Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T19:34:22.345Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): A Genetically Sensitive Investigation of Cognitive and Behavioral Development From Childhood to Young Adulthood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 October 2012

Claire M. A. Haworth*
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Oliver S. P. Davis
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Robert Plomin
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
*
address for correspondence: Claire M. A. Haworth, Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, P080, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) is a large longitudinal sample of twins born in England and Wales between 1994 and 1996. The focus of TEDS has been on cognitive and behavioral development, including difficulties in the context of normal development. TEDS began when multiple births were identified from birth records and the families were invited to take part in the study; 16,810 pairs of twins were originally enrolled in TEDS. More than 10,000 of these twin pairs remain enrolled in the study to date. DNA has been collected for more than 7,000 pairs, and genome-wide genotyping data for two million DNA markers are available for 3,500 individuals. The TEDS families have taken part in studies when the twins were aged 2, 3, 4, 7, 8, 9, 10, 12, 14, and 16 years of age. Data collection is currently underway to assess the adult destinations of the twins as they move from school to university and the workplace. Between January 2012 and December 2014, all of the TEDS twins will turn 18, and the study will transition to an adult sample. TEDS represents an outstanding resource for investigating the developmental effects of genes and environments on complex quantitative traits from childhood to young adulthood and beyond.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

The Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) follows the development of a large cohort of twins born in England and Wales between 1994 and 1996. The initial aims of TEDS were to consider genetic and environmental influences on cognitive and behavioral development, and in particular to recruit a large representative sample to allow the investigation of common developmental difficulties in the context of typical development. A unique aspect of TEDS has been its focus on language, cognition, and academic achievement, but we have also collected data on adjustment and some information about health. It is a momentous time for TEDS as we transition from a childhood to an adult sample. Here, we update the previous descriptions of TEDS (Oliver & Plomin, Reference Oliver and Plomin2007; Trouton et al., Reference Trouton, Spinath and Plomin2002) to include information about our latest assessments in adolescence and early adulthood.

TEDS From Recruitment at Birth to Adulthood

Twin births between January 1994 and December 1996 were identified through birth records. The UK Office for National Statistics contacted these families after screening for infant mortality, and 16,810 families responded to acknowledge their interest in taking part in the study. The TEDS team made their first contact with these families when the twins were about 18 months of age to collect general demographic information, including zygosity and information about pregnancy and birth. Since initial enrolment the families have been invited to take part in studies when the twins were aged 2, 3, 4, 7, 8, 9, 10, 12, 14, and 16 years of age. In addition, there was a subsample of twins who were assessed in their homes at 4½ years (see below for more details), and the whole sample is being contacted as the twins turn 18 to collect information on the adult destinations of the twins as they move from school to university and the workplace. Zygosity was assigned using a parent-reported questionnaire of physical similarity, which is over 95% accurate when compared to DNA testing (Price et al., Reference Price, Freeman, Craig, Petrill, Ebersole and Plomin2000). For cases where zygosity was unclear, DNA testing was conducted.

Table 1 includes information about the sample size from first contact to adolescence, and details of the continued representativeness of the sample. At first contact almost 14,000 families provided data; these families were highly representative of the general UK population (Table 1). For example, 91.7% of the families at first contact self-reported as white, compared to 93% in the general population at that time (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Maher, Coulthard, Goddard and Thomas2001). The percentage white has remained very stable from early childhood (92.9%), into middle childhood (93.2%), and adolescence (92.8%). FIGURE 1 shows the approximate location of the twins in adolescence, which is representative of the distribution of the general population in England and Wales. Although all of the twins were born in England and Wales, over the years a small number of families have spread out across the United Kingdom and abroad.

TABLE 1 Representativeness of the TEDS Sample at First Contact, Early Childhood, Middle Childhood, and Adolescence

The equivalent UK population percentages for this generation are 93% white; 32% for A-levels or higher; 49% for mother employed; and 89% for father employed (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Maher, Coulthard, Goddard and Thomas2001). Early childhood refers to families who provided any data when the twins were aged 2, 3, or 4 years. Middle childhood refers to families who provided any data when the twins were aged 7, 9, or 10 years. Adolescence refers to families who provided any data when the twins were aged 12, 14, or 16 years. A-levels are the national educational qualification taken at 18 years of age in the UK, and refer to parental educational qualifications. % female relates to the sex of the twins. MZ = monozygotic twins. These percentages for MZ twins match the expected distribution of twin births in the United Kingdom.

