Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T08:19:56.718Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Measuring pragmatic competence of discourse output among Chinese-speaking individuals with traumatic brain injury

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2023

Anthony Pak-Hin Kong*
Affiliation:
Academic Unit of Human Communication, Development, and Information Sciences, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong The Aphasia Research and Therapy (ART) Laboratory, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong
Dustin Kai-Yan Lau
Affiliation:
Department of Chinese and Bilingual Studies, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong, Hong Kong
Daisy Ho-Ying Lai
Affiliation:
Department of Chinese and Bilingual Studies, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong, Hong Kong
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Objective:

Discourse analysis is one of the clinical methods commonly used to assess the language ability of individuals with traumatic brain injury (TBI). However, the majority of published analytic frameworks are not geared for highlighting the pragmatic aspect of discourse deficits in acquired language disorders, except for those designed for quantifying conversational samples. This study aimed to examine how pragmatic competence is impaired and reflected in spoken monologues in Chinese speakers with TBI.

Methods:

Discourse samples of five tasks (personal narrative, storytelling, procedural, single- and sequential picture description) were elicited from ten TBI survivors and their controls. Each discourse sample was measured using 16 indices (e.g., number of informative words, percentage of local/global coherence errors, repeated words or phrases) that corresponded to the four Gricean maxims. Twenty-five naïve Chinese speakers were also recruited to perform perceptual rating of the quality of all 50 TBI audio files (five discourse samples per TBI participant), in terms of erroneous/inaccurate information, adequacy of amount of information given, as well as degree of organization and clarity.

Results:

The maxim of quantity best predicted TBI’s pragmatic impairments. Naïve listeners’ perception of pragmatics deficits correlated to measures on total and informative words, as well as number and length of terminable units. Clinically, personal narrative and storytelling tasks could better elicit violations in pragmatics.

Conclusion:

Applying Gricean maxims in monologic oral narratives could capture the hallmark underlying pragmatic problems in TBI. This may help provide an additional approach of clinically assessing social communications in and subsequent management of TBI.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Australasian Society for the Study of Brain Impairment

Introduction

Individuals with traumatic brain injury (TBI) often demonstrate a variety of communication problems. The communication problems are suggested to result from disruptions in the complex interactions between cognitive, linguistic, and social demands (Coelho, Liles, & Duffy, Reference Coelho, Liles and Duffy1995). Instead of showing linguistic impairments in formal language assessments, individuals with TBI are usually observed to demonstrate deficits in linguistic processing (e.g., Sarno, Reference Sarno1980, Reference Sarno1984). For example, in terms of sentence production, Ellis and Peach (Reference Ellis and Peach2009) reported longer initiation times in the sentences produced by speakers with TBI and concluded that speakers with TBI suffered from problems with sentence planning. Similarly, Lau, Kong, and Chan (Reference Lau, Kong and Chan2022) obtained language samples from Chinese-speaking participants and observed that the TBI group produced more errors, different varieties of sentence types, and lower syntactic complexity in their sentence production compared with the control group. The manifestation of language impairments of individuals with TBI is usually reported to be even more pronounced beyond syntactic measures. Coelho et al. (Reference Coelho, Grela, Corso, Gamble and Feinn2005), for example, obtained narrative samples and reported disorganization of spoken output in individuals with TBI that manifest across sentential boundaries. Kong, Lau, and Cheng (Reference Kong, Lau and Cheng2020) also reported deficits in the thematic organization of main ideas in the narratives produced by individuals with TBI. Overall, the language disruptions observed among TBI survivors appeared to be less apparent when measured in terms of syntactic forms of language, but more apparent when the use of language is quantified (e.g., Hagan, Reference Hagan and Holland1984; Kong, Lau, & Chan, Reference Kong, Lau and Chan2019).

Pragmatic competence is the ability to use language effectively in a contextually appropriate fashion (Penn, Reference Penn1999) and to communicate effectively across contexts (Dahlberg et al., Reference Dahlberg, Cusick, Hawley, Newman, Morey, Harrison-Felix and Whiteneck2007). Many survivors of TBI demonstrate a relatively intact language ability, but their pragmatic skills are impaired, causing considerable barriers to effective communication. For example, some recent reports examining the communicative-pragmatic disorders in speakers with TBI have suggested that both impaired cognitive performance in executive functions and theory of mind could contribute to their pragmatic difficulties (e.g., Bosco, Gabbatore, Angeleri, Zettin, & Parola, Reference Bosco, Gabbatore, Angeleri, Zettin and Parola2018; Bosco, Parola, Sacco, Zettin, & Angeleri, Reference Bosco, Parola, Sacco, Zettin and Angeleri2017). Rowley, Rogish, Alexander, and Riggs (Reference Rowley, Rogish, Alexander and Riggs2017) have also reported significant correlations between cognitive measures (e.g., declarative or working memory, attention, and executive functions) and pragmatic comprehension in adults with TBI. In addition, Carlomagno et al. (Reference Carlomagno, Giannotti, Vorano and Marini2011) and Marini et al. (Reference Marini, Galetto, Zampieri, Vorano, Zettin and Carlomagno2011) have previously stated that Italian and English speakers with TBI, respectively, demonstrated intact language ability in terms of the amount of information content within discourse production (elicited using a picture description task of single and sequential pictures) being comparable to healthy controls; in contrast, pragmatic deficits were found including errors of cohesion and coherence. Furthermore, individuals with TBI have been reported to demonstrate the following impairments, including overly talkative (Hagan, Reference Hagan and Holland1984), tangential and inappropriate (Kong et al., Reference Kong, Lau and Chan2019) as well as reduced (Gao, Kong, & Lau, Reference Gao, Kong and Lau2016) speech content, unable to convey sufficiently detailed information (McDonald & Van Sommers, Reference McDonald and Van Sommers1993), inappropriate verbal (e.g., excessive foul language, yelling, or shouting) as well as non-verbal (e.g., difficulty perceiving personal space, limited facial expressions, or reduced prosody and intonation) behaviors (Mahar & Fraser, Reference Mahar and Fraser2011), and excessive verbal requests (McDonald & Van Sommers, Reference McDonald and Van Sommers1993) or inappropriate initiation, termination, or turn-taking during conversations (Mahar & Fraser, Reference Mahar and Fraser2011).

