Introduction
Sow welfare and productivity are negatively affected by the risk factors for chronic stress during gestation (Martinez-Miro et al. Reference Martinez-Miro, Tecles, Ramon, Escribano, Hernandez, Madrid, Orengo, Martinez-Subiela, Manteca and Ceron2016; Lagoda et al. Reference Lagoda, Marchewka, O’Driscoll and Boyle2022). Many of these are associated with the physical environment, which can cause physical stress due to discomfort and pain, as well as psychological stress due to an inability to perform highly motivated species-specific behaviours (Jensen Reference Jensen1988; Lawrence et al. Reference Lawrence, McLean, Jarvis, Gilbert and Petherick1997; Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008; Oczak et al. Reference Oczak, Maschat, Berckmans, Vranken and Baumgartner2015). Building and pen designs are not easily altered, and changes are costly (Winkel et al. Reference Winkel, von Meyer-Höfer and Heise2020). Nevertheless, sow welfare could be improved in conventional buildings through multiple smaller, incremental changes that may be more feasible for the pig producer and cumulatively may reduce chronic stress.
The widespread use of fully slatted concrete floors is a significant risk factor for sow welfare. These are uncomfortable to lie on (Tuyttens Reference Tuyttens2005; Spoolder et al. Reference Spoolder, Geudeke, Van der Peet-Schwering and Soede2009), are associated with leg injuries and lameness (Elmore et al. Reference Elmore, Garner, Johnson, Richert and Pajor2010; Calderon Diaz et al. Reference Calderon Diaz, Fahey, KilBride, Green and Boyle2013), and preclude the provision of rooting material that could provide an outlet for investigatory behaviours (Tuyttens Reference Tuyttens2005). However, rubber floor mats can at least improve comfort while resting, and as a consequence reduce the risk of physical discomfort, thereby reducing stress (Boyle et al. Reference Boyle, Regan, Leonard, Lynch and Brophy2000; Tuyttens et al. Reference Tuyttens, Wouters, Struelens, Sonck and Duchateau2008; Elmore et al. Reference Elmore, Garner, Johnson, Richert and Pajor2010; Calderon Diaz et al. Reference Calderon Diaz, Fahey, KilBride, Green and Boyle2013; Ostovic et al. Reference Ostovic, Rafaj, Mencik, Kabalin, Grahovac, Matkovic, Vucinic, Nenadovic, Zaja and Pavicic2017). This is because the cushioning effect enables greater ease of changing posture, and reduces the risk of claw lesions and lameness (Boyle et al. Reference Boyle, Regan, Leonard, Lynch and Brophy2000; Tuyttens et al. Reference Tuyttens, Wouters, Struelens, Sonck and Duchateau2008; Elmore et al. Reference Elmore, Garner, Johnson, Richert and Pajor2010; Calderon Diaz et al. Reference Calderon Diaz, Fahey, KilBride, Green and Boyle2013).
Nevertheless, rubber mats do not provide the other benefits of straw bedding which is an outlet for many natural, species-specific sow behaviours, such as exploration, chewing, rooting and foraging (Tuyttens Reference Tuyttens2005; Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008). Indeed, most conventional gestation housing systems inhibit expression of these highly motivated, natural behaviours in sows as a result of insufficient environmental enrichment (Calvert et al. Reference Calvert, Haskell, Wemelsfelder, Lawrence and Mendl1996; van de Weerd & Ison Reference van de Weerd and Ison2019). Performance of such behaviours is important to the sow’s welfare (Studnitz et al. Reference Studnitz, Jensen and Pedersen2007). As well as providing an outlet for foraging behaviour, straw is also a high-fibre, ingestible material which can provide a degree of satiation to feed-restricted sows (Tuyttens Reference Tuyttens2005; Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008). This can reduce stress and oral stereotypies resulting from unsatisfied feeding motivation (Whittaker et al. Reference Whittaker, Edwards, Spoolder, Lawrence and Corning1999; Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Plush, Ralph, Morrison, Acharya and Doyle2019). Straw cannot be provided as bedding on slatted floors, but ‘off the floor’ structures such as racks and rooting towers are alternative delivery options (Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008). While this limits the potential of straw as rooting material, it nevertheless remains an effective source of fibre and enrichment as it is investigable, manipulable, chewable/destructible, and edible (van de Weerd & Ison Reference van de Weerd and Ison2019), and also facilitates species-specific behaviours, such as exploration (Whittaker et al. Reference Whittaker, Edwards, Spoolder, Lawrence and Corning1999; Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008).