FIGURE 1 Geographical distribution of TEDS. Note: Plotting the local density of 10,000 TEDS families on a map of the United Kingdom — using a color scale from dark red (low density) to yellow (high density) — shows that they are distributed in the same way as the general population in England and Wales. Only a small proportion of the initial sample has migrated outside the United Kingdom (253 families). For these families, the main destinations have been Australia (44), New Zealand (33), the United States (33), Canada (30), the Republic of Ireland (21), and France (20). The underlying map was provided by Ordnance Survey OpenData (www.ordnancesurvey.co.uk).

Informed consent has been collected from the parents for each assessment, and informed assent was collected from the twins in adolescence. Not all of the families are invited to take part at all ages; for example, due to budget constraints the 1996 birth cohort has not been included in all of the assessment waves. Families are always given the option to withdraw from the study, or simply to not participate in a particular stage of the study. The TEDS research team works extremely hard to retain the study families, including sending handwritten birthday cards, annual newsletters, and conducting competitions and prize draws. As the twins have grown up, we have also been providing work experience and career advice, and have set up twin advice panels called the TEDS Ideas Panels (TIPs). Information about the prize draws and our latest research news can be found on our website: www.teds.ac.uk.

Measures

FIGURE 2 represents the timeline of assessments in TEDS, including information about the measures included at each age, and example TEDS publications. More detailed information about the measures is available from the TEDS team on request. These main TEDS assessments have been made possible by 20 years of continuous funding from the UK Medical Research Council.

FIGURE 2 Timeline of TEDS assessments. This figure represents the main TEDS assessments and major measures. More detailed information is available from the TEDS team on request. Following are examples at each age of papers reporting twin analyses of TEDS data. The papers were selected from over 300 in order to represent the diversity of authors and measures in TEDS. Examples of other TEDS twin analyses and DNA analyses are included in the text. First contact: Koeppen-Schomerus et al. (Reference Koeppen-Schomerus, Eley, Wolke, Gringras and Plomin2000); Price et al. (Reference Price, Grosser, Plomin and Jaffee2010); Ronald et al. (Reference Ronald, Happé, Dworzynski, Bolton and Plomin2010b). 2 years: Dale et al. (Reference Dale, Simonoff, Bishop, Eley, Oliver, Price and Plomin1998); Dionne et al. (Reference Dionne, Dale, Boivin and Plomin2003); Hayiou-Thomas et al. (Reference Hayiou-Thomas, Dale and Plomin2012); Petrill et al. (Reference Petrill, Saudino, Wilkerson and Plomin2001). 3 years: Davis et al. (Reference Davis, Haworth and Plomin2009); Gregory et al. (Reference Gregory, Eley, O'Connor and Plomin2004); Iervolino et al. (Reference Iervolino, Hines, Golombok, Rust and Plomin2005); Spinath et al. (Reference Spinath, Price, Dale and Plomin2004). 4 years: Asbury et al. (Reference Asbury, Wachs and Plomin2005); Eley et al. (Reference Eley, Bolton, O'Connor, Perrin, Smith and Plomin2003); Knafo & Plomin (Reference Knafo and Plomin2006); Larsson et al. (Reference Larsson, Viding, Rijsdijk and Plomin2008). 4½ years: Hayiou-Thomas et al. (Reference Hayiou-Thomas, Kovas, Harlaar, Plomin, Bishop and Dale2006); Oliver et al. (Reference Oliver, Dale and Plomin2004); Viding et al. (Reference Viding, Spinath, Price, Bishop, Dale and Plomin2004). 7 years: Arden & Plomin (Reference Arden and Plomin2006); Harlaar et al. (Reference Harlaar, Dale and Plomin2007); Saudino et al. (Reference Saudino, Ronald and Plomin2005); Viding et al. (Reference Viding, Blair, Moffitt and Plomin2005); Walker et al. (Reference Walker, Petrill, Spinath and Plomin2004). 8 years: Greven et al. (Reference Greven, Asherson, Rijsdijk and Plomin2011); Loat et al. (Reference Loat, Haworth, Plomin and Craig2008); Ronald et al. (Reference Ronald, Happe, Bolton, Butcher, Price, Wheelwright and Plomin2006). 9 years: Hanscombe et al. (Reference Hanscombe, Haworth, Davis, Jaffee and Plomin2010); Haworth et al. (Reference Haworth, Dale and Plomin2009a); Oliver et al. (Reference Oliver, Pike and Plomin2008); Trzaskowski et al. (Reference Trzaskowski, Zavos, Haworth, Plomin and Eley2012); Wardle et al. (Reference Wardle, Carnell, Haworth and Plomin2008). 10 years: Asbury et al. (Reference Asbury, Almeida, Hibel, Harlaar and Plomin2008); Davis et al. (Reference Davis, Kovas, Harlaar, Busfield, McMillan, Frances and Plomin2008); Greven et al. (Reference Greven, Harlaar, Kovas, Chamorro-Premuzic and Plomin2009); Kovas et al. (Reference Kovas, Haworth, Harlaar, Petrill, Dale and Plomin2007b). 12 years: Dale et al. (Reference Dale, Harlaar, Hayiou-Thomas and Plomin2010b); Davis et al. (Reference Davis, Haworth, Lewis and Plomin2012); Greven et al. (Reference Greven, Rijsdijk, Asherson and Plomin2012); Haworth et al. (Reference Haworth, Asbury, Dale and Plomin2011); Haworth et al. (Reference Haworth, Kovas, Harlaar, Hayiou-Thomas, Petrill, Dale and Plomin2009b); Robinson et al. (Reference Robinson, Koenen, McCormick, Munir, Hallett, Happé, Plomin and Ronald2011). 14 years: Dale et al. (Reference Dale, Harlaar, Haworth and Plomin2010a); Hanscombe et al. (Reference Hanscombe, Trzaskowski, Haworth, Davis, Dale and Plomin2012); Haworth et al. (in press).