To ensure appropriate and effective communication, Grice (Reference Grice1975) proposed the Cooperative Principle and its four maxims, namely Quality, Quantity, Relevance, and Manner, to describe how effective communication in conversation can be achieved in common social situations. Violations in Gricean maxims may result in misunderstanding and false information exchange between speakers and listeners; these violations demonstrated by those with TBI would be more apparent (i.e., more problematic communication) and lead to more devastating effects in pragmatics (Douglas, Reference Douglas2010). For instance, compromised maxim of quantity and quality may manifest in leaving out of important/essential details or giving false information, respectively, in TBI’s output (see additional examples in Douglas, Bracy, & Snow, Reference Douglas, Bracy and Snow2007).

There are three prominent pragmatic language skills that are frequently disrupted in adults with pragmatic impairments, such as those caused by a TBI, including the interpretation of implicature, the use of reference, and the management of information (Cummings, Reference Cummings2021). However, relative to impaired semantics and phonology, much less research have been conducted to investigate pragmatic impairments and this has resulted in fewer evidence-based rehabilitation programs/protocols or therapy approaches readily available for clinical application (Cummings, Reference Cummings2021). Most existing standardized language batteries for acquired communication disorders (such as aphasia) focus primarily on language comprehension (e.g., reading or auditory passage comprehension tasks) and expression (e.g., naming or sentence production tasks) and do not consider one’s pragmatic competence – see Comprehensive Aphasia Test (Swinburn, Porter, & Howard, Reference Swinburn, Porter and Howard2004) or Western Aphasia Battery (Kertesz, Reference Kertesz1982) and their Cantonese versions (Kong & Ng, Reference Kong and Ng2022; Yiu, Reference Yiu1992). There are also assessments that are cognitive communication focused, such as the Measure of Cognitive Linguistic Abilities (MCLA; Ellmo, Reference Ellmo1995), Montreal Protocol for the Evaluation of Communication (MEC; Joanette et al., Reference Joanette, Ska, Cote, Ferre, LaPointe, Coppens and Small2015), or Birmingham Cognitive Screen (BCoS; Humphreys, Bickerton, Samson, & Riddoch, Reference Humphreys, Bickerton, Samson and Riddoch2012) and its Cantonese version (Kong et al., Reference Kong, Chan, Lau, Bickerton, Weekes and Humphreys2018; Pan et al., Reference Pan, Chen, Bickerton, Lau, Kong, Rotshtein and Humphreys2015). Clinicians often need to use specific checklists or profiles targeting verbal and non-verbal pragmatic behaviors to assess one’s pragmatic skills. For example, Prutting and Kirchner’s (Reference Prutting and Kirchner1987) Pragmatic Protocol is based on the use of a speech act theory as the means of organizing pragmatic parameters. Based on this protocol, breakdowns in pragmatic behaviors could be categorized according to utterance acts, propositional acts, illocutionary and perlocutionary acts. In another example, Bishop (Reference Bishop2003) proposed a Children’s Communication Checklist, based on results obtained from a questionnaire given to families of 87 children with pragmatic language impairments. Although these checklists vary in the number and range of pragmatic behaviors that were examined, it was concluded that corresponding results were comprehensive enough to clinically guide intervention.

On the other hand, there is an increasing number of studies that reported the use of assessments based on conversation (Angeleri et al., Reference Angeleri, Bosco, Zettin, Sacco, Colle and Bara2008) or discourse analysis (Steel & Togher, Reference Steel and Togher2019) to characterize the pragmatic impairments of one’s spoken output. These assessments tended to be labor- and time-intensive because they typically required interviews and/or running of questionnaires with a client and the communicative partner, involved language sample transcriptions, and extensive coding or annotation of appropriate verbal and non-verbal pragmatic behaviors. However, proper analyses of the final results can help reveal the comprehensive nature of pragmatic disorders, ranging from areas of speech acts (such as topic management or turn-taking) to inter-sentential cohesion and coherence (Cummings, Reference Cummings2009).

Kong et al. (Reference Kong, Lau and Cheng2020) have recently reported impaired global and local coherence in Chinese-speaking TBI survivors. Specifically, two groups of speakers with neurogenic communication disorders, including one group of stroke survivors with aphasia and another group of individuals with TBI, provided language samples of single- and sequential picture description and storytelling. The elicited discourse samples were quantified in terms of global and local coherence ratings, sequence of content (e.g., main events), and informativeness. The authors found that the problematic sequence of main events produced by the TBI group correlated significantly with their reduced global coherence. Their impaired language integrity was also associated with problems of local coherence. It was concluded that secondary to the inherent linguistic and cognitive dysfunction, these speakers tended to demonstrate a greater degree of difficulties not only in maintaining and coordinating the overall organization of spoken discourse, but also the association between adjacent utterances. Note also that according to Bliss and McCabe (Reference Bliss and McCabe2006), common discourse elicitation tasks, such as the ones mentioned previously mentioned in this Introduction section, are different in terms of the linguistic and cognitive demand (and therefore the level of difficulty) they placed on a speaker. Capilouto, Wright, and Wagovich (Reference Capilouto, Wright and Wagovich2006) have also suggested the number of pictorial stimuli and the target information embedded in these pictures would play a role in how much lexical content one would provide in a spoken discourse task; this applied to both participants with and without a language disorder. It remains unclear if, and how, pragmatic competence (or impairments) would potentially vary across different genres in the TBI population.

Aims

In short, previous studies have highlighted the disruptions in language pragmatics among individuals with TBI (Hagan, Reference Hagan and Holland1984; Mahar & Fraser, Reference Mahar and Fraser2011; McDonald & Van Sommers, Reference McDonald and Van Sommers1993), but its manifestation in Chinese speakers with TBI remains unclear. As suggested by González (Reference González2005), pragmatic markers in discourse are essential to the organization of oral narrative, but the overall quantity, occurrence, and distribution of these markers may vary not just across text genre but also languages. The current study aimed to explore how Chinese-speaking TBI individuals are pragmatically impaired in spoken discourse as measured by violations of Gricean maxims. In addition, the current study further explored how the pragmatic performance of the Chinese-speaking TBI individuals may vary according to the cognitive and social demands (e.g., Coelho et al., Reference Coelho, Liles and Duffy1995) by observing the significance of the effects of discourse genre and visual support in the discourse. Finally, despite being subtle, the language impairments of TBI individuals may interact with their pragmatic performance. Hence, we also intended to explore if any pragmatic impairments observed among the TBI individuals are associated with the severity of the individuals’ language impairments. In particular, there were five aims:

  1. 1. To examine if pragmatic measures can be used to differentiate between Chinese speakers with TBI and non-brain injured (NBI). With reference to Carlomagno et al. (Reference Carlomagno, Giannotti, Vorano and Marini2011) and Marini et al. (Reference Marini, Galetto, Zampieri, Vorano, Zettin and Carlomagno2011), it was predicted that violations of Gricean maxims would be found in the Chinese TBI population.

