Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T12:38:59.918Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Resting-state functional connectivity patterns associated with childhood maltreatment in a large bicentric cohort of adults with and without major depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2022

Janik Goltermann*
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Nils Ralf Winter
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Susanne Meinert
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany University of Münster, Institute for Translational Neuroscience, Münster, Germany
Lisa Sindermann
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Hannah Lemke
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Elisabeth J. Leehr
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Dominik Grotegerd
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Alexandra Winter
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Katharina Thiel
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Lena Waltemate
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Fabian Breuer
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Jonathan Repple
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Marius Gruber
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Maike Richter
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Vanessa Teckentrup
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
Nils B. Kroemer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany Department of Psychiatry & Psychotherapy, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany
Katharina Brosch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
Tina Meller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
Julia-Katharina Pfarr
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
Kai Gustav Ringwald
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
Frederike Stein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
Walter Heindel
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Department of Clinical Radiology, Münster, Germany
Andreas Jansen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
Tilo Kircher
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
Igor Nenadić
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
Udo Dannlowski
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
Nils Opel
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany University of Münster, Interdisciplinary Centre for Clinical Research (IZKF), Münster, Germany Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Jena University Hospital, Jena, Germany
Tim Hahn
Affiliation:
University of Münster, Institute for Translational Psychiatry, Münster, Germany
*
Author for correspondence: Janik Goltermann, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Childhood maltreatment (CM) represents a potent risk factor for major depressive disorder (MDD), including poorer treatment response. Altered resting-state connectivity in the fronto-limbic system has been reported in maltreated individuals. However, previous results in smaller samples differ largely regarding localization and direction of effects.

Methods

We included healthy and depressed samples [n = 624 participants with MDD; n = 701 healthy control (HC) participants] that underwent resting-state functional MRI measurements and provided retrospective self-reports of maltreatment using the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. A-priori defined regions of interest [ROI; amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)] were used to calculate seed-to-voxel connectivities.

Results

No significant associations between maltreatment and resting-state connectivity of any ROI were found across MDD and HC participants and no interaction effect with diagnosis became significant. Investigating MDD patients only yielded maltreatment-associated increased connectivity between the amygdala and dorsolateral frontal areas [pFDR < 0.001; η2partial = 0.050; 95%-CI (0.023–0.085)]. This effect was robust across various sensitivity analyses and was associated with concurrent and previous symptom severity. Particularly strong amygdala-frontal associations with maltreatment were observed in acutely depressed individuals [n = 264; pFDR < 0.001; η2partial = 0.091; 95%-CI (0.038–0.166)). Weaker evidence – not surviving correction for multiple ROI analyses – was found for altered supracallosal ACC connectivity in HC individuals associated with maltreatment.

Conclusions

The majority of previous resting-state connectivity correlates of CM could not be replicated in this large-scale study. The strongest evidence was found for clinically relevant maltreatment associations with altered adult amygdala-dorsolateral frontal connectivity in depression. Future studies should explore the relevance of this pathway for a maltreated subgroup of MDD patients.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Childhood maltreatment (CM) has been identified as a major risk factor for the development of various mental health disorders, particularly major depressive disorder (MDD) (Nemeroff, Reference Nemeroff2016). Depressive patients with maltreatment experiences show a higher likelihood of a chronic disease trajectory as well as poorer treatment outcomes, pointing to a distinct psychopathological phenotype (Nanni, Uher, & Danese, Reference Nanni, Uher and Danese2012; Opel et al., Reference Opel, Redlich, Dohm, Zaremba, Goltermann, Repple and Dannlowski2019; Teicher & Samson, Reference Teicher and Samson2013). Understanding the neurobiological consequences of CM as risk factor is crucial to develop early and effective therapeutic interventions (Teicher, Samson, Anderson, & Ohashi, Reference Teicher, Samson, Anderson and Ohashi2016). Against this backdrop, previous research indicates that extreme or enduring stress during childhood leads to long-lasting neurobiological changes in the stress-processing system (Nemeroff, Reference Nemeroff2016). These changes might be marked by an altered brain structure and function, especially affecting prefrontal and limbic areas (Dannlowski et al., Reference Dannlowski, Kugel, Huber, Stuhrmann, Redlich, Grotegerd and Suslow2013, Reference Dannlowski, Stuhrmann, Beutelmann, Zwanzger, Lenzen, Grotegerd and Kugel2012; Frodl et al., Reference Frodl, Janowitz, Schmaal, Tozzi, Dobrowolny, Stein and Grabe2017; Heany et al., Reference Heany, Groenewold, Uhlmann, Dalvie, Stein and Brooks2018; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Schmitt, Sauder, Stein, Yüksel, Grotegerd and Jansen2020; Lim, Radua, & Rubia, Reference Lim, Radua and Rubia2014; Opel et al., Reference Opel, Redlich, Dohm, Zaremba, Goltermann, Repple and Dannlowski2019, Reference Opel, Zwanzger, Redlich, Grotegerd, Dohm, Arolt and Dannlowski2016; Popovic et al., Reference Popovic, Ruef, Dwyer, Antonucci, Eder, Sanfelici and Piccin2020; Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Samson, Anderson and Ohashi2016; Tozzi et al., Reference Tozzi, Garczarek, Janowitz, Stein, Wittfeld and Dobrowolny2020). Paralleling effects of CM, structural and functional neural correlates of depression have been localized in similar regions such as the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the hippocampus or the amygdala (Gray, Müller, Eickhoff, & Fox, Reference Gray, Müller, Eickhoff and Fox2020). This observed overlap in neural correlates of depression and CM has nourished the interpretation that brain alterations could play an important role in the link between CM and depression (Meinert et al., Reference Meinert, Repple, Nenadic, Krug, Jansen, Grotegerd and Dannlowski2019; Opel et al., Reference Opel, Redlich, Zwanzger, Grotegerd, Arolt, Heindel and Dannlowski2014; Teicher & Samson, Reference Teicher and Samson2013).

In addition to alterations in gray matter structure and function, research on brain connectivity and its putative role for psychopathology has received increased attention in recent years (Kovner, Oler, & Kalin, Reference Kovner, Oler and Kalin2019). Particularly, the connectivity between frontal and limbic regions has been implied in emotion regulation, thus taking a central role in neurobiological models of affective disorders (Dixon, Thiruchselvam, Todd, & Christoff, Reference Dixon, Thiruchselvam, Todd and Christoff2017). Studies investigating resting-state functional connectivity (i.e. synchronization of the spontaneous fluctuations of activity between brain regions during rest) support this notion, linking patterns of functional connectivity to CM (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Dai, Peng, Tan, Ding, He and Zhang2014), as well as depression (Iwabuchi et al., Reference Iwabuchi, Krishnadas, Li, Auer, Radua and Palaniyappan2015; Mulders, van Eijndhoven, Schene, Beckmann, & Tendolkar, Reference Mulders, van Eijndhoven, Schene, Beckmann and Tendolkar2015). More specifically, MDD patients have shown lower subgenual ACC connectivity and higher amygdala connectivity with other subcortical areas (Oathes, Patenaude, Schatzberg, & Etkin, Reference Oathes, Patenaude, Schatzberg and Etkin2015). In addition, functional connectivity of different frontal and limbic regions has been shown to predict antidepressant treatment outcomes (Chin Fatt et al., Reference Chin Fatt, Jha, Cooper, Fonzo, South, Grannemann and Trivedi2020; van Waarde et al., Reference van Waarde, Scholte, Van Oudheusden, Verwey, Denys and van Wingen2015).

Paralleling resting-state findings in depression research, altered fronto-limbic functional connectivity has also been observed in individuals with a history of CM, both in clinical and non-clinical samples. In a community sample of n = 64 late-adolescents a higher burden of CM experiences was associated with lower connectivity of amygdala and hippocampus seed regions, specifically with the sgACC (Herringa et al., Reference Herringa, Birn, Ruttle, Burghy, Stodola, Davidson and Essex2013). In contrast, a specific investigation of CM associations with the sgACC in a sample of healthy adolescents (n = 68) yielded a weaker positive connectivity with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), supramarginal gyrus and cuneus, but failed to replicate an effect involving the amygdala or hippocampus (Hoffmann et al., Reference Hoffmann, Viding, Puetz, Gerin, Sethi, Rankin and McCrory2018). Others have shown that CM is negatively associated with the connectivity of amygdala and hippocampus seed regions with the prefrontal cortex (PFC), including portions of the ACC (n = 27) (Birn, Patriat, Phillips, Germain, & Herringa, Reference Birn, Patriat, Phillips, Germain and Herringa2014). Similarly, van der Werff et al., reported on reduced positive amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex connectivity associated with emotional maltreatment in a mixed sample with and without psychiatric disorders (n = 88) (van der Werff et al., Reference van der Werff, Pannekoek, Veer, van Tol, Aleman, Veltman and van der Wee2013). In contrast, Kaiser et al., found increased negative connectivity between amygdala and dlPFC but no connectivity alterations of ACC or hippocampus associated with CM in a sample of n = 70 adult individuals with affective and anxiety disorders (Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Clegg, Goer, Pechtel, Beltzer, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2018). To sum up, altered fronto-limbic functional connectivity in individuals with CM experiences could represent a neurobiological link to psychiatric symptoms (Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Samson, Anderson and Ohashi2016).