In-person cognitive testing of the large, geographically dispersed TEDS sample has not been feasible; instead, we have used a variety of innovative methods to collect data on the largest sample possible. In early childhood (ages 2, 3, and 4), we used booklets sent to the twins’ parents. These included measures of cognition and language ability, as well as questionnaire measures of behavior for the parents to complete. Our early parent-administered cognitive measures have been validated in a subsample, as reported elsewhere (Oliver et al., Reference Oliver, Dale, Saudino, Petrill, Pike and Plomin2002; Saudino et al., Reference Saudino, Dale, Oliver, Petrill, Richardson, Rutter and Plomin1998). We used telephone testing for the first time when the twins were 7 years of age, which provided data highly comparable to more traditional in-person cognitive testing (Petrill et al., Reference Petrill, Rempell, Oliver and Plomin2002). We used telephone testing again at 12 years for a short reading test. Telephone testing is expensive and time consuming for the broad battery of tests we wanted to include, so we returned to postal booklets for our main assessments at 9 years, which were completed by the twins under the parents’ supervision. In 2004, by the time the twins were 10 years old, we had developed a diverse battery of web-based cognitive tests, including reading, mathematics, and general cognitive ability. These tests have been updated and expanded to include a battery of language and scientific enquiry tests, and used for cognitive testing online when the twins were 12, 14, and 16 years of age. We successfully validated our web-based tests by comparing the results for a subsample with more traditional paper and pencil in-person tests (Haworth et al., Reference Haworth, Harlaar, Kovas, Davis, Oliver, Hayiou-Thomas and Plomin2007). For the most recent web assessments at 16 years, we found an average correlation of 0.72 between online and offline versions of our language, reading, and general cognitive ability tests administered 2 months apart, providing further support for the validity and reliability of our web-based cognitive tests in adolescence.

Although cognition has been a major focus of TEDS, we have also collected questionnaire data on behavioral problems, health, and home and school environments. Parents completed questionnaires at first contact and when the twins were aged 2, 3, 4, 7, 8, 9, 12, 14, and 16 years. These questionnaires included measures of the home environment, parenting, and behavioral problems, such as anxiety, depression, conduct problems, autistic spectrum, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The twins completed questionnaires when they were 9, 12, 14, and 16 years of age, including measures of behavioral problems and environmental experiences in the home and at school. We have also collected academic achievement data and behavioral problem measures from the twins’ teachers at ages 7, 9, 10, 12, and 14 years. However, it became increasingly difficult to get teacher reports when the twins moved to secondary school where they often have a different teacher for each subject. For this reason, we phased out the teacher reports when the twins were 14 years old, and began collecting academic achievement data from the TEDS families, based on the National Curriculum levels reported to them by the schools. We are now in the process of matching TEDS to the UK National Pupil Database, which is a government-administered repository of school quality and achievement in the United Kingdom. The twins and their parents have provided exam results for the General Certificate of Secondary Education (GCSEs), which are the exams taken at 16 years at the end of compulsory schooling. We are now also collecting A-level results for the national exams taken by those twins who remain in education at 18 years in the United Kingdom.