  2. 2. To determine if there is a genre effect (i.e., different discourse tasks of storytelling, single- and sequential picture description, procedural discourse, and personal monologues) on the pragmatic ability of the TBI and NBI groups. With the reference to Bliss and McCabe (Reference Bliss and McCabe2006), it was predicted that the pragmatic competence rating would be more well-preserved in storytelling as illustrations (in)directly informing the story structure of the expected discourse output were available for the TBI group; this would be followed by personal and procedural narratives that would require planning and organization of utterances around a theme. A higher degree of wordiness in the narrative and procedural tasks was also expected which would reflect violation of the maxim of quantity. The results of this examination were anticipated to inform which of the five genres could provide the best evaluation of pragmatic ability following TBI.

  3. 3. To investigate how the existence of visual support (pictured vs. pictureless tasks), types of pictures (line-drawing, colored pictures, vs. real photos), and the number of pictures would play a role in reflecting pragmatic competence in the TBI and NBI groups. In previous studies, numbers of main events produced were found to increase with number of elicitation pictures (Capilouto et al., Reference Capilouto, Wright and Wagovich2006); which would be shown by a better performance in maxim of quantity of the present study. On the other hand, speakers would tend to use pronouns with shifting reference and pay more attention to the hearer’s mental representations (maxim of relation) in the absence of pictorial support (Cummings, Reference Cummings2021). In other words, it is anticipated that different elicitation tasks might place different cognitive demands on TBI speaker’s organization, resulting in the difference in the amount of information (maxim of quantity) as well as the degree and quality of coherence (maxim of relation).

  4. 4. To examine how pragmatic performance would vary as a function of the severity of language impairment, and types of aphasia or cognitive deficits in TBI. According to Cummings (Reference Cummings2009), pragmatic competence is highly correlated with cognition and language ability. It is predicted that the TBI group, who were comorbid with aphasia, would demonstrate poorer pragmatic performance.

  5. 5. To explore which of four pragmatic maxims would show a stronger relationship with naïve listeners’ subjective rating on discourse production from Chinese-speaking individuals with TBI. It was hypothesized that the maxims of quantity and relation would best correlate with naive listeners’ ratings. This is because the characteristics of pragmatic deficits associated with TBI (e.g., insufficient or too much information, poor local or global coherence) are easier to show in spoken discourse production, and these features are frequently perceived as limited output or increased verbosity and tangentiality in subjective judgment (Kong et al., Reference Kong, Lau and Chan2019).

Methods

Part 1: Discourse sample collection and analysis

Participants

A total of ten adults (aged 18 years or older; see Table 1) with TBI were recruited from the Guangdong Work Injury Rehabilitation Hospital. They included five native Cantonese and five native Mandarin speakers born in the Guangdong province of Mainland China. All TBI participants were diagnosed by neurologists and/or medical internists with a confirmed closed head brain injury that had occurred at least 5 months prior to the testing. Each participant was diagnosed with fluent aphasia with the Cantonese (CAB; Yiu, Reference Yiu1992) or Mandarin version of the Western Aphasia Battery (MAB; Peking University Health Science Center, 1988). None of the TBI participants received formal language interventions that addressed their spoken discourse. However, prior to the time this study was conducted, they had received training focusing on swallowing (primarily) and functional communication with treatment goals not overlapping with the aims of the present study (and hence posing any influences on the results). In addition, each of them received an evaluation of cognitive ability using the Chinese adaption of the Cognitive Language Quick Test (CLQT; Helm-Estabrooks, Reference Helm-Estabrooks2001).

Table 1. Background Information on Participants in the Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) and Non-brain Injured (NBI) Groups

a Based on Cantonese (CAB; Yiu, Reference Yiu1992) or Mandarin (Peking University, 1988) version of the Western Aphasia Battery. A cutoff of 96.4 and 98.4 was suggested for the Cantonese and Mandarin version, respectively.

b Chinese adaption of Cognitive Linguistic Quick Test (CLQT; Helm-Estabrooks, Reference Helm-Estabrooks2001). It was used as a standalone overview assessment that gives a broad perspective across relevant domains of cognition (attention, memory, executive functions, visuospatial skills) and language.

c Based on results of paired samples t-test, two groups were not significantly different for age, t(9) = −.755, p = .47, and years of education, t(9) = −1.616, p = .14.

Another ten NBI control participants (five native Cantonese and five native Mandarin speakers) matched in age (Young: below 40 years of age, Middle-age: 40–60 years, and Elderly: above 60 years) and education level (low: 0–11 years for the two younger groups, and 0–6 years for the elderly group; high: at least 12 years for the two younger groups, and at least 7 years for the elderly group) were also invited to join the study. The NBI group had no reported history of neurological deficits, head injuries, or other medical conditions that would affect their receptive and expressive language abilities.

Language samples

Elicitation of language samples followed the Cantonese Aphasia Bank Protocol (Kong & Law, Reference Kong and Law2019) modified for TBI. A total of five discourse productions with various types (i.e., line-drawing, colored pictures, real photos) and amounts of pictorial supports (i.e., 0 in the personal narrative task to 6 in a multiple picture description task), and time of display (i.e., withdrawn before or along the production) of visual support of each participant were elicited.

