Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T08:19:35.941Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Time-varying living arrangements and suicide death in the general population sample: 14-year causal survival analysis via pooled logistic regression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 May 2024

Z. Narita*
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Medicine, National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Tokyo, Japan
T. Shinozaki
Affiliation:
Department of Information and Computer Technology, Faculty of Engineering, Tokyo University of Science, Katsushika-ku, Tokyo, Japan
A. Goto
Affiliation:
Department of Health Data Science, Graduate School of Data Science, Yokohama City University, Yokohama, Kanagawa, Japan
H. Hori
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Medicine, National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Tokyo, Japan
Y. Kim
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Medicine, National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Tokyo, Japan
H. C. Wilcox
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
M. Inoue
Affiliation:
Division of Prevention, National Cancer Center Institute for Cancer Control, Chuo-ku, Tokyo, Japan
S. Tsugane
Affiliation:
Division of Cohort Research, National Cancer Center Institute for Cancer Control, Chuo-ku, Tokyo, Japan International University of Health and Welfare Graduate School of Public Health, Minato City, Tokyo, Japan
N. Sawada
Affiliation:
Division of Cohort Research, National Cancer Center Institute for Cancer Control, Chuo-ku, Tokyo, Japan
*
Corresponding author: Zui Narita; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims

While past research suggested that living arrangements are associated with suicide death, no study has examined the impact of sustained living arrangements and the change in living arrangements. Also, previous survival analysis studies only reported a single hazard ratio (HR), whereas the actual HR may change over time. We aimed to address these limitations using causal inference approaches.

Methods

Multi-point data from a general Japanese population sample were used. Participants reported their living arrangements twice within a 5-year time interval. After that, suicide death, non-suicide death and all-cause mortality were evaluated over 14 years. We used inverse probability weighted pooled logistic regression and cumulative incidence curve, evaluating the association of time-varying living arrangements with suicide death. We also studied non-suicide death and all-cause mortality to contextualize the association. Missing data for covariates were handled using random forest imputation.

Results

A total of 86,749 participants were analysed, with a mean age (standard deviation) of 51.7 (7.90) at baseline. Of these, 306 died by suicide during the 14-year follow-up. Persistently living alone was associated with an increased risk of suicide death (risk difference [RD]: 1.1%, 95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.3–2.5%; risk ratio [RR]: 4.00, 95% CI: 1.83–7.41), non-suicide death (RD: 7.8%, 95% CI: 5.2–10.5%; RR: 1.56, 95% CI: 1.38–1.74) and all-cause mortality (RD: 8.7%, 95% CI: 6.2–11.3%; RR: 1.60, 95% CI: 1.42–1.79) at the end of the follow-up. The cumulative incidence curve showed that these associations were consistent throughout the follow-up. Across all types of mortality, the increased risk was smaller for those who started to live with someone and those who transitioned to living alone. The results remained robust in sensitivity analyses.

Conclusions

Individuals who persistently live alone have an increased risk of suicide death as well as non-suicide death and all-cause mortality, whereas this impact is weaker for those who change their living arrangements.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press.

Introduction

Suicide represents a pressing public health concern globally, with approximately 700,000 deaths annually (World Health Organization, Reference World Health Organization2021a). It is vital to identify factors leading to suicidal behaviours to facilitate effective prevention efforts. Among various social factors, living alone has been suggested as important in the development of suicide (Nestadt, Reference Nestadt2022). While the mechanism for this development remains unclear, living alone may increase a sense of thwarted belongingness, a crucial factor in the interpersonal theory of suicide (Van Orden et al., Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner2010). Also, living alone may reduce the opportunities for intervention by others, contributing to the volitional phase in the integrated motivational–volitional model of suicide (O’Connor and Kirtley, Reference O’Connor and Kirtley2018).

Previous studies have reinforced the evidence of an association between living alone and suicide death (Olfson et al., Reference Olfson, Cosgrove, Altekruse, Wall and Blanco2022; Poudel-Tandukar et al., Reference Poudel-Tandukar, Nanri, Mizoue, Matsushita, Takahashi, Noda, Inoue and Tsugane2011; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Cullen, Graham, Lyall, Mackay, Okolie, Pearsall, Ward, John and Smith2021). A cohort study for the Japanese population suggested that men living without a spouse faced an elevated risk of suicide death (Poudel-Tandukar et al., Reference Poudel-Tandukar, Nanri, Mizoue, Matsushita, Takahashi, Noda, Inoue and Tsugane2011). In the analysis of middle-aged individuals from the UK Biobank, even after adjusting for loneliness, the increase in suicide rates due to living alone was approximately twice as high for men (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Cullen, Graham, Lyall, Mackay, Okolie, Pearsall, Ward, John and Smith2021). Furthermore, analysis of American Community Survey data showed an increased hazard of suicide death among individuals living alone (Olfson et al., Reference Olfson, Cosgrove, Altekruse, Wall and Blanco2022). These findings underscore the importance of targeted interventions and support for individuals living alone.

Despite the aforementioned findings, two limitations merit attention in this area of research. First, past research only assessed living alone at a given point in time as a time-fixed exposure, controlling for time-fixed confounders. While living arrangements may change over time and be considered as a time-varying variable (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Liang, Krause, Akiyama, Sugisawa and Fukaya2002; U.S. Census Bureau, Reference U.S. Census Bureau2020), no study has examined how sustained living arrangements and the change in living arrangements impact suicide death. Traditional regression analyses, such as the Cox model, are inadequate for handling time-varying variables, which may introduce bias due to exposure-confounder feedback (Hernán and Robins, Reference Hernán, Robins and Boca2020). To overcome this limitation, the application of g-methods, such as inverse probability weighting, is necessary (Hernán and Robins, Reference Hernán, Robins and Boca2020). Second, previous survival analysis studies examining suicide death have only reported a single hazard ratio (HR) averaged over time based on the proportional hazard assumption. Nonetheless, the actual HR may change during the follow-up period, and relying solely on a single HR may potentially lead to misleading interpretations (Hernán, Reference Hernán2010; Prentice et al., Reference Prentice, Pettinger and Anderson2005). To avoid such interpretations, it is imperative to evaluate the survival and cumulative incidence throughout follow-up periods (Hernán, Reference Hernán2010; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Caniglia and Petito2021).

Given the multifaceted nature of suicide (Shaw, Reference Shaw2022), identifying associated factors is crucial to developing effective prevention strategies. Understanding the association between living alone and suicide death may help identify individuals at a higher risk and enable targeted prevention efforts (Eaton, Reference Eaton2012). By addressing the limitations of past research, we aimed to evaluate the association of time-varying living arrangements with suicide death and examine how this association changed over time. We used multi-point data from the Japan Public Health Center (JPHC)-based Prospective Study, accounting for time-fixed and time-varying confounders in the analyses. To contextualize the association, we also studied non-suicide death and all-cause mortality. Furthermore, given the heterogeneity in suicide rates (World Health Organization, 2021b), we conducted age-stratified and gender-stratified analyses as an exploratory aim.

Methods

Study population

We analysed data from the JPHC Study in Japan. The study design has been extensively detailed in a prior study (Tsugane and Sawada, Reference Tsugane and Sawada2014). Participants who completed the questionnaire were considered to have given their consent to participate in the study. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the National Cancer Center and the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry. Our general population sample comprised individuals from local municipalities in 11 public health centre areas with 2 separate cohorts. Cohort I consisted of individuals aged 40–59 years, while Cohort II consisted of individuals aged 40–69 years. We defined wave 1 as the period of data collection from 1995 to 1999, with the specific timing dependent on each public health centre. Wave 2 was defined as the period 5 years after wave 1, i.e., from 2000 to 2004.

Cause of death

Our primary outcome was suicide death, while non-suicide death and all-cause mortality were also assessed. All outcomes were evaluated up to 14 years after wave 2. With the permission of the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare, we obtained information on the causes of death via death certificates (Sawada et al., Reference Sawada, Nakaya, Kashima, Yorifuji, Hanibuchi, Charvat, Yamaji, Iwasaki, Inoue, Iso and Tsugane2022). The cause of death was defined according to the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, tenth revision (ICD-10) (World Health Organization, 1990). Suicide death was defined using ICD-10 codes X60–X84. Non-suicide death was defined as any death registered under other ICD-10 codes.

Living arrangements

At waves 1 and 2, we asked participants about their living arrangements by posing the question, ‘Are you currently living with someone (spouse, child(ren), parent(s), others, alone)?’ Here, ‘others’ included both non-family members and additional family members like siblings, grandparents, uncles, aunts, cousins and in-laws.

Covariates

We evaluated covariates at wave 1 that might serve as potential confounders. We selected covariates per the disjunctive cause criterion, controlling for each potential cause of the exposure, the outcome or both while excluding instrumental variables and including covariates that act as proxies for unmeasured variables that are common causes of both the exposure and the outcome (VanderWeele, Reference VanderWeele2019). We included the following variables that would fulfil the disjunctive cause criterion: age (continuous), gender (dichotomous, female or male), body mass index (continuous) (Amiri and Behnezhad, Reference Amiri and Behnezhad2018), smoking status (dichotomous, no or yes) (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Munafò, Davey Smith and Wootton2020), alcohol consumption (dichotomous, <once a week or ≥once a week) (Isaacs et al., Reference Isaacs, Smith, Sherry, Seno, Moore and Stewart2022), physical activity (dichotomous, <once a week or ≥once a week) (Vancampfort et al., Reference Vancampfort, Hallgren, Firth, Rosenbaum, Schuch, Mugisha, Probst, Van Damme, Carvalho and Stubbs2018), employment status (dichotomous, employed or homemaker or unemployed) (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Grebner, Schnabel, Hampel, Georgi and Seidler2011), sleep duration (dichotomous, >6 hours or ≤6 hours) (Dolsen et al., Reference Dolsen, Prather, Lamers and Penninx2021), history of cancer (dichotomous, no or yes) (Zaorsky et al., Reference Zaorsky, Zhang, Tuanquin, Bluethmann, Park and Chinchilli2019), history of cerebrovascular or cardiovascular disease (dichotomous, no or yes) (Chan et al., Reference Chan, Lyness and Conwell2007; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Chang, Kuo, Liu, Chen, Yeh, Luo and See2018) and consumption of vegetables (continuous) (Nanri et al., Reference Nanri, Mizoue, Poudel-Tandukar, Noda, Kato, Kurotani, Goto, Oba, Inoue and Tsugane2013), fruits (continuous) (Narita et al., Reference Narita, Nozaki, Shikimoto, Hori, Kim, Mimura, Tsugane and Sawada2022), fish (continuous) (Nanri et al., Reference Nanri, Mizoue, Poudel-Tandukar, Noda, Kato, Kurotani, Goto, Oba, Inoue and Tsugane2013) and meat (continuous) (Molendijk et al., Reference Molendijk, Molero, Ortuño Sánchez-Pedreño, Van der Does and Angel Martínez-González2018), as well as region (categorical, Ninohe, Yokote, Saku, Chubu, Katsushika, Mito, Nagaoka, Chuo-higashi, Kamigoto, Miyako and Suita) (Fedina et al., Reference Fedina, Mushonga, Bessaha, Jun, Narita and DeVylder2021; Narita et al., Reference Narita, Knowles, Fedina, Oh, Stickley, Kelleher and DeVylder2020a; Yoshioka et al., Reference Yoshioka, Hanley, Sato and Saijo2021). Employment status was included as a surrogate for socio-economic status (Bartley and Owen, Reference Bartley and Owen1996), for which data were not available. Food consumption was determined by multiplying the frequency of consumption by the relative portion size and energy-adjusted using the residual method (Willett et al., Reference Willett, Howe and Kushi1997).

Moreover, we controlled for the following factors at wave 2 as time-varying confounders, which may be affected by living arrangements at wave 1 and also confound the association of living arrangements at wave 2 with suicide death, non-suicide death and all-cause mortality: smoking status, employment status, sleep duration, history of cancer, history of cerebrovascular and cardiovascular disease and consumption of vegetables, fruits, fish and meat.

Statistical analyses

All analyses were conducted using R-4.3.1. We used inverse probability weighted pooled logistic regression models for discrete-time hazards (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Caniglia and Petito2021). Thereