Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T17:48:16.420Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Restricting night-time eating reduces daily energy intake in healthy young men: a short-term cross-over study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 May 2013

James D. LeCheminant*
Affiliation:
Department of Exercise Sciences, Brigham Young University, 269 SFH, Provo, UT84602, USA
Ed Christenson
Affiliation:
Department of Exercise Sciences, Brigham Young University, 116 RB, Provo, UT84602, USA
Bruce W. Bailey
Affiliation:
Department of Exercise Sciences, Brigham Young University, 267 SFH, Provo, UT84602, USA
Larry A. Tucker
Affiliation:
Department of Exercise Sciences, Brigham Young University, 237 SFH, Provo, UT84602, USA
*
*Corresponding author: J. D. LeCheminant, fax +1 801 422 0555, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Few experimental data are available to support the notion that reducing night-time eating changes total daily energy intake (EI) or body weight in healthy adults. The present study primarily examined the short-term effect of night eating restriction (NER) on daily EI in healthy young men. It secondarily examined body weight and moods associated with NER. Using a cross-over design, twenty-nine men (20·9 (sd 2·5) years; 24·4 (sd 2·5) kg/m2) initiated a 2-week NER intervention (elimination of EI from 19.00 to 06.00 hours) and a 2-week control condition, counterbalanced and separated by a 1-week washout period. EI and macronutrient intake were assessed using computerised, multiple-pass 24 h food recalls, body weight via a digital scale and mood using the Profile of Mood States survey. Of the twenty-nine participants, twenty-seven (93 %) completed all aspects of the study. During the NER condition, the participants consumed less total energy per d than during the control condition (10 125 v. 11 146 kJ/d; F= 6·41; P= 0·018). During the NER condition, no energy was reported consumed between 19.00 and 06.00 hours; however, during the control condition, the energy intake of participants was 2920 (sd 1347) kJ/d between 19.00 and 06.00 hours. There was a significant difference in weight change between the NER ( − 0·4 (sd 1·1) kg) and control (+0·6 (sd 0·9) kg) conditions (F= 22·68; P< 0·001). Differences in total mood score or mood subscales between the NER and control conditions were not apparent (P>0·05). These findings provide support for NER decreasing short-term EI in healthy young men.

Type
Full Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2013 

Unhealthy dietary patterns have been linked to gradual weight gain in adults over time(Reference Mozaffarian, Hao and Rimm1). However, young adults appear to be at a particular risk for poor dietary habits and significant weight gain(Reference Gropper, Simmons and Connell2, Reference Racette, Deusinger and Strube3) and, thus, warrant particular attention. To prevent weight gain, some researchers have focused on sustainable lifestyle modifications including a small-changes approach that emphasises increased physical activity and reduced energy intake(Reference Rodearmel, Wyatt and Stroebele4, Reference Hill5). In addition, modest improvements in environmental conditions that promote excessive eating(Reference Wansink6) may attenuate the gradual weight gain experienced by many adults across their lifespan(Reference Hill5).

It has been suggested that a greater volume of food is consumed as the day progresses(Reference de Castro7). In addition, the evening hours typically result in increased television viewing, which has been associated with increased snacking and food consumption(Reference Thomson, Spence and Raine8). Thus, the evening hours probably represent a high-risk environmental condition for overeating(Reference Nelson, Kocos and Lytle9) in many adults.

Interestingly, anecdotal weight management recommendations often include advice to limit food consumption at night. This is logical; however, there are surprisingly few experimental data to support the notion that a reduction in or elimination of eating after a certain time of night results in a lower daily energy intake or effective weight management in healthy adults without the night eating syndrome(Reference Stunkard, Grace and Wolff10, Reference Colles, Dixon and O'Brien11) or those who do not work the night-time shift(Reference Atkinson, Fullick and Grindey12).

To provide initial experimental data and proof of concept, the present study primarily examined the short-term trends in energy intake and macronutrient intake resulting from the elimination of night-time eating. Young adult men were chosen in the present study as many are likely to engage in unhealthy behaviours(Reference Hager, George and LeCheminant13) and could potentially benefit from the intervention. The present study secondarily examined whether or not there was a trend in body weight change and whether there were differences in mood as a result of this intervention. We hypothesised that night-time eating restriction would be associated with a lower total daily energy intake compared with the control condition.

Methods

Participants

The present study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the university. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. The participants were recruited through fliers, posted advertisements, word of mouth and Facebook. Each participant gave his consent to participate in the study and was given a signed copy of the consent form for future reference. As part of the recruitment process, all interested participants were screened, via telephone, to ensure that they met the study criteria given below.

The participants of the present study were unmarried men, 18–26 years of age, healthy, and non-smokers or drinkers and had their weight stable for the previous month. Participant characteristics are given in Table 1. Each participant was screened for the night eating syndrome, on the basis of the night eating syndrome characterisation proposed by Stunkard et al. (Reference Stunkard, Grace and Wolff10, Reference Stunkard, Berkowitz and Wadden14), and excluded if the participant reported that he (1) had no appetite for breakfast, (2) had evening hyperphagia (greater than half of his daily energy intake after 19.00 hours) or (3) had trouble getting to sleep or staying asleep three or more days per week, all for the previous 3 months. Nevertheless, the participants were not required to be regular breakfast eaters. Additional exclusion criteria were as follows: current dieting or extreme dietary practices; the presence of an eating disorder (anorexia, bulimia or binge-eating); digestive disorders; a metabolic disease that could affect energy balance (e.g. diabetes mellitus, cancer or heart disease). As exercise may influence food-seeking behaviour(Reference Hanlon, Larson and Bailey15), the participants were also excluded for excessive exercise training (vigorous-intensity physical activity >4 d/week and 30 min per session), participation in college athletics or any elite sports, or inability to exercise at a moderate-intensity level.

Table 1 Participant characteristics at baseline (Mean values and standard deviations, n 29)

Using a cross-over design, twenty-nine participants initiated a 2-week night eating restriction (NER) intervention (elimination of energy intake from 19.00 to 06.00 hours) and a 2-week control condition separated by a 1-week washout period. The order was counterbalanced with half of the participants completing the NER intervention first and the other half completing the control condition first. The order of conditions was determined on the basis of the order of recruitment. The time of 19.00 hours was chosen as many individuals may have eaten their evening meal by this time and because the night eating syndrome has previously been described as excessive food consumption after 19.00 hours(Reference Striegel-Moore, Franko and Thompson16). The following outcomes were assessed during each condition: energy intake and macronutrient intake via computerised, multiple-pass 24 h food recalls; pre-breakfast hunger level using visual analogue scales (VAS); body weight; mood profile.

Procedures

After the initial phone screening, interested and qualifying participants presented at the Human Performance Laboratory to reconfirm that they fit within the parameters of the study, to discuss the required study protocol and to undergo body composition testing. In addition, at this time, each participant was trained by a member of the research staff on how to properly complete a computerised, multiple-pass 24 h food recall by completing one for the previous day. Furthermore, to determine the pre-study dietary intake, the participants were instructed to complete two additional 24 h food recalls during the next week (one weekday and one weekend day), determined at random, and were then scheduled for their next visit a week later. Each participant was asked to go about his usual schedule and normal lifestyle patterns during this ‘lead-in’ week.

On the last day of the lead-in week, each participant presented again to the Human Performance Laboratory where he received instruction about the expectations and protocol of the first condition (NER or control) and reviewed again as to how to properly complete the computerised, multiple-pass 24 h dietary recalls. The participants were also weighed, completed a mood survey and were scheduled for their next visit a week later.

At the end of the first and second weeks of each condition, the participants presented again to the Human Performance Laboratory to be weighed and turn in their pre-breakfast hunger VAS (see below). Additionally, after the second week of each condition, a post-condition mood survey was completed. The protocol for each condition was identical except that the participants were instructed according to the current condition.

Night eating restriction condition

For the NER condition, the participants were instructed to avoid consumption of all food or beverages that included any energy (kJ) from 19.00 to 06.00 hours for two consecutive weeks. The consumption of water after 19.00 hours was considered acceptable and preferable. There were no additional instructions or recommendations on the amount or type of food consumed during the daytime hours of 06.00 to 19.00 hours. There were also no recommendations or restrictions on their general lifestyle patterns. To increase compliance, each participant was called 3 d/week to report on whether or not he had adhered to the NER protocol and to encourage further compliance.

Control condition

During the control condition (2 weeks), the participants were encouraged to follow their normal pre-study daily lifestyle and eating patterns. There were no other instructions or recommendations provided for this period.

Washout period

During the washout period (1 week) between conditions, the participants were asked to resume their normal lifestyle patterns. The half of the participants who completed the NER condition first were encouraged to resume their pre-study dietary routine. No other instructions or recommendations were provided for this period.

Measurements

Energy intake and macronutrient intake

Energy intake and macronutrient intake were assessed with a goal of three separate days (two weekdays and one weekend day) during the lead-in week, each week of the NER condition and each week of the control condition using the Automated Self-Administered 24-Hour system used by the National Cancer Institute. The Automated Self-Administered 24-Hour is a multiple-pass 24 h recall that determines the amount, type and time of food and beverage consumption. A multiple-pass 24 h recall is often preferred as a valid and reliable measure of energy intake and macronutrient intake(Reference Johnson17). Its most significant advantages are: (1) low participant burden compared with other measures, such as weighed food records, and (2) relatively non-reactive compared with other dietary assessment methods(Reference Johnson17). To further reduce the likelihood of reactivity, dietary assessment days were determined randomly and the participants were not informed of the days on which they would complete a 24 h recall until the evening hours. Subsequently, each participant was asked to complete the multiple-pass recall as soon as possible the following day, preferably early in the morning.

To ensure that the energy and macronutrient data were reliable and represented the normal diet of the participants during the 2-week control condition, a 1-week lead-in period was completed before the intervention, as noted previously. There was no statistical difference in energy intake (F= 0·32; P= 0·578), carbohydrate intake (F= 0·10; P= 0·757), fat intake (F= 1·30; P= 0·265) or protein intake (F= 0·56; P= 0·462) between the lead-in period and the 2-week control condition.

Body composition measures

Body weight and composition were assessed during the lead-in week for descriptive purposes. In addition, body weight was assessed at the beginning and end of each condition. Body weight was obtained using a digital scale (Tanita) accurate to ± 0·01 kg with the participants being barefoot and wearing a standardised bathing suit. Height was measured using a stadiometer (Seca), and BMI was calculated as kg/m2. Body composition was assessed using dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry, which is considered a valid and reliable measure of body composition(Reference Bailey, Tucker and Peterson18, Reference Maddalozzo, Cardinal and Snow19).

Pre-breakfast hunger level

Pre-breakfast hunger level was assessed each day during the second week of each condition. The participants rated their hunger level before eating breakfast using a VAS. A VAS is often used to rate subjective feelings of hunger or appetite(Reference Kral, Roe and Rolls20, Reference Lemmens, Martens and Kester21). The VAS included the question ‘How hungry do you feel right now?’ above a line with the words ‘Not at All’ on the left side of the scale and ‘Extremely’ on the right side of the scale. The participants were asked to place a vertical slash along the continuum that best described their hunger level at that moment.

Mood

Mood was assessed using the Profile of Mood States (POMS) scale at the beginning and end of each condition. The POMS survey includes sixty-five questions and has been validated previously(Reference Nyenhuis, Yamamoto and Luchetta22). In addition to the total mood score, the POMS scale includes six subscales: tension; depression; anger; vigour; fatigue; confusion. The POMS scale ranges from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely) to address feelings such as how friendly, tense, angry, confused, shaky, peeved, energetic, relaxed, uneasy, exhausted, cheerful, etc. a person feels at that moment(Reference Nyenhuis, Yamamoto and Luchetta22).

Statistical analyses

The statistical software package PC-SAS (version 9.3; SAS Institute, Inc.) was employed for all statistical analyses. The level of significance was set at 0·05 for all statistical tests. Descriptive statistics (mean, standard deviation, etc.) are reported for all dependent measures. The mixed-model procedure in SAS was used to detect differences between conditions for all variables of interest (energy intake and macronutrient intake, body weight change, VAS and POMS scale). To account for individual variability, participant was included in the model as a random term. In addition, baseline (lead-in period) energy or macronutrient data were used as a control variable and the order of conditions was used as a control variable. To determine daily energy intake and macronutrient intake, data from the 24 h recalls across each 2-week intervention were averaged by person and are reported in Table 2 (macronutrients as a percentage of energy intake). Night eating was determined as energy intake between 19.00 and 06.00 hours. As the POMS scale was completed by each participant pre and post for each condition, the results section presents the condition × time interaction for total mood score and for each individual mood subscale.

Table 2 Energy intake and macronutrient intake as a percentage of energy by condition* (Mean values and standard deviations)

* Macronutrient percentages are estimates and were determined by multiplying the number of grams of the macronutrient by 16·736 kJ per g of carbohydrate, 37·656 kJ per g of fat, and 16·736 kJ per g of protein.

Lead-in dietary intake represents dietary intake during the week before the first experimental condition.

Difference = night eating restriction − control condition.

§ F and P represent the difference between the night eating restriction and control conditions with statistical control of the lead-in value for each macronutrient and the order of completion of the condition.

There was no statistical difference between the lead-in dietary intake and the control condition (P>0·05).

Results

At baseline, the participants tended to be young (20·9 (sd 2·5) years), of normal weight (24·4 (sd 2·5) kg/m2) and with a body fat percentage of 21·3 (sd 7·1) %. Of the twenty-nine participants who initiated the study, twenty-seven (93 %) completed all aspects of the study. The two non-completers cited withdrawal for personal reasons and inability to comply with the protocol due to travel. Furthermore, the participants self-reported following the NER protocol on 93·6 (sd 5·6) % (approximately thirteen out of fourteen) of the prescribed days.

During the NER condition, the participants consumed less daily average energy (10 125 v. 11 146 kJ/d) than during the control condition (F= 6·41; P= 0·018; Table 2) and significantly less dietary fat (95 v. 110 g/d) than during the control condition (F= 7·53; P= 0·011). Carbohydrate and protein intakes (g/d) were also reduced during the NER condition but did not reach statistical significance, though carbohydrate intake (g/d) trended towards significance (F= 3·87; P= 0·060). When carbohydrate, fat and protein intakes were expressed as a percentage of energy intake, there were no statistical differences (P>0·05; Table 2). During the control condition, the energy intake of participants was 2920 (sd 1347) kJ/d between 19.00 and 06.00 hours, while they did not report consuming any energy between 19.00 and 06.00 hours during the NER condition.

The average weight change during the NER condition was − 0·4 (sd 1·1) kg and during the control condition was +0·6 (sd 0·9) kg (F= 22·68; P< 0·001). The total BMI change during the NER condition was − 0·1 (sd 0·3) kg/m2 and during the control condition was +0·2 (sd 0·3) kg/m2 (F= 22·73; P< 0·001).

The VAS revealed that the participants experienced greater hunger upon waking during the NER condition than during the control condition. On a scale of 0–10 (10 being most hungry), the participants reported an average pre-breakfast hunger value of 5·7 (sd 1·5) during the NER condition and that of 4·8 (sd 1·1) during the control condition (F= 7·82; P= 0·001).

Change in mood and its six subscales are given in Table 3. There was no pre/post-change for total mood score or its subscales within the NER and control conditions (P>0·05). A condition × time interaction for total mood disturbance was not observed (F= 0·47; P= 0·494). Likewise, there was no condition × time interaction for the tension, depression, anger, vigour or confusion subscale (P>0·05). The fatigue subscale trended towards more fatigue during the NER condition and less during the control condition but did not reach statistical significance (F= 3·61; P= 0·06).

Table 3 Mood scores by condition (Mean values and standard deviations)

* No within-group difference was observed for the night eating restriction or control condition.

F and P values represent a condition × time interaction.

Discussion

The present study provides initial evidence that restricting night-time eating may promote a short-term reduction in total energy intake in a young male population. Our original hypothesis was thus correct. Several findings from the present study are given brief attention below.

First, daily energy intake was approximately 1000 kJ lower during the NER condition. This level of energy reduction is modest but similar to other ‘small-changes’ approaches(Reference Hill5, Reference Stroebele, de Castro and Stuht23) that may be effective for body weight maintenance. We observed that a potential advantage of the NER intervention to reduce energy intake is its simplicity. There were no gadgets or record keeping and the intervention was simple to understand and implement. Second, the participants reported modestly higher levels of pre-breakfast hunger during the NER condition than during the control condition. While there was no difference in average energy intake during morning time (breakfast) between conditions (data not shown), it is unclear how the increase in pre-breakfast hunger would influence subsequent hunger and energy intake if an NER intervention was followed long term. Third, during the NER condition, the participants lost a small amount of body weight, and during the control condition, they gained a small amount of body weight. In conjunction with the average decrease in energy intake during the NER condition, these data suggest that the decreased energy intake during the NER intervention partially explained the slight weight loss. However, we note that the gain in weight during the control condition is surprising given that there was no difference in energy intake between the lead-in and control conditions. Therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that there was under-reporting of energy intake or that some other study factor influenced energy intake, particularly during the control condition.

The major strength of the present study is that it experimentally examined the influence of NER on energy intake and body weight. Using a cross-sectional design or performing a secondary analysis, others have suggested that night-time/late-night eating is positively associated (r 0·14) with total daily energy intake in young/middle-aged adults(Reference de Castro7), influences diet and body weight in college-aged students(Reference Nelson, Kocos and Lytle9) and is associated with BMI and obesity(Reference Lissner, Lindroos and Sjostrom24, Reference Baron, Reid and Kern25). On the other hand, Sullivan et al. (Reference Sullivan, Daniels and Koegler26) reported that in rhesus monkeys, energy consumption at night did not correlate well with body weight gain. In a study related to the present investigation, Waller et al. (Reference Waller, Vander Wal and Klurfeld27) studied the effects of a post-dinner snack intervention in overweight or obese ‘night snackers’ over 4 weeks. They showed that their intervention resulted in less energy intake of approximately 1561 kJ/d than in the control group(Reference Waller, Vander Wal and Klurfeld27). The implication of their findings was that minimising night-time snacking may result in lower overall energy intake in that sample. However, it is difficult to compare the Waller et al. (Reference Waller, Vander Wal and Klurfeld27) results with those of the present study, as the present study did not exclusively use overweight or obese subjects and did not provide snacks. Nevertheless, the present study appears to support other related studies of the potential effect of NER on energy intake and weight management outcomes.

Notable caution should be exercised when interpreting these data. First, the NER and control conditions each lasted for a period of only 2 weeks. The present study provides no information regarding the long-term effectiveness, practicality or compensatory effects that may occur over a longer period of time. Second, though being a student was not a requirement for participation, all participants in the final sample were university students. This prevents the broad generalisation of the results of the study. In addition, the participants in the present study were excluded for alcohol use. Other studies have shown that in young adults classified as ‘current drinkers’, increasing alcohol consumption (drinks/d) may be associated with higher energy intake(Reference Breslow, Guenther and Juan28). Furthermore, in drinkers, it has been shown that alcohol consumption tends to increase during the evening hours(Reference de Castro7). Therefore, the present study does not generalise to a drinking population. Additional studies of the effect of NER in broader populations, including obese adults, children or families, may be particularly beneficial.

In conclusion, compared with the control condition, 2 weeks of NER resulted in a significant difference in energy intake and body weight in healthy young men. Therefore, these data provide initial proof of concept for NER. If NER proves sustainable in the long term, it may be a promising tool to prevent weight gain in this population sample. However, examination of the long-term effectiveness of NER and application to additional sample populations are needed.

Acknowledgements

There is no external funding source to report. The authors' contributions are as follows: J. D. L. conceived the study design with significant input from E. C., B. W. B. and L. A. T; E. C. L. coordinated the data recruitment and collection; L. A. T. provided additional statistical input; J. D. L. was the primary author of the manuscript with input and critical review from E. C., B. W. B. and L. A. T. There are no conflicts of interest to report.

