Patients with personality disorders have been reported to use psychiatric services excessively (Reference Seivewright, Tyrer and CaseySeivewright et al, 1991; Reference Saarento, Nieminten and HakkoSaarento et al, 1997). Nevertheless, the economic impact of personality disorders on the health service remains largely unexplored. Only one previously published study has examined the cost of this patient group. Smith et al (Reference Smith, Shah and Wright1995) estimated that the cost of personality disorders to the National Health Service (NHS) in 1986, was £ 61.3 million. However, this study relied on national data sources to measure prevalence and service costs and provided no comparative group. Using a sample of consecutive attenders at four general practices in the London area, we set out to test the hypothesis that people with a personality disorder have higher mean health and non-health costs compared with those without a personality disorder.
METHOD
Recruitment and baseline assessments
The study was a follow-up of a cohort of 303 primary care attenders who had been recruited and assessed 1 year previously for the presence of personality disorders. Full details of the method and the characteristics of the study have been published elsewhere (Reference Moran, Jenkins and TyleeMoran et al, 2000). In brief, 374 patients were recruited from a convenience sample of four general practices in the London area. Personality was assessed using a revised ICD-10 and DSM-IV version of the Standardised Assessment of Personality (SAP; Reference Pilgrim, Mellers and BoothbyPilgrim et al, 1993). The SAP is a semi-structured interview designed for use with an informant and generates data that can be used to make a diagnosis of personality disorder according to ICD-10 and DSM-IV criteria. The personalities of 303 participants were rated using the SAP. A total of 72 (24%) of the patients were assessed as having at least one personality disorder according to ICD-10 and DSM—IV criteria. Demographic information was also recorded at baseline.
One year follow-up
One year after recruitment all participants were sent a questionnaire by post, containing the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12; Reference GoldbergGoldberg, 1972), the physical function sub-scale of the SF-36 (Reference Jenkinson, Wright and CoulterJenkinson et al, 1993), a life events questionnaire (Reference Brugha and CraggBrugha & Cragg, 1990) and a use of services questionnaire. The GHQ-12 is a 12-item self-report questionnaire that screens for the presence of psychiatric morbidity and specifically ‘common mental disorders’, such as anxiety and depression (an increase in GHQ-12 score indicates greater psychiatric morbidity). The physical function sub-scale of the SF-36 is a self-report measure of physical functioning (an increase in physical function score indicates better physical functioning).
The use of services questionnaire was a variant of the Client Service Receipt Inventory (CSRI; Reference Beecham, Knapp, Thornicroft, Brewin and WingBeecham & Knapp, 1992). This questionnaire was used to collect retrospective data on service utilisation in the last 6 months and covers the following domains: general practitioner (GP) consultations; practice nurse visits; hospital in-patient stays; hospital out-patient episodes; seeing a social worker; counselling and therapy contacts. Participants were asked about the duration of consultations and any costs incurred for private treatment. Information was also collected on employment status and time taken off work because of ill health.
Estimated unit costs were calculated for all services used, drugs prescribed and time taken off work because of illness. All costs were calculated in £ sterling at 1998/99 price levels. Service cost estimates were based on figures published in Netten et al (Reference Netten, Dennett and Knight1999) and allowed for capital, overheads, travelling time, non-patient contact time, support services and London weighting. Where costs were not available in Netten et al, other sources and equivalent costs were used. Lost productivity costs, because of illness or unemployment, were calculated as the only non-health service costs. Figures were based on national average gross earnings by occupational status. The researcher responsible for costing the data (A. R.) was blind to the personality status of the participants.
Sample size
A pre-study power calculation showed that a sample of 300 participants would be required to detect a 50 unit difference in mean costs between participants with personality disorders and those without, with 80% power and 95% significance. This assumes a 30% prevalence of personality disorder (Reference Casey and TyrerCasey & Tyrer, 1990) and a standard deviation of 125.
Statistical analysis
Analyses were performed using SPSS and Stata (Reference NorusisNorusis, 1993; StataCorp, 1999). A strategy for the statistical analysis of the association between baseline measures and 1-year cost was drawn up before inspecting the data. The main outcome measures used were the arithmetic means for health service, non-health service and total costs. Univariate associations between personality disorder and costs were investigated using t-tests; the results were checked using non-parametric bootstrap analyses (based on 2000 replications). Multiple linear regression was then used to identify variables that predicted variations in costs. The following variables are associated with health service use and were therefore entered into the multiple regression models: age; gender; physical health and psychiatric morbidity (Reference Gill and SharpeGill & Sharpe 1999; Reference Dowrick, Bellon and GomezDowrick et al, 2000). The following additional variables were entered into the multiple regression models on a priori grounds: personality disorder status, life events, marital status and age on finishing education (a proxy measure for socio-economic status). Interactions between personality disorder and SF—36 physical function score and General Health Questionnaire score were also included in the model.
Results from the multiple regression models were subject to two checks. First, they were compared with the results from non-parametric bootstrap regression to assess the robustness of confidence intervals and P values to non-normality in the cost distribution (Reference Efron and TibshiraniEfron & Tibshirani, 1993). Confidence limits were obtained from bias-corrected estimates based on 2000 resamples; significance levels were obtained from percentiles of the distribution of bootstrapped re-samples. Second, the results were compared with those obtained from a generalised linear model, where a non-normal (gamma distribution) was assumed for costs.
RESULTS
Completeness of follow-up
Of the 303 participants rated at baseline, 254 (84%) returned completed questionnaires. Participants with personality disorders were not over-represented among those lost to follow-up (χ2=0.56; P=0.81). Those lost to follow-up were more likely to be male (χ2=3.97; P=0.05) and younger (t-test, 2-tailed P=0.01).
Univariate analyses
Mean costs were consistently higher for patients with personality disorders compared with those without personality disorders, although the difference was only statistically significant for total costs (Table 1). Non-parametric bootstrap analyses confirmed these findings.
Cost | Personality disorder (n=60) | Non-personality disorder (n=192) | t-test P value |
---|---|---|---|
Total health service costs | 507.0 (884.5) | 313.6 (854.3) | 0.13 |
Total non-health service costs | 2544.8 (4994.8) | 1312.5 (3584.7) | 0.08 |
Total cost | 3094.3 (5324.2) | 1632.9 (3779.0) | 0.05 |
Multivariate analyses
Health service costs
A percentage (12%) of the variance of health service costs was explained by the variables in the multivariate model (Table 2). The SF—36 physical function sub-scale score was the only variable that was significantly associated with health service costs; a one-point increase in physical function score was associated with a £46 decrease in health service costs. Personality disorder was not significantly associated with health service costs in this model. Bootstrap regression analysis and generalised linear modelling confirmed these findings.
Multivariate predictor | Coefficient (95% CI)1 | P |
---|---|---|
Age (years) | -0.43 (-9.8 to 8.9) | 0.92 |
Gender (male compared with female) | 193 (-33.9 to 419.1) | 0.10 |
Marital status (married compared with being single) | 21 (-196.7 to 237.8) | 0.85 |
Age on finishing education (years) | -54 (-124.8 to 16.4) | 0.13 |
Total number of life events | 55 (-35.7 to 145.6) | 0.23 |
PF score | -46 (-71.2 to −20.0) | <0.001 |
GHQ-12 score | 32 (-5.9 to 69.8) | 0.10 |
Personality disorder status (non-personality disorder compared with personality disorder) | -719 (-1994.2 to 555.4) | 0.27 |
Interaction term between personality disorder and PF score | 33 (-12.8 to 79.3) | 0.16 |
Interaction term between personality disorder and GHQ-12 score | -12 (-83.4 to 59.5) | 0.74 |
Constant | 1620 (580.4 to 2658.7) | 0.003 |
Non-health service costs
A percentage (24%) of the variance of non-health service costs was explained by the variables in the multivariate model (Table 3). Three variables were significantly associated with non-health service costs: GHQ-12 score, gender and marital status. In addition, non-health service costs were significantly associated with an interaction between personality disorder and GHQ—12 score. For those participants without a personality disorder, a one-point increase in GHQ—12 score was associated with a £253 increase in non-health service costs. For patients with personality disorders, a one-point increase in GHQ—12 score was associated with a £764 increase in non-health service costs.
Multivariate predictor | Coefficient (95% CI)1 | P |
---|---|---|
Age (years) | 1 (-38.1 to 40.1) | 0.96 |
Gender (male compared with female) | 1251 (305.4 to 2197.4) | 0.01 |
Marital status (married compared with being single) | -975 (-1882.6 to −67.4) | 0.04 |
Age on finishing education (years) | -279 (-573.8 to 16.0) | 0.06 |
Total number of life events | 218 (-160.1 to 596.9) | 0.26 |
PF score | -54 (-161.3 to 52.4) | 0.32 |
GHQ-12 score | 253 (95.3 to 414.2) | 0.002 |
Personality disorder status (non-personality disorder compared with personality disorder) | -1535 (-6890.8 to 3790.7) | 0.57 |
Interaction term between personality disorder and GHQ-12 score | 511 (212.2 to 809.2) | < 0.001 |
Interaction term between personality disorder and PF score | 24 (-168.2 to 216.5) | 0.81 |
Constant | 3602 (-738.9 to 7942.9) | 0.1 |
Bootstrap regression analyses largely confirmed these results, although marital status failed to remain significantly associated with non-health service costs. A generalised linear model of non-health service costs did not find the interaction between GHQ—12 score and personality disorder to be statistically significant.
Total costs
Some of the variance of total costs (28%) was explained by variables in the multivariate model. Four variables were significantly associated with total costs: GHQ—12 score, gender, age on finishing education and marital status (Table 4).
Multivariate predictor | Coefficient (95% CI)1 | P |
---|---|---|
Age (years) | 0.59 (-40.1 to 41.3) | 0.98 |
Gender (male compared with female) | 1444 (499.3 to 2332) | 0.004 |
Marital status (married compared with being single) | -955 (-1923 to −99.3) | 0.05 |
Age on finishing education (years) | -333 (-593.2 to −38.5) | 0.03 |
Total number of life events | 273 (-120.3 to 667) | 0.17 |
PF score | -100 (-186 to −4.2) | 0.08 |
GHQ-12 score | 285 (165 to 474.7) | 0.001 |
Personality disorder status (non-personality disorder compared with personality disorder) | -2255 (-7793.3 to 3283.7) | 0.42 |
Interaction term between personality disorder and GHQ-12 score | 499 (180.1 to 626.2) | 0.002 |
Interaction term between personality disorder and PF score | 57 (-142.7 to 257.4) | 0.57 |
Constant | 5222 (683.7 to 9759.5) | 0.02 |
In addition, an interaction between GHQ—12 score and personality disorder was significantly associated with total costs. For participants without personality disorders, a one-point increase in GHQ—12 score was associated with a £285 increase in total costs. For participants with personality disorders, a one-point increase in GHQ—12 score was associated with a £784 increase in total service costs.
The non-parametric bootstrap analysis confirmed the presence of a statistically significant interaction between GHQ—12 score and personality disorder in association with total costs. However, a generalised linear model of total costs failed to confirm the presence of this interaction.
DISCUSSION
Personality disorder and cost
Personality disorder was not significantly associated with mean health or non-health service costs at either a univariate or multivariate level. Personality disorder was significantly associated with higher mean total costs, although this association failed to remain statistically significant in the multivariate analysis. Our main hypothesis was therefore not supported.
A significant interaction emerged between personality disorder and common mental disorders in the multivariate analyses of non-health and total costs; the costs for patients with personality disorder were significantly higher in the presence of common mental disorders. The presence of this interaction was confirmed by non-parametric bootstrap analyses (which assume an additive model of costs), although it was not confirmed by generalised linear modelling (which assumes a multiplicative model of costs). This makes us more cautious in attributing significance to the interaction. Nevertheless, it also raises methodological questions as to the need for multiple confirmatory analysis (Reference Knapp, Chisholm and LeeseKnapp et al, 2002).
