5, 6 and 7 using validated tests of verbal fluency, immediate recall and delayed recall. In the verbal fluency test (Rosen, Reference Rosen1980), participants were instructed to name as many different animals as they could think of in 1 minute. The score was the total number of correctly named animals, with a higher score indicating higher verbal fluency. Immediate and delayed recall were assessed using an adapted 10-word delayed-recall test (Harris and Dowson, Reference Harris and Dowson1982). In the immediate-recall test, participants first listened to a 10-word list that was read out loud by the interviewer. Then, immediately after the reading of this list, participants were asked to recall as many words as possible. At the end of the cognitive testing session, the participants were asked again to recall any of the words from the list, which captured the delayed recall score. Both scores ranged from 0 to 10, with a higher score indicating greater performance.
Covariates
Models were adjusted for sociodemographic and health-related characteristics identified as potentially confounding and mediating factors in the association between PA and CP level and decline (Trost et al., Reference Trost, Owen, Bauman, Sallis and Brown2002; Kirk and Rhodes, Reference Kirk and Rhodes2011; Bauman et al., Reference Bauman, Reis, Sallis, Wells, Loos, Martin and Group2012; Choi et al., Reference Choi, Chen, Stein, Klimentidis, Wang, Koenen and Smoller2019; Cheval et al., Reference Cheval, Maltagliati, Sieber, Beran, Chalabaev, Sander, Cullati and Boisgontier2021). The value of the covariates was the one at baseline if all PA and CP data were available. In case of missing data at baseline, the value of the covariate was taken from the closest available wave. The selected sociodemographic factors were area of Europe (Western Europe, Scandinavia, Southern Europe, Central and Eastern Europe), age (years), sex (male, female), education (seven categories based on the International Standard Classification of Education 1997), (United Nations Educational, 2006) residence (big city, suburbs or outskirts of a big city, large town, small town, rural area or village), household size (number of members), a partner in a household (yes, no), household net worth (standardised difference between household gross financial assets and financial liabilities), current job situation (working, not working), number of children, number of grandchildren and attrition (non-participation in all the waves or death during the follow-up; yes, no). Health-related characteristics were the number of limitations in instrumental activities of daily living (IADL), number of depressive symptoms assessed with the EURO-D scale (Prince et al., Reference Prince, Reischies, Beekman, Fuhrer, Jonker, Kivela, Lawlor, Lobo, Magnusson and Fichter1999), number of chronic diseases, body mass index (continuous variable), mobility limitations index (number of limitations), smoking (ever smoked daily, never smoked daily), alcohol use (more than two glasses of alcohol almost every day, less), frequency of eating fruits and vegetables (every day, 3–6 times a week, twice a week, once a week, less than once a week).
Statistical analyses
PA trajectories
As in our previous study on trajectories of depressive symptoms (Formánek et al., Reference Formánek, Csajbók, Wolfová, Kučera, Tom, Aarsland and Cermakova2020), growth mixture modelling with maximum likelihood estimation was used to identify latent trajectories of PA (Jung and Wickrama, Reference Jung and Wickrama2008). This approach estimates latent classes following similar trajectories of PA over time with a high probability. Consistent with previous guidelines and literature (Van De Schoot et al., Reference Van De Schoot, Sijbrandij, Winter, Depaoli and Vermunt2017; Formánek et al., Reference Formánek, Csajbók, Wolfová, Kučera, Tom, Aarsland and Cermakova2020), we first freely estimated time slopes in a latent basis growth model that was entered to the growth mixture model (i.e., the classification). Second, the most parsimonious model among those with a different number of PA trajectories (i.e., classes) was determined using the following indicators: Akaike information criterion, Bayesian information criterion, sample-size adjusted Bayesian information criterion, Vuong–Lo–Mendell–Rubin likelihood ratio test, Luo–Mendell–Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test and bootstrap likelihood ratio test (see Supplementary Methods). Finally, we checked the number of individuals within each PA trajectory group to ensure an adequate sample size.