However, despite some degree of replicability of altered connectivity in frontal and limbic areas (most prominently amygdala, hippocampus, and PFC regions including the ACC), findings differ regarding the specific regions involved. Even within similar regions, inconsistencies with respect to the direction of CM effects are evident in the current literature. While the findings cited above indicate a negative association between CM and fronto-limbic connectivity (resulting either in a decreased correlation or increased anticorrelation between brain regions), others have reported on opposite effects (Jedd et al., Reference Jedd, Hunt, Cicchetti, Hunt, Cowell, Rogosch and Thomas2015; Thomason et al., Reference Thomason, Marusak, Tocco, Vila, Mcgarragle and Rosenberg2015). Furthermore, it should be noted that some studies have failed to replicate fronto-limbic connectivity alterations associated with CM altogether and tentatively point to effects located elsewhere – e.g. connectivity alterations of the cerebellum, precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex and fusiform gyrus (Boccadoro et al., Reference Boccadoro, Siugzdaite, Hudson, Maeyens, Hamme and Mueller2019; Philip et al., Reference Philip, Sweet, Tyrka, Price, Bloom and Carpenter2013b).

Heterogeneity within the current literature may result from several methodological aspects. First, study samples differed massively regarding the degree of maltreatment experiences, presence of clinical diagnoses, as well as in operationalizations of CM. While most of the above studies conduct cutoff-based group comparisons of CM v. non-CM samples (Boccadoro et al., Reference Boccadoro, Siugzdaite, Hudson, Maeyens, Hamme and Mueller2019; Hoffmann et al., Reference Hoffmann, Viding, Puetz, Gerin, Sethi, Rankin and McCrory2018; Jedd et al., Reference Jedd, Hunt, Cicchetti, Hunt, Cowell, Rogosch and Thomas2015; Philip et al., Reference Philip, Kuras, Valentine, Sweet, Tyrka, Price and Carpenter2013a; Thomason et al., Reference Thomason, Marusak, Tocco, Vila, Mcgarragle and Rosenberg2015; van der Werff et al., Reference van der Werff, Pannekoek, Veer, van Tol, Aleman, Veltman and van der Wee2013), others have employed a continuous rather than categorical operationalization, thus expressing CM severity (Birn et al., Reference Birn, Patriat, Phillips, Germain and Herringa2014; Herringa et al., Reference Herringa, Birn, Ruttle, Burghy, Stodola, Davidson and Essex2013; Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Clegg, Goer, Pechtel, Beltzer, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2018). It is yet unclear how such differences in operationalizations of CM could impact the findings, although continuous conceptualizations may implicate higher psychometric qualities (Goltermann et al., Reference Goltermann, Meinert, Hülsmann, Dohm, Grotegerd, Redlich and Dannlowski2021).

Second, although some studies include psychiatric as well as healthy participants, a systematic investigation of the role of MDD to CM-related functional connectivity effects is yet missing. Likewise, the current clinical state of individuals with a psychiatric disorder could pose another source of heterogeneity, as evidence suggests that resting-state connectivity differs largely across acute and remitted bipolar (Wang, Feng, Mitchell, Wang, & Si, Reference Wang, Feng, Mitchell, Wang and Si2020a, Reference Wang, Gao, Tang, Lu, Zhang, Bu and Huang2020b) and unipolar depressive patients (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Feng, Mitchell, Wang and Si2020a). Third, previous studies are based on small samples (with the vast majority reporting on N < 70) and are likely to be underpowered considering the common small effect sizes in the neuroimaging domain (Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Knodt, Ireland, Morris, Poulton, Ramrakha and Hariri2020). Despite some reports of large effect sizes in single studies (Birn et al., Reference Birn, Patriat, Phillips, Germain and Herringa2014), small sample sizes suggest that those could be overestimated (Kühberger, Fritz, & Scherndl, Reference Kühberger, Fritz and Scherndl2014). Therefore, it is crucial to secure previous findings in a sufficiently large sample to reevaluate our knowledge on the effects of CM on functional connectivity within the adult brain.

In this study, we therefore aim to shed light on the current heterogeneity of findings and investigate the association between CM and resting-state functional connectivity in an unprecedented large adult cohort of individuals both with and without MDD. Based on the literature presented above, we test the hypotheses of CM-associated altered resting-state connectivity of the (1) amygdala, (2) hippocampus, and (3) ACC as seed regions of interest (ROIs), investigating their whole-brain voxel-wise connectivity. We conducted joint as well as subgroup analyses for healthy control (HC) and MDD samples to compare effects with previously published findings obtained from clinical and non-clinical samples. Further, we explore the impact of differential CM operationalizations as well as current remission status on resting-state effects.

Materials and methods

Participants and design

A total of n = 624 MDD and n = 701 HC were included in the current analysis (age 18–65, mean age 36.17 years, s.d. 13.06; 65.40% female), as part of the Marburg Münster Affective Disorders Cohort study (MACS) that has been extensively described elsewhere (Kircher et al., Reference Kircher, Wöhr, Nenadic, Schwarting, Schratt, Alferink and Dannlowski2018; Vogelbacher et al., Reference Vogelbacher, Möbius, Sommer, Schuster, Dannlowski, Kircher and Bopp2018). Clinical and demographic sample characteristics are displayed in online Supplementary Table S1. For exclusion criteria, see the online Supplementary Material. Participants were recruited at the Departments of Psychiatry at the university hospitals in Münster and Marburg, Germany. They received financial compensation and gave written and informed consent. The MACS was approved by the ethics committees of the Medical Faculties, University of Marburg (AZ: 07/14) and University of Münster (AZ: 2014-422-b-S).

Clinical and maltreatment measures

The German 28-item version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) (Wingenfeld et al., Reference Wingenfeld, Spitzer, Mensebach, Grabe, Hill, Gast and Driessen2010) was administered to assess CM experienced before age 18. The CTQ is a retrospective self-report questionnaire assessing different types of CM. The questionnaire has been widely used across a variety of countries for the assessment of CM experiences in clinical and non-clinical samples and has been extensively validated in different languages (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Fink, Handelsman, Foote, Lovejoy, Wenzel and Ruggiero1994, Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia and Zule2003; Karos, Niederstrasser, Abidi, Bernstein, & Bader, Reference Karos, Niederstrasser, Abidi, Bernstein and Bader2014; Spinhoven et al., Reference Spinhoven, Penninx, Hickendorff, van Hemert, Bernstein, Elzinga and Elzinga2014; Viola et al., Reference Viola, Salum, Kluwe-Schiavon, Sanvicente-Vieira, Levandowski, Grassi-Oliveira and Grassi-Oliveira2016). The sum score can be used to represent an overall maltreatment severity. Categorical cutoffs defined by Walker and colleagues were used to categorize participants into maltreated and non-maltreated for additional analyses (see online Supplementary Material) (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Gelfand, Katon, Koss, Van Korff, Bernstein and Russo1999). Information on the categorical prevalence of CM subtypes across HC and MDD participants can be found in online Supplementary Table S3.

Psychiatric diagnoses or the lack thereof was confirmed by trained personnel using the Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (SCID-I) (Wittchen, Wunderlich, Gruschwitz, & Zaudig, Reference Wittchen, Wunderlich, Gruschwitz and Zaudig1997). According to the SCID-I, participants were classified as HC (not fulfilling lifetime criteria for any psychiatric diagnosis) and MDD (fulfilling lifetime MDD criteria). Further, a subset of lifetime MDD participants that additionally fulfilled the criteria of a current depression were denoted as acute MDD (n = 265). A detailed description of psychiatric comorbidities based on the SCID-I can be found in online Supplementary Table S2. For clinical characterization current symptomatology was assessed using the self-report Beck depression inventory (BDI) (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, Reference Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock and Erbaugh1961), the global level of functioning was assessed by trained raters based on the DSM-IV (Saß, Wittchen, Zaudig, & Houben, Reference Saß, Wittchen, Zaudig and Houben2003), and the previous course of disease was assessed via self-reported number of psychiatric hospitalizations.

Resting-state fMRI acquisition and preprocessing

MRI data were acquired using a 3T whole body MRI scanner (Marburg: Tim Trio, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany; Münster: Prisma, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). Due to the two scanner sites and a change of the scanner body-coil in Marburg during recruitment, two dummy variables for three different scanner specifications were used as covariates in all analyses. Resting-state functional images were acquired over a duration of eight minutes (eyes closed). Preprocessing was done with the CONN (v18b) MATLAB toolbox using the default volume-based MNI preprocessing pipeline (https://web.conn-toolbox.org/) (Whitfield-Gabrieli & Nieto-Castanon, Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon2012) and SPM12 (Welcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Additional information on scanner sequence, preprocessing parameters and quality check procedures are presented in the online Supplementary Materials.