Sows value fibrous enrichment resources such as straw very highly (Whittaker et al. Reference Whittaker, Edwards, Spoolder, Lawrence and Corning1999; Roy et al. Reference Roy, Lippens, Kyeiwaa, Seddon, Connor and Brown2019; van de Weerd & Ison Reference van de Weerd and Ison2019). Hence, a drawback of rooting towers and racks is the risk of competition (and thus sustained aggression), due to the potential for such structures to be monopolised by dominant individuals (Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008). Nevertheless, this could be mitigated by strategic placement and provision of a number of straw delivery structures (Lagoda et al. 2021 [Au: Reference Lagoda, O’Driscoll, Marchewka, Foister, Turner and Boyle2021a], Reference Lagoda, Marchewka, O’Driscoll and Boyle2022). Natural fibre rope could also help satisfy sows’ behavioural motivation to chew, as it is destructible, chewable, manipulable, and investigable (Horback et al. Reference Horback, Pierdon and Parsons2016; Mkwanazi et al. Reference Mkwanazi, Ncobela, Kanengoni and Chimonyo2019; van de Weerd & Ison Reference van de Weerd and Ison2019), with a potential positive effect on the performance of stereotypical behaviour (Casal-Plana et al. Reference Casal-Plana, Manteca, Dalmau and Fàbrega2017). Indeed, Horback et al. (Reference Horback, Pierdon and Parsons2016) showed that sows made contact with cotton rope more frequently than with rubber sticks or fixed woodblocks, and that this preference was observed day and night for two weeks. Providing rope in different locations to the straw dispensers could help to reduce competition around this valuable resource.
There are several methods of estimating chronic stress in sows (Lagoda et al. Reference Lagoda, Marchewka, O’Driscoll and Boyle2022), besides physiological measurements such as cortisol level (Herman et al. Reference Herman, McKlveen, Ghosal, Kopp, Wulsin, Makinson, Scheimann and Myers2016; Carroll et al. Reference Carroll, Boyle, Hanlon, Palmer, Collins, Griffin, Armstrong and O’Connell2018). One is through observation of stereotypical behaviours; these become established when animals are unable to cope with a challenge, or have no control over their environment (Martinez-Miro et al. Reference Martinez-Miro, Tecles, Ramon, Escribano, Hernandez, Madrid, Orengo, Martinez-Subiela, Manteca and Ceron2016). For instance, oral stereotypies in sows are commonly used as an indicator of current or previous unsatisfied feeding motivation (Tatemoto et al. Reference Tatemoto, Bernardino, Rodrigues and Zanella2019). Tear staining (chromodacryorrhea) around the eyes is a rarely measured yet a promising method of estimating levels of chronic stress (DeBoer et al. Reference DeBoer, Garner, McCain, Lay, Eicher and Marchant-Forde2015; Telkänranta et al. Reference Telkänranta, Marchant-Forde and Valros2016; Larsen et al. Reference Larsen, Gustafsson, Marchant-Forde and Valros2019). The stain results from the secretion of porphyrin from the Harderian gland, and is thought to be under autonomic endocrine control of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic adrenomedullary system (DeBoer & Marchant-Forde Reference DeBoer and Marchant-Forde2013). Indeed, sows housed in free lactation pens had less tear staining around their left eye at weaning, than those confined to farrowing crates for the duration of lactation (Kinane et al. Reference Kinane, Butler and O’Driscoll2022).
There have been few studies investigating the combined effect of multiple minor adjustments to fully slatted pens on stress and welfare of pregnant sows (Elmore et al. Reference Elmore, Garner, Johnson, Kirkden, Richert and Pajor2011; Quesnel et al. Reference Quesnel, Peuteman, Pere, Louveau, Lefaucheur, Perruchot, Prunier, Meunier-Salaun, Gardan-Salmon, Gondret and Merlot2019). However, this approach could provide additional benefits over research targeting individual risk factors for chronic stress (e.g. Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008; Horback et al. Reference Horback, Pierdon and Parsons2016; Merlot et al. Reference Merlot, Calvar and Prunier2017), as in reality sows rarely experience stressors in isolation (Lagoda et al. Reference Lagoda, Marchewka, O’Driscoll and Boyle2022). Hence, the aim of this study was to investigate the effect of housing sows in a physically more comfortable and enriched environment on indicators of chronic stress and animal welfare. We hypothesised that sows housed in the improved environment would have lower levels of chronic stress reflected in reduced performance of oral stereotypies and lower levels of tear staining, culminating in better welfare compared to sows in the conventional pens.
Materials and methods
Ethical approval
Experimental work was authorised by the Teagasc Animal Ethics Committee (Approval no: TAEC 2020-266).
Assignment of animals to trial, housing and management
This study took place on a 2,000-sow, commercial, farrow-to-finish farm in County Cork, Ireland, between July 2021 and April 2022. Oestrous was not synchronised on the farm. Sows (Large White × Landrace) were artificially inseminated in gestation stalls (2.30 × 0.55 m; length × width), at the onset of standing oestrous, and again within 24 h, and remained locked in stalls for 28 (28.9 [± 0.37]) days post-insemination. Sows went on trial over two three-week periods (period 1: 2–16 August 2021, where pens of replicate 1, 2, and 3 were formed; period 2: 15–29 November 2021, where pens of replicate 4, 5, and 6 were formed), whereby 40 served sows were enrolled in the study every week, with 20 sows assigned to conventional (CONTROL) and 20 sows assigned to treatment (IMPROVED) pens. In total, the study used 240 sows of parity 1 to 5 (mean [± SD]; 2.4 [± 1.03]) in six replicates. Sows were selected for the experiment on day 25 post-insemination out of approximately 60 to 80 sows per batch. Sows were restrained in gestation stalls to measure their back fat. Hair was shaved (and samples saved for hair cortisol analysis as part of a companion paper; Lagoda et al. Reference Lagoda, O’Driscoll, Galli, Ceron, Ortin-Bustillo, Marchewka and Boyle2023) from the dorso-lumbar region, identified by measuring 6.5 cm left and right from the mid-point at the spine marked by the position of the last rib. Back-fat depth (mm) was measured at the two identified sites using a Renco LEAN-MEATER® device [Renco; Minneapolis, MN, USA], and the average of the two values taken. The parity of each sow was noted, and overall health status evaluated, with those showing poor body condition and lameness excluded from the study. Blocks of two sows (i.e. 20 blocks per replicate) were created and balanced for back fat and parity and sows within each block were randomly assigned to either the CONTROL or IMPROVED treatment. The coefficient of variation for back fat was 25% in CONTROL, and 22% in IMPROVED, and for parity 42% in CONTROL, and 44% in IMPROVED.
The experiment started on the day that sows were moved to the gestation pens and mixed (day 28.9 [± 0.37] post-insemination) into their stable treatment groups. Each pen had 20 individual free access feeding/lying stalls (2.30 × 0.55 m; length × width), and sows were free to move around the remainder of the pen (7.3× 7.2 m; roaming area behind feeding stalls: 7.3 × 2.7 m). CONTROL pens had fully slatted concrete floors, two blocks of wood and two chains suspended within the group area. In replicates 4 to 6, pens also had a rubber toy (Astro 200, EasyFix Rubber Products, Ballinasloe, County Galway, Ireland) suspended from a chain. IMPROVED pens were the same, but with the addition of a length of natural fibre rope (1-m manila rope; Marine Suppliers & Co Ltd, Howth, Dublin, Ireland) suspended from the feed trough within each feeding stall, and straw provided from three custom-made structures (two straw racks at each end of the pen, and a rooting tower in the middle of the roaming area; Figure 1). Additionally, in the IMPROVED treatment, the slats in each feeding stall, as well as in front of the rooting tower, were covered with rubber mats (EasyFix Rubber Products, Ballinasloe, Co Galway, Ireland).
Sows were restrictedly fed (approximately 60–70% of ad libitum intake) a standard commercial, gestation liquid diet twice per day, and had ad libitum access to water via three nipple drinkers at one end of the pen.
Behavioural observations
Sow behaviour was observed directly three times throughout pregnancy: 72 h post-mixing (day 33 [± 0.5]), in mid pregnancy (day 60 [± 4.1]), and in late pregnancy (day 106 [± 1.6]). There were three 1-h observation sessions per sampling day at the following times (scheduled to avoid feeding times and routine checks on the animals performed by farm staff): 0800–0900, 0930–1030 and 1230–1330h. Twenty-four hours prior to the observations, sows were identified by a number spray-marked onto their back to facilitate viewing from outside of the pen. The two observers practiced scoring behaviour until at least 90% intra- and inter-observer scores for repeatability were achieved prior to the onset of the study. Observers could not be blinded to the treatment. Pens of sows were scanned every 6 min (with the exception of replicate 1, 72 h post-mixing when scans were at 10-min intervals). Thus, in total, there were 30 recordings per sow per sampling day (with the exception of replicate 1; 72 h post-mixing there were 18 recordings per sow per sampling day). Observers switched between IMPROVED and CONTROL groups at the beginning of each session to balance the time spent observing each treatment. Behaviour, as well as posture (lying ventrally or laterally, standing, dog sitting) and location (group area, stalls) of each animal was recorded on every scan, based on an ethogram adapted from Cronin and Wiepkema (Reference Cronin and Wiepkema1984; Table 1).
Stereotypical behaviour
On each scan, sows were observed for oral stereotypies, including: sham chewing, mouth stretching, palate grinding, sucking, tongue flicking, and licking. The proportion of scans (as a proxy for duration) sows spent performing oral stereotypies was calculated as a percentage of the total number of scans.
Interaction with enrichment items
On each scan, sows were observed for their interaction with different types of enrichment (chain/wood, Astro 200 toy, manila rope, straw rack, rooting tower, straw on the ground). The proportion of scans (as a proxy for duration) sows spent interacting with enrichment items was calculated as a percentage of the total number of scans.
Movement index
Data on the location of each sow in the pen (either in stalls or in the group area) on every scan was used to calculate a ‘movement index’, whereby a score of 1 was assigned each time a sow’s location changed between scans, with the sum of these scores representing a proxy for the amount of movement by a sow on each observation day.
Aggressive behaviour
Aggressive behaviour of individual sows was recorded using all-occurrence sampling simultaneous to the observations of stereotypical behaviour, as well as sow posture and location in the pen, resulting in 3 h of observation per sow per sampling day (following the same scoring arrangement as for behavioural observations, with observers switching between IMPROVED and CONTROL groups at the beginning of each session to balance the time spent observing each treatment). An ethogram of the aggressive behaviours recorded was adapted from Stewart et al. (Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008; Table 2). The pen location (stalls, group area) in which aggressive behaviours took place was also recorded. Occurrences of all aggressive behaviours were summed to yield a total count of aggressive behaviours per sow for each sampling day. Aggressive behaviours were also categorised as either ‘non-contact’ (chasing, threat, avoidance), or ‘contact’ (fighting, biting, head knocking, chasing with vulva biting), and summed to yield a total count per sow for each category on each sampling day.
1 ‘Contact’ aggressive behaviour
2 ‘Non-contact’ aggressive behaviour
Skin lesions
Skin lesions were counted 24 h post-mixing, three weeks post-mixing (day 52 [± 0.4]) and in late pregnancy (day 109), following a method validated by Turner et al. (Reference Turner, Farnworth, White, Brotherstone, Mendl, Knap, Penny and Lawrence2006). Skin lesions were counted on the anterior (head, neck, shoulders, front legs), middle (flanks, back), and posterior (rump, hind legs), on the left and right sides of the body. Counts included fresh skin lesions only, identified by colour and the estimated age of scabbing. The length or diameter of skin lesions was not weighted. All counts were summed to calculate a total skin lesion count for each sow per inspection.