Association between PA trajectories and baseline characteristics
First, independent samples t-tests and χ 2 tests were used to compare the differences in baseline characteristics between PA trajectories. Second, a multivariable analysis using logistic regression was performed to estimate the odds ratio (OR) with a 95% confidence interval (CI) for the association between participants´ baseline characteristics and PA trajectories.
Association between PA trajectories and the level and rate of decline of CP
We used linear mixed-effects models to investigate the associations of PA trajectories with the level of CP and its rate of decline over time. The models included time in years since baseline, PA trajectory, their interaction term (PA trajectory × time) and covariates. Model 1 was adjusted for age and sex. Other sociodemographic covariates (birth cohort, region, education, residence, household size, partner in household, household net worth, current job situation, number of children, number of grandchildren and attrition) were included in Model 2. Health-related characteristics (limitations in IADL, depressive symptoms, number of chronic diseases, body mass index, mobility limitations index, smoking, alcohol use and eating behaviour) were included in Model 3. We adjusted for covariates group-wise in three steps to assess whether sociodemographic and health-related characteristics may explain the observed associations. The model random structure encompassed random intercepts for participants and random linear slopes for time. In addition, we stratified the dataset according to PA trajectories, and (1) fitted a crude model containing only time and (2) Models 1–3 (except interaction term) for each cognitive test, separately. Based on the fit of the crude stratified model, we visualised the cognitive decline across trajectories of PA and per cognitive tests.
Sensitivity and robustness analyses
We performed three sensitivity analyses (SA), in which we replicated the growth mixture modelling on (1) a sample of participants having at least two measures of PA, two measures of CP and no diagnosis of dementia (SA1: N = 67 270), (2) a sample only including surviving participants (SA2: N = 36 248) and (3) a sample of participants who neither dropped out nor died during the follow-up of the study (SA3: N = 29 115). We conducted two robustness analyses, in which we performed the growth mixture modelling on (a) the moderate physical activity measure only and (b) on the vigorous physical activity measure only.
Data availability
Access to the SHARE data is provided free of charge on the basis of a release policy that gives quick and convenient access to all scientific users worldwide after individual registration. All details about the application and registration process can be found on this website: http://www.share-project.org. The study protocol and syntax of the statistical analysis will be shared upon request from the corresponding author of this study.
Results
PA trajectories
The study sample included 38 729 adults 50 years of age or older (63 years on average, 57% women). Two PA trajectory groups emerged from the growth mixture modelling as the best model for describing the longitudinal data: constantly high PA (N = 27 634: 71%) and decreasing PA (N = 11 095; 29%) (Fig. 2). This two-trajectory solution showed an acceptable fit to the data, although with relatively low entropy (0.603), thereby indicating some overlap between the two classes. Results from the process of model selection, including the results of the sensitivity and robustness analyses, are provided in the supplementary materials.
PA trajectories and baseline characteristics of the participants
Table 1 summarises the characteristics of the participants stratified by PA trajectory. Compared with the constantly high PA group, individuals with decreasing PA showed lower baseline level in the three measures of CP (mean verbal fluency 17.9 v. 21.5; mean immediate recall 4.7 v. 5.5; mean delayed recall 3.2 v. 4.1; ps < 0.001). In addition, individuals with decreasing PA were older (p < 0.001) and predominantly women (p < 0.001).
s.d., standard deviation; IADL, instrumental activities of daily living.
a International Standard Classification of Education level 5 or 6.
b Big city, its suburbs or outskirts.
c 4 or more points on EURO-D scale.
d Body mass index 30 and more.
e Fruits or vegetables less than every day.
Table 2 shows the results of the logistic regression models testing the association between PA trajectories and participants' sociodemographic and health-related characteristics. Compared with individuals with the constantly high PA, decreasing PA was associated with older age, lower education, lower household wealth, non-urban residence, not working, living without a partner, greater depressive symptoms, more limitations in IADL and mobility, more chronic diseases, smoking, obesity and poor diet. When compared to Western Europe, decreasing PA was more likely to occur in Southern Europe and Israel and less likely in Scandinavia and Central and Eastern Europe.
OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.
All characteristics were entered into the model. In addition, the model included attrition and cognition (mean of z-scores of all three cognitive tests). Because of collinearity, birth cohort was not included into the model.
a International Standard Classification of Education level 5 or 6.
b Big city, its suburbs or outskirts.
c 4 or more points on EURO-D scale.
d Body mass index 30 and more.
e Fruits or vegetables less than every day.
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.001.
Physical-activity trajectories and cognitive performance
Table 3 shows the results of the linear mixed-effects models. Compared with individuals from the constantly high PA group, individuals in the decreasing PA group had a significantly lower level of baseline CP, independently of all covariates (p < 0.001 for all three measures in Model 1, 2 and 3). Individuals in the decreasing PA group also had a steeper decline of CP in all CP measures, independently from covariates, as shown by the PA trajectory × time interaction (p < 0.001 for all three measures in Models 1, 2 and 3).
CI, confidence interval.
Results are derived from linear mixed-effects models.
Model 1: adjusted for age and sex.
Model 2: adjusted for age, sex, birth cohort, region, education, residence, household size, a partner in household, household net worth, current job situation, number of children, number of grandchildren and attrition.
Model 3: adjusted for age, sex, birth cohort, region, education, residence, household size, a partner in household, household net worth, current job situation, number of children, number of grandchildren, attrition, limitations in IADL, depressive symptoms, number of chronic diseases, body mass index, mobility limitations index, smoking, alcohol use and eating behaviour.
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.001.
When stratified by the PA trajectory, individuals from the constantly high PA group had negligible rates of decline in immediate recall, verbal fluency and none in delayed recall, while individuals from the decreasing PA group showed steeper rates of decline in all three cognitive measures (Fig. 3). Adjustment for covariates only slightly attenuated the rates of decline. In the fully adjusted models, individuals from the decreasing PA group had a steeper decline in all cognitive measures (immediate recall: ß = −0.04; 95% CI = −0.05 to −0.04; verbal fluency: ß = −0.22; 95% CI = −0.24 to −0.21; delayed recall: ß = −0.04; 95% CI = −0.05 to −0.04), in comparison to the other group. In contrast, individuals from the constantly high PA group only demonstrated a small decline in immediate recall (ß −0.01; 95% CI −0.01 to −0.01) and verbal fluency (ß = −0.05; 95% CI −0.06 to −0.04) and none in delayed recall (ß = 6.0 × 10−4; 95% CI −2.2 × 10−3 to 3.4 × 10−3).
Sensitivity and robustness analyses
The SA and robustness analyses (Supplementary tables S4, S5 and S6) yielded similar results as the main analysis. Specifically, we replicated the 2-class solution (see supplementary methods). Furthermore, the linear mixed-effects models showed that, compared with individuals from the constantly high PA group, individuals from the decreasing PA group had a significantly lower baseline level of the three CP measures and exhibited a steeper decline of those measures. The only difference is that the associations between the PA groups and the decline of immediate and delayed recall over time did not remain significant after the adjustment for all the covariates.
Discussion
In the present large-scale longitudinal multicentric study of adults aged 50 years and older, we identified two distinct PA trajectories: constantly high PA and decreasing PA. These profiles of PA trajectories were both associated with the level of CP and the rate of its decline over time. Compared with the constantly high PA profile, the decreasing PA profile was significantly associated with lower baseline levels of CP, and with a significantly steeper decline of CP over time. These associations were only slightly attenuated after adjustment for the sociodemographic and health-related characteristics. These findings suggest that the CP decline over time is mirrored in the longitudinal changes of PA.
Our results complement the literature on the association of PA and CP during ageing from a longitudinal perspective. Using growth mixture modelling, we identified two distinct PA trajectories over time. One PA trajectory showed PA levels that remained constant across time, a finding that is consistent with previous studies that observed stable PA trajectories in old age (Laddu et al., Reference Laddu, Parimi, Cauley, Cawthon, Ensrud, Orwoll, Stefanick and Langsetmo2018; Aggio et al., Reference Aggio, Papachristou, Papacosta, Lennon, Ash, Whincup, Wannamethee and Jefferis2020). The second PA trajectory that emerged showed a decreased PA across time. This result is in line with previous studies that have observed a reduction in PA at old age (Lounassalo et al., Reference Lounassalo, Salin, Kankaanpää, Hirvensalo, Palomäki, Tolvanen, Yang and Tammelin2019; Sanchez-Sanchez et al., Reference Sanchez-Sanchez, Izquierdo, Carnicero-Carreño, García-García and Rodríguez-Mañas2020). However, in addition to these two PA trajectories, other studies found increasing PA trajectories in middle-aged and older adults (Lounassalo et al., Reference Lounassalo, Salin, Kankaanpää, Hirvensalo, Palomäki, Tolvanen, Yang and Tammelin2019; Saint-Maurice et al., Reference Saint-Maurice, Coughlan, Kelly, Keadle, Cook, Carlson, Fulton and Matthews2019). Moreover, studies often reported three or four PA trajectory groups, which contrasts with the two PA trajectories that have emerged from the current data (Lounassalo et al., Reference Lounassalo, Salin, Kankaanpää, Hirvensalo, Palomäki, Tolvanen, Yang and Tammelin2019). This discrepancy may be explained by the features of the scale that was used to measure PA (i.e., 2-items measuring the frequency of moderate and vigorous PA), which was associated with low variance. As such, it was difficult to identify clearly distinct PA trajectories as indicated by the low level of entropy observed between our two selected PA groups.
In the present study, 71% of our sample were classified in the more favourable profile (i.e., constantly high PA), while only 29% were classified in the unfavourable profile (i.e., decreasing PA). This result is rather consistent with previous studies observing that about 30% of older adults showed a decreasing PA trajectory across time (Saint-Maurice et al., Reference Saint-Maurice, Coughlan, Kelly, Keadle, Cook, Carlson, Fulton and Matthews2019; Aggio et al., Reference Aggio, Papachristou, Papacosta, Lennon, Ash, Whincup, Wannamethee and Jefferis2020; Sanchez-Sanchez et al., Reference Sanchez-Sanchez, Izquierdo, Carnicero-Carreño, García-García and Rodríguez-Mañas2020). However, while this finding is very encouraging in terms of public health, it must be interpreted with caution given that, in our study, PA was assessed using a self-reported questionnaire, which may have biased the estimation of participants' PA behaviours. Furthermore, when age is used as the time scale instead of the wave of measurement (i.e., using an accelerated longitudinal design assessing PA evolution from 65 to 100 years), results showed a clear decline in PA across ageing in all the PA groups (Laddu et al., Reference Laddu, Parimi, Cauley, Cawthon, Ensrud, Orwoll, Stefanick and Langsetmo2018, Reference Laddu, Parimi, Stone, Lapidus, Hoffman, Stefanick and Langsetmo2020).
We observed that a number of sociodemographic and health-related characteristics are associated with the PA trajectories. In particular, older age, worse socioeconomic status, poor lifestyle profile and worse health were associated with increased odds of belonging to the unfavourable PA group. Our findings are consistent with previous studies that have investigated the multiple correlates of higher engagement in PA (Trost et al., Reference Trost, Owen, Bauman, Sallis and Brown2002; Bauman et al., Reference Bauman, Reis, Sallis, Wells, Loos, Martin and Group2012). For example, chronic conditions (Cheval et al., Reference Cheval, Maltagliati, Sieber, Beran, Chalabaev, Sander, Cullati and Boisgontier2021), depressive symptoms (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Chen, Stein, Klimentidis, Wang, Koenen and Smoller2019), or lower-level education (Kirk and Rhodes, Reference Kirk and Rhodes2011), have been found to be associated with a lower level of engagement in PA. Our results confirm these associations but also reveal that these factors are not only associated with the level of PA engagement, but also with its evolution across time. Even though causality cannot be established due to the observational design of this study, these findings indicate that decline in PA and CP could be predicted. Focusing health care and preventive efforts on subgroups of individuals with low socioeconomic status and poor health profile could contribute to the prevention of decreasing PA and CP.
The strong relationship between higher levels of PA and better cognitive function are well-established (Cheval et al., Reference Cheval, Sieber, Guessous, Orsholits, Courvoisier, Kliegel, Stringhini, Swinnen, Burton-Jeangros, Cullati and Boisgontier2018b, Reference Cheval, Orsholits, Sieber, Courvoisier, Cullati and Boisgontier2020c; Sebastiani et al., Reference Sebastiani, Andersen, Sweigart, Du, Cosentino, Thyagarajan, Christensen, Schupf and Perls2020), and our results confirm this association. To the best of our knowledge, our study was the first to investigate this association using a data-driven approach to identify PA trajectories. One study has estimated trajectories of PA over 28 years in people with or without dementia and found that PA started to decline 9 years before the diagnosis of dementia (Sabia et al., Reference Sabia, Dugravot, Dartigues, Abell, Elbaz, Kivimäki and Singh-Manoux2017). A result that rather suggests that changes in PA may simply result from the decline in CP, which is consistent with other studies that have observed an association from changes in CP to changes in PA (Cheval et al., Reference Cheval, Orsholits, Sieber, Courvoisier, Cullati and Boisgontier2020c). Moreover, intervention studies in older adults observed a protective effect of an increased PA on CP (Colcombe and Kramer, Reference Colcombe and Kramer2003; Angevaren et al., Reference Angevaren, Aufdemkampe, Verhaar, Aleman and Vanhees2008), although other studies did not (Sink et al., Reference Sink, Espeland, Castro, Church, Cohen, Dodson, Guralnik, Hendrie, Jennings and Katula2015; Young et al., Reference Young, Angevaren, Rusted and Tabet2015). However, these studies focused on relatively short-term changes in PA (i.e., from one time of measurement to another one; or before and after an intervention), but disregarded the long-term trajectories of PA across time. Overall, our results are in line with previous studies showing that changes in PA are associated with changes in CP.
Several mechanisms have been suggested to explain the association between a higher level of PA and a maintained level of CP (Cotman and Berchtold, Reference Cotman and Berchtold2002; Colcombe and Kramer, Reference Colcombe and Kramer2003; Cotman et al., Reference Cotman, Berchtold and Christie2007; Hillman et al., Reference Hillman, Erickson and Kramer2008; Raichlen and Alexander, Reference Raichlen and Alexander2017). For example, PA has been associated with increased brain plasticity, angiogenesis, synaptogenesis and neurogenesis primarily through the release of growth factors such as brain-derived neurotrophic factor, insulin-like growth factor-1 and vascular endothelial growth factor (Cotman and Berchtold, Reference Cotman and Berchtold2002; Cotman et al., Reference Cotman, Berchtold and Christie2007; Hillman et al., Reference Hillman, Erickson and Kramer2008). Moreover, the cognitive demands inherent in many types of PA, which may include planning, reasoning, decision-making and multitasking, may also have positive effects on the brain (Raichlen and Alexander, Reference Raichlen and Alexander2017; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Saliasi, Van Den Berg, Uijtdewilligen, De Groot, Jolles, Andersen, Bailey, Chang and Diamond2019). Alternatively, additional mechanisms have been suggested to explain the association in the other direction – i.e., from cognitive functioning to higher engagement in physical activity (Cheval et al., Reference Cheval, Radel, Neva, Boyd, Swinnen, Sander and Boisgontier2018a, Reference Cheval, Rebar, Miller, Sieber, Orsholits, Baranyi, Courvoisier, Cullati, Sander and Boisgontier2019, Reference Cheval, Daou, Cabral, Bacelar, Parma, Forestier, Orsholits, Sander, Boisgontier and Miller2020a). In particular, anchored within the theory of effort minimisation in physical activity (TEMPA) (Cheval and Boisgontier, Reference Cheval and Boisgontier2021), these studies showed that cognitive functions are critical to counteract the automatic tendency to effort minimisation, thereby favouring engagement in PA.
Among the strengths of the present study are the large sample size, the longitudinal design, the data-driven approach to identify PA trajectories across time and the reliance on three measures of CP based on established procedures. However, some limitations should be noted. First, PA was assessed using a self-reported questionnaire, which may have reduced measurement validity. Second, the two PA groups differed in both the initial PA levels (i.e., high v. moderate) and the evolution of PA across time (i.e., maintaining v. decreasing). As such, it was not possible to disentangle the influence of these two factors (i.e., level or slope) on CP. Future research should rely on a more detailed and reliable questionnaire or on device-based measures of PA to be able to better discriminate between PA trajectories, and their associations with CP. Likewise, as the PA groups differed regarding overall health-related condition, we cannot exclude that a steeper cognitive decline observed in the unfavourable PA profile can result from this poorer health condition rather than the change in PA. Furthermore, as it cannot be avoided in long-term longitudinal studies, we cannot exclude a selection bias due to attrition. Yet, to minimise this bias, our statistical analyses were adjusted for attrition and we conducted SA excluding participants who dropped out or died during the follow-up. Finally, the correlational nature of the SHARE design cannot guarantee causal links between PA trajectories and CP decline.
In conclusion, these findings confirm the hypothesis that an unfavourable PA profile may be associated with weaker CP in old age, thereby supporting the need to promote effective strategies to help individuals to maintain their PA levels over the life course.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796021000688.
Data
This SHARE dataset is available at http://www.share-project.org/data-access.html.
Acknowledgements
This paper uses data from SHARE Waves 1, 2, 3 (SHARELIFE), 4, 5, 6, 7 and 8 (DOIs: 10..6103/SHARE.w1.600, 10..6103/SHARE.w2.600, 10..6103/SHARE.w3.600, 10..6103/SHARE.w4.600, 10..6103/SHARE.w5.600, 10..6103/SHARE.w6.600, 10.6103/SHARE.w7.711, 10.6103/SHARE.w8cabeta.001). The SHARE data collection was primarily funded by the European Commission through FP5 (QLK6-CT-2001-00360), FP6 (SHARE-I3: RII-CT-2006-062193, COMPARE: CIT5-CT-2005-028857, SHARELIFE: CIT4-CT-2006-028812) and FP7 (SHARE-PREP: no.211909, SHARE-LEAP: no.227822, SHARE M4: no.261982). Additional funding from the German Ministry of Education and Research, the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science, the U.S. National Institute on Aging (U01_AG09740-13S2, P01_AG005842, P01_AG08291, P30_AG12815, R21_AG025169, Y1-AG-4553-01, IAG_BSR06-11, OGHA_04-064, HHSN271201300071C) and from various national funding sources is gratefully acknowledged (see www.share-project.org).
Author contributions
B.C.: conceptualisation, writing – original draft; Z.C.: conceptualisation, formal analysis, writing – original draft; T.F.: formal analysis, writing – review & editing; S.S.: writing – review & editing; M.P.B.: writing – review & editing; S.C.: supervision, writing – review & editing; P.C.: conceptualisation, formal analysis, writing – original draft.
Financial support
B.C. is supported by an Ambizione grant (PZ00P1_180040) from the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF). Z.C. is supported by Charles University Research Centre programme UNCE/HUM/025 (204056) and PRIMUS grant (247066). P.C. is supported by grant PRIMUS (247066) conducted at Charles University and the Ministry of Health of the Czech Republic (grant NU20J-04-00022). M.P.B. is supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (RGPIN-2021-03153) and by a grant from The Banting Research Foundation.
Ethical standards
This study was part of the SHARE study, approved by the relevant research ethics committees in the participating countries.
Informed consent
All participants provided written informed consent.