Selection of regions of interest

Based on the previous literature we identified the amygdala, hippocampus, and ACC as most prominent regions with repeatedly reported CM effects on resting-state connectivity. As previous findings suggest a functional distinction of different parts of the ACC (Rolls, Huang, Lin, Feng, & Joliot, Reference Rolls, Huang, Lin, Feng and Joliot2020), we applied distinct region of interest (ROI) masks for the subgenual, pregenual and the supracallosal ACC (sgACC, pgACC, scACC). We decided against the definition of further PFC ROIs due to substantial uncertainty regarding the exact localization of PFC effects based on previous findings outlined above (involving medial, dorsolateral and orbitofrontal regions of the PFC, overall covering a large area). Thus, we limited our ROIs to amygdala, hippocampus and ACC regions in a seed-to-voxel approach, which however is also sensitive to identify potential connectivity effects between selected ROIs and PFC regions.

The outlined ROIs were taken from the Automated Anatomical Labelling Atlas version 3 (AAL3) (Rolls et al., Reference Rolls, Huang, Lin, Feng and Joliot2020) to create ROI masks for seed-based analyses. In order to accommodate for occasional lateralized resting-state findings (Hoffmann et al., Reference Hoffmann, Viding, Puetz, Gerin, Sethi, Rankin and McCrory2018; Philip et al., Reference Philip, Valentine, Sweet, Tyrka, Price and Carpenter2014; van der Werff et al., Reference van der Werff, Pannekoek, Veer, van Tol, Aleman, Veltman and van der Wee2013), we applied each ROI for the left and right hemisphere separately, resulting in a total of 10 ROIs (ROIs are depicted in online Supplementary Fig. S1). For each seed ROI the fMRI signal was averaged across voxels resulting in one signal time series per ROI. Pearson correlations between each seed ROI time series and each whole-brain voxel time series were conducted as a measure of functional connectivity (seed-to-voxel analysis).

Statistical analyses

A general linear model was applied to investigate the main effects of CTQ sum, controlling for age, sex, and two scanner condition dummies. In a first step, the pooled sample (HC and MDD) was investigated for the main effects of CM, as well as an interaction of CM with MDD diagnosis. Within the pooled sample, the diagnosis was added as an additional control variable. Subsequently, HC and MDD samples were investigated separately for two reasons: Firstly, this was done due to substantial differences in CTQ sum distributions across HC and MDD subgroups with regard to mean, variance and skewness (see online Supplementary Fig. S2), thus raising concerns about the validity of joint analysis of both heterogeneous subgroups in one statistical model. We further ran separate analyses for a subset of the MDD sample currently fulfilling the criteria for an acute depressive episode to account for the findings of differing resting-state connectivity during the presence of concurrent affective symptoms outlined above (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Feng, Mitchell, Wang and Si2020a, Reference Wang, Gao, Tang, Lu, Zhang, Bu and Huang2020b). The second reason for subgroup analysis is that it enables us to control for medication effects within the MDD group that may influence limbic connectivity (McCabe & Mishor, Reference McCabe and Mishor2011). Follow-up analyses additionally controlling for psychiatric medication, educational attainment, and motion parameters, as well as exploring the role of sex and maltreatment subtypes were conducted for effects significant at a Bonferroni-corrected level at p < 0.05. Psychiatric medication load was operationalized by calculating a medication index incorporating the number and dosage of concurrent medication (described in more detail within the supplements), educational attainment was operationalized as the reported years of education, and the individual mean motion was used as a motion covariate. The CTQ subscale scores were separately used as predictors instead of CTQ sum to probe CM subtype effects.

Further, exploratory analyses with different CM operationalizations were conducted and are reported within the online Supplementary Results (investigating categorical definitions and extreme forms of CM).

For connectivity analyses a combination of a False Discovery Rate (FDR)-corrected cluster threshold of p < 0.05 and an uncorrected voxel threshold of p < 0.001 were used as adjunctive significance criteria. Effects were tested using two-sided contrasts due to inconsistent findings regarding the direction of CM associations with resting-state connectivity. FDR-corrected cluster-size p values and partial η2 as cluster-level effect size measure are reported with all significant results. Due to non-normality of the CTQ data we calculated non-parametric bias corrected accelerated bootstrapped confidence intervals for all effect sizes (N = 1000 bootstrap samples). This method accounts for the skewness of the bootstrapped data and has been shown to produce robust and valid confidence intervals under non-normality circumstances (Davison & Hinkley, Reference Davison and Hinkley1997; Kelley, Reference Kelley2005). In addition to within-analysis cluster-size FDR-correction, we further computed and report Bonferroni-corrected p values controlling for the false-positive rate across the number of ROIs (implying correction for ten tests).

Achieved statistical sensitivity (i.e. the minimum required effect size to detect a main effect of CTQ) was computed for the available samples using g*Power software (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Lang and Buchner2007). For a statistical power of 95% and α = 0.001, required effect sizes were η 2partial = 0.018 for the whole sample, η 2partial = 0.034 for the HC sample, η 2partial = 0.038 for the lifetime MDD sample, and η 2partial = 0.086 for the acute MDD sample (details within the supplements).

Results

Resting-state functional connectivity correlates of childhood maltreatment

Pooled clinical and non-clinical sample

Testing a CTQ sum effect in the whole sample (pooling MDD and HC participants together) yielded no significant suprathreshold clusters for any of the ten seed ROIs, neither with nor without multiple-ROI correction. Testing an interaction effect between CTQ sum and diagnosis yielded no significant effects for any of the ten seed ROIs on a Bonferroni-corrected level. Applying a more lenient threshold without correcting for multiple ROI analyses yielded an interaction effect on the connectivity between the left sgACC and a cluster in the right superior temporal region (p FDR = 0.018), but no interaction effect for any of the other nine ROIs.

Sample with lifetime MDD diagnosis

Within lifetime MDD participants, higher CTQ sum scores were associated with higher positive connectivity of the right amygdala ROI with a single cluster in the dlPFC (pFDR < 0.001, located mainly within the right superior and middle frontal gyrus, corresponding to Brodmann areas 8, 6 and 9, Table 1). This effect remained significant when applying Bonferroni correction for multiple ROI analyses.

Table 1. Significant clusters with main effects of continuous childhood maltreatment operationalization (CTQ sum) in ROI-based seed-to-voxel analyses

HC, healthy control; MDD, major depressive disorder; CTQ, childhood trauma questionnaire; CM, childhood maltreatment; sc, supracallosal; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; L, left; R, right; CI, confidence interval.

Note. FDR-corrected cluster-size p values are presented with and without additional Bonferroni correction. Results in bold font are still significant after Bonferroni correction across multiple region of interest analyses. The direction of effects indicates if the association between CTQ sum and seed-to-voxel connectivity is positive (higher positive or less negative connectivity with higher CTQ sum scores) or negative (higher negative or less positive connectivity with higher CTQ sum scores). Bootstrapped (N = 1000 samples) bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa) 95% confidence intervals are presented for all effect sizes.

Connectivity of the right amygdala with two additional clusters in the right fusiform gyrus (pFDR = 0.036) and bilateral middle cingulate cortex (pFDR = 0.036) were also positively associated with CTQ sum, but did not survive Bonferroni correction for multiple ROI analyses (Table 1). Effect size estimates of the positive relationship between CTQ sum and the observed amygdala connectivities ranged from η 2partial = 0.032 to 0.050 (Table 1). CM-associated amygdala connectivities are presented in Fig. 1. No significant associations were observed in this sample between CTQ sum and the connectivity of the left amygdala or any hippocampus or ACC ROI (also not for the left sgACC where the interaction with the diagnosis was found in the pooled sample).

Fig. 1. Clusters with altered connectivity to the right amygdala associated with CTQ sum in lifetime MDD participants. (a) Right amygdala seed region of interest (ROI) presented in red. (b) Significant clusters (before Bonferroni correction) with altered connectivity associated with maltreatment. Slice position is displayed using MNI-coordinates. (c) Scatter plots with individual CTQ sum scores and connectivity values between seed ROI and significant clusters.

As the effect of CTQ sum on right amygdala connectivity to the dlPFC cluster in the lifetime MDD sample was the only effect among all primary planned analyses that survived Bonferroni-correction for multiple comparisons, we followed up on this main finding with additional robustness analyses. This finding was not qualitatively changed when controlling for additional covariates (medication index, educational attainment, mean motion), applying Threshold-free Cluster Enhancement as an alternate approach to control for false-positives, or when excluding participants with a comorbid eating disorder, substance use disorder, or psychotic disorder (see online Supplementary Results). On a descriptive level, the largest effect sizes for the amygdala-dlPFC connectivity were observed for the emotional abuse and physical neglect subscales, with the lowest effect size obtained for sexual abuse (see supplements, online Supplementary Table S4). CTQ subscales were strongly correlated (supplements, online Supplementary Table S9). In order to investigate the robustness of the finding of altered amygdala connectivity we further applied an alternate more conservative motion thresholding, resulting in a reduced sample of n = 569 MDD participants with at least 5 min of non-scrubbed resting-state scans. This alternate analysis yielded three clusters with a positive effect of CTQ sum on right amygdala connectivity: a cluster in the left hippocampal and temporal regions, a cluster left and right middle cingulate cortex, precuneus and paracentral lobule as well as one cluster located in the right (para)hippocampal and fusiform region. However, the cluster in the right dlPFC did not reach significance using this alternate motion thresholding (supplements, online Supplementary Table S8).