Specifically, for the single picture description tasks, one line-drawing of the ‘Cat rescue’ picture and a colored photograph of ‘Flood’ was presented to the participants during description. Concerning the sequential picture description task, a set of ‘Refused Umbrella’ line-drawing pictures with six panels was provided during the production. For the storytelling task, participants were asked to tell the story of ‘The Boy Who Cried Wolf’; five colored pictures were shown and then withdrawn before production. For the procedural discourse task, everyone was asked to describe the procedures to make an ‘Egg and Ham Sandwich’; photos and written text of the ingredients were provided only if a participant failed to give verbal responses. Prompts specific to the content of the above four discourse tasks were avoided. Finally, in the personal narrative task, participants were asked to recount an important event. This task was first designed as a personal monologue, but neutral prompts and conversation feedback (with or without follow up questions) were provided when participants presented limited production. All samples produced by the participants were audio-recorded.

Each discourse sample was measured using 16 indices, in terms of the four Gricean maxims, adopted and modified from Andreetta, Cantagallo, and Marini (Reference Andreetta, Cantagallo and Marini2012), Cummings (Reference Cummings2021), Galski, Tompkins, and Johnston (Reference Galski, Tompkins and Johnston1998), and Kong and Law (Reference Kong and Law2004). A detailed description of the procedure for calculating these indices is given in Table 2.

Table 2. Indices of Measuring Pragmatics

Part 2: Naïve listeners’ rating of discourse samples

A total of 25 naïve Chinese speakers were recruited to perform perceptual rating of all 50 audio files (five discourse samples per TBI participant). These listeners were, included three men and twenty-three women, all between 22 and 27 years. They were divided into four listening groups (three groups of six people and one group of seven people). The 50 files were randomized into four different sets of sequences, one for each listening group, before the participants were asked to listen and rate each file independently. Each naïve listener was asked to refer to the four statements (see Table 3) and to rate the quality of each TBI production using a 5-point Likert scale (5: strongly agree, agree, neutral, disagree, to 1: strongly disagree) immediately after listening to each audio recording. The four statements were presented simultaneously in both English and Chinese to the raters, and they could not communicate with other listeners when rating the samples.

Table 3. Statements and Corresponding Categories for Naïve Listeners’ Perceptual Rating of Audio Files

The listening session lasted for around 1.75 h, including a 15-min introduction and training, and a 5-min break. At the beginning of the rating session, the third author first introduced and explained the definition of the four rating statements, followed by a mock scoring using two different audio files, to ensure that all listeners fully understood the rating procedures and a common rating standard was established. The raters could also raise any inquiries in these practice trails.

Statistical analysis

To address Aim 1, pragmatic deficits consistent with previous research studies (e.g., errors of cohesion and coherence or violations of Gricean maxims) were measured. Independent sample t-tests were conducted between the TBI and NBI groups across all objective pragmatic measurements.

For Aim 2, in order to investigate the effect of group and genres (single picture description, multiple picture description, storytelling, procedural discourse, and personal narratives), the means of the ratings from naïve listeners were compared between the four Gricean maxims. To determine whether and how the existence of visual supports and types of pictorial stimuli would influence the pragmatic performance of individuals in the TBI group (Aim 3), the naïve listener ratings were compared with the pragmatic measures.

As for Aim 4, Pearson’s r correlations were conducted to examine the relationship between pragmatic competence and TBI severity. In addition, correlations between the unit-weighted composite score of the five cognitive domains in CLQT (attention, memory, language, executive functions, and visuospatial skill) and each discourse measure were administered. Finally, a Pearson’s r correlation was conducted to examine the relationships of the 16 pragmatic measures and naïve listeners’ ratings (Aim 5).

Results

Measures of maxims

Descriptive results of the TBI and NBI performance across all 16 pragmatic measures are summarized in Table 4. Correlation results between the pragmatic measures and the corresponding maxim are displayed in Table 5.

Table 4. Pragmatic Performance in the TBI and NBI Groups

* Values are presented in ‘mean (standard deviation), range’.

Table 5. Correlation (Pearson’s r) Among the Objective Measures Under the Four Maxims

*p < .05; **p < .01.

Relative to Measures of Quality, results of independent sample t-tests indicated that the TBI group made significantly more errors than the NBI group [t(98) = 2.46, p < .05], scored significantly lower in the Index of Error (IEr) [t(98) = 2.96, p < .01], and demonstrated significantly fewer repairs and revision of errors [t(98) = 3.89, p < .001]. However, the two groups did not differ in terms of syntactic accuracy. The results of Pearson’s r correlation (Table 5) further indicated that the ratings of the maxim of quality were significantly correlated with the number of errors produced, but not with IEr and number of repairs.

In terms of Measures of Quantity, results of independent sample t-tests indicated that the TBI group produced significantly shorter discourse than the NBI counterpart [t(98) = 4.70, p < .001], produced significantly fewer information words (I-words) [t(98) = 3.48, p < .001], terminable units (T-units) [t(98) = 3.57, p = .001], and words per T-unit [t(98) = 6.35, p < .001]. The results of Pearson’s r correlation (Table 5) also indicated that the ratings of the maxim of quantity were significantly correlated with all the four measures (v–viii).

Regarding Measure of Relation, the results of independent sample t-tests indicated that the two groups did not differ in terms of number of global and local coherence errors (p > .1). There was also a lack of significant correlation between the ratings of the maxim of relation and both global and local coherence errors (Table 5).

Finally, concerning Measure of Manner, the results of independent sample t-tests indicated the TBI group scored significantly lower in the Index of Communication Efficiency (ICE) [t(96) = 5.63, p < .001] and used significantly fewer cohesive devices per T-unit [t(98) = 2.21, p < .05] than the NBI group. However, the two groups did not differ in terms of Index of lexical efficiency (ILE) and the number of repetitions of words and phrases (p > .1). The results of Pearson’s r correlation (Table 5) further revealed that the ratings of the maxim of manner were significantly correlated with ICE only, but not the other three measures (ix, x, and xii).

Effect of genre, pictorial supports and language-cognitive deficits on pragmatic performance

Table 6 summarizes the means of the subjective ratings obtained from the naïve listeners by elicitation tasks. The results revealed a trend of increasing violation of maxims as visual supports during the elicitation decreased. Specifically, the personal narratives by the TBI speakers that contained the least amount of visual support were judged to contain the highest degree of violation of three out of four maxims. Interesting, the most violation in terms of the maxim of quality was found in the storytelling task, in which five picture cards were first provided and then removed during the participants’ production. As displayed in Table 7 a significant correlation was found only between the maxim of quantity and language ability (but not other cognitive domains).