Collaborations

In addition to these main TEDS assessments, there have been subsample studies (e.g., in-home testing of cognition and language for more than 800 pairs at 4½ years), and numerous spin-off projects have been funded. For example, TEDS has provided access to samples for the Environmental Risk (E-Risk) study, spin-offs on eating behavior, anxiety, ADHD, attachment, wellbeing, schizotypy, and myopia to name but a few. The TEDS family of researchers and collaborators continues to grow. Over the past 18 years, more than 25 students have completed PhDs that primarily focused on TEDS data, and many more have included TEDS data as part of their doctoral dissertations. Now ‘students of students’ are completing PhDs, resulting in a multigenerational family of TEDS collaborators. To date, around 300 research papers have been published using TEDS data, involving more than 300 authors. In addition, large portions of the raw data have been released and are freely available for analyses. For example, we have released all of the cognitive and academic achievement data collected on TEDS when the twins were 7, 9, and 10 years of age, to accompany a monograph that summarizes our findings in middle childhood (Kovas et al., Reference Kovas, Haworth, Dale and Plomin2007a).

DNA Studies in TEDS

Only a small proportion of the heritability of common disorders and complex traits has been accounted for despite enormous research efforts (Plomin, Reference Plomin2012). Nonetheless, as some genes are identified, large, multivariate, longitudinal phenomes like TEDS will be increasingly valuable. TEDS collected DNA from more than 14,000 twins (7,000 pairs) and has been at the forefront of molecular genetic research in the cognitive domain, for example, conducting the first genome-wide association (GWA) study of cognitive ability before DNA microarrays were available (Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, Hill, Craig, McGuffin, Purcell, Sham and Owen2001). With the advent of DNA microarrays, we developed the cost-effective SNPMaP (SNP Microarrays and Pooling) method to facilitate the first array-based GWA studies of cognitive abilities (Butcher et al., Reference Butcher, Davis, Craig and Plomin2008; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Butcher, Docherty, Meaburn, Curtis, Simpson and Plomin2010; Docherty et al., Reference Docherty, Davis, Kovas, Meaburn, Dale, Petrill and Plomin2010; Meaburn et al., Reference Meaburn, Harlaar, Craig, Schalkwyk and Plomin2008) and behavioral problems such as psychopathy and autism spectrum disorders (Ronald et al., Reference Ronald, Butcher, Docherty, Davis, Schalkwyk, Craig and Plomin2010a; Viding et al., Reference Viding, Hanscombe, Curtis, Davis, Meaburn and Plomin2010).

TEDS now has 2 million DNA microarray genotypes (Affymetrix 6.0 GeneChip) for 3,500 individuals (one member of a twin pair). Because effect sizes of associations for complex traits are so small, we now know that samples of tens, or even hundreds of thousands are needed to achieve adequate power to detect reliable associations after correcting for multiple testing in GWA analyses. For this reason, TEDS is involved in several collaborations that aim to achieve the necessary power to detect associations of small effect size through meta-analyses across many studies. These DNA microarray genotype data also make it possible to use any genes or polygenic predictors identified in the literature to follow up at the DNA level on our twin results investigating developmental, multivariate, and gene–environment interplay issues (Harlaar et al., Reference Harlaar, Butcher, Meaburn, Sham, Craig and Plomin2005; Haworth et al., Reference Haworth, Carnell, Meaburn, Davis, Plomin and Wardle2008). We are also conducting analyses of copy number variants using the 1 million non-polymorphic DNA markers on the Affymetrix 6.0 GeneChip that are especially useful for these analyses. Another new development that relies on DNA microarray genotype data is genome-wide complex trait analysis, which is a quantitative genetic technique that is based solely on DNA rather than familial relatedness as in twin studies, and which we have shown to verify twin study estimates of heritability for cognitive abilities (Plomin et al., in press). Finally, the next major development is whole-genome sequencing, which can identify all DNA polymorphisms throughout the genome; whole-genome sequencing is ongoing for the 150 most cognitively impaired individuals in TEDS.

Future Directions

The large family of TEDS collaborators provides a lasting legacy for TEDS research. TEDS will continue to capitalize on the longitudinal data collected over the past 18 years, and link this to measured genetic variants and specific measures of the environment to understand the developmental interplay between genes and environments in creating individual differences in cognition and behavior. Twin analyses still have a prominent role to play in understanding nature and nurture (Haworth & Plomin, Reference Haworth and Plomin2010), and a key strength for TEDS is in combining our twin and DNA approaches (e.g., Harlaar et al., Reference Harlaar, Butcher, Meaburn, Sham, Craig and Plomin2005; Haworth et al., Reference Haworth, Carnell, Meaburn, Davis, Plomin and Wardle2008; Plomin et al., in press). It is our hope that TEDS can continue to capitalize on advances in genetic technology; for example, as mentioned above, we already have a project underway in whole genome sequencing; we are also conducting research on biomarkers and a study on DNA methylation in identical twins discordant for autism spectrum disorder. New phenotypic measures collected in emerging adulthood and beyond will add considerable value to our well-characterized sample, facilitating lifespan approaches to individual differences in behavioral and cognitive outcomes.

Acknowledgments

The authors are enormously grateful to the twins, parents, and the twins’ teachers who have supported the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) for the past 18 years. TEDS has also benefited from an exceptionally gifted group of researchers and support staff. The authors would especially like to thank the current TEDS team, including Rachel Ogden, Andrew McMillan, Kathryn Carter, Francesca Lewis, Neil Harvey, Rae Gardner-Kimball, and Louise Webster, for their enduring dedication to the project. TEDS has been continuously supported by the UK Medical Research Council since 1995 (G9424799, G0500079, and now G0901245). The authors' work on environments and academic achievement is also supported by grants from the US National Institutes of Health (HD044454, HD046167, and HD059215). The authors' molecular genetic research has been partly supported by grants from the NIH (HD049861), Wellcome Trust (GR075492, WT084728), Autism Speaks (4743), and John Templeton Foundation (13575). CMAH is supported by a research fellowship from the British Academy; OSPD is supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Fellowship (WT088984); RP is supported by a research professorship from the UK Medical Research Council (G19/2) and a European Research Council Advanced Investigator Award (295366).

References

Arden, R., & Plomin, R. (2006). Sex differences in variance of intelligence across childhood. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 3948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asbury, K., Almeida, D., Hibel, J., Harlaar, N., & Plomin, R. (2008). Clones in the classroom: A daily diary study of the nonshared environmental relationship between monozygotic twin differences in school experience and achievement. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 11, 586595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asbury, K., Wachs, T. D., & Plomin, R. (2005). Environmental moderators of genetic influence on verbal and nonverbal abilities in early childhood. Intelligence, 33, 643661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butcher, L. M., Davis, O. S. P., Craig, I. W., & Plomin, R. (2008). Genome-wide quantitative trait locus association scan of general cognitive ability using pooled DNA and 500K single nucleotide polymorphism microarrays. Genes, Brain and Behaviour, 7, 435446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S., Harlaar, , Haworth, N., Plomin, C. M. A., & , R. (2010a). Two by two: A twin study of second-language acquisition. Psychological Science, 21 (5), 635640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S., Harlaar, N., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., & Plomin, R. (2010b). The etiology of diverse receptive language skills at 12 years. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 982992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S., Simonoff, E., Bishop, D. V. M., Eley, T. C., Oliver, B., Price, T. S., . . . Plomin, R. (1998). Genetic influence on language delay in two-year-old children. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 324328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, O. S. P., Butcher, L. M., Docherty, S. J., Meaburn, E. M., Curtis, C. J. C., Simpson, A., . . . Plomin, R. (2010). A three-stage genome-wide association study of general cognitive ability: Hunting the small effects. Behavior Genetics, 40, 759767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, O. S. P., Haworth, C. M. A., Lewis, C. M., & Plomin, R. (2012). Visual analysis of geocoded twin data puts nature and nurture on the map. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 867874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, O. S. P., Haworth, C. M. A., & Plomin, R. (2009). Dramatic increase in heritability of cognitive development from early to middle childhood: An 8-year longitudinal study of 8,700 pairs of twins. Psychological Science, 20, 13011308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, O. S. P., Kovas, Y., Harlaar, N., Busfield, P., McMillan, A., Frances, J., . . . Plomin, R. (2008). Generalist genes and the Internet generation: Etiology of learning abilities by web testing at age 10. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 7, 455462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., Boivin, M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic evidence for bidirectional effects of early lexical and grammatical development. Child Development, 74, 394412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Docherty, S. J., Davis, O. S. P., Kovas, Y., Meaburn, E. L., Dale, P. S., Petrill, S. A., . . . Plomin, R. (2010). A genome-wide association study identifies multiple loci associated with mathematics ability and disability. Genes, Brain and Behaviour, 9, 234237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eley, T. C., Bolton, D., O'Connor, T. G., Perrin, S., Smith, P., & Plomin, R. (2003). A twin study of anxiety-related behaviours in pre-school children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 945960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregory, A. M., Eley, T. C., O'Connor, T. G., & Plomin, R. (2004). Etiologies of associations between childhood sleep and behavioral problems in a large twin sample. Journal of the American Academic of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 744751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greven, C. U., Asherson, P., Rijsdijk, F. V., & Plomin, R. (2011). A longitudinal twin study on the association between inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive ADHD symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 623632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greven, C. U., Harlaar, N., Kovas, Y., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Plomin, R. (2009). More than just IQ: School achievement is predicted by self-perceived abilities for genetic reasons. Psychological Science, 20, 753762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greven, C. U., Rijsdijk, F. V., Asherson, P., & Plomin, R. (2012). A longitudinal twin study on the association between ADHD symptoms and reading. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53 (3), 234242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanscombe, K. B., Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P., Jaffee, S. R., & Plomin, R. (2010). The nature (and nurture) of children's perceptions of family chaos. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 549553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanscombe, K. B., Trzaskowski, M., Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). Socioeconomic status (SES) and children's intelligence (IQ): In a UK-representative sample SES moderates the environmental, not genetic, effect on IQ. PLoS ONE, 7, e30320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlaar, N., Butcher, L. M., Meaburn, E., Sham, P., Craig, I. W., & Plomin, R. (2005). A behavioural genomic analysis of DNA markers associated with general cognitive ability in 7-year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 10971107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlaar, N., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2007). From learning to read to reading to learn: Substantial and stable genetic influence. Child Development, 78, 116131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haworth, C. M. A., Asbury, K., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2011). Added value measures in education show genetic as well as environmental influence. PLoS ONE, 6, e16006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haworth, C. M. A., Carnell, S., Meaburn, E. L., Davis, O. S. P., Plomin, R., & Wardle, J. (2008). Increasing heritability of BMI and stronger associations with the FTO gene over childhood. Obesity, 16, 26632668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haworth, C. M. A., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2009a). The etiology of science performance: Decreasing heritability and increasing importance of the shared environment from 9 to 12 years of age. Child Development, 80, 662673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P., Hanscombe, K. B., Kovas, Y., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (in press). Understanding the science-learning environment: A genetically sensitive approach. Learning and Individual Differences. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.07.018.Google Scholar
Haworth, C. M. A., Harlaar, N., Kovas, Y., Davis, O. S. P., Oliver, B. R., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., . . . Plomin, R. (2007). Internet cognitive testing of large samples needed in genetic research. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 554563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haworth, C. M. A., Kovas, Y., Harlaar, N., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Petrill, S. A., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2009b). Generalist genes and learning disabilities: A multivariate genetic analysis of low performance in reading, mathematics, language and general cognitive ability in a sample of 8000 12-year-old twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 13181325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haworth, C. M. A., & Plomin, R. (2010). Quantitative genetics in the era of molecular genetics: Learning abilities and disabilities as an example. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 783793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). The etiology of variation in language skills changes with development: A longitudinal twin study of language from 2 to 12 years. Developmental Science, 15, 233249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Kovas, Y., Harlaar, N., Plomin, R., Bishop, D. V. M., & Dale, P. S. (2006). Common aetiology for diverse language skills in 4½-year-old twins. Journal of Child Language, 33, 339368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iervolino, A. C., Hines, M., Golombok, S. E., Rust, J., & Plomin, R. (2005). Genetic and environmental influences on sex-typed behavior during the preschool years. Child Development, 76, 826840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knafo, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Prosocial behavior from early to middle childhood: Genetic and environmental influences on stability and change. Developmental Psychology, 42, 771786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koeppen-Schomerus, G., Eley, T. C., Wolke, D., Gringras, P., & Plomin, R. (2000). The interaction of prematurity with genetic and environmental influences on cognitive development in twins. Journal of Pediatrics, 137, 527533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovas, Y., Haworth, C. M. A., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2007a). The genetic and environmental origins of learning abilities and disabilities in the early school years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72, vii160.Google ScholarPubMed