Locomotion
To score locomotion (locomotory ability), sows were encouraged to take at least six strides on the fully slatted, concrete floors of the group pen in mid (day 57.3 [± 0.82]) and in late (day 108) pregnancy, using a visual analogue scale (VAS) developed by Lagoda et al. (Reference Lagoda, Boyle, Marchewka and O’Driscoll2021b). The scale consisted of a 150-mm horizontal line. Locomotion was scored by marking a point along the scale, with increasing impairment represented by a mark further to the right of the line (0 mm representing perfect locomotion, and the very right end, 150 mm representing severely impaired locomotion). The distance from the left-hand end of the scale was measured and the value for each recorded in millimetres. Thus, the greater the number, the more impaired the locomotion.
Tear staining
Tear staining was scored for each sow in late pregnancy (day 103 [± 0.5]), on the right and left eye separately, according to a scale developed by DeBoer et al. (Reference DeBoer, Garner, McCain, Lay, Eicher and Marchant-Forde2015; Table 3).
Statistical analysis
SAS v9.4 was used for all statistical analyses (SAS Inst Inc, Cary, NC, USA) with sow or pen as the experimental unit depending on the analysis. Differences were reported when P ≤ 0.05, while statistical trends were reported when P > 0.05 and P ≤ 0.10. Residuals were checked for normality by examination of histograms, quantile-quantile and normal distribution plots using the univariate procedure. Degrees of freedom were estimated using the Kenwood-Rogers adjustment, and P-values adjusted using the Tukey-Kramer adjustment where mixed models were used. Data are presented as least square (LS) means and standard errors (SE).
All general linear models included the interactive effect of treatment and time, as well as replicate, time as a repeated effect, and pen as a random effect. Covariance structure was selected on the basis of best fit, using the minimum finite-sample corrected Akaike Information Criteria (AIC). Further details of each model are described below.
Oral stereotypy % (proportion of scans sows were performing stereotypies as a percentage of the total number of scans), skin lesion counts, locomotion scores, and movement index were analysed using general linear models (PROC MIXED) and individual sow data used in the analysis (sow as experimental unit).
Enrichment interaction % (proportion of scans sows interacted with different enrichment items as a percentage of the total number of scans) was analysed using general linear models (PROC MIXED) which included the interactive effect of enrichment type, treatment and time, and the repeated effect of time. Sow was used as the experimental unit.
The sum of all aggressive behaviours for each sow were averaged per pen (pen as experimental unit) to normalise the data which were also analysed using general linear models (PROC MIXED). We conducted two analyses. The first investigated the interaction between treatment, time and location in the pen, and the second the interaction between treatment, time and aggression type.
The proportion of scans sows spent in different pen locations and postures was used as a proxy for time spent in different pen locations and postures. Here, individual sow values were averaged per pen (pen as experimental unit) and summed for each location and posture to normalise the distribution of the data. Location and posture were then analysed separately using general linear models (PROC MIXED). The first model included the interaction between treatment nested within location, and time nested within location, and the second the interactive effect between treatment nested within posture, and time nested within posture.
The Mann-Whitney test (PROC Npar1Way) was used to compare tear stains for both the right and left eyes of sows from CONTROL and IMPROVED pens, in late pregnancy. Sow was used as the experimental unit. Right and left eyes were analysed separately, as previous work showed differences in tear staining for both eyes in response to stressors (DeBoer et al. Reference DeBoer, Garner, McCain, Lay, Eicher and Marchant-Forde2015).
Results
Oral stereotypies
There was an interaction between treatment and time on performance of oral stereotypies (P = 0.001; Table 4). There was no effect of treatment at 72 h post-mixing (P > 0.05; Table 4). However, sows in CONTROL pens performed more oral stereotypies than sows in IMPROVED pens in mid (P < 0.001) and late (P < 0.001; Table 4) pregnancy.
Interaction with enrichment items
There was an interaction between treatment, time and enrichment item on the % of scans sows spent (proxy for duration) interacting with enrichment items (P < 0.001; Figure 2). There were no differences in the level of use of chain/wood and Astro 200 toy between treatments at any time-point. Understandably, straw (rack, rooting tower, straw on the ground) and manila rope were the most often used enrichment items in the IMPROVED pens (P < 0.05; Figure 2). While IMPROVED sows began and continued to interact with rope from 72 h post-mixing (immediately after mixing), meaningful interactions with straw enrichment were only recorded from mid pregnancy onwards (P < 0.001 for each enrichment item; Figure 2).
Location and posture of sows during behaviour observations
There tended to be an interaction between treatment, time and location of sows during behavioural observations (P = 0.086; Figure 3). In late pregnancy, sows in the IMPROVED pens tended to spend more time in the group area (P = 0.081), and tended to spend less time in the stalls (P = 0.081) compared to sows in CONTROL pens (Figure 3). However, sows in both treatments spent more time inside the stalls than in the group area as pregnancy progressed (P < 0.05; Figure 3).
No interaction was found between treatment, time and the posture of sows (P = 0.952; Figure 3). However, there was an effect of time on sow posture, with sows spending most time lying ventrally throughout pregnancy (P < 0.001; Figure 3).
Movement index
There was an interaction between treatment and time on the movement index (P = 0.003; Table 4). Although there was no effect of treatment 72 h post-mixing (P > 0.05), in mid and late pregnancy, sows in the IMPROVED pens had a higher movement index than CONTROL sows (Mid: P = 0.003; Late: P = 0.013; Table 4).
Aggressive behaviour
There was also an interaction between treatment and time when it came to total aggressive behaviours (P = 0.018; Table 4). Although there was no difference 72 h post-mixing and in mid pregnancy (P > 0.05), there were more aggressive behaviours among sows in the IMPROVED pens than in CONTROL pens in late pregnancy (P = 0.016; Table 4).
Location of aggressive behaviour
There was an interaction between treatment, time and the location of aggression in the pen (P = 0.023; Figure 4). Levels of aggression were higher in the group area of IMPROVED pens compared to CONTROL pens in late pregnancy (P < 0.001; Figure 4). In addition, there were higher levels of aggression in the group area compared to the stalls of the IMPROVED pens in late pregnancy (P < 0.001), whereas this difference was not significant in CONTROL pens.
Type of aggressive behaviour
Overall, there was no interaction between treatment, time and type of aggression (P = 0.672; Figure 4). However, there was an effect of type of aggression, with sows experiencing more non-contact than contact aggression throughout pregnancy in both IMPROVED and CONTROL pens (P = 0.014; Figure 4).
Skin lesion counts
Generally, skin lesion counts were low throughout pregnancy (IMPROVED 12.3 [± 12.66]; CONTROL 13.5 [± 14.21]). There was no interaction between treatment and time, and no effect of treatment on total skin lesion counts at any point throughout pregnancy (P > 0.05; Table 4).
Locomotion
Sows were considered lame if they scored 60 mm or higher (≥ 60) on the VAS. Locomotion scores were low throughout pregnancy (IMPROVED 7.0 [± 10.65]; CONTROL 7.9 [± 12.81]), with low occurrence of lameness (CONTROL n = 4; IMPROVED n = 2 lame sows). There was no interaction between treatment and time, or effect of treatment on locomotion at any point throughout pregnancy (P > 0.05; Table 4).
Discussion
Current gestation housing systems pose multiple risks for chronic stress in sows (Salak-Johnson Reference Salak-Johnson2017; Lagoda et al. Reference Lagoda, Marchewka, O’Driscoll and Boyle2022), and it is difficult to eliminate/ameliorate them all (Winkel et al. Reference Winkel, von Meyer-Höfer and Heise2020) within the constraints of existing commercial buildings. We implemented simultaneous minor modifications to conventional fully slatted gestation pens with the aim of improving sow welfare. The results of the current study provide good evidence that even the minor modifications that were made to a typical conventional system design in the IMPROVED treatment enhanced the physical and psychological comfort of pregnant sows. The positive effects on oral stereotypies, tear stains, and also the lack of negative impacts on locomotion despite increased levels of movement, suggest that the incremental improvements made in the pens did have an additive positive effect on overall levels of sow welfare.
As predicted, both forms of enrichment were the target of sows’ investigatory behaviour. Sows in IMPROVED pens not only investigated, but also ingested the straw (ME Lagoda, personal observation 2022, as based on the rate of disappearance of straw, as well as lack of evidence of it beneath the racks, or floating in the slurry beneath the slats), which likely provided additional gut fill, and increased satiety (Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008). In line with Whittaker et al. (Reference Whittaker, Spoolder, Edwards, Lawrence and Corning1998) and Bernardino et al. (Reference Bernardino, Tatemoto, de Moraes, Morrone and Zanella2021), this was reflected in substantially lower levels of oral stereotypies both in mid and late pregnancy compared to CONTROL sows. Although inedible and perhaps not the ideal form of enrichment for sows, the manila rope in the individual stalls likely also contributed to this effect (Casal-Plana et al. Reference Casal-Plana, Manteca, Dalmau and Fàbrega2017) by providing an outlet for oral stereotypies or by redirecting their sham-chewing behaviour. This was reflected in sows’ high level of interaction with the ropes in the individual stalls. Hence, the additive effects of straw and rope provision in IMPROVED pens was likely beneficial to the sows’ psychological and physiological needs, and thereby reduced levels of chronic stress.
There is a possibility that the tendency for increased time out of stalls in the IMPROVED sows could indicate a lower preference for the rubber mats provided inside the stalls. However, placing this result in the context of the presence of enrichment in the group area, it is more likely that sows were encouraged out of the stalls to interact with enrichment sources which they valued highly, reflected in high levels of interaction with the straw resources. It is also in line with the levels of aggression recorded in the IMPROVED pens.
Levels of aggression recorded in late pregnancy were higher in the IMPROVED pens, and particularly in the group area compared to the stalls. Based on the high levels of sow interaction with enrichment items provided in the group area of the pen, this likely reflects competition between the sows for access to those sources of enrichment material (Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008). Contrary to our expectations, providing three spatially separated points of straw delivery did not ameliorate competition for access to these resources.
Aggressive behaviours performed by sows in the IMPROVED and CONTROL pens were mostly threat and avoidance based (i.e. there was little physical contact between the sows). Therefore, it is not surprising that in spite of the higher levels of aggression recorded in late pregnancy there was no treatment effect on skin lesion counts at any stage. This is in line with Stewart et al. (Reference Stewart, O’Connell and Boyle2008) and Horback et al. (Reference Horback, Pierdon and Parsons2016) and suggests that even with fewer sources of enrichment the aggression associated with competition for access to such resources is less severe compared to aggression associated with competition for access to food in feed-restricted sows (Horback et al. Reference Horback, Pierdon and Parsons2016).
Sows in the IMPROVED pens moved around the pen more than CONTROL sows in mid and late pregnancy. Arguably, their higher movement index could have been detrimental for hoof health given that the majority of the pen flooring consisted of concrete slats which is a major risk factor for lameness, especially when combined with higher levels of agonistic behaviour (Philipot et al. Reference Philipot, Pluvinage, Cimarosti, Sulpice and Bugnard1994; Calderon Diaz & Boyle Reference Calderon Diaz and Boyle2014). However, there was no negative effect on the locomotion of sows in the IMPROVED pens. Indeed, more exercise (Perrin & Bowland Reference Perrin and Bowland1977; Marchant & Broom Reference Marchant and Broom1996; Schenck et al. Reference Schenck, McMunn, Rosenstein, Stroshine, Nielsen, Richert, Marchant-Forde and Lay2008) combined with better comfort while lying provided by the rubber mats (Boyle et al. Reference Boyle, Regan, Leonard, Lynch and Brophy2000; Elmore et al. Reference Elmore, Garner, Johnson, Richert and Pajor2010; Calderon Diaz et al. Reference Calderon Diaz, Fahey, KilBride, Green and Boyle2013) should possibly have improved the locomotory ability of sows in the IMPROVED pens. The absence of a difference between the treatments probably reflects the low levels of lameness in general for this sow herd. Moreover, sows in both treatments spent more time inside the stalls than in the group area throughout pregnancy, which is typical for this housing system (Olsson et al. Reference Olsson, Samuelsson, Collins and Boom1993; Arey & Edwards Reference Arey and Edwards1998). Indeed, in the IMPROVED pens, this meant that sows spent a greater proportion of scans, and therefore more time on the rubber flooring which could have compensated for any potential detrimental effect of the increased movement around the slatted concrete of the group area.
Lower tear stain scores around the left and right eyes of sows in IMPROVED pens reflect lower levels of chronic stress in these animals (DeBoer et al. Reference DeBoer, Garner, McCain, Lay, Eicher and Marchant-Forde2015; Telkänranta et al. Reference Telkänranta, Marchant-Forde and Valros2016; Larsen et al. Reference Larsen, Gustafsson, Marchant-Forde and Valros2019). This is in agreement with the lower performance of stereotypical behaviour by sows in the IMPROVED pens described in this paper. Additionally, our companion paper (Lagoda et al. Reference Lagoda, O’Driscoll, Galli, Ceron, Ortin-Bustillo, Marchewka and Boyle2023), describes lower levels of haptoglobin (inflammatory marker), improved reproductive performance, and better health of piglets born to sows from IMPROVED pens. Moreover, these findings emphasise the potential of tear-stain scoring to discern cumulative benefits to welfare in a systems study. Our finding is similar to that of Telkänranta et al. (Reference Telkänranta, Marchant-Forde and Valros2016) where growing-finishing pigs tended to have lower tear-stain scores around the left but not the right eye in pens designed to improve welfare (e.g. equipped with manipulable objects made of fresh wood or polythene plastic), compared to control pens. Similarly, sows housed in pens with piglets and access to sisal ropes tended to have lower tear-staining scores than control sows with no access to rope (Telkänranta et al. Reference Telkänranta, Marchant-Forde and Valros2016). Kinane et al. (Reference Kinane, Butler and O’Driscoll2022) also recorded less tear staining around the left eye of sows housed in free lactation pens at weaning, compared to those confined to farrowing crates for the duration of lactation. Moreover, despite having access to manipulable objects (Astro 200 toy, chain/wood), sows in CONTROL pens still had higher tear-stain scores than sows in IMPROVED pens. Hence, it is possible that of all the enrichment items provided to sows in IMPROVED pens, straw had the most powerful impact on improving welfare and reducing stress levels, likely by improving gut fill and thereby satiety, and by enriching the overall diet, recently identified as an important factor for the welfare of pigs (Kobek-Kjeldager et al. Reference Kobek-Kjeldager, Schönherz, Canibe and Pedersen2022). Nonetheless, as this was a systems study, this effect cannot be ascribed to straw alone, as it is possible that other improvements in the pen also had partial beneficial effects that contributed to the lower scores for tear staining.
Animal welfare implications and conclusion
The current study indicates that it is possible to improve sow welfare even within the constraints of fully slatted conventional gestation housing systems by implementing a number of modifications designed to lower chronic stress. It seems likely that the benefits accruing from the provision of enrichment resources and comfortable resting surfaces compensated for any potential detrimental effects of the increased aggression and associated stress caused by competition for straw provided in racks and rooting towers. Thus, implementing such improvements to pen design can be an effective alternative to major structural alterations, and has a cumulative beneficial effect in addressing both physical and psychological stressors experienced by sows.
Acknowledgements
MEL was funded by the Teagasc Walsh Scholarship Programme. The work was conducted as part of a Teagasc funded grant-in-aid project ‘Effects of chronic stress on sow welfare and performance, and on the resilience of their offspring (SowWeanWel)’ ref 0370. The authors would like to thank the farm owner and his staff for facilitating the work. The authors would also like to thank L Markland, M Harrison, and L Serai for their help with data collection.
Competing interest
None.