References

1Mozaffarian, D, Hao, T, Rimm, EB, et al. (2011) Changes in diet and lifestyle and long-term weight gain in women and men. N Engl J Med 364, 23922404.Google Scholar
2Gropper, SS, Simmons, KP, Connell, LJ, et al. (2012) Changes in body weight, composition, and shape: a 4-year study of college students. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 37, 11181123.Google Scholar
3Racette, SB, Deusinger, SS, Strube, MJ, et al. (2008) Changes in weight and health behaviors from freshman through senior year of college. J Nutr Educ Behav 40, 3942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4Rodearmel, SJ, Wyatt, HR, Stroebele, N, et al. (2007) Small changes in dietary sugar and physical activity as an approach to preventing excessive weight gain: The America on the Move Family Study. Pediatrics 120, e869e879.Google Scholar
5Hill, JO (2009) Can a small-changes approach help address the obesity epidemic? A report of the Joint Task Force of the American Society for Nutrition, Institute of Food Technologists, and International Food Information Council. Am J Clin Nutr 89, 477484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6Wansink, B (2004) Environmental factors that increase the food intake and consumption volume of unknowing consumers. Annu Rev Nutr 24, 455479.Google Scholar
7de Castro, JM (2004) The time of day of food intake influences overall intake in humans. J Nutr 134, 104111.Google Scholar
8Thomson, M, Spence, JC, Raine, K, et al. (2008) The association of television viewing with snacking behavior and body weight of young adults. Am J Health Promot 22, 329335.Google Scholar
9Nelson, MC, Kocos, R, Lytle, LA, et al. (2009) Understanding the perceived determinants of weight-related behaviors in late adolescence: a qualitative analysis among college youth. J Nutr Educ Behav 41, 287292.Google Scholar
10Stunkard, AJ, Grace, WJ & Wolff, HG (1955) The night-eating syndrome; a pattern of food intake among certain obese patients. Am J Med 19, 7886.Google Scholar
11Colles, SL, Dixon, JB & O'Brien, PE (2007) Night eating syndrome and nocturnal snacking: association with obesity, binge eating and psychological distress. Int J Obes 31, 17221730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12Atkinson, G, Fullick, S, Grindey, C, et al. (2008) Exercise, energy balance and the shift worker. Sports Med 38, 671685.Google Scholar
13Hager, R, George, JD, LeCheminant, JD, et al. (2012) Evaluation of a university general education health and wellness course delivered by lecture or online. Am J Health Promot 26, 263269.Google Scholar
14Stunkard, A, Berkowitz, R, Wadden, T, et al. (1996) Binge eating disorder and the night-eating syndrome. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 20, 16.Google Scholar
15Hanlon, B, Larson, MJ, Bailey, BW, et al. (2012) Neural response to pictures of food after exercise in normal-weight and obese women. Med Sci Sports Exerc 44, 18641870.Google Scholar
16Striegel-Moore, RH, Franko, DL, Thompson, D, et al. (2006) Night eating: prevalence and demographic correlates. Obesity 14, 139147.Google Scholar
17Johnson, RK (2002) Dietary intake – how do we measure what people are really eating? Obes Res 10, 63S68S.Google Scholar
18Bailey, BW, Tucker, LA, Peterson, TR, et al. (2001) Test–retest reliability of body fat percentage results using dual energy X-ray absorptiometry and the Bod Pod. Med Sci Sports Exerc 33, 174S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19Maddalozzo, GF, Cardinal, BJ & Snow, CA (2002) Concurrent validity of the BOD POD and dual energy X-ray absorptiometry techniques for assessing body composition in young women. J Am Diet Assoc 102, 16771679.Google Scholar
20Kral, TV, Roe, LS & Rolls, BJ (2004) Combined effects of energy density and portion size on energy intake in women. Am J Clin Nutr 79, 962968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Lemmens, SG, Martens, EA, Kester, AD, et al. (2011) Changes in gut hormone and glucose concentrations in relation to hunger and fullness. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 717725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22Nyenhuis, DL, Yamamoto, C, Luchetta, T, et al. (1999) Adult and geriatric normative data and validation of the profile of mood states. J Clin Psychol 55, 7986.Google Scholar
23Stroebele, N, de Castro, JM, Stuht, J, et al. (2009) A small-changes approach reduces energy intake in free-living humans. J Am Coll Nutr 28, 6368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24Lissner, L, Lindroos, AK & Sjostrom, L (1998) Swedish obese subjects (SOS): an obesity intervention study with a nutritional perspective. Eur J Clin Nutr 52, 316322.Google Scholar
25Baron, KG, Reid, KJ, Kern, AS, et al. (2011) Role of sleep timing in caloric intake and BMI. Obesity 19, 13741381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26Sullivan, EL, Daniels, AJ, Koegler, FH, et al. (2005) Evidence in female rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) that nighttime caloric intake is not associated with weight gain. Obes Res 13, 20722080.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
27Waller, SM, Vander Wal, JS, Klurfeld, DM, et al. (2004) Evening ready-to-eat cereal consumption contributes to weight management. J Am Coll Nutr 23, 316321.Google Scholar
28Breslow, RA, Guenther, PM, Juan, W, et al. (2010) Alcoholic beverage consumption, nutrient intakes, and diet quality in the US adult population, 1999–2006. J Am Diet Assoc 110, 551562.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Participant characteristics at baseline (Mean values and standard deviations, n 29)

Figure 1

Table 2 Energy intake and macronutrient intake as a percentage of energy by condition* (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 2

Table 3 Mood scores by condition (Mean values and standard deviations)