Table 6. Means of the Results of Naïve Listeners’ Rating

Ratings in bolded indicates the highest degree of violation found in the corresponding maxim.

a The least amount of visual support on personal narrative task to the most support on sequential picture description.

b The higher the rating was, the more violation of the Maxim of Quality was found.

Table 7. Correlations (Pearson’s r) Between the Language-Cognitive Deficits of TBI Individuals and Their Pragmatic Performance by Maxims

CAB/MAB: Cantonese/Mandarin version of Western Aphasia Battery; CLQT: Cognitive Language Quick Test.

* p < .05.

Naïve listeners’ ratings

Results of the curve estimation (Fig. 1) indicated that Total Number of Words per task [F(2,47) = 26.452, p < .001], Number of I-word [F(2,47) = 33.931, p < .001], Number of Terminable Units [F(2,47) = 26.629, p < .001] and words per T-unit [F(2,47) = 6.997, p < .01] produced significantly predicted maxim of quantity in a quadratic model. Results of Pearson’s r correlation (Supplementary Table 1) also showed significant correlations of all ratings of maxims, but that between the maxims of quantity and manner.

Figure 1. Quadratic models analyze among the indices of Maxims of Quantity.

Inter-rater and intra-rater reliability

Of the samples, 20% were randomly selected and re-analyzed by the third author to estimate the degree of intra-rater reliability. The same samples were also independently analyzed by a second independent examiner blinded to this study to obtain inter-rater reliability. The results of Cronbach’s alpha (see Supplementary Table 2) indicated good to excellent inter-rater and intra-rater.

Discussion

TBI and pragmatic impairments

The current study compared discourse production between Chinese speakers with and without TBI from the perspective of pragmatics with reference to the four maxims of quality, quantity, relevance, and manner (Grice, Reference Grice1975). The results supported our prediction of significantly more maxim violation in the TBI group. In particular, the two speaker groups differed the most in terms of the maxim of quantity and the Index of Communication Efficiency (ICE). This finding extended earlier conclusions by Carlomagno et al. (Reference Carlomagno, Giannotti, Vorano and Marini2011) and Marini et al. (Reference Marini, Galetto, Zampieri, Vorano, Zettin and Carlomagno2011) regarding the distinctive discourse feature of inadequate informativeness but relatively intact language ability in the TBI population of Indo-European languages; here we argue the same difficulty exists in TBI survivors (in our case, to be more specific, TBI speakers with anomic aphasia) in the East. A recent neurophysiological study to examine speech act and lexico-semantic processing in healthy adults (Egorova, Shtyrov, & Pulvermuller, Reference Egorova, Shtyrov and Pulvermuller2013) has suggested parallel activation of pragmatic and linguistic processing; the behavioral evidence from the present study also seems to be in line with that.

Moreover, the discourse measures related to maxim of quality were highly correlated to the listeners’ ratings on the appropriate amount and informativeness with discourse production in adults with TBI. Interestingly, the quadratic relationship (in Fig. 1) suggested that the amount of information provided in a discourse and one’s judgment of a ‘good’ production was not in a linear relation. This observation, in line with Steel and Togher (Reference Steel and Togher2019), confirmed the uniqueness of successful social communication which only requires the right amount of information to convey appropriate content (in a way that urges the communication partner to sustain an exchange of messages). Similarly, the two groups’ significant differences in ICE and usage of cohesive ties were also consistent with previous reports that listeners relied on the rate (of how quickly) the information was provided and whether it was presented in an organized way (Cummings, Reference Cummings2021) to judge spoken discourse naturalness.

One unexpected finding, however, was that the two groups did not differ in terms of the maxim of manner which was associated with local and global coherence. Global and local coherence were related to the maxim of relation as they concern how well each sentence relates to the overall theme or to adjacent sentences, i.e., the aspect of relevancy in discourse as defined by Grice (Reference Grice1975). Unlike previous findings specific to discourse impairments in Chinese-speaking TBI survivors, in terms of microstructural (e.g., across-sentence analysis of cohesion and coherence; Kong et al., Reference Kong, Lau and Cheng2020; Mok, Kong, & Lau, Reference Mok, Kong and Lau2016) as well as macrostructural (e.g., local and global coherence; Chow, Kong, & Lau, Reference Chow, Kong and Lau2016) deficits, the current study did not find any significant differences between the TBI and NBI groups. All language samples presented in this study were monologic discourse while communication problems were suggested to be more readily apparent in a conversational interaction (Van Leer & Turkstra, Reference Van Leer and Turkstra1999). It was unclear if the lack of conversational samples considered or the current pool of TBI participants being milder in terms of aphasia severity impairment (average aphasia quotient of 87.5, ranging from 76 to 92; Worrall et al., Reference Worrall, Simmons-Mackie, Wallace, Rose, Brady, Kong and Hallowell2016) might have contributed to this phenomenon. Similar to earlier reports that had failed to identify differences of coherence competency between individuals with and without TBI (Biddle, McCabe, & Bliss, Reference Biddle, McCabe and Bliss1996; Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Culhane, Levin, Harward, Mendelsohn, Ewing-Cobbs and Bruce1992), conversational samples may be essential to allow a more holistic examination of coherence performance in TBI.

Of the four Gricean maxims, our results indicated that a significantly higher correlation was found between pragmatics indices and listeners’ rating in terms of Quantity. Compared with the other three maxims, the definition of this aspect (focusing on content, length, and depth) is more objective, concrete, and possibly easier for naïve listeners to rate. One may also argue that the other maxims are not entirely mutually exclusive to each other. For example, violation of maxim of quality (i.e., truth) may lead to perception of a lower degree of relevance (maxim of relation) and/or clarity (maxim of manner).

With the use of carefully selected pragmatic measures to compare differences between the TBI and NBI groups, the current study added support to the notion that pragmatic impairments are observed in speakers with TBI. However, one potential limitation here was the small sample size. One may also notice the relatively large variation in months post onset among the participants with TBI in this study. Due to the small sample size of two subgroups (i.e., speakers with a recent TBI vs. those with a longer time post TBI), group difference on pragmatic problems was not compared here; future extension involving this systematic comparison with a larger pool of participants is warranted. Considering the uniqueness of pragmatics (e.g., highly individualized for which proper and objective quantification is far from simple), assessments containing only monologic discourse tasks without conversation samples may not be comprehensive enough to provide a holistic picture of the pragmatic problems in TBI. There is need for the further investigation of theoretically based evaluation of discourse pragmatics in TBI.

Discourse stimuli and pragmatics

It was clear that the presence of picture stimuli, their characteristics, and how they were utilized in a discourse task influenced our TBI speakers’ performance, which was in turn identified by naïve listeners. While the different types of images used in the present study varied in terms of the levels of cognitive support (i.e., number of visual stimuli provided, the duration of presentation, and presence vs. retraction during a speaker’s production), it was found that personal narratives (elicited with the least amount of visual supports among the five tasks) had the most violations of pragmatic maxims. In contrast, the sequential picture description task yielded the best listeners’ perception (Table 6). It is believed that the picture series presented an ongoing story with connections between the events, which could better elicit a more organized and efficient discourse from speakers in the TBI group (Potechin, Nicholas, & Brookshire, Reference Potechin, Nicholas and Brookshire1987). The cognitive demand in planning and organizing the information was also lower on the part of the speakers when the pictures were present, freeing up mental buffer for the pragmatic aspects during the production and hence reducing the violation in maxims of quantity, manner, and relation. In line with the hypothesis that the visual stimuli manipulated the speakers’ level of cognitive load and arousal, it was also believed that the visual supports with the extralinguistic context embedded in the sequential pictures also made it easier to analyze and determine the content and accuracy of the production (Potechin et al., Reference Potechin, Nicholas and Brookshire1987).

When interpreting the current results, it is worth mentioning that comparing (and controlling) the complexity across spoken discourse tasks is not straightforward. It was found that there were more violations the maxim of quality in the storytelling task, which contained the second largest amount of pictorial support (five picture cards). However, since the TBI participants were actually required to provide the story after these pictures had been removed, the higher degree of violations could be attributed to the increase memory loading induced by withdrawal of visual stimuli during the production. In order to tell the story, the speakers would need to create mental representations to understand, remember, and organize details. Hence, the overall performance could have been compromised by their difficulties with sustained attention, memory, and planning. On the other hand, the task of personal narratives contained the least amount of visual support and yielded the lowest degree of violations of maxim of quality. A possible explanation for this was the higher personal relevancy of this task to the speakers, for which they could select the information they were confident to share and avoid making errors (Lai, Law, & Kong, Reference Lai, Law and Kong2017; Law, Kong, Lai, & Lai, Reference Law, Kong, Lai and Lai2015).

Clinical implications

Discourse analysis is one of the clinical methodologies that is commonly used to assess TBI speakers’ language ability. However, the majority of published analytic frameworks are not geared for highlighting the pragmatic aspect of discourse deficits in acquired language disorders, except for a small number of investigations that attempted to quantify samples of picture description (e.g., Snow, Douglas, & Ponsfordoe, Reference Snow, Douglas and Ponsfordoe1999) or procedural discourse (e.g., Snow, Douglas, & Ponsfordoe, Reference Snow, Douglas and Ponsfordoe1997). The fact that we applied Gricean maxims in monologic oral narratives and revealed its clinical feasibility to capture the hallmark of pragmatic problems underlying TBI may help provide an additional approach of clinical assessment.

With reference to our results, personal narration (albeit of its high personal relevancy to the speaker) and the storytelling task (that taps and requires a higher level of information planning) seemed to better elicit violations in pragmatics. Specifically, it was observed that the participants with TBI demonstrated more violation of maxims than their NBI counterparts. Such violations may be particularly prominent in discourse tasks that provide less visual support and/or pose a higher demand of organizational and cognitive skills. As suggested by Coelho (Reference Coelho2007), discourse impairments in TBI may persist for years after its onset, which can pose a negative and long-lasting impact on quality of life. It is hoped that the finding of this study will render clinicians additional evidence on assessing social communication among adults with TBI and to support their decision-making on informative management of TBI.

Discourse analysis is one of the clinical methodologies that is commonly used to assess TBI speakers’ language ability (Kong, Reference Kong2009).

Supplementary materials

For supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/BrImp.2022.36

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to express gratitude to Dr. Jie Zhu and clinicians in the Speech Therapy Department of the Guangdong Work Injury Rehabilitation Hospital (Guangzhou, China) for their help in participant recruitment and testing. The authors would also like to show utmost appreciation to all TBI and NBI participants for their help and cooperation during data collection, as well as all the naïve listeners who were willing to spend hours of times for the study without any monetary compensation.