In an exploratory analysis in the MDD subsample displaying acute depressive symptomatology (n = 264), the positive relationship between the CTQ sum and right amygdala connectivity with the reported cluster in the dlPFC was confirmed (pFDR < 0.001, η2partial = 0.091). This effect also survived the Bonferroni correction.

Sample without a history of psychiatric disorders

In HC, no CM effects on ROI connectivity survived Bonferroni correction. However, applying a more lenient significance threshold without correcting for multiple ROI analyses yielded an association of CTQ sum with higher positive connectivity of the right scACC ROI and three clusters in the bilateral temporal and superior frontal gyrus (left temporal: pFDR = 0.005, frontal: pFDR = 0.008, right temporal: pFDR = 0.009, Table 1, Fig. 2), as well as with higher negative connectivity between the left hippocampus ROI and a cluster in the cerebellum (pFDR = 0.049, Fig. 3). CTQ sum accounted for about three percent of the variance in reported ROI-cluster connectivities (η 2partial = 0.030 to 0.038, Table 1). CTQ sum showed no significant connectivity associations in this sample with the right hippocampus, either amygdala ROI or any of the ACC ROIs (also not for the left sgACC where the interaction was found).

Fig. 2. Clusters with altered connectivity to the scACC associated with CTQ sum in healthy controls. (a) Right scACC seed region of interest (ROI) presented in red. (b) Significant clusters (before Bonferroni correction) with altered connectivity associated with maltreatment. Slice position is displayed using MNI-coordinates. (c) Scatter plots with individual CTQ sum scores and connectivity values between seed ROI and significant clusters.

Fig. 3. Clusters with altered connectivity to the left hippocampus associated with CTQ sum in healthy controls. (a) Left hippocampus seed region of interest (ROI) presented in red. (b) Significant clusters (before Bonferroni correction) with altered connectivity associated with maltreatment. Slice position is displayed using MNI-coordinates. (c) Scatter plot with individual CTQ sum scores and connectivity values between seed ROI and significant cluster.

Connectivity analyses using different maltreatment operationalizations

Categorical group comparisons of cutoff-defined CM v. non-CM and between groups with extreme CM v. non-CM were mostly consistent with CTQ sum analyses, although overall the CTQ sum analyses seemed somewhat more sensitive to detect CM effects. As for the CTQ sum analyses, other operationalizations of CM yielded no main effects when pooling the sample. Occasional main effects were found within subgroup analyses when applying a lenient significance threshold without correction for multiple ROI analyses. With additional Bonferroni correction none of the combinations between ROIs and CM operationalizations yielded significant results. Results are presented in the online Supplementary Table S6.

Clinical correlates of maltreatment-associated fronto-limbic connectivity in depression

Exploratory analyses were conducted to investigate the potential clinical relevance of CM-related connectivity effects in the MDD sample. To this end we extracted individual connectivity values of the amygdala-dlPFC cluster significantly associated with CTQ sum in our analysis and correlated them with measures of acute illness severity and previous course of disorder. Higher connectivity between the amygdala and the frontal cluster was significantly associated with higher concurrent depression severity [BDI, rho = 0.141, BCa 95%-CI (0.061–0.219), p < 0.001, n = 616], lower global levels of functioning [GAF, rho = −0.152, BCa 95%-CI (−0.058 to −0.258), p = 0.001, n = 497), and a higher number of previous psychiatric hospitalizations (rho = 0.102, BCa 95%-CI (0.023–0.174), p = 0.011, n = 615] (Fig. 4). Further, higher connectivity between the amygdala and the right fusiform cluster was associated with higher BDI scores [rho = 0.090, BCa 95%-CI (0.003–0.173), p = 0.026, n = 616]. All other correlations between CM-associated amygdala connectivity to the fusiform cluster, as well as to the middle cingulate gyrus with clinical measures were non-significant (all p > 0.079).

Fig. 4. Scatter plots of associations between individual amygdala-frontal connectivity and clinical measures within the MDD sample. Individual amygdala seed connectivities with frontal cluster significantly associated with childhood maltreatment are displayed on the y-axis. BDI, Beck depression inventory; GAF, global assessment of functioning.

Discussion

In this study, we present findings on fronto-limbic functional connectivity in adults with a history of CM in an unprecedented large sample of healthy and depressive individuals. While the majority of previous resting-state connectivity correlates of maltreatment could not be replicated in this study, subgroup analyses suggest that maltreatment experiences could be associated with higher fronto-limbic connectivity in MDD patients. However, our findings also indicate that these correlates of maltreatment are more subtle than suggested by earlier studies and do not generalize across clinical and non-clinical samples, potentially explaining the heterogeneous and often contradictory reports in the previous literature.

We did not observe a CM effect when pooling healthy and depressed samples despite large statistical power. Testing various maltreatment definitions rule out that obtained null-findings were produced by a specific CM operationalization. Even investigating subgroups with severe levels of CM did not change this pattern of results. Taken together, these findings contradict previously published associations between experiences of CM and resting-state functional connectivity of the investigated regions (Birn et al., Reference Birn, Patriat, Phillips, Germain and Herringa2014; Herringa et al., Reference Herringa, Birn, Ruttle, Burghy, Stodola, Davidson and Essex2013; Hoffmann et al., Reference Hoffmann, Viding, Puetz, Gerin, Sethi, Rankin and McCrory2018; Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Clegg, Goer, Pechtel, Beltzer, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2018; van der Werff et al., Reference van der Werff, Pannekoek, Veer, van Tol, Aleman, Veltman and van der Wee2013). One possible explanation could be that previous small-scale studies have been reporting false-positive results.

When analyzing the MDD sample only, evidence for altered fronto-limbic connectivity emerged. Individuals with a lifetime MDD displayed effects of CM on resting-state functional connectivity of the amygdala to frontal regions but not the hippocampus or any ACC region. More specifically, CM severity was associated with higher positive connectivity of the right amygdala with a cluster located in the right superior and middle frontal gyrus. This effect showed a modest effect size and was the only connectivity effect surviving correction for multiple testing. This frontal cluster covers an area often referred to as the dlPFC, which has been implied to be a key region for emotion regulation processes (Dixon et al., Reference Dixon, Thiruchselvam, Todd and Christoff2017; Phillips, Ladouceur, & Drevets, Reference Phillips, Ladouceur and Drevets2008). Neurobiological models of fronto-limbic emotion regulation postulate that the dlPFC reflects on, appraises and if necessary downregulates emotional responses of the amygdala (Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, Reference Ochsner, Bunge, Gross and Gabrieli2002). Altered amygdala-dlPFC connectivity has been repeatedly suggested to constitute a neural correlate of emotion dysregulation which is a symptom often observed in children (Gruhn & Compas, Reference Gruhn and Compas2020; Kim & Cicchetti, Reference Kim and Cicchetti2010) and adults (Burns, Jackson, & Harding, Reference Burns, Jackson and Harding2010) with a history of CM. Emotion dysregulation has further been proposed to be one of the potential pathways between CM and depression (O'Mahen, Karl, Moberly, & Fedock, Reference O'Mahen, Karl, Moberly and Fedock2015). Better recruitment of the dlPFC during an emotion regulation task is correlated with higher amygdala downregulation and less severe depressive symptoms two years later in children with maltreatment experiences (Rodman, Jenness, Weissman, Pine, & McLaughlin, Reference Rodman, Jenness, Weissman, Pine and McLaughlin2019). Weak but significant correlations between amygdala-dlPFC connectivity and clinical variables in our study support the interpretation that this CM effect might be clinically meaningful. We provide evidence that the CM-related altered connectivity can be associated with current depression severity, global level of functioning and previous disease severity, as indicated by the frequency of psychiatric hospitalizations. Further, the association between CM and amygdala-dlPFC connectivity was aggravated within MDD participants experiencing an acute depressive episode, which could point to state-dependent relevance of neurobiological emotion regulation abnormalities. The dlPFC is part of a network involved in emotion regulation, together with the ventrolateral and dorsomedial PFC that regulate activation in the amygdala and hypothalamus via the ACC (Berboth & Morawetz, Reference Berboth and Morawetz2021; Lopez, Denny, & Fagundes, Reference Lopez, Denny and Fagundes2018). Our findings indicate that CM could impact emotion regulation abilities particularly by affecting the connectivity of the amygdala to the dlPFC within this network. Notably, within individuals with an acute MDD, CM severity explained over 9% of the variance in amygdala-frontal connectivity indicating a moderate to large effect. Considering that small effect sizes are rather common in the domain of psychiatric neuroimaging (Jia et al., Reference Jia, Zhao, Barton, Burciu, Carrière, Cerasa and Zang2018; Schmaal et al., Reference Schmaal, Hibar, Sämann, Hall, Baune, Jahanshad and Veltman2017, Reference Schmaal, Veltman, van Erp, Sämann, Frodl, Jahanshad and Hibar2016), this observation points to the potential relevance of CM for this particular subgroup.