Kovas, Y., Haworth, C. M. A., Harlaar, N., Petrill, S. A., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2007b). Overlap and specificity of genetic and environmental influences on mathematics and reading disability in 10-year-old twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 914922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, H., Viding, E., Rijsdijk, F. V., & Plomin, R. (2008). Relationships between parental negativity and childhood antisocial behavior over time: A bidirectional effects model in a longitudinal genetically informative design. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 633645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loat, C. S., Haworth, C. M. A., Plomin, R., & Craig, I. W. (2008). A model incorporating potential skewed X-inactivation in MZ girls suggests that X-linked QTLs exist for several social behaviours including autism spectrum disorder. Annals of Human Genetics, 72, 742751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meaburn, E. L., Harlaar, N., Craig, I. W., Schalkwyk, L. C., & Plomin, R. (2008). Quantitative Trait Loci association scan of early reading disability and ability using pooled DNA and 100K SNP microarrays in a sample of 5760 children. Molecular Psychiatry, 13, 729740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliver, B., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2004). Verbal and nonverbal predictors of early language problems: An analysis of twins in early childhood back to infancy. Journal of Child Language, 31, 609631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliver, B., Dale, P. S., Saudino, K. J., Petrill, S. A., Pike, A., & Plomin, R. (2002). The validity of a parent-based assessment of cognitive abilities in 3-year olds. Early Child Development and Care, 172, 337348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliver, B. R., Pike, A., & Plomin, R. (2008). Nonshared environmental influences on teacher-reported behaviour problems: Monozygotic twin differences in perceptions of the classroom. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 646653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliver, B. R., & Plomin, R. (2007). Twins’ Early Development Study (TEDS): A multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems from childhood through adolescence. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 96105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrill, S. A., Rempell, J., Oliver, B., & Plomin, R. (2002). Testing cognitive abilities by telephone in a sample of 6-to 8-year olds. Intelligence, 30, 353360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrill, S. A., Saudino, K. S., Wilkerson, B., & Plomin, R. (2001). Genetic and environmental molarity and modularity of cognitive functioning in 2-year-old twins. Intelligence, 29, 3143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R. (2012). Child development and molecular genetics: 14 years later. Child Development. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01757.x.Google ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., Haworth, C. M. A., Meaburn, E. L., Price, T., Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2, & Davis, O. S. P. (in press). Common DNA markers can account for more than half of the genetic influence on cognitive abilities. Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., Hill, L., Craig, I. W., McGuffin, P., Purcell, S., Sham, P., . . . Owen, M. J. (2001). A genome-wide scan of 1842 DNA markers for allelic associations with general cognitive ability: A five-stage design using DNA pooling and extreme selected groups. Behavior Genetics, 31, 497509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, T. S., Freeman, B., Craig, I., Petrill, S. A., Ebersole, L., & Plomin, R. (2000). Infant zygosity can be assigned by parental report questionnaire data. Twin Research, 3, 129133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, T. S., Grosser, T., Plomin, R., & Jaffee, S. R. (2010). Fetal genotype for the xenobiotic metabolizing enzyme NQO1 influences intrauterine growth among infants whose mothers smoked during pregnancy. Child Development, 81, 101114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, E. B., Koenen, K. C., McCormick, M. C., Munir, K., Hallett, V., Happé, F., Plomin, R., & Ronald, A. (2011). Evidence that autistic traits show the same etiology in the general population and at the quantitative extremes (5%, 2.5% and 1%). Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 11131121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ronald, A., Butcher, L. M., Docherty, S., Davis, O. S. P., Schalkwyk, L. C., Craig, I. W., & Plomin, R. (2010a). A genome-wide association study of social and non-social autistic-like traits in the general population using pooled DNA, 500K SNP microarrays and both community and diagnosed autism replication samples. Behavior Genetics, 40, 3145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ronald, A., Happe, F., Bolton, P., Butcher, L. M., Price, T. S., Wheelwright, S., . . . Plomin, R. (2006). Genetic heterogeneity between the three components of the autism spectrum: A twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 691699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ronald, A., Happé, F., Dworzynski, K., Bolton, P., & Plomin, R. (2010b). Exploring the relationship between prenatal and neonatal complications and later autistic-like features in a representative community sample of twins. Child Development, 81, 166182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saudino, K. J., Dale, P. S., Oliver, B., Petrill, S. A., Richardson, V., Rutter, M., . . . Plomin, R. (1998). The validity of parent-based assessment of the cognitive abilities of 2-year-olds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 349363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saudino, K. J., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2005). The etiology of behavior problems in 7-year-old twins: Substantial genetic influence and negligible shared environmental influence for parent ratings and ratings by same and different teachers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 113130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinath, F. M., Price, T. S., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2004). The genetic and environmental origins of language disability and ability. Child Development, 75, 445454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trouton, A., Spinath, F. M., & Plomin, R. (2002). Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): A multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems in childhood. Twin Research, 5, 444448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trzaskowski, M., Zavos, H. M. S., Haworth, C. M. A, Plomin, R., & Eley, T. C. (2012). Stable genetic influence on anxiety-related behaviours across middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 8594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, E., Blair, R. J. R., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2005). Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 592597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, E., Hanscombe, K., Curtis, C., Davis, O. S. P., Meaburn, E. L., & Plomin, R. (2010). In search of genes associated with risk for psychopathic tendencies in children: A two-stage genome-wide association study of pooled DNA. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 780788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, E., Spinath, F. M., Price, T. S., Bishop, D. V. M., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2004). Genetic and environmental influence on language impairment in 4-year-old same-sex and opposite-sex twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 315325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, A., Maher, J., Coulthard, M., Goddard, E., & Thomas, M. (2001). Living in Britain: Results from the 2000/2001 General Household Survey. London: TSO.Google Scholar
Walker, S. O., Petrill, S. A., Spinath, F. M., & Plomin, R. (2004). Nature, nurture and academic achievement: A twin study of teacher assessments of 7-year-olds. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 323342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wardle, J., Carnell, S., Haworth, C. M. A., & Plomin, R. (2008). Evidence for a strong genetic influence on childhood adiposity despite the force of the obesogenic environment. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 87, 398404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Representativeness of the TEDS Sample at First Contact, Early Childhood, Middle Childhood, and Adolescence