Financial support

This work was supported by The 2016 Departmental General Research Funds sponsored by the Research Office of The Polytechnic University of Hong Kong (project code 4-ZZFP) to the first and second authors.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Andreetta, S., Cantagallo, A., & Marini, A. (2012). Narrative discourse in anomic aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 50(8), 17871793. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angeleri, R., Bosco, F. M., Zettin, M., Sacco, K., Colle, L., & Bara, B. G. (2008). Communicative impairment in traumatic brain injury: A complete pragmatic assessment. Brain and Language, 107(3), 229245. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.01.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biddle, K. R., McCabe, A., & Bliss, L. S. (1996). Narrative skills following traumatic brain injury in children and adults. Journal of Communication Disorders, 29(6), 447469. doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(95)00038-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). The Children’s Communication Checklist, version 2 (CCC-2). London: Pearson.Google Scholar
Bliss, L. S., & McCabe, A. (2006). Comparison of discourse genres: Clinical implications. Contemporary Issues in Communication Science and Disorders, 33(Fall), 126167. doi: 10.1044/cicsd_33_F_126 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosco, F. M., Gabbatore, I., Angeleri, R., Zettin, M., & Parola, A. (2018). Do executive function and theory of mind predict pragmatic abilities following traumatic brain injury? An analysis of sincere, deceitful and ironic communicative acts. Journal of Communication Disorders, 75, 102117. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2018.05.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosco, F. M., Parola, A., Sacco, K., Zettin, M., & Angeleri, R. (2017). Communicative-pragmatic disorders in traumatic brain injury: The role of theory of mind and executive functions. Brain and Language, 168, 7383. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.01.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capilouto, G. J., Wright, H. H., & Wagovich, S. A. (2006). Reliability of main event measurement in the discourse of individuals with aphasia. Aphasiology, 20(2-4), 205216. doi: 10.1080/02687030500473122 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlomagno, S., Giannotti, S., Vorano, L., & Marini, A. (2011). Discourse information content in non-aphasic adults with brain injury: A pilot study. Brain Injury, 25(10), 10101018. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2011.605097 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, S. B., Culhane, K. A., Levin, H. S., Harward, H., Mendelsohn, D., Ewing-Cobbs, L., …Bruce, D. (1992). Narrative discourse after closed head injury in children and adolescents. Brain and Language, 43(1), 4265. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(92)90020-F CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chow, W., Kong, A., & Lau, K. (2016). An investigation of global and local coherence of spontaneous personal versus descriptive narratives in native Chinese speakers with traumatic brain injury: Preliminary data. Frontiers in Psychology. doi: 10.3389/conf.fpsyg.2016.68.00018 Google Scholar
Coelho, C. A. (2007). Management of discourse deficits following traumatic brain injury: Progress, caveats, and needs. Seminars in Speech and Language, 28(2), 122135. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-970570 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coelho, C. A., Grela, B., Corso, M., Gamble, A., & Feinn, R. (2005). Microlinguistic deficits in the narrative discourse of adults with traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 19(13), 11391145. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050500110678 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coelho, C. A., Liles, B. Z., & Duffy, R. J. (1995). Impairments of discourse abilities and executive functions in traumatically brain-injured adults. Brain Injury, 9(5), 471477. doi: 10.3109/02699059509008206 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, L. (2009). Clinical pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummings, L. (2021). Pragmatic impairment. In J. S. Damico, N. Müller, & M. J. Ball (Eds.) Handbook of language and speech disorders (pp. 192208). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119606987.ch10 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahlberg, C. A., Cusick, C. P., Hawley, L. A., Newman, J. K., Morey, C. E., Harrison-Felix, C. L., & Whiteneck, G. G. (2007). Treatment efficacy of social communication skills training after traumatic brain injury: A randomized treatment and deferred treatment-controlled trial. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 88(12), 15611573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, J. M. (2010). Relation of executive functioning to pragmatic outcome following severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(2), 365382. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0205) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, J. M., Bracy, C. A., & Snow, P. C. (2007). Exploring the factor structure of the La Trobe Communication Questionnaire: Insights into the nature of communication deficits following traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology, 21(12), 11811194. doi: 10.1080/02687030600980950 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egorova, N., Shtyrov, Y., & Pulvermuller, F. (2013). Early and parallel processing of pragmatic and semantic information in speech acts: Neurophysiological evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 86. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00086 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, C., & Peach, R. (2009). Sentence planning following traumatic brain injury. NeuroRehabilitation, 24(3), 255266. doi: 10.3233/NRE-2009-0476 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellmo, W. J. (1995). MCLA: Measure of cognitive-linguistic abilities. Vero Beach, FL: The Speech Bin, Inc.Google Scholar
Galski, T., Tompkins, C., & Johnston, M. V. (1998). Competence in discourse as a measure of social integration and quality of life in persons with traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 12(9), 769782. doi: 10.1080/026990598122160 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, G., Kong, A., & Lau, K. (2016). Production of main concepts by Mandarin-speakers with traumatic brain injury in China: A pilot study. Frontiers in Psychology. 10.3389/conf.fpsyg.2016.68.00005 Google Scholar
González, M. (2005). Pragmatic markers and discourse coherence relations in English and Catalan oral narrative. Discourse Studies, 7(1), 5386. doi: 10.1177/1461445605048767 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. Speech Acts, 3, 4158. doi: 10.1163/9789004368811_003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagan, C. (1984). Language disorders in head trauma. In Holland, A. (Ed.), Language disorders in adults. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Helm-Estabrooks, N. (2001). Cognitive linguistic quick test. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar
Hilger, A., Ramsberger, G., Gilley, P., Menn, L., & Kong, A. P. H. (2014). Analysing speech problems in a longitudinal case study of logopenic variant PPA. Aphasiology, 28(7), 840861. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2014.895974 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G., Bickerton, W.-L., Samson, D., & Riddoch, J. (2012). Birmingham cognitive screen: Examiner’s booklet B. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Joanette, Y., Ska, B., Cote, H., Ferre, P., LaPointe, L., Coppens, P., & Small, S. L. (2015). Montreal Protocol for the Evaluation of Communication (MEC) manual. Sydney, Australia: ASSBI Resources.Google Scholar
Kertesz, A. (1982). Western Aphasia Battery. New York: Grune and Stratton.Google Scholar
Kong, A. P. H. (2006). A Cantonese Linguistic Communication Measure for evaluating aphasic narrative production (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Kong, A. P. H. (2009). The use of main concept analysis to measure discourse production in Cantonese-speaking persons with aphasia: A preliminary report. Journal of Communication Disorders, 42(6), 442464. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kong, A. P. H., Chan, J., Lau, J. K. L., Bickerton, W. L., Weekes, B., & Humphreys, G. (2018). Developing a Cantonese version of Birmingham Cognitive Screen for stroke survivors in Hong Kong. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 39(3), 387401. doi: 10.1177/1525740117720382 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kong, A. P. H., Lau, D. K. Y., & Chan, K. T.-Y. (2019). Measuring tangentiality of discourse output among Chinese-speaking individuals with acquired neurogenic disorders: A pilot study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. doi: 10.3389/conf.fnhum.2019.01.00011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kong, A. P. H., Lau, D. K. Y., & Cheng, C. Y. Y. (2020). Analysing coherence of oral discourse among Cantonese speakers in Mainland China with traumatic brain injury and cerebrovascular accident. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22(1), 3747. doi: 10.1080/17549507.2019.1581256 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kong, A. P. H., & Law, S. P. (2004). A Cantonese linguistic communication measure for evaluating aphasic narrative production: Normative and preliminary aphasic data. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 2(2), 124146. doi: 10.1080/14769670310001659708 Google Scholar