Notably, the effect of continuous CM on amygdala-frontal connectivity was not found when applying a more conservative motion thresholding. This finding suggests that the identification of maltreatment effects on resting-state functional connectivity is not robust across different preprocessing/quality assurance pipelines which could explain the heterogeneity in previous findings. Motion thresholding always aims at finding the right balance between removing too little artifactual and too much true variance in the data. The researcher degrees of freedoms created by the multitude of different settings possible in neuroimaging analysis may be one major reason for low replicability that has been observed in this domain (Marek et al., Reference Marek, Tervo-Clemmens, Calabro, Montez, Kay, Hatoum and Dosenbach2022). Unfortunately, there is no broad consensus regarding the optimal preprocessing steps and parameters. The utilization of largely automated preprocessing pipelines such as fMRIPrep can reduce the researcher degrees of freedom and could possibly contribute to higher replicability of findings (Esteban et al., Reference Esteban, Markiewicz, Blair, Moodie, Isik, Erramuzpe and Gorgolewski2019).

While interactions of CM with comorbidity profiles and with sex were not significant, we observed descriptively larger effect sizes in an MDD group with comorbid anxiety disorders and in female as compared to male MDD patients, thus tentatively pointing to the potential relevance of these additional subgroups. Importantly, the relevance of the acute MDD group was not driven by a higher load of comorbidities or differences in age and sex characteristics as compared to the remitted MDD sample.

Null-findings yielded by analyses with alternate CM operationalizations (i.e. categorical) raise the question how robust CM effects on resting-state connectivity are (whereas depending on specific operationalizations). Findings could tentatively point to a higher sensitivity of continuous operationalizations of maltreatment severity to identify functional connectivity correlates of CM. This is in line with recent findings suggesting superior psychometric qualities of continuous operationalizations (Goltermann et al., Reference Goltermann, Meinert, Hülsmann, Dohm, Grotegerd, Redlich and Dannlowski2021).

Of note, even though amygdala-frontal connectivity alterations are generally in line with previous studies on CM, there is no consensus on the direction of the effects (Birn et al., Reference Birn, Patriat, Phillips, Germain and Herringa2014; Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Clegg, Goer, Pechtel, Beltzer, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2018; van der Werff et al., Reference van der Werff, Pannekoek, Veer, van Tol, Aleman, Veltman and van der Wee2013). While we observed stronger positive connectivity between the amygdala and frontal regions with increasing CM severity, the above-cited studies reported increased anticorrelation (Birn et al., Reference Birn, Patriat, Phillips, Germain and Herringa2014), decreased positive correlation (van der Werff et al., Reference van der Werff, Pannekoek, Veer, van Tol, Aleman, Veltman and van der Wee2013) and a mixture of both (Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Clegg, Goer, Pechtel, Beltzer, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2018) which can be summarized as a negative relationship of CM with connectivity as opposed to our findings.

In contrast, we did not find any evidence for altered amygdala connectivity as a function of CM severity in healthy subjects. Instead, weaker evidence was observed for an association with increased positive connectivity of the scACC with temporal and frontal regions, and with increased negative connectivity of the hippocampus with parts of the cerebellum. However, these effects did not survive multiple comparison corrections. No effects involving any other subdivisions of the ACC were found.

Furthermore, no evidence was found for a CM effect involving the resting-state connectivity of the subgenual portion of the ACC, contrasting findings of previous studies on adolescents (Herringa et al., Reference Herringa, Birn, Ruttle, Burghy, Stodola, Davidson and Essex2013; Hoffmann et al., Reference Hoffmann, Viding, Puetz, Gerin, Sethi, Rankin and McCrory2018) and adult samples (Thomason et al., Reference Thomason, Marusak, Tocco, Vila, Mcgarragle and Rosenberg2015). Similarly, only very limited evidence for CM effects of altered pgACC connectivity was found. Merely in a secondary subgroup analysis, comparing severe CM with no CM within the MDD sample CM was associated with higher connectivity between the pgACC and clusters located in the supramarginal and superior temporal gyrus tentatively pointing to differential connectivity patterns for this population.

The significant and non-significant findings in the subgroup analyses of MDD and HC individuals could suggest differing effects depending on depressive psychopathology. However, the non-significant interaction effect between CM and diagnosis opposed to the differential findings obtained from HC and MDD samples in separate subgroup analyses seem to contradict each other. This could be due to the small effect sizes that were obtained within groups, potentially leading to insufficient slope differences to produce a significant interaction effect. In addition, the validity of investigating a CM main effect within the pooled samples, as well as the validity of the interaction effect is somewhat limited due to considerable heterogeneity in variance and distribution of CM reports across groups.

The effect sizes obtained in the current study, as well as the conducted power analysis suggest that even the largest previous studies (e.g. N = 88 in the study by van der Werff et al., Reference van der Werff, Pannekoek, Veer, van Tol, Aleman, Veltman and van der Wee2013) could have been underpowered to detect relevant maltreatment-related connectivity alterations, potentially accounting for the large heterogeneity in previous findings. In contrast, our study should have had sufficient statistical power to detect effect sizes reported in previous studies. Overall, out of 30 primary statistical tests on different ROIs and subsamples, only one analysis of ROI connectivity produced robust significant results after correcting for multiple tests (right amygdala ROI in MDD). Even when applying a more lenient significance threshold, only two additional ROIs showed altered connectivity associated with CM (scACC and Hippocampus in HCs), again pointing to rather subtle CM effects.

An important limitation of the current study are the retrospective self-reports that were used to measure CM which rely on the ability of participants to accurately recall and report upon their childhood experiences despite potential cognitive depressive bias (Hardt & Rutter, Reference Hardt and Rutter2004). However, recent findings of our work group indicate that retrospective self-reports using the CTQ are very stable over time and do not vary greatly as a function of changes in depressive symptoms (Goltermann et al., Reference Goltermann, Meinert, Hülsmann, Dohm, Grotegerd, Redlich and Dannlowski2021). Another limiting factor of the reported findings arises from the specific methodology and sample used. While we aimed to account for a variety of different CM operationalizations in order to preclude that null-findings arise only as a result of a specific analytical approach, there still remains a variety of other methods that have previously been used, such as different preprocessing strategies or analysis approaches (e.g. investigation of ROI-to-ROI, voxel-to-voxel or dynamic resting-state connectivity, or regional homogeneity), as well as alternate cross-site harmonization approaches (e.g. NeuroCombat; Fortin et al., Reference Fortin, Cullen, Sheline, Taylor, Aselcioglu, Cook and Shinohara2018), that might be more sensitive to detect neurobiological effects of maltreatment. Further, the neurobiological and clinical consequences of CM could heavily depend on the exact type and timing of the CM experiences, as indicated by an increasing number of studies (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Humphreys, Flannery, Goff, Telzer, Shapiro and Tottenham2013; Herzog et al., Reference Herzog, Thome, Demirakca, Koppe, Ende, Lis and Schmahl2020; Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, King, Sacchet, Camacho, Colich, Ordaz and Gotlib2019; Pechtel, Lyons-Ruth, Anderson, & Teicher, Reference Pechtel, Lyons-Ruth, Anderson and Teicher2014; Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, Lowen, Anderson, Ohashi, Khan and Teicher2019), as well as the subjective perception of stressors (Slavich & Shields, Reference Slavich and Shields2018). As this information is not captured by the CTQ this constitutes an additional important limitation of the current study. Another reason for heterogeneous and null-findings within the MDD sample could be the inherent heterogeneity of the disorder itself. Major depression is a disorder with various clinical manifestations and subtypes that could have differential neurobiological underpinnings (Drysdale et al., Reference Drysdale, Grosenick, Downar, Dunlop, Mansouri, Meng and Liston2017; Fried & Nesse, Reference Fried and Nesse2015; Liu, Li, Li, Ren, & Ma, Reference Liu, Li, Li, Ren and Ma2021; Milaneschi, Lamers, Berk, & Penninx, Reference Milaneschi, Lamers, Berk and Penninx2020; Vassilopoulou et al., Reference Vassilopoulou, Papathanasiou, Michopoulos, Boufidou, Oulis, Kelekis and Lykouras2013). It would be conceivable that CM-related connectivity changes vary across these subtypes of depression and future research may profit from thorough stratification. In summary, we present evidence for only subtle fronto-limbic connectivity alterations as a neural correlate of self-reported CM in depression. Experiences of maltreatment may play a particular role for the functional connectivity between the amygdala and the dlPFC during the presence of acute depressive symptoms. However, our findings illustrate the need for large-scale replications of previous studies with small sample sizes in the domain of psychiatric neuroimaging research in order to secure and reevaluate our knowledge in this domain. In the face of the enormous heterogeneity in localization and direction of neurobiological effects in previous studies, such large-scale replications are crucial to differentiate true and meaningful effects from false-positive findings. Future studies should further investigate the reliability as well as mechanistic nature of this relationship and whether this knowledge can be translated into personalized interventions to improve clinical treatment of affective disorders.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722001623