Figure 1

FIGURE 1 Geographical distribution of TEDS. Note: Plotting the local density of 10,000 TEDS families on a map of the United Kingdom — using a color scale from dark red (low density) to yellow (high density) — shows that they are distributed in the same way as the general population in England and Wales. Only a small proportion of the initial sample has migrated outside the United Kingdom (253 families). For these families, the main destinations have been Australia (44), New Zealand (33), the United States (33), Canada (30), the Republic of Ireland (21), and France (20). The underlying map was provided by Ordnance Survey OpenData (www.ordnancesurvey.co.uk).

Figure 2

FIGURE 2 Timeline of TEDS assessments. This figure represents the main TEDS assessments and major measures. More detailed information is available from the TEDS team on request. Following are examples at each age of papers reporting twin analyses of TEDS data. The papers were selected from over 300 in order to represent the diversity of authors and measures in TEDS. Examples of other TEDS twin analyses and DNA analyses are included in the text. First contact: Koeppen-Schomerus et al. (2000); Price et al. (2010); Ronald et al. (2010b). 2 years: Dale et al. (1998); Dionne et al. (2003); Hayiou-Thomas et al. (2012); Petrill et al. (2001). 3 years: Davis et al. (2009); Gregory et al. (2004); Iervolino et al. (2005); Spinath et al. (2004). 4 years: Asbury et al. (2005); Eley et al. (2003); Knafo & Plomin (2006); Larsson et al. (2008). 4½ years: Hayiou-Thomas et al. (2006); Oliver et al. (2004); Viding et al. (2004). 7 years: Arden & Plomin (2006); Harlaar et al. (2007); Saudino et al. (2005); Viding et al. (2005); Walker et al. (2004). 8 years: Greven et al. (2011); Loat et al. (2008); Ronald et al. (2006). 9 years: Hanscombe et al. (2010); Haworth et al. (2009a); Oliver et al. (2008); Trzaskowski et al. (2012); Wardle et al. (2008). 10 years: Asbury et al. (2008); Davis et al. (2008); Greven et al. (2009); Kovas et al. (2007b). 12 years: Dale et al. (2010b); Davis et al. (2012); Greven et al. (2012); Haworth et al. (2011); Haworth et al. (2009b); Robinson et al. (2011). 14 years: Dale et al. (2010a); Hanscombe et al. (2012); Haworth et al. (in press).