Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T10:04:21.299Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The impact of data-driven learning form-focused tasks on learners’ task engagement: An intervention study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 April 2024

Javad Zare
Affiliation:
Kosar University of Bojnord, Bojnord, Iran ([email protected])
Mostafa Azari Noughabi
Affiliation:
Hakim Sabzevari University, Sabzevar, Iran ([email protected])
Ahmad Al-Issa
Affiliation:
American University of Sharjah, Sharjah, United Arab Emirates ([email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Data-driven learning (DDL) form-focused tasks are a relatively new concept. These tasks involve using concordance lines to teach language in a way that integrates discovery learning, authentic language use, consciousness-raising, and the communicative use of language. Given their novelty, there haven’t been many studies on how they impact learners’ engagement. Therefore, this study sought to study whether DDL form-focused tasks influence English as a foreign language (EFL) learners’ task engagement. A total of 114 Iranian EFL learners were randomly divided between comparison and intervention groups in a study that utilized an experimental (comparison group, pretest, and post-test) design within a sequential explanatory mixed-methods design. The comparison group completed 10 non-DDL form-focused tasks, whereas the intervention group completed 10 DDL form-focused tasks. The results of t-tests and repeated-measures ANOVA indicated that incorporating DDL form-focused tasks into English classes enhanced EFL learners’ task engagement in the short run. However, the impact of DDL form-focused tasks on EFL learners’ task engagement was not durable. Moreover, analyzing semi-structured interview data suggested that using DDL-enhanced tasks with a form-focused approach increases EFL learners’ task engagement by triggering their curiosity, improving their autonomy, enhancing their concentration and interest, and facilitating their discovery learning. The present study lends more credence to the application of such tasks. The paper ends with implications for English language teaching and materials development.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of EUROCALL, the European Association for Computer-Assisted Language Learning

1. Introduction

Exposure to a certain amount of language is key to learning a second or foreign language. This is where data-driven learning (DDL) comes into use in second language acquisition (SLA) (Boulton, Reference Boulton, Thorne and May2017). DDL deals with using corpora and corpus analytic tools to learn about how the target language works in different contexts (Boulton, Reference Boulton2009). Corpora as collections of naturally occurring language provide learners with several instances of target language forms. DDL turns language learners into researchers by embracing language discovery (Johns, Reference Johns1986). In addition to learning language forms, DDL involves generalizing contextualized patterns of language use. The real advantage of DDL is in turning learners into better language learners and users (Boulton, Reference Boulton, Thorne and May2017). That is, given its constructivist and inductive method, which allows individuals to draw their own conclusions that hold personal significance to them, DDL enables learners to master the learning process, rather than simply teaching them (Boulton, Reference Boulton, Thorne and May2017). Regarding its effectiveness in SLA, Boulton and Cobb (Reference Boulton and Cobb2017) conclude in their meta-analysis that “DDL works pretty well in almost any context where it has been extensively tried” (p. 386). This may be in part due to its compatibility with the current understanding of task-based language teaching (TBLT) – that is, form-focused instruction (Leńko-Szymańska & Boulton, Reference Leńko-Szymańska, Boulton, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015). In form-focused instruction, learners are directed to specific language forms while using language meaningfully (Ellis, Reference Ellis2016). This is what DDL entails: attention to forms in contexts associated with language use and meaning. In this respect, Zare and Aqajani Delavar (Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2023) integrated DDL with form-focused tasks and proposed a new task type – that is, DDL form-focused tasks (defined as consciousness-raising tasks aimed at facilitating the communicative use of language through access to authentic input and discovery learning) – and investigated its effect on improving students’ comprehension of academic English lectures. Their results pointed to both temporary and durable gains.

DDL is a flourishing field of research (Boulton & Cobb, Reference Boulton and Cobb2017). Several studies have pointed to the beneficial effects of DDL on language learning (e.g. Zare, Reference Zare2020; Zare & Aqajani Delavar, Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2023; Zare, Karimpour & Aqajani Delavar, Reference Zare, Karimpour and Aqajani Delavar2023). DDL is effective in SLA contexts due to its positive impact on learners, including motivation, discovery learning, awareness-raising, autonomous learning, exposure to authentic language use, and cognitive engagement (Boulton, Reference Boulton, Thorne and May2017; Flowerdew, Reference Flowerdew, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015; O’Keeffe, Reference O’Keeffe2021). Despite its effectiveness in improving language learning outcomes, it has not taken ground in mainstream English language teaching and learning (O’Keeffe, Reference O’Keeffe2021). This could be partly because, despite the valuable insights gained so far, some issues remain as barriers to our thorough understanding of DDL. One such issue is the concept of task engagement, which has slipped the minds of DDL researchers. As one of the concepts that have recently sparked much attention in SLA research, engagement is defined as “how actively involved a student is in a learning task and the extent to which that physical and mental activity is goal-directed and purpose-driven” (Hiver, Al-Hoorie, Vitta & Wu, Reference Hiver, Al-Hoorie, Vitta and Wu2024: p. 202). It is so essential in second/foreign language learning that no learning is likely to take place without it (Philp & Duchesne, Reference Philp and Duchesne2016). More importantly, it is highly situated and context-dependent (Hiver et al., Reference Hiver, Al-Hoorie, Vitta and Wu2024; Philp & Duchesne, Reference Philp and Duchesne2016). That is, engagement is a dynamic attribute that shifts according to the specific learning environment and the tasks at hand. Therefore, engagement responds differently to different language learning situations and tasks. As such, it needs to be studied in relation to different tasks, such as DDL form-focused tasks. Yet, to the best of our knowledge, no study has so far investigated the impact of such tasks on learners’ engagement.

With the above points in consideration, the present analysis, drawing on an experimental design within a sequential explanatory design, set out to explore the impact of using DDL form-focused tasks on 114 Iranian English as a foreign language (EFL) learners’ task engagement. The findings could inform the research and practice of SLA regarding DDL and task engagement.

2. Review of the literature

2.1 DDL and TBLT

A DDL approach to learning has several advantages, although it has been treated marginally in real classroom practice (Leńko-Szymańska & Boulton, Reference Leńko-Szymańska, Boulton, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015). As a bottom-up inductive approach to learning, DDL involves “identification,” “classification,” and “generalization” (Johns, Reference Johns1991), which are crucial for discovery learning (Bernardini, Reference Bernardini and Sinclair2004). This requires mastering several cognitive skills, such as predicting, observing, noticing, thinking, reasoning, analyzing, interpreting, reflecting, exploring, making inferences (inductively or deductively), focusing, guessing, comparing, differentiating, theorizing, hypothesizing, and verifying (O’Sullivan, Reference O’Sullivan2007: 277). Acquiring these skills enables learners to manage their own learning (Boulton, Reference Boulton and Crosthwaite2019; O’Keeffe, Reference O’Keeffe2021; O’Sullivan, Reference O’Sullivan2007). In other words, instead of relying solely on the teacher’s instructions, learners are empowered to take charge of their own learning, using the skills acquired through DDL to pursue their learning objectives at their own pace and according to their individual needs. Additionally, it gives learners access to rich authentic examples of language use (Leńko-Szymańska & Boulton, Reference Leńko-Szymańska, Boulton, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015), which increases their motivation to learn. Consciousness-raising is another benefit of DDL (Boulton, Reference Boulton2009). DDL raises learners’ awareness of the target language by giving them access to multiple examples of target language forms, which may be construed as input flooding. In DDL, teachers may use complete sentences from the corpora for the students. DDL may also expose learners to language forms in key-word-in-context (KWIC) format, which may be seen as equivalent to input enhancement. Input enhancement through “bolding, italics, underlining, or highlighting” features (Schenck & Baldwin, Reference Schenck and Baldwin2019: 12) and input flooding by providing multiple samples of target language use (Hernández, Reference Hernández and Liontas2018) increase the saliency of target language forms, which can turn into linguistic intake (VanPatten & Smith, Reference VanPatten and Smith2015). Input enhancement and input flooding are the two variants of form-focused instruction that, due to drawing learners’ attention to linguistic forms in context, has a central role in TBLT (Long, Reference Long, Lambert and Shohamy2000). Form-focused (focus on form) instruction stresses attention to language forms besides meaning (Ellis, Reference Ellis2003). This is done by engaging learners in language use while their attention is focused on forms (Long, Reference Long2015). Form-focused instruction “overtly draws students’ attention to linguistic elements as they arise incidentally in lessons whose overriding focus is on meaning, or communication” (Long, Reference Long, de Bot, Ginsberg and Kramsch1991: 45–46). Hence, it enables learners to develop both fluency and accuracy by creating conditions for restructuring interlanguage (Ellis, Reference Ellis2015). Incorporating discovery learning activities in form-focused instruction can help learners develop certain cognitive skills such as connecting, generalizing, and hypothesizing (Tomlinson, Reference Tomlinson1994). Students can also use the explicit knowledge presented in form-focused instruction tasks to develop consciously implicit knowledge by actively observing and exploring the features of the material being taught (Tomlinson, Reference Tomlinson, Fotos and Nassaji2007).

In this connection, Zare and Aqajani Delavar (Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2023) integrated DDL with form-focused instruction and proposed DDL form-focused tasks. Such tasks borrowed consciousness-raising and communicative language use from form-focused instruction, and discovery learning and authentic language use examples from DDL. These tasks raised learners’ attention to language forms through KWIC concordance lines while they were involved in the communicative use of language. Additionally, as the tasks provided learners with authentic language use examples without any metalinguistic descriptions of how the target language forms worked, it required discovery learning on the part of learners. In an intervention study, Zare and Aqajani Delavar (Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2023) investigated the impact of using such tasks against form-focused tasks on learners’ understanding of academic English lectures. Their findings indicated that learners experienced both short-term and long-term improvements in comprehension as a result of increased noticing, metalinguistic awareness, discovery learning, and agency. They concluded that integrating discovery learning and authentic language use from DDL and consciousness-raising and communicative language use from form-focused instruction is an efficient approach for improving EFL learners’ academic English lecture comprehension.

2.2 DDL and task engagement

Engagement is a complex concept (Philp & Duchesne, Reference Philp and Duchesne2016). Part of its complexity is due to its highly individual, dynamic, and contextual nature (Hiver et al., Reference Hiver, Al-Hoorie, Vitta and Wu2024; Reschly & Christenson, Reference Reschly, Christenson, Christenson, Reschly and Wylie2012). Its multidimensional nature also adds to its complexity. As Philp and Duchesne (Reference Philp and Duchesne2016) note, engagement is “a state of heightened attention and involvement, in which participation is reflected not only in the cognitive dimension, but in social, behavioral, and affective dimensions as well” (p. 51). While scholars disagree on its components, engagement can be categorized into behavioral, cognitive, social, affective, and agentic types. Behavioral engagement concerns actual participation and on-task behavior. Cognitive engagement deals with attention and mental effort in completing the task (Helme & Clarke, Reference Helme and Clarke2001). In this connection, research shows that DDL requires learners’ cognitive engagement in tasks that call for the activation of higher-order cognitive skills (O’Sullivan, Reference O’Sullivan2007). However, the connection between DDL and cognitive engagement has not been studied (O’Keeffe, Reference O’Keeffe2021). Emotional engagement concerns emotional stimulation and showing positive affective emotions/attitudes towards the task at hand (Skinner, Kindermann & Furrer, Reference Skinner, Kindermann and Furrer2009). In this regard, previous research shows that incorporating a DDL approach impacts and fosters language learners’ positive emotions (Zare, Karimpour & Aqajani Delavar, Reference Zare, Karimpour and Aqajani Delavar2022). Social engagement involves negotiating and collaborating with peers and the teacher. Socially engaged learners “listen to one another, draw from one another’s expertise and ideas, and provide feedback to one another” (Philp & Duchesne, Reference Philp and Duchesne2016: 57). Research indicates that DDL leads learners toward higher levels of social engagement in negotiating form-focused episodes (O’Keeffe, Reference O’Keeffe2021). Agentic engagement concerns how learners contribute to the learning and teaching environment and shape instruction (Oga-Baldwin, Reference Oga-Baldwin2019). This is mostly done by asking questions from the teacher or other students. According to Lee, Warschauer and Lee (Reference Lee, Warschauer and Lee2019), the discovery-oriented nature of DDL is related to learners’ agentic involvement with concordances. According to Egbert et al. (Reference Egbert, Shahrokni, Zhang, Abobaker, Bantawtook, He, Bekar, Roe and Huh2021), task engagement can be measured through various levels or indicators. Their model identifies authenticity, social interaction, learning support, interest, autonomy, and challenge as factors that promote engagement.

These facilitators are directly or indirectly relevant to DDL. Authenticity, as one of the core facilitators of task engagement, is a principal feature of DDL. As Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton (Reference Leńko-Szymańska, Boulton, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015) note, DDL gives learners access to authentic language, “which they can query in a variety of ways for the information which is interesting and relevant to them” (p. 3). Social interaction is another key facilitator of engagement, which underscores interacting with others (peers and the teacher) and using language (Batstone, Reference Batstone2010). In DDL, however, learning is mediated through metatalk, which is the result of interaction with corpora and concordances (Flowerdew, Reference Flowerdew, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015). Yet, given the challenges learners face in concordancing, providing scaffolding prompts by the teacher seems necessary (Chang & Sun, Reference Chang and Sun2009). Related to social interaction is learning support, another facilitator of task engagement, which involves addressing learners’ concerns by providing feedback, scaffolds, and structured tasks that seem important to DDL. Interest, another crucial facilitator of task engagement, is relevant to DDL as well. The authenticity and richness of the samples of language DDL provides learners with make it interesting to them (Mishan, Reference Mishan2004). Autonomy, another facilitator of language task engagement, concerns the learners’ ability to take control of their learning. DDL increases learner autonomy and lifelong learning (Boulton & Cobb, Reference Boulton and Cobb2017). As Gilquin and Granger (Reference Gilquin, Granger, O’Keeffe and McCarthy2010) highlight, DDL involves frequently formulating and testing hypotheses and rules, which makes learners more active, involved, and autonomous in their learning paths. Task engagement increases with the right amount of challenge or where the difficulty of the task aligns with the learners’ skills (Csikszentmihályi, Reference Csikszentmihályi1990). In this respect, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton (Reference Leńko-Szymańska, Boulton, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015) consider DDL a challenging technique, as it requires certain technical and cognitive skills. To work with corpora, learners need to be able to manipulate concordancers and formulate and verify appropriate queries. Whether the challenge DDL brings to the learner is optimal or not is an issue that needs to be investigated through research.

2.3 The present study

Although there appears to be a strong connection between DDL and task engagement, no empirical study has explored the relationship between these two concepts. This study aims to address this gap by answering the following research questions:

  1. 1. Does using DDL form-focused tasks enhance the EFL learners’ task engagement significantly in the short and long run?

  2. 2. What are EFL learners’ perceptions of the role of DDL form-focused tasks in their task engagement?

3. Methods

3.1 Setting and design

The study was conducted in an EFL context. Initially, an experimental comparison group design was followed to implement the intervention and investigate the impact of using DDL form-focused tasks on the learners’ task engagement. To this end, learners were pre-tested and randomly divided into intervention and comparison groups. Both groups underwent the respective intervention and control treatments and were post-tested immediately and after one month. A sequential explanatory mixed-methods design was also implemented to collect both quantitative and qualitative data on the impact of using DDL form-focused tasks on the learners’ task engagement. In sequential explanatory designs, researchers first collect quantitative data and then use qualitative data to explain their findings. In this design, qualitative data are collected to contextualize and enrich the quantitative findings while highlighting the participants’ viewpoints. Here, qualitative data (i.e. interviews and narrative frames) were collected to explore EFL learners’ perceptions of how (in qualitative terms) the DDL form-focused tasks affected their task engagement. What is important to note is that the study relied on self-report measures. While we acknowledge that self-report data are susceptible to several biases and weaknesses, including honesty, social desirability, response bias, and introspective ability (Demetriou et al., Reference Demetriou, Ozer, Essau, Cautin and Lilienfeld2015), given the mostly mental, dynamic, and multidimensional nature of engagement, we relied on the learners’ own perceptions of their engagement to assess their task engagement. More importantly, given the fact that we needed quantitative data (repeated measures) to analyze the short- and long-term effects of DDL form-focused tasks, we couldn’t think of other means to measure task engagement. Although prior studies have used time on task and the number of turns, for example, to measure engagement, these means mostly assess behavioral engagement, and other aspects of engagement, including affective, social, cognitive, and agentic, are left unexplored. So we assumed the most reliable source for assessing learners’ engagement was the learner herself.

3.2 Participants

A total of 114 Iranian English learners volunteered to take part in the study. They were all BA university students majoring in English language teaching at a state-run single-sex (female) university with the age range of 19 to 23 (M = 20.31, SD = 1.08). Sixty-six (57.90%) were second-year students and 48 (42.10%) were third-year students. Before the study, they were asked to sign a written informed consent form where the outlines and steps of the study were explained. Their general English language proficiency, English-lecturing ability, and task engagement were gauged using the Oxford Online Placement Test (OOPT), an English-lecturing test, and a task engagement questionnaire, respectively, before the study. Their English language proficiency level was B1 according to the Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR). Their lecturing ability and task engagement in English varied within one standard deviation from the mean.

3.3 Instruments

3.3.1 English language proficiency test

The study utilized OOPT to gauge the English proficiency level of the students. OOPT was used solely for participant sampling purposes. OOPT is a computer-adaptive test used for English placement. It generates a candidate’s score on the CEFR score band. The test comprises two sections: Use of English and Listening. The Use of English component evaluates the candidate’s grammar and vocabulary knowledge, while the Listening section measures the ability to comprehend spoken language. The test takes approximately 45–60 minutes and provides a score ranging from 1 to 120.

3.3.2 English-lecturing test

In addition to the OOPT, an English-lecturing test, developed and validated by Zare and Aqajani Delavar (Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2022), was used to measure the learners’ English-lecturing ability (see Appendix A) and sample participants. The purpose was to ensure that all learners had comparable English-lecturing abilities before the intervention so that any post-intervention changes in their task engagement could be attributed to the intervention and not their initial English-lecturing abilities. The test provided learners with some information on “the role of culture in language learning” where some pieces of information were marked in red as a sign of importance. The test then asked them to prepare and audio-record a lecture on the same topic with the presented information. Learners were given the same information and asked to give a lecture on the same topic to ensure that their background knowledge of the subject was comparable. This was done to prevent it from affecting their ability to lecture in English. Grading the test was based on four criteria: organization, content, language, and the correct use of importance markers. The “organization” criterion evaluated the proper arrangement of information; “content” assessed if the information was conveyed accurately; and “language” evaluated correct language use. “The correct use of importance markers” dealt with the use of discourse structuring expressions.

3.3.3 Task engagement questionnaire

See Appendix B and C for information regarding the piloting of the task engagement questionnaire.

3.3.4 DDL and non-DDL form-focused tasks

Ten DDL form-focused English-lecturing tasks (see Appendix D), which had been developed in a former study by Zare and Aqajani Delavar (Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2022), were used for the intervention group. DDL form-focused English-lecturing tasks integrated DDL activities, based on a corpus-based study of importance markers in English academic lectures by Zare and Keivanlou-Shahrestanaki (Reference Zare and Keivanlou-Shahrestanaki2017), in form-focused tasks to improve the English-lecturing abilities of learners. Importance markers are defined as lexicogrammatical expressions that are used to highlight information in discourse (e.g. the point is, what I’m trying to say is, what you need to know is) (Zare & Keivanlou-Shahrestanaki, Reference Zare and Keivanlou-Shahrestanaki2017). These tasks borrowed consciousness-raising and communicative use of language from form-focused instruction and discovery learning and authentic language use examples (concordance lines) from DDL. Overall, the tasks promoted learning and use of importance markers in giving English lectures. They followed a similar design but focused on different topics. A typical DDL form-focused English-lecturing task comprised three main parts. First, a short audio lecture was given with important information highlighted, followed by comprehension questions. Students were required to listen to the lecture and answer the related questions, which took an average of 10 minutes to complete. Next, some concordance lines from a study by Zare and Keivanlou-Shahrestanaki (Reference Zare and Keivanlou-Shahrestanaki2017) were presented, and students had to analyze how importance was emphasized in English lectures. This was followed by a written explanation of how these markers were produced, which took an average of 25 minutes. Lastly, some facts on a particular topic were given in the form of bullet points, some of which were highlighted as important. The students were supposed to study them and record an English audio lecture where these facts were marked as important through the use of importance markers. This part took 45 minutes on average.

Ten non-DDL form-focused English-lecturing tasks (see Appendix E) were also used for the comparison group. Like the DDL form-focused English-lecturing tasks, these tasks were developed by Zare and Aqajani Delavar (Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2022). The tasks were designed based on the principles of form-focused instruction to improve the English-lecturing ability of learners by enabling them to use importance markers (Zare & Aqajani Delavar, Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2022). As such, they promoted learning and use of importance markers in giving English lectures via consciousness-raising while learners were engaged in using language for communicative purposes. However, they did not feature concordance lines and discovery learning, which made them different from DDL form-focused tasks. DDL promotes discovery learning by exposing learners to several instances of the target language structure through concordance lines (Zare, Reference Zare2020). Each form-focused task followed the same design but focused on a different topic. A typical form-focused English-lecturing task consisted of three parts. First, a short audio lecture was presented, followed by comprehension questions. This took an average of 10 minutes. The second part involved a written transcript of the previous lecture, with importance markers highlighted in it, and an explanation of how they were used in English lectures. This took around 25 minutes. Lastly, bullet points on a specific topic were presented, with some being highlighted as important. The learners were required to record an English audio lecture with the important points marked using importance markers. On average, this part took 45 minutes to complete.

The DDL form-focused and non-DDL form-focused English-lecturing tasks were paired based on their topic and content. This means that the first DDL form-focused English-lecturing task was similar to the first non-DDL form-focused English-lecturing task in topic and content; the second DDL form-focused task was similar to the second non-DDL form-focused task, and so on. The content of the lectures was in English, while the directions for the tasks were given in Persian.

3.3.5 Narrative frame

A narrative frame was used to complement the results of the task engagement questionnaire and provide answers to the second research question. The narrative frame was designed in Google Forms format. It included a sentence prompt in Persian that asked students to explain how the tasks and their features kept them engaged (What kept me dis/engaged in the tasks was …). The students were supposed to fill it out every two weeks. The narrative frame was used to elicit learners’ perceptions regarding how the tasks affected their dis/engagement.

3.3.6 Semi-structured interviews

In addition to the narrative frame, online (via WhatsApp) one-on-one semi-structured interviews were also held in Persian (the students’ native language) with five students to expand the results of the task engagement questionnaire and provide answers to the second research question. The overall purpose of the interviews, along with narrative frames, was to investigate what and how aspects of the tasks affected the students’ task engagement. A typical interview began by introducing its aims. Next, the students were supposed to answer the following questions: (1) Were you dis/engaged in the tasks? (2) What do you think of the tasks? (3) What feature of the tasks affected your engagement? In addition to these questions, they were allowed to share their concerns regarding the tasks and their engagement. Each interview lasted 30 minutes on average.

3.4 Procedures

First, 138 students were pre-tested in their general English language proficiency, English-lecturing ability, and task engagement via the OOPT, English-lecturing test, and task engagement questionnaire (see section 3.2). The purpose of administering the OOPT, English-lecturing test, and task engagement questionnaire before the intervention was to ensure that any post-intervention changes in the learners’ task engagement were due to the intervention and not pre-intervention differences in their general English language proficiency, English-lecturing abilities, and task engagement. As a result, 114 English learners with a B1 level of English language proficiency and similar English-lecturing ability and task engagement were selected. We split the students into two groups: intervention (57 students) and comparison (57 students), taking into account their age and years of study, and assigned them to the groups, using stratified random sampling. Stratified random sampling involves forming strata or subgroups. In this case, subgroups were formed based on the learners’ age and years of study. And then they were divided between the two groups. The two groups were created by selecting an equal number of learners of the same age, based on the frequency in the sample. In terms of year of study, each group consisted of 33 second-year and 24 third-year students. This was done to ensure that the two groups were similar in age and years of study. Each group underwent a different treatment. The intervention group received 10 one-and-a-half-hour sessions of instruction, during which they completed 10 DDL form-focused English-lecturing tasks. The control group received the same number of sessions, but instead completed 10 non-DDL form-focused English-lecturing tasks. The students in both groups received assistance from the same English teacher to complete their tasks while receiving feedback and guidance. The reason we employed one teacher for both groups was to ensure that the teacher and the way he provided learners with guidance and feedback did not affect learners’ task engagement. The intervention and control treatments lasted for five months. Every two weeks, the students were required to complete an online narrative frame using Google Forms. This narrative frame required them to explain how the tasks and their associated features affected their engagement with the task. Upon completion of the program, the students were asked to fill out the online task engagement questionnaire through Google Forms. The purpose of this implementation of the questionnaire was to investigate the immediate effects of completing DDL form-focused English-lecturing tasks on the students’ task engagement. Next, during online one-on-one semi-structured interviews with five students who volunteered to participate in the interviews, they explained the different aspects of the tasks that influenced their task engagement in English lecturing. To protect students’ confidentiality, aliases are used in the paper instead of their real names. Finally, after a one-month interval, the students were asked to fill out the online task engagement questionnaire again. The purpose of this implementation of the questionnaire was to investigate the delayed effects of completing DDL form-focused English-lecturing tasks on the students’ task engagement.

3.5 Data analysis

The present investigation involved collecting and analyzing both quantitative and qualitative analyses. The quantitative data were collected to address the first research question, while the qualitative data were collected to provide answers to the second research question. As with every sequential explanatory design, qualitative data are analyzed to complement the results of quantitative data. As the results of running Shapiro–Wilk’s test for the pre-task survey, immediate post-task survey, and delayed post-task survey confirmed normality of the data (p = 0.11, 0.21, 0.83 > 0.05) using SPSS, parametric tests were used. This required running three independent-samples t-tests using SPSS: one for the task engagement questionnaire used as a pre-task survey, one for the task engagement questionnaire as an immediate post-task survey, and the last one as a delayed post-task survey. We also ran two separate repeated-measures ANOVAs in SPSS, one for the comparison group and one for the intervention group, to investigate whether the students’ task engagement statistically significantly differed from the pre-task survey to the immediate and delayed post-task survey stages.

Next, we analyzed the qualitative data by following Gao and Zhang’s (Reference Gao and Zhang2020) framework. The data included students’ responses to narrative frames (in text format) and semi-structured interviews (in audio format). We transcribed the audio interviews verbatim and combined them with the narrative frame responses. After removing errors and irrelevant responses, we used bottom-up coding. The bottom-up coding helped us identify codes without any preconceived notions. It also helped us maintain the confirmability of the analysis, a quality criterion for qualitative data analysis. Next, we reviewed the emerged codes and grouped them into themes. Last, we wrote a detailed report of the analysis with the emerged codes and themes, complemented with associated excerpts from the students’ responses to interviews and narrative frames, to enhance the transferability of the analysis. We also invited a foreign coder (Chinese) to code the data and develop themes. This was done independently to maintain both emic (the native coders) and etic (the Chinese coder) perspectives by considering researcher positioning. It also helped us enhance the dependability of the analysis. Inter-coder and intra-coder reliabilities were also estimated with Cohen’s kappa (α = 0.77, 0.81, respectively) to enhance dependability. Finally, through member checking or participant validation, we sent the report of the analysis presenting the emerged codes and themes along with examples from the students’ responses to the five interviewed students by email and asked them to check if their concerns had been addressed in interpreting their responses.

In our analysis, we also took ethical considerations into account, including confidentiality and informed consent. To protect confidentiality, we presented the results using aliases. Additionally, we only included students who volunteered for the study and signed a written informed consent form before the study. This form was prepared according to the guidelines set forth by the British Educational Research Association for educational research in 2018.

4. Results

4.1 Research question one (quantitative results)

The first research question set out to investigate whether using DDL form-focused tasks enhances EFL learners’ task engagement significantly in the short and long run. Table 1 shows descriptive statistics of the task engagement questionnaire as pre-task, immediate, and delayed post-task surveys.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for task engagement

Table 1 points to different means for the students in the comparison and intervention groups on the pre-task survey, immediate post-task survey, and delayed post-task survey. To determine statistical significance, we conducted three independent-samples t-tests: one for the pre-task, one for the immediate post-task, and one for the delayed post-task survey. The results of these three t-tests are displayed in Table 2.

Table 2. Independent-samples t-tests for task engagement

Note. CI = confidence interval.

As Table 2 shows, the difference between the two groups in their task engagement before the intervention was not statistically significant (p = 0.93 > 0.05). In terms of task engagement immediately following the intervention, however, the results pointed to a small (Plonsky & Oswald, Reference Plonsky and Oswald2014) yet statistically significant difference between the intervention and comparison groups (p = 0.01 < 0.05). In other words, DDL form-focused tasks significantly enhanced the learners’ short-term task engagement.

Additionally, in terms of delayed task engagement, the results pointed to a small (Plonsky & Oswald, Reference Plonsky and Oswald2014) and statistically insignificant difference between the two groups in their delayed task engagement (p = 0.28 > 0.05). In other words, DDL form-focused tasks did not enhance the students’ task engagement statistically significantly in the long run.

To investigate if the difference between students’ task engagement was statistically significant at different time points in the study (i.e. before the intervention, immediately after the intervention, and with a one-month interval following the intervention), we ran two separate repeated-measures ANOVAs for both the comparison and intervention groups. The results of the repeated-measures ANOVAs are displayed in Tables 36.

Table 3. Tests of within-subjects effects for the comparison group

Table 4. Pairwise comparisons for the comparison group

Note. CI = confidence interval.

Table 5. Tests of within-subjects effects for the intervention group

Table 6. Pairwise comparisons for the intervention group

Note. CI = confidence interval.

As Table 3 shows, the repeated-measures ANOVA with a Greenhouse–Geisser correction did not point to a statistically significant difference for the comparison group, F(1.982, 22680.374) = 0.409, p = 0.664 > 0.05.

According to Table 4, the post hoc analysis with a Bonferroni adjustment did not reveal any significant difference in task engagement for the comparison group learners. This was observed from the pre-task survey to both the immediate and delayed post-task surveys (p = 1.0 > .05), as well as between the immediate and delayed post-task surveys (p = 1.0 > .05).

The Greenhouse–Geisser corrected repeated-measures ANOVA pointed to a statistically significant difference for the intervention group, F(1.921, 107.602) = 4.271, p = 0.01 < 0.05, as shown in Table 5.

According to Table 6, the post hoc analysis with a Bonferroni adjustment revealed that there was a significant difference in the task engagement of learners in the intervention group between the pre-task survey and the immediate post-task survey (p = 0.01 < .05). However, there was no significant difference in task engagement between the pre-task survey and the delayed post-task survey (p = 0.48 > .05) or between the immediate and delayed post-task surveys (p = 0.45 > .05).

4.2 Research question two (qualitative results)

The results of quantitative analyses pointed to the significant effect of using DDL form-focused tasks in enhancing EFL learners’ task engagement. To further shed light on this finding, the second research question set out to address the learners’ perceptions of the role of DDL form-focused tasks in impacting their task engagement. In this regard, thematic analysis of the learners’ responses to the narrative frames and semi-structured interviews led to the emergence of seven themes: (1) motivation, (2) rich unteachable materials, (3) curiosity, (4) interest and challenge, (5) discovery learning, (6) concentration, and (7) autonomy (see Appendix F for a further discussion).

Motivation is the initial intention or drive that leads to subsequent action or engagement, as Reschly and Christenson (Reference Reschly, Christenson, Christenson, Reschly and Wylie2012) note. Hence, motivation is an antecedent or precursor of engagement. Motivation is associated with DDL (Curado Fuentes, Reference Curado Fuentes and Boulton2015). Interestingly, motivation was one of the recurrent themes regarding DDL tasks that increased learners’ engagement in them (n = 24). This may deal with the authenticity and richness of DDL materials. In this respect, Mia (an alias) wrote in her narrative frame:

At first, I didn’t know what the tasks were and didn’t feel much like doing them. But later when I realized what interesting English phrases I could learn from the activities I became motivated and did the rest of the tasks with energy and excitement (Mia, a 22-year-old English learner).

Closely related to motivation is the theme of “rich unteachable materials,” which was repeatedly mentioned by learners (n = 9). The authenticity and richness of DDL materials, a core facilitator of task engagement (Egbert et al., Reference Egbert, Shahrokni, Zhang, Abobaker, Bantawtook, He, Bekar, Roe and Huh2021), make them suitable for learning language forms that are “normally untaught and possibly unteachable” (Johns, Reference Johns1991: 28). In this regard, Olivia said in the interview:

I think what I found interesting about the tasks was that I learned things that I had never learned before. For instance, I learned to use “the thing is” or “the point is”. So simple but useful. These were new to me (Olivia, a 20-year-old English learner).

“Curiosity” was another theme that came up frequently in the learners’ responses (n = 22). Although it is not discussed among facilitators of engagement (Egbert et al., Reference Egbert, Shahrokni, Zhang, Abobaker, Bantawtook, He, Bekar, Roe and Huh2021), several learners pointed out that their curiosity was raised in the course of tasks and that’s why their engagement increased. In this regard, Ava said in her interview:

Doing the tasks raised my curiosity and interest. There was a problem that needed to be solved. And the clue was in the task itself. So, I just needed to focus. When going back and forth in the task, I felt I was getting closer to the answer. So, this way my curiosity and interest to do the task increased (Ava, a 20-year-old English learner).

“Interest and challenge,” which were mostly reported with each other, were also repeatedly mentioned by learners as reasons for increasing their engagement in DDL form-focused tasks (n = 14). Interest and challenge are both key facilitators of task engagement (Egbert et al., Reference Egbert, Shahrokni, Zhang, Abobaker, Bantawtook, He, Bekar, Roe and Huh2021). Presenting an optimal level of challenge may drive learners to invest more in the task than become disengaged (Aubrey, King & Almukhaild, Reference Aubrey, King and Almukhaild2022). As Ainley (Reference Ainley, Christenson, Reschly and Wylie2012) notes, personal interest could enhance situational interest and promote persistence in tasks. In this connection, Kate commented in her narrative frame:

The tasks were both interesting and challenging. They also helped me make more progress in English. They seemed difficult and challenging at first. But when I got to know how to do it, it became easier and more interesting (Kate, a 21-year-old English learner).

“Discovery learning” was also mentioned by learners as one of the reasons that promoted their engagement in DDL form-focused tasks (n = 17). As a bottom-up inductive approach to learning, DDL enhances learners’ awareness of target language forms through the three major steps of identification, classification, and generalization (Johns, Reference Johns1991), which reflects discovery learning (Bernardini, Reference Bernardini and Sinclair2004; Boulton, Reference Boulton and Crosthwaite2019). Such an approach makes learning more motivating and fun (Gilquin & Granger, Reference Gilquin, Granger, O’Keeffe and McCarthy2010). Concerning “discovery learning,” Charlotte wrote in her narrative frame:

Completing each task was exciting because it was like exploring, you know. It was like doing something new or discovering something (Charlotte, a 19-year-old English learner).

“Concentration” was another common theme among learners’ responses to narrative frames and semi-structured interviews (n = 13). DDL requires focusing on and analyzing particular language forms to induce rules of how such forms work in different contexts. Such efforts promote learners’ language consciousness (Flowerdew, Reference Flowerdew, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015). This underscores the noticing hypothesis according to which conscious attention to linguistic forms is necessary for input to turn into intake (Schmidt, Reference Schmidt1990, Reference Schmidt and Robinson2001). In this respect, Molly highlighted in the narrative frame:

Doing the tasks helped me concentrate more. I couldn’t miss a thing in them. It was like the pieces of a puzzle. So, I had to concentrate and think more about what each piece had to do to make the whole picture (Molly, a 21-year-old English learner).

Several learners pointed to “autonomy” as another reason for their engagement in DDL form-focused tasks (n = 12). In this regard, Rose mentioned in her interview:

It helped me learn better. I think things that I learn this way stay longer in my mind. I think it was because I learned them by myself. So, in a sense, not only did I learn new things about English, but also I became a better English learner (Rose, a 20-year-old English learner).

As Rose and other learners pointed out, DDL form-focused tasks promoted their autonomy as English language learners. As a core facilitator of engagement (Egbert et al., Reference Egbert, Shahrokni, Zhang, Abobaker, Bantawtook, He, Bekar, Roe and Huh2021), autonomy enhances learners’ engagement in tasks (Mozgalina, Reference Mozgalina2015). Putting learners in the driving seat and hence promoting their autonomy are also core features of DDL (Boulton, Reference Boulton and Crosthwaite2019; Boulton & Cobb, Reference Boulton and Cobb2017). The constructivist inductive approach DDL involves helps learners reach their own conclusions that are meaningful to them (Boulton, Reference Boulton, Thorne and May2017). Also, the cognitive processes involved in DDL may lead to longer retention (Boulton, Reference Boulton, Thorne and May2017). In other words,

the real advantages lie not so much in the explicit knowledge gained as in the processes involved – ability to deal with authentic texts in different genres; awareness of frequency, chunking, and collocation; noticing forms and variation; formulating hypotheses and inferring meanings; and so on. In other words, it should help students become better language learners and users. (Boulton, Reference Boulton, Thorne and May2017: 2).

5. Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the role of DDL focus-on-form tasks in EFL learners’ task engagement. The results of quantitative analyses indicated that incorporating DDL form-focused tasks into English language classes significantly increased L2 learners’ task engagement in the short run. Additionally, the results of qualitative analyses showed that DDL form-focused tasks enhanced learners’ task engagement by promoting their motivation, curiosity, interest, discovery learning, concentration, and autonomy and giving them access to rich unteachable materials. The findings further support the arguments made in previous studies regarding the beneficial role of DDL form-focused tasks in learners’ academic English lecture comprehension, task motivation, and autonomy (Lin & Lee, Reference Lin and Lee2015; Zare & Aqajani Delavar, Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2022, Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2023). The present study lends more credence to the facilitative role of DDL tasks incorporated in a form-focused approach to English language teaching (Boulton, Reference Boulton, Thorne and May2017, Reference Boulton and Crosthwaite2019; Leńko-Szymańska & Boulton, Reference Leńko-Szymańska, Boulton, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015; Zare & Aqajani Delavar, Reference Zare and Aqajani Delavar2023). It is interpreted that the direct use of classroom concordancing provides L2 learners with rich, naturally occurring linguistic input (Leńko-Szymańska & Boulton, Reference Leńko-Szymańska, Boulton, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015), which subsequently develops their autonomy and motivation for language learning (O’Keeffe, Reference O’Keeffe2021). With regard to the tenets of self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2017), it is believed that L2 learners with higher levels of motivation and autonomy are more likely to be engaged in language learning despite hurdles and difficulties (Mercer, Reference Mercer and Gao2019).

In addition, as the findings suggested, the significant impact of the proposed DDL tasks on task engagement was not durable. This finding can be attributed to the complicated nature of engagement (Philp & Duchesne, Reference Philp and Duchesne2016) being affected by a multitude of social, contextual, and psychological factors (Hiver et al., Reference Hiver, Al-Hoorie, Vitta and Wu2024). Furthermore, as stressed by Zhang (Reference Zhang2022), a combination of different aspects of learner engagement with appropriate language learning tasks coupled with productive learning contexts is conducive to L2 achievement. Moreover, it might be interpreted that discovery-based language learning can be challenging and disengaging for learners in longer periods of time (Boulton, Reference Boulton2010). The temporary impact of form-focused DDL on EFL learners’ task engagement can also be explained by the fact that DDL causes an increase in learners’ foreign language anxiety (Zare et al., Reference Zare, Karimpour and Aqajani Delavar2022), which negatively impacts language learners’ engagement (O’Reilly & García-Castro, Reference O’Reilly and García-Castro2022). Accordingly, to obtain more conclusive evidence, further longitudinal studies are required to explore whether DDL tasks exert any significant influence on learners’ task engagement in the long run.

In particular, in line with the model of task engagement proposed by Egbert et al. (Reference Egbert, Shahrokni, Zhang, Abobaker, Bantawtook, He, Bekar, Roe and Huh2021), the qualitative findings of the present study revealed that DDL tasks in form-focused instruction can pave the path for increasing task engagement through offering authentic language materials, a sense of autonomy, and interesting learning challenges. The findings also corroborate with the study of Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton (Reference Leńko-Szymańska, Boulton, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015), noting that authenticity, interaction with concordancers, and autonomy are key features of DDL tasks that can facilitate the enhancement of L2 learners’ engagement. One point worthy of mentioning is that to improve learners’ involvement in learning, DDL tasks need to be authentically interesting and logically challenging (Csikszentmihályi, Reference Csikszentmihályi1990; Mishan, Reference Mishan2004). Furthermore, analyzing the learners’ perceptions revealed that DDL form-focused tasks could lead to increased engagement with working with corpora for formulating proper queries because doing such discovery-oriented tasks calls for high levels of concentration, interest, curiosity, and independence (Boulton & Cobb, Reference Boulton and Cobb2017; Gilquin & Granger, Reference Gilquin, Granger, O’Keeffe and McCarthy2010; Leńko-Szymańska & Boulton, Reference Leńko-Szymańska, Boulton, Leńko-Szymańska and Boulton2015; Zare et al., Reference Zare, Karimpour and Aqajani Delavar2022).

In sum, the present study revealed that incorporating DDL form-focused tasks into English language classes has the potential to increase EFL learners’ engagement in the short run. Experiencing such an inductive, discovery-oriented approach to doing tasks would help EFL learners remain motivated and engaged (Gilquin & Granger, Reference Gilquin, Granger, O’Keeffe and McCarthy2010). This is in line with the study of Daskalovska (Reference Daskalovska2015), which argues that corpus-based language tasks are motivational and engaging enough and L2 learners discover the information autonomously. In other words, DDL requires that EFL learners become engaged in their learning process by using various cognitive skills such as comparing, classifying, reasoning, analyzing, inferencing, and interpreting (Boulton & Cobb, Reference Boulton and Cobb2017). The students’ perspective also noted a series of distinguishing DDL form-focused task features – that is, motivating, authentic, autonomy-raising, intriguing (curiosity), engrossing (concentration), and interesting, albeit challenging – required for task engagement. However, based on the findings of the current study, doing such tasks is less likely to have lasting significant effects on L2 learners’ commitment due to their demanding and time-consuming nature. It might also be due to the novelty of the DDL form-focused approach to English language teaching (Zare et al., Reference Zare, Karimpour and Aqajani Delavar2022) that calls for a revision in language teachers’ and learners’ roles in task-based classes.

The present study has some limitations that should be acknowledged. First, only female language learners with intermediate general English language proficiency levels were recruited to participate in the current study, which restricts the generalizability of the findings. Further studies are required to study both male and female learners with various levels of language proficiency. Second, the participants’ task engagement was measured with self-report data. Self-report measures are prone to biases like honesty, social desirability, response bias, and introspective ability that can affect their reliability and validity (Demetriou et al., Reference Demetriou, Ozer, Essau, Cautin and Lilienfeld2015). Hence, avid researchers are recommended to employ a more rigorous methodological triangulation (e.g. observing language learners while completing the form-focused DDL tasks, using think-aloud protocols, and utilizing video-stimulated recall) to depict a broader picture of the way the proposed tasks impact their task engagement. The use of tech-rich methods, such as eye tracking and automatic logging, is also recommended for measuring engagement (Dewan, Murshed & Lin, Reference Dewan, Murshed and Lin2019). Last but not least, exploring the role of contextual factors was out of the scope of the present study. Future ecological studies are required to explore EFL learners’ gains as a result of doing form-focused DDL tasks inside the dynamic nature of the classroom.

6. Conclusion

The current study set out to contribute to the understanding of the effect of DDL form-focused tasks on learners’ task engagement. The results revealed that providing EFL learners with DDL form-focused tasks can be one way to enhance their task engagement in the short run by promoting their motivation, curiosity, interest, discovery learning, concentration, and autonomy. Moreover, it was indicated that DDL form-focused tasks did not have delayed significant effects on EFL learners’ task engagement. These findings, while contributing to research on DDL learning, call for further investigation of the applicability of DDL form-focused tasks and their influence on the complicated network of learners’ psychological states.

Based on the results of the present study, it might be concluded that DDL form-focused tasks improve language learners’ task engagement by increasing their motivation, curiosity, consciousness, autonomy, and discovery learning by means of offering authentic unteachable materials that are interesting, albeit challenging. A pedagogical implication is that incorporating form-focused DDL tasks into English language classes can enhance learners’ agency, motivation, and interest to sustain their engagement. Accordingly, materials developers are highly recommended to integrate DDL form-focused tasks to enhance L2 learners’ task engagement as a prerequisite for academic success. Language teachers should bear in mind that modeling how to use concordancing for academic purposes coupled with the provision of scaffolded prompts are a useful means to familiarize L2 learners with the application of concordances in general and DDL form-focused tasks in particular. Another implication is that L2 teachers can take an integrated approach to their teaching with the inclusion of consciousness-raising, communicative tasks that enhance language learners’ metalinguistic awareness and orientation towards discovery learning. We believe that such integration will result in more task engagement and language achievement.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0958344024000120

Acknowledgements

The researchers gratefully acknowledge financial support from Kosar University of Bojnord. The author Javad Zare was supported by Kosar University of Bojnord with grant number 0208161714.

Ethical statement and competing interests

Ethical considerations, such as voluntary participation, anonymity, and confidentiality of the learners, were considered. The authors declare no competing interests.

About the authors

Javad Zare is Associate Professor of Applied Linguistics at Kosar University of Bojnord, Bojnord, Iran. His most recent publications have appeared in Applied Linguistics, System, Computer Assisted Language Learning, Journal of Second Language Writing, Language Teaching Research, ReCALL, Frontiers in Psychology, Educational Review, British Journal of Educational Studies, International Journal of Applied Linguistics, Current Psychology, Brain Sciences, Journal of Pragmatics, Lingua, English for Specific Purposes, Journal of English for Academic Purposes, Discourse Processes, and Text & Talk. His research areas of interest include positive psychology, computer-assisted language learning, task-based language teaching, corpus linguistics, and English for academic purposes.

Mostafa Azari Noughabi has a PhD in applied linguistics from Hakim Sabzevari University, Iran. He has published articles in various journals, including System, Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, Journal of Multilingual & Multicultural Development, Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching, The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher, English Teaching & Learning, Current Psychology, and Porta Linguarum. He is a member of the editorial board of Current Psychology. His research interests are positive psychology in SLA, computer-assisted language learning, L2 teacher education, and L2 assessment.

Ahmad Al-Issa is Professor of English Linguistics at the American University of Sharjah in the United Arab Emirates. His research interests are in the areas of sociolinguistics, intercultural communication, language and identity, global English, pragmatics, language planning, and language policy.

References

Ainley, M. (2012) Students’ interest and engagement in classroom activities. In Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L. & Wylie, C. (eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement. Boston: Springer, 283302. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_13 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aubrey, S., King, J. & Almukhaild, H. (2022) Language learner engagement during speaking tasks: A longitudinal study. RELC Journal, 53(3): 519533. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033688220945418 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batstone, R. (ed.) (2010) Sociocognitive perspectives on language use and language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bernardini, S. (2004) Corpora in the classroom: An overview and some reflections on future developments. In Sinclair, M. H. (ed.), How to use corpora in language teaching. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 1536. https://doi.org/10.1075/scl.12.05ber CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boulton, A. (2009) Testing the limits of data-driven learning: Language proficiency and training. ReCALL, 21(1): 3754. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0958344009000068 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boulton, A. (2010) Data‐driven learning: Taking the computer out of the equation. Language Learning, 60(3), 534572. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00566.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boulton, A. (2017) Data-driven learning and language pedagogy. In Thorne, S. & May, S. (eds.), Language, education and technology: Encyclopedia of language and education (3rd ed.). Cham: Springer, 181192. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-02237-6_15 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boulton, A. (2019) Data-driven learning for younger learners: Obstacles and optimism. In Crosthwaite, P. (ed.), Data-driven learning for the next generation: Corpora and DDL for pre-tertiary learners. Abingdon: Routledge, 1420.Google Scholar
Boulton, A. & Cobb, T. (2017) Corpus use in language learning: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 67(2): 348393. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12224 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, W.-L. & Sun, Y.-C. (2009) Scaffolding and web concordancers as support for language learning. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 22(4): 283302. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588220903184518 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihályi, M. (1990) Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Curado Fuentes, A. (2015) Exploiting keywords in a DDL approach to the comprehension of news texts by lower-level learners. In , A. LeńkoSzymańskab & A. Boulton, (eds.), Multiple affordances of language corpora for data-driven learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 177198. https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688211001289 Google Scholar
Daskalovska, N. (2015) Corpus-based versus traditional learning of collocations. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 28(2): 130144. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2013.803982 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demetriou, C., Ozer, B. U. & Essau, C. A. (2015) Self-Report questionnaires. In Cautin, R. L. & Lilienfeld, S. O. (eds.), The encyclopedia of clinical psychology. New Jersey: Wiley, 16. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118625392.wbecp507 Google Scholar
Dewan, M. A. A., Murshed, M. & Lin, F. (2019) Engagement detection in online learning: A review. Smart Learning Environments, 6: Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40561-018-0080-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egbert, J. L., Shahrokni, S. A., Zhang, X., Abobaker, R., Bantawtook, P., He, H., Bekar, M., Roe, M. F. & Huh, K. (2021) Language task engagement: An evidence-based model. TESL-EJ, 24(4): 134. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1288722.pdf Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2003) Task-based language learning and teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2015) The importance of focus on form in communicative language teaching. Eurasian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 1(2): 112. https://doi.org/10.32601/ejal.460611 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2016) Focus on form: A critical review. Language Teaching Research, 20(3): 405428. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168816628627 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flowerdew, L. (2015) Data-driven learning and language learning theories: Whither the twain shall meet. In Leńko-Szymańska, A. & Boulton, A. (eds.), Multiple affordances of language corpora for data-driven learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 1536. https://doi.org/10.1075/scl.69.02flo Google Scholar
Gao, L. G., & Zhang, L. J. (2020) Teacher learning in difficult times: Examining foreign language teachers’ cognitions about online teaching to tide over COVID-19. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.549653 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilquin, G. & Granger, S. (2010) How can data-driven learning be used in language teaching? In O’Keeffe, A. & McCarthy, M. (eds.), The Routledge handbook of corpus linguistics. London: Routledge, 359370.10.4324/9780203856949-26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helme, S. & Clarke, D. (2001) Identifying cognitive engagement in the mathematics classroom. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 13(2): 133153. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03217103 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernández, T. A. (2018) Input flooding. In Liontas, J. I. (ed.), The TESOL encyclopedia of English language teaching. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, 17. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118784235.eelt0073 Google Scholar
Hiver, P., Al-Hoorie, A. H., Vitta, J. P. & Wu, J. (2024) Engagement in language learning: A systematic review of 20 years of research methods and definitions. Language Teaching Research, 28(1): 201230. https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688211001289 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johns, T. (1986) Micro-concord: A language learner’s research tool. System, 14(2): 151162. https://doi.org/10.1016/0346-251X(86)90004-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johns, T. (1991) From printout to handout: Grammar and vocabulary teaching in the context of data-driven learning. ELR Journal, 4: 2745.Google Scholar
Lee, H., Warschauer, M. & Lee, J. H. (2019) The effects of corpus use on second language vocabulary learning: A multilevel meta-analysis. Applied Linguistics, 40(5): 721753. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amy012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leńko-Szymańska, A., & Boulton, A. (2015) Data-Driven learning in language pedagogy. In Leńko-Szymańska, A. & Boulton, A. (eds.), Multiple affordances of language corpora for data-driven learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 114. https://doi.org/10.1075/scl.69.01int Google Scholar
Lin, M. H. & Lee, J.-Y. (2015) Data-driven learning: Changing the teaching of grammar in EFL classes. ELT Journal, 69(3): 264274. https://doi.org/10.1093/elt/ccv010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. (1991) Focus on form: A design feature in language teaching methodology. In de Bot, K., Ginsberg, R. B. & Kramsch, C. (eds.), Foreign language research in cross-cultural perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamin, 3952. https://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.2.07lon CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. (2000) Focus on form in task-based language teaching. In Lambert, R. D. & Shohamy, E. (eds.), Language policy and pedagogy: Essays in honor of A. Ronald Walton. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 179192. https://doi.org/10.1075/z.96.11lon CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. (2015) Second language acquisition and task-based language teaching. Malden: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Mercer, S. (2019) Language learner engagement: Setting the scene. In Gao, X. (ed.), Second handbook of English language teaching. Cham: Springer, 643660. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02899-2_40 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mishan, F. (2004) Authenticating corpora for language learning: A problem and its resolution. ELT Journal, 58(3): 219227. https://doi.org/10.1093/elt/58.3.219 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mozgalina, A. (2015) More or less choice? The influence of choice on task motivation and task engagement. System, 49: 120132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2015.01.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Keeffe, A. (2021) Data-driven learning – A call for a broader research gaze. Language Teaching, 54(2): 259272. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0261444820000245 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Reilly, J. & García-Castro, V. (2022) Exploring the relationship between foreign language anxiety and students’ online engagement at UK universities during the Covid-19 pandemic. Language Learning in Higher Education, 12(2): 409427. https://doi.org/10.1515/cercles-2022-2054 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Sullivan, Í. (2007) Enhancing a process-oriented approach to literacy and language learning: The role of corpus consultation literacy. ReCALL, 19(3): 269286. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095834400700033X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oga-Baldwin, W. L. Q. (2019) Acting, thinking, feeling, making, collaborating: The engagement process in foreign language learning. System, 86: Article 102128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2019.102128 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philp, J. & Duchesne, S. (2016) Exploring engagement in tasks in the language classroom. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 36: 5072. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190515000094 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plonsky, L. & Oswald, F. L. (2014) How big is “big”? Interpreting effect sizes in L2 research. Language Learning, 64(4): 878912. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12079 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reschly, A. L. & Christenson, S. L. (2012) Jingle, jangle, and conceptual haziness: Evolution and future directions of the engagement construct. In Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L. & Wylie, C. (eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement. Boston: Springer, 319. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, R. M. & Deci, E. L. (2017) Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. New York: Guilford Press. https://doi.org/10.1521/978.14625/28806 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schenck, A. & Baldwin, M. (2019) Is form-focused instruction really a waste of time? A review of past mistakes and future possibilities through the analysis of input enhancement. Journal of Linguistics and Language Teaching (JLLT), 10(1): 1128.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. (2001) Attention. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Cognition and second language instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 332. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139524780.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R. W. (1990) The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11(2): 129158. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/11.2.129 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A. & Furrer, C. J. (2009) A motivational perspective on engagement and disaffection: Conceptualization and assessment of children’s behavioral and emotional participation in academic activities in the classroom. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 69(3): 493525. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164408323233 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomlinson, B. (1994) Pragmatic awareness activities. Language Awareness, 3(3–4): 119129. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658416.1994.9959850 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomlinson, B. (2007) Using form-focused discovery approaches. In Fotos, S. & Nassaji, H. (eds.), Form-focused instruction and teacher education: Studies in honour of Rod Ellis. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 177192.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. & Smith, M. (2015) Aptitude as grammatical sensitivity and the initial stages of learning Japanese as a L2: Parametric variation and case marking. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(1): 135165. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263114000345 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zare, J. (2020) Awareness of discourse organizers and comprehension of academic lectures: The effect of using concordancers. Current Psychology, 39(2): 419427. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00579-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zare, J. & Aqajani Delavar, K. (2022) Enhancing English learning materials with data-driven learning: A mixed-methods study of task motivation. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2022.2134881 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zare, J. & Aqajani Delavar, K. (2023) A data-driven learning focus on form approach to academic English lecture comprehension. Applied Linguistics, 44(3): 485504. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amac021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zare, J., Karimpour, S. & Aqajani Delavar, K. (2022) The impact of concordancing on English learners’ foreign language anxiety and enjoyment: An application of data-driven learning. System, 109: Article 102891. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2022.102891 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zare, J., Karimpour, S. & Aqajani Delavar, K. (2023) Classroom concordancing and English academic lecture comprehension: An implication of data-driven learning. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 36(5–6): 885905. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2021.1953081 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zare, J. & Keivanlou-Shahrestanaki, Z. (2017) The language of English academic lectures: The case of field of study in highlighting importance. Lingua, 193: 3650. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2017.04.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Z. (2022) Learner engagement and language learning: A narrative inquiry of a successful language learner. The Language Learning Journal, 50(3): 378392. https://doi.org/10.1080/09571736.2020.1786712 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for task engagement

Figure 1

Table 2. Independent-samples t-tests for task engagement

Figure 2

Table 3. Tests of within-subjects effects for the comparison group

Figure 3

Table 4. Pairwise comparisons for the comparison group

Figure 4

Table 5. Tests of within-subjects effects for the intervention group

Figure 5

Table 6. Pairwise comparisons for the intervention group

Supplementary material: File

Zare et al. supplementary material

Zare et al. supplementary material
Download Zare et al. supplementary material(File)
File 175 KB