Acknowledgements

Principal investigators in the FOR2107 consortium that are not co-authors of the current paper are: Work package (WP) 6, multi-method data analytics: Bertram Müller-Myhsok, Astrid Dempfle. Central project (CP) 1, biobank: Petra Pfefferle, Harald Renz. CP2, administration: Carsten Konrad. Further involvement in recruitment, data collection and management by: Henrike Bröhl, Bruno Dietsche, Rozbeh Elahi, Jennifer Engelen, Sabine Fischer, Jessica Heinen, Svenja Klingel, Felicitas Meier, Torsten Sauder, Annette Tittmar, Dilara Yüksel (Dept. of Psychiatry, Marburg University). Mechthild Wallnig, Rita Werner (Core-Facility Brainimaging, Marburg University). Carmen Schade-Brittinger, Maik Hahmann (Coordinating Centre for Clinical Trials, Marburg). Michael Putzke (Psychiatric Hospital, Friedberg). Rolf Speier, Lutz Lenhard (Psychiatric Hospital, Haina). Birgit Köhnlein (Psychiatric Practice, Marburg). Peter Wulf, Jürgen Kleebach, Achim Becker (Psychiatric Hospital Hephata, Schwalmstadt-Treysa). Ruth Bär (Care facility Bischoff, Neukirchen). Matthias Müller, Michael Franz, Siegfried Scharmann, Anja Haag, Kristina Spenner, Ulrich Ohlenschläger (Psychiatric Hospital Vitos, Marburg). Matthias Müller, Michael Franz, Bernd Kundermann (Psychiatric Hospital Vitos, Gießen). Ronny Redlich, Katharina Dohm, Katharina Förster, Christian Bürger, Fanni Dzvonyar, Kordula Vorspohl, Bettina Walden, Dario Zaremba, Joscha Böhnlein, Tiana Borgers, Verena Enneking, Stella Fingas, Marius Gruber, Carina Hülsmann, Melissa Klug, Elisabeth Schrammen (Dept. of Psychiatry, University of Münster). Harald Kugel, Birgit Vahrenkamp (Dept. of Clinical Radiology, University of Münster). Gereon Heuft, Gudrun Schneider (Dept. of Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Münster). Thomas Reker (LWL-Hospital Münster). Gisela Bartling (IPP Münster). Ulrike Buhlmann (Dept. of Clinical Psychology, University of Münster). Helene Dukal, Christine Hohmeyer, Lennard Stütz, Viola Schwerdt, Fabian Streit, Josef Frank, Lea Sirignano (Dept. of Genetic Epidemiology, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University). Stefanie Heilmann-Heimbach, Stefan Herms, Per Hoffmann (Institute of Human Genetics, University of Bonn, School of Medicine & University Hospital Bonn). Andreas J. Forstner (Institute of Human Genetics, University of Bonn, School of Medicine & University Hospital Bonn; Centre for Human Genetics, Marburg University). Anastasia Benedyk, Miriam Bopp, Roman Keßler, Maximilian Lückel, Verena Schuster, Christoph Vogelbacher (Dept. of Psychiatry, Marburg University). Jens Sommer, (Core-Facility Brainimaging, Marburg University). Thomas W.D. Möbius (Institute of Medical Informatics and Statistics, Kiel University). Julian Glandorf, Fabian Kormann, Arif Alkan, Fatana Wedi, Lea Henning, Alena Renker, Karina Schneider, Elisabeth Folwarczny, Dana Stenzel, Kai Wenk, Felix Picard, Alexandra Fischer, Sandra Blumenau, Beate Kleb, Doris Finholdt, Elisabeth Kinder, Tamara Wüst, Elvira Przypadlo, Corinna Brehm (Comprehensive Biomaterial Bank Marburg, Marburg University). We are further deeply indebted to all participants of this study.

Financial support

This work is part of the German multicenter consortium ‘Neurobiology of Affective Disorders. A translational perspective on brain structure and function’, funded by the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, DFG; Forschungsgruppe/Research Unit FOR2107). Funding for this research project was received by: Tilo Kircher (speaker FOR2107; DFG grant numbers KI 588/14-1, KI 588/14-2, KI 588/15-1, KI 588/17-1), Udo Dannlowski (co-speaker FOR2107; DA 1151/5-1, DA 1151/5-2, DA 1151/6-1), Axel Krug (KR 3822/5-1, KR 3822/7-2), Igor Nenadic (NE 2254/1-2), Carsten Konrad (KO 4291/3-1, KO 4291/4-1), Marcella Rietschel (RI 908/11-1, RI 908/11-2), Markus Nöthen (NO 246/10-1, NO 246/10-2), Stephanie Witt (WI 3439/3-1, WI 3439/3-2), Andreas Jansen (JA 1890/7-1, JA 1890/7-2), Tim Hahn (HA7070/2-2, HA7070/3, HA7070/4), Bertram Müller-Myhsok (MU1315/8-2), Astrid Dempfle (DE 1614/3-1, DE 1614/3-2), Petra Pfefferle (PF 784/1-1, PF 784/1-2), Harald Renz (RE 737/20-1, 737/20-2)

Conflict of interest

Tilo Kircher received unrestricted educational grants from Servier, Janssen, Recordati, Aristo, Otsuka, neuraxpharm. No further potential conflicts of interest are declared by the authors.

Footnotes

*

Equally contributing first authors.

Equally contributing senior authors.