Evaluation of the study design
This is the first published study of the economic impact of personality disorder in the UK health service. The study is characterised by three positive features. First, it is a prospective study in which the assessment of costs was not influenced by the assessment of personality. Second, the researcher costing the data was blind to the personality status of participants, therefore minimising observer bias in the estimation of costs. Third, the use of an informant-based assessment of personality minimised the possibility of concurrent abnormal mental state biasing the assessment of personality. Despite these features, the study has a number of limitations. First, the patients in the study were primary care attenders and are likely to be different from the patients seen by general psychiatrists. This therefore limits the extent to which the findings can be generalised to psychiatric populations. Second, a convenience and not a random sample of practices was used and the findings might not therefore be generalisable to patients from other UK practices. Third, our sample of consecutive attenders could have included a greater number of heavy service users. This could have inflated the costs for the whole sample, although it should not have affected the relative costs of attenders with personality disorders compared with attenders without personality disorders. Fourth, our power calculation was based on a prevalence of 30% for personality disorder, although the detected prevalence was only 24%. Therefore, our study could have been underpowered to detect an association between personality disorder and cost. Underpowering is a problem common to health economic studies, especially in the mental health field (Reference Gray, Marshall and LockwoodGray et al, 1997), although in our case, we had used the best available prior data to try to avoid this difficulty. Finally, the basis for our measure of cost, the CSRI, is a self-report instrument and the estimated costs could, therefore, be susceptible to recall bias.
Explained variation in cost
Health service costs were associated with physical function status. This is consistent with the finding that heavy general practice service users are less likely to report excellent and good health and more likely to have physical disease, compared to controls (Reference Gill and SharpeGill & Sharpe, 1999). However, our final regression model only explained 12% of the variance in total health service costs. Unmeasured variables of relevance to the prediction of health service costs could include factors such as cigarette and alcohol consumption, physical activity, access to services and socio-economic status (Reference KnappKnapp, 1998). Our finding of no association between personality disorder and health service costs is at odds with previous reports of an association between personality disorder and high psychiatric service utilisation (Reference Seivewright, Tyrer and CaseySeivewright et al, 1991; Reference Saarento, Nieminten and HakkoSaarento et al, 1997). However, as noted above, this could reflect the fact that, despite our best efforts, the study was underpowered to detect such an association.
In our sample of GP attenders, the presence of common mental disorders predicted both non-health service costs (lost productivity) and total costs. This finding is consistent with other reports of a significant association between common mental disorders and disability (Reference Ormel, Korff and BrinkOrmel et al, 1993). However, our findings suggest that common mental disorders interacted with personality disorders in predicting both non-health service and total costs; patients with personality disorders were only more expensive if they also had common mental disorders. Personality disorders are often associated with a poor prognosis for the treatment of associated mental illness (Reference Patience, McGuire and ScottPatience et al, 1995; Reference Mennin and HeimbergMennin & Heimberg, 2000). We therefore suggest that increased total costs for the patients with personality disorders could have occurred as a result of chronicity of associated mental illness (Reference Seivewright, Tyrer and JohnsonSeivewright et al, 1998).
Personality disorders are currently the subject of great debate. However, the current debate is focused on the small proportion of individuals who have a ‘severe personality disorder’ and could pose a danger to the public. By examining the economic impact of the whole diagnostic group of patients with personality disorders, we have shown that personality disorders could have a subtle effect on non-health service and total costs through an interaction with psychiatric comorbidity. Clearly our findings need replication in a larger, more representative sample of patients. However, we believe that this study is an important step towards improving the evidence base in an area that is overburdened with opinion rather than fact.
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
-
• General practitioner (GP) patients with personality disorders have higher mean total costs compared with those without personality disorders.
-
• Physical function was significantly associated with health service costs. Common mental disorders were significantly associated with both non-health service costs and total costs.
-
• An interaction between personality disorders and common mental disorders was significantly associated with non-health service costs and total costs. These increased costs could be incurred as a result of the chronicity of associated mental illness.
LIMITATIONS
-
• The results apply to a population of GP attenders and might not be generalisable to populations of patients with psychiatric disorders.
-
• The multiple regression models explained only a small proportion of the variations in cost.
-
• The study could have failed to identify and measure variables that have a significant impact on cost.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Morven Leese and Jack Astin for their assistance with data analysis and Lisa Green and Saddaff Piracha for help in recruitment of participants and entering data. We are grateful to the patients, informants and GPs that participated in this study. Finally, we want to thank two anonymous referees for their comments on an earlier draft of this paper. The study was supported by grants from the Department of Health and the Medical Research Council.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.