Kong, A. P. H., & Ng, C. Y.-T. (2022). Psycholinguistic considerations for adapting the Cantonese version of Comprehensive Aphasia Test (Cant-CAT): A feasibility study. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 7(4), 12111222. doi: 10.1044/2022_PERSP-22-00044 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kong, A. P. H., & Wong, C. W. Y. (2018). An integrative analysis of spontaneous storytelling discourse in aphasia: Relationship with listeners’ rating and prediction of severity and fluency status of aphasia. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 27(4), 14911505. doi: 10.1044/2018_AJSLP-18-0015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kong, A.P.H. & Law, S.P. (2019). Cantonese AphasiaBank: An annotated database of spoken discourse and co-verbal gestures by healthy and language-impaired native Cantonese speakers. Behavior Research Methods, 51(3), 11311144. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1043-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, C. C. T., Law, S. P., & Kong, A. P. H. (2017). A quantitative study of right dislocation in Cantonese spoken discourse. Language and Speech, 60(4), 633642. doi: 10.1177/0023830916688028,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lau, D. K.-Y., Kong, A. P. H., & Chan, M. S.-W. (2022). Sentence types and complexity of spontaneous discourse productions by Cantonese-speakers with traumatic brain injury – A preliminary report. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 36(4-5), 381397. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2021.1984582 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Law, S. P., Kong, A. P. H., Lai, L. W. S., & Lai, C. (2015). Effects of context and word class on lexical retrieval in Chinese speakers with anomic aphasia. Aphasiology, 29(1), 81100. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2014.951598,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liles, B. Z., Coelho, C. A., Duffy, R. J., & Zalagens, M. R. (1989). Effects of elicitation procedures on the narratives of normal and closed head-injured adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54(3), 356366. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5403.356 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahar, C., & Fraser, K. (2011). Strategies to facilitate successful community reintegration following Acquired Brain Injury (ABI). International Journal of Disability Management, 6(1), 6878. doi: 10.1375/jdmr.6.1.68 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marini, A., Galetto, V., Zampieri, E., Vorano, L., Zettin, M., & Carlomagno, S. (2011). Narrative language in traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 49(10), 29042910. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, S., & Yip, V. (2013). Cantonese: A comprehensive grammar. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, S., & Van Sommers, P. (1993). Pragmatic language skills alter closed head injury: Ability to negotiate requests. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10(4), 297315. doi: 10.1080/02643299308253466 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mok, K., Kong, A., & Lau, K. (2016). Cohesion in oral discourse of Mandarin-speaking adults with traumatic brain injury: Report of pilot data on story telling. Frontiers in Psychology. doi: 10.3389/conf.fpsyg.2016.68.00047 Google Scholar
Pan, X., Chen, H., Bickerton, W. L., Lau, J. K. L., Kong, A. P. H., Rotshtein, P., …Humphreys, G. W. (2015). Preliminary findings on the reliability and validity of the Cantonese Birmingham Cognitive Screen in patients with acute ischemic stroke. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 2015(11), 23772390. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S85698,Google Scholar
Peking University Health Science Center (PUHSC) (1988). Mandarin aphasia battery. Unpublished work.Google Scholar
Penn, C. (1999). Pragmatic assessment and therapy for persons with brain damage: What have clinicians gleaned in two decades? Brain and Language, 68(3), 535552. doi: 10.1006/brln.1999.2127 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potechin, G. C., Nicholas, L. E., & Brookshire, R. H. (1987). Effects of picture stimuli on discourse production by aphasic patients. Clinical Aphasiology, 17, 216220. http://aphasiology.pitt.edu/id/eprint/937 Google Scholar
Prutting, C. A., & Kirchner, D. M. (1987). A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 52(2), 105119. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5202.105 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowley, D. A., Rogish, M., Alexander, T., & Riggs, K. J. (2017). Cognitive correlates of pragmatic language comprehension in adult traumatic brain injury: A systematic review and meta-analyses. Brain Injury, 31(12), 15641574. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2017.1341645 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarno, M. T. (1980). The nature of verbal impairment after closed head injury. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 168(11), 685692. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198011000-00008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarno, M. T. (1984). Verbal impairment after closed head injury. Report of a replication study. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 172(8), 475479. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198408000-00006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, C. M., & Nippold, M. A. (1988). Spoken and written syntax. In Nippold, M. A. (Ed.), Latter language development: Ages nine through nineteen (pp. 2759). Boston, MA: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Snow, P. C., Douglas, J., & Ponsfordoe, J. (1997). Procedural discourse following traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology, 11(10), 947968. doi: 10.1080/02687039708249421,CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, P. C., Douglas, J. M., & Ponsfordoe, J. L. (1999). Narrative discourse following severe traumatic brain injury: A longitudinal follow-up. Aphasiology, 13(7), 529551. doi: 10.1080/026870399401993 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steel, J., & Togher, L. (2019). Social communication assessment after traumatic brain injury: A narrative review of innovations in pragmatic and discourse assessment methods. Brain Injury, 33(1), 4861. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2018.1531304 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swinburn, K., Porter, G., & Howard, D. (2004). Comprehensive aphasia test. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Van Leer, E., & Turkstra, L. (1999). The effect of elicitation task on discourse coherence and cohesion in adolescents with brain injury. Journal of Communication Disorders, 32(5), 327349. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9924(99)00008-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Worrall, L., Simmons-Mackie, N., Wallace, S., Rose, T., Brady, M., Kong, A. P. H., …Hallowell, B. (2016). Let’s call it “aphasia”: Rationales for eliminating the term “dysphasia”. International Journal of Stroke, 11(8), 848851. doi: 10.1177/1747493016654487 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yiu, E. M. (1992). Linguistic assessment of Chinese-speaking aphasics: Development of a Cantonese aphasia battery. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 7(4), 379424. doi: 10.1016/0911-6044(92)90025-R CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Background Information on Participants in the Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) and Non-brain Injured (NBI) Groups

Figure 1

Table 2. Indices of Measuring Pragmatics

Figure 2

Table 3. Statements and Corresponding Categories for Naïve Listeners’ Perceptual Rating of Audio Files

Figure 3

Table 4. Pragmatic Performance in the TBI and NBI Groups

Figure 4

Table 5. Correlation (Pearson’s r) Among the Objective Measures Under the Four Maxims

Figure 5

Table 6. Means of the Results of Naïve Listeners’ Rating

Figure 6

Table 7. Correlations (Pearson’s r) Between the Language-Cognitive Deficits of TBI Individuals and Their Pragmatic Performance by Maxims

Figure 7

Figure 1. Quadratic models analyze among the indices of Maxims of Quantity.

Supplementary material: File

Kong et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S2

Download Kong et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15.3 KB