References

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561571. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berboth, S., & Morawetz, C. (2021). Amygdala-prefrontal connectivity during emotion regulation: A meta-analysis of psychophysiological interactions. Neuropsychologia, 153, 107767. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, D. P., Fink, L., Handelsman, L., Foote, J., Lovejoy, M., Wenzel, K., … Ruggiero, J. (1994). Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 11321136.Google ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E. A., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., … Zule, W. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27, 169190. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00541-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birn, R. M., Patriat, R., Phillips, M. L., Germain, A., & Herringa, R. J. (2014). Childhood maltreatment and combat posttraumatic stress differentially predict fear-related fronto-subcortical connectivity. Depression and Anxiety, 31, 880892. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boccadoro, S., Siugzdaite, R., Hudson, A. R., Maeyens, L., Hamme, V., & Mueller, S. C. (2019). Women with early maltreatment experience show increased resting-state functional connectivity in the theory of mind (ToM) network. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 10(1), 1647044. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2019.1647044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, E. E., Jackson, J. L., & Harding, H. G. (2010). Child maltreatment, emotion regulation, and posttraumatic stress: The impact of emotional abuse. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma, 19(8), 801819. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2010.522947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chin Fatt, C. R., Jha, M. K., Cooper, C. M., Fonzo, G., South, C., Grannemann, B., … Trivedi, M. H. (2020). Effect of intrinsic patterns of functional brain connectivity in moderating antidepressant treatment response in major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(2), 143154. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.18070870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dannlowski, U., Kugel, H., Huber, F., Stuhrmann, A., Redlich, R., Grotegerd, D., … Suslow, T. (2013). Childhood maltreatment is associated with an automatic negative emotion processing bias in the amygdala. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 28992909. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dannlowski, U., Stuhrmann, A., Beutelmann, V., Zwanzger, P., Lenzen, T., Grotegerd, D., … Kugel, H. (2012). Limbic scars: Long-term consequences of childhood revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 71(4), 286293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davison, A. C., & Hinkley, D. V. (1997). Bootstrap methods and their application (1st ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, M. L., Thiruchselvam, R., Todd, R., & Christoff, K. (2017). Emotion and the prefrontal cortex: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143(10), 10331081. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drysdale, A. T., Grosenick, L., Downar, J., Dunlop, K., Mansouri, F., Meng, Y., … Liston, C. (2017). Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression. Nature Medicine, 23(1), 2838. https://doi.org/10.1038/nm.4246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, M. L., Knodt, A. R., Ireland, D., Morris, M. L., Poulton, R., Ramrakha, S., … Hariri, A. R. (2020). What is the test-retest reliability of common task-functional MRI measures? New empirical evidence and a meta-analysis. Psychological Science, 31(7), 792806. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620916786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esteban, O., Markiewicz, C. J., Blair, R. W., Moodie, C. A., Isik, A. I., Erramuzpe, A., … Gorgolewski, K. J. (2019). fMRIPrep: a robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nature Methods, 16(1), 111116. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fortin, J. P., Cullen, N., Sheline, Y. I., Taylor, W. D., Aselcioglu, I., Cook, P. A., … Shinohara, R. T. (2018). Harmonization of cortical thickness measurements across scanners and sites. NeuroImage, 167, 104120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, E. I., & Nesse, R. M. (2015). Depression sum-scores don't add up: Why analyzing specific depression symptoms is essential. BMC Medicine, 13(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-015-0325-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frodl, T., Janowitz, D., Schmaal, L., Tozzi, L., Dobrowolny, H., Stein, D. J., … Grabe, H. J. (2017). Childhood adversity impacts on brain subcortical structures relevant to depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 86, 5865. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.11.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, D. G., Humphreys, K. L., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Telzer, E. H., Shapiro, M., … Tottenham, N. (2013). A developmental shift from positive to negative connectivity in human amygdala-prefrontal circuitry. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(10), 45844593. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3446-12.2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goltermann, J., Meinert, S. L., Hülsmann, C., Dohm, K., Grotegerd, D., Redlich, R., … Dannlowski, U. (2021). Temporal stability and state-dependence of retrospective self-reports of childhood maltreatment in major depression: A two-year longitudinal analysis of the childhood trauma questionnaire. MedRxiv. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.08.31.21262884.Google Scholar
Gray, J. P., Müller, V. I., Eickhoff, S. B., & Fox, P. T. (2020). Multimodal abnormalities of brain structure and function in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(5), 422434. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19050560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruhn, M. A., & Compas, B. E. (2020). Effects of maltreatment on coping and emotion regulation in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review. Child Abuse and Neglect, 103, 104446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardt, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: Review of the evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 260273. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00218.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heany, S. J., Groenewold, N. A., Uhlmann, A., Dalvie, S., Stein, D. J., & Brooks, S. J. (2018). The neural correlates of Childhood Trauma Questionnaire scores in adults: A meta-analysis and review of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Development and Psychopathology, 30(4), 14751485. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417001717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herringa, R. J., Birn, R. M., Ruttle, P. L., Burghy, C. A., Stodola, D. E., Davidson, R. J., & Essex, M. J. (2013). Childhood maltreatment is associated with altered fear circuitry and increased internalizing symptoms by late adolescence. PNAS, 110(47), 1911919124. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1310766110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herzog, J. I., Thome, J., Demirakca, T., Koppe, G., Ende, G., Lis, S., … Schmahl, C. (2020). Influence of severity of type and timing of retrospectively reported childhood maltreatment on female amygdala and hippocampal volume. Scientific Reports, 10, 110. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-57490-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffmann, F., Viding, E., Puetz, V. B., Gerin, M. I., Sethi, A., Rankin, G., & McCrory, E. J. (2018). Evidence for depressogenic spontaneous thoughts and altered resting-state connectivity in adolescents with a maltreatment history. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(9), 687695. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2018.05.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, K. L., King, L. S., Sacchet, M. D., Camacho, M. C., Colich, N. L., Ordaz, S. J., … Gotlib, I. H. (2019). Evidence for a sensitive period in the effects of early life stress on hippocampal volume. Developmental Science, 22(3), 110. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iwabuchi, S. J., Krishnadas, R., Li, C., Auer, D. P., Radua, J., & Palaniyappan, L. (2015). Localized connectivity in depression: A meta-analysis of resting state functional imaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 7786. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jedd, K., Hunt, R. H., Cicchetti, D., Hunt, E., Cowell, R. A., Rogosch, F. A., … Thomas, K. M. (2015). Long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment: Altered amygdala functional connectivity. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 15771589. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jia, X.-Z., Zhao, N., Barton, B., Burciu, R., Carrière, N., Cerasa, A., … Zang, Y.-F. (2018). Small effect size leads to reproducibility failure in resting-state fMRI studies. BioRxiv. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1101/285171.Google Scholar
Kaiser, R. H., Clegg, R., Goer, F., Pechtel, P., Beltzer, M., Vitaliano, G., … Pizzagalli, D. A. (2018). Childhood stress, grown-up brain networks: Corticolimbic correlates of threat-related early life stress and adult stress response. Psychological Medicine, 48, 11571166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karos, K., Niederstrasser, N., Abidi, L., Bernstein, D. P., & Bader, K. (2014). Factor structure, reliability, and known groups validity of the German version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (Short-Form) in Swiss patients and non-patients. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 23(4), 418430. https://doi.org/10.1080/10538712.2014.896840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, K. (2005). The effects of nonnormal distributions on confidence intervals around the standardized mean difference: Bootstrap and parametric confidence intervals. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 65(1), 5169. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164404264850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R., Schmitt, S., Sauder, T., Stein, F., Yüksel, D., Grotegerd, D., … Jansen, A. (2020). Long-term neuroanatomical consequences of childhood maltreatment: Reduced amygdala inhibition by medial prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 14(28), 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2020.00028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(6), 706716. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02202.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kircher, T., Wöhr, M., Nenadic, I., Schwarting, R., Schratt, G., Alferink, J., … Dannlowski, U. (2018). Neurobiology of the major psychoses: A translational perspective on brain structure and function---the FOR2107 consortium. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 269, 949962. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-018-0943-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovner, R., Oler, J. A., & Kalin, N. H. (2019). Cortico-limbic interactions mediate adaptive and maladaptive responses relevant to psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 987999. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19101064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kühberger, A., Fritz, A., & Scherndl, T. (2014). Publication bias in psychology: A diagnosis based on the correlation between effect size and sample size. PLoS ONE, 9(9), 18. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, L., Radua, J., & Rubia, K. (2014). Gray matter abnormalities in childhood maltreatment: A voxel-wise meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 854863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, X., Li, L., Li, M., Ren, Z., & Ma, P. (2021). Characterizing the subtype of anhedonia in major depressive disorder: A symptom-specific multimodal MRI study. Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging, 308(November 2020), 111239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2020.111239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez, R. B., Denny, B. T., & Fagundes, C. P. (2018). Neural mechanisms of emotion regulation and their role in endocrine and immune functioning: A review with implications for treatment of affective disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 95(October), 508514. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.10.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marek, S., Tervo-Clemmens, B., Calabro, F. J., Montez, D. F., Kay, B. P., Hatoum, A. S., … Dosenbach, N. U. F. (2022). Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature, 603, 654660. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, C., & Mishor, Z. (2011). Antidepressant medications reduce subcortical-cortical resting-state functional connectivity in healthy volunteers. NeuroImage, 57(4), 13171323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meinert, S. L., Repple, J., Nenadic, I., Krug, A., Jansen, A., Grotegerd, D., … Dannlowski, U. (2019). Reduced fractional anisotropy in depressed patients due to childhood maltreatment rather than diagnosis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(12), 20652072. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-019-0472-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milaneschi, Y., Lamers, F., Berk, M., & Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2020). Depression heterogeneity and its biological underpinnings: Towards immuno-metabolic depression. Biological Psychiatry, 88(5), 369380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.01.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulders, P. C., van Eijndhoven, P. F., Schene, A. H., Beckmann, C. F., & Tendolkar, I. (2015). Resting-state functional connectivity in major depressive disorder: A review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 56, 330344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.07.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nanni, V., Uher, R., & Danese, A. (2012). Childhood maltreatment predicts unfavorable course of illness and treatment outcome in depression: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 141151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nemeroff, C. B. (2016). Paradise lost: The neurobiological and clinical consequences of child abuse and neglect. Neuron, 89(5), 892909. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oathes, D. J., Patenaude, B., Schatzberg, A. F., & Etkin, A. (2015). Neurobiological signatures of anxiety and depression in resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 77(4), 385393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.08.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 12151229. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203496190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Mahen, H. A., Karl, A., Moberly, N., & Fedock, G. (2015). The association between childhood maltreatment and emotion regulation: Two different mechanisms contributing to depression? Journal of Affective Disorders, 174, 287295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.11.028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opel, N., Redlich, R., Dohm, K., Zaremba, D., Goltermann, J., Repple, J., … Dannlowski, U. (2019). Mediation of the influence of childhood maltreatment on depression relapse by cortical structure: A 2-year longitudinal observational study. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 6(4), 318326. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30044-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opel, N., Redlich, R., Zwanzger, P., Grotegerd, D., Arolt, V., Heindel, W., … Dannlowski, U. (2014). Hippocampal atrophy in major depression: A function of childhood maltreatment rather than diagnosis? Neuropsychopharmacology, 39, 27232731. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opel, N., Zwanzger, P., Redlich, R., Grotegerd, D., Dohm, K., Arolt, V., … Dannlowski, U. (2016). Differing brain structural correlates of familial and environmental risk for major depressive disorder revealed by a combined VBM/pattern recognition approach. Psychological Medicine, 46(2016), 277290. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715001683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pechtel, P., Lyons-Ruth, K., Anderson, C. M., & Teicher, M. H. (2014). Sensitive periods of amygdala development: The role of maltreatment in preadolescence. NeuroImage, 97, 236244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philip, N. S., Kuras, Y. I., Valentine, T. R., Sweet, L. H., Tyrka, A. R., Price, L. H., & Carpenter, L. L. (2013a). Regional homogeneity and resting state functional connectivity: Associations with exposure to early life stress. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 214(3), 247253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.07.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philip, N. S., Sweet, L. H., Tyrka, A. R., Price, L. H., Bloom, R. F., & Carpenter, L. L. (2013b). Decreased default network connectivity is associated with early life stress in medication-free healthy adults. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 23(1), 2432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.10.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philip, N. S., Valentine, T. R., Sweet, L. H., Tyrka, A. R., Price, L. H., & Carpenter, L. L. (2014). Early life stress impacts dorsolateral prefrontal cortex functional connectivity in healthy adults: Informing future studies of antidepressant treatments. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 52, 6369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.01.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, M. L., Ladouceur, C. D., & Drevets, W. C. (2008). A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: Implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(9), 833857. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2008.65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Popovic, D., Ruef, A., Dwyer, D. B., Antonucci, L. A., Eder, J., Sanfelici, R., … Piccin, S. (2020). Traces of trauma: A multivariate pattern analysis of childhood trauma, brain structure, and clinical phenotypes. Biological Psychiatry, 88(11), 829842. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.05.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodman, A. M., Jenness, J. L., Weissman, D. G., Pine, D. S., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2019). Neurobiological markers of resilience to depression following childhood maltreatment: The role of neural circuits supporting the cognitive control of emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 86(6), 464473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.04.033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. T., Huang, C. C., Lin, C. P., Feng, J., & Joliot, M. (2020). Automated anatomical labelling atlas 3. NeuroImage, 206, 116189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saß, H., Wittchen, H.-U., Zaudig, M., & Houben, I. (2003). Diagnostische kriterien des diagnostischen und statistischen manuals psychischer störungen. In American Psychiatric Association (Ed.), DSM-IV-TR (pp. 4749). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schmaal, L., Hibar, D. P., Sämann, P. G., Hall, G. B., Baune, B. T., Jahanshad, N., … Veltman, D. J. (2017). Cortical abnormalities in adults and adolescents with major depression based on brain scans from 20 cohorts worldwide in the ENIGMA major depressive disorder working group. Molecular Psychiatry, 22, 900909. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2016.60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmaal, L., Veltman, D. J., van Erp, T. G. M., Sämann, P. G., Frodl, T., Jahanshad, N., … Hibar, D. P. (2016). Subcortical brain alterations in major depressive disorder: Findings from the ENIGMA major depressive disorder working group. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(6), 806812. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2015.69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., & Shields, G. S. (2018). Assessing lifetime stress exposure using the stress and adversity inventory for adults (Adult STRAIN): An overview and initial validation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 80(1), 1727. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinhoven, P., Penninx, B. W., Hickendorff, M., van Hemert, A. M., Bernstein, D. P., Elzinga, B. M., … Elzinga, B. M. (2014). Childhood trauma questionnaire: Factor structure, measurement invariance, and validity across emotional disorders. Psychological Assessment, 26(3), 717729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., & Samson, J. A. (2013). Childhood maltreatment and psychopathology: A case for ecophenotypic variants as clinically and neurobiologically distinct subtypes. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 11141133. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12070957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., Samson, J. A., Anderson, C. M., & Ohashi, K. (2016). The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(10), 652666. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomason, M. E., Marusak, H. A., Tocco, M. A., Vila, A. M., Mcgarragle, O., & Rosenberg, D. R. (2015). Altered amygdala connectivity in urban youth exposed to trauma. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(11), 14601468. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tozzi, L., Garczarek, L., Janowitz, D., Stein, D. J., Wittfeld, K., Dobrowolny, H., … for the ENIGMA-MDD Consortium. (2020). Interactive impact of childhood maltreatment, depression, and age on cortical brain structure: Mega-analytic findings from a large multi-site cohort. Psychological Medicine, 50(6), 10201031. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171900093X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Werff, S. J. A., Pannekoek, J. N., Veer, I. M., van Tol, M.-J., Aleman, A., Veltman, D. J., … van der Wee, N. J. A. (2013). Resting-state functional connectivity in adults with childhood emotional maltreatment. Psychological Medicine, 43, 18251836. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712002942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Waarde, J. A., Scholte, H. S., Van Oudheusden, L. J. B., Verwey, B., Denys, D., & van Wingen, G. A. (2015). A functional MRI marker may predict the outcome of electroconvulsive therapy in severe and treatment-resistant depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 20(5), 609614. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2014.78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vassilopoulou, K., Papathanasiou, M., Michopoulos, I., Boufidou, F., Oulis, P., Kelekis, N., … Lykouras, L. (2013). A magnetic resonance imaging study of hippocampal, amygdala and subgenual prefrontal cortex volumes in major depression subtypes: Melancholic versus psychotic depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 146(2), 197204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.09.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viola, T. W., Salum, G. A., Kluwe-Schiavon, B., Sanvicente-Vieira, B., Levandowski, M. L., Grassi-Oliveira, R., … Grassi-Oliveira, R. (2016). The influence of geographical and economic factors in estimates of childhood abuse and neglect using the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: A worldwide meta-regression analysis. Child Abuse & Neglect, 51, 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.11.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogelbacher, C., Möbius, T. W. D., Sommer, J., Schuster, V., Dannlowski, U., Kircher, T., … Bopp, M. H. A. (2018). The Marburg-Münster Affective Disorders Cohort Study (MACS): A quality assurance protocol for MR neuroimaging data. NeuroImage, 172, 450460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, E. A., Gelfand, A., Katon, W. J., Koss, M. P., Van Korff, M., Bernstein, D. P., & Russo, J. (1999). Adult health status of women with histories of childhood abuse and neglect. The American Journal of Medicine, 107, 332339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, L., Dai, Z., Peng, H., Tan, L., Ding, Y., He, Z., … Zhang, L. (2014). Overlapping and segregated resting-state functional connectivity in patients with major depressive disorder with and without childhood neglect. Human Brain Mapping, 1166(August 2012), 11541166. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, L., Feng, L., Mitchell, P. B., Wang, C.-Y., & Si, T.-M. (2020a). Striatal resting-state connectivity abnormalities associated with different clinical stages of major depressive disorder. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 81(2), 4265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., Gao, Y., Tang, S., Lu, L., Zhang, L., Bu, X., … Huang, X. (2020b). Large-scale network dysfunction in the acute state compared to the remitted state of bipolar disorder: A meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. EBioMedicine, 54, 102742. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ebiom.2020.102742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., & Nieto-Castanon, A. (2012). Conn: A functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connectivity, 2(3), 125141. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2012.0073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wingenfeld, K., Spitzer, C., Mensebach, C., Grabe, H. J., Hill, A., Gast, U., … Driessen, M. (2010). The German version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ): Preliminary psychometric properties. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie, 60, 442450. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0030-1253494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H.-U., Wunderlich, U., Gruschwitz, S., & Zaudig, M. (1997). SKID-I. Strukturiertes klinisches interview für DSM-IV. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Zhu, J., Lowen, S. B., Anderson, C. M., Ohashi, K., Khan, A., & Teicher, M. H. (2019). Association of prepubertal and postpubertal exposure to childhood maltreatment with adult amygdala function. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(8), 843853. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Significant clusters with main effects of continuous childhood maltreatment operationalization (CTQ sum) in ROI-based seed-to-voxel analyses

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Clusters with altered connectivity to the right amygdala associated with CTQ sum in lifetime MDD participants. (a) Right amygdala seed region of interest (ROI) presented in red. (b) Significant clusters (before Bonferroni correction) with altered connectivity associated with maltreatment. Slice position is displayed using MNI-coordinates. (c) Scatter plots with individual CTQ sum scores and connectivity values between seed ROI and significant clusters.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Clusters with altered connectivity to the scACC associated with CTQ sum in healthy controls. (a) Right scACC seed region of interest (ROI) presented in red. (b) Significant clusters (before Bonferroni correction) with altered connectivity associated with maltreatment. Slice position is displayed using MNI-coordinates. (c) Scatter plots with individual CTQ sum scores and connectivity values between seed ROI and significant clusters.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Clusters with altered connectivity to the left hippocampus associated with CTQ sum in healthy controls. (a) Left hippocampus seed region of interest (ROI) presented in red. (b) Significant clusters (before Bonferroni correction) with altered connectivity associated with maltreatment. Slice position is displayed using MNI-coordinates. (c) Scatter plot with individual CTQ sum scores and connectivity values between seed ROI and significant cluster.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Scatter plots of associations between individual amygdala-frontal connectivity and clinical measures within the MDD sample. Individual amygdala seed connectivities with frontal cluster significantly associated with childhood maltreatment are displayed on the y-axis. BDI, Beck depression inventory; GAF, global assessment of functioning.

Supplementary material: File

Goltermann et al. supplementary material

Goltermann et al. supplementary material

Download Goltermann et al. supplementary material(File)
File 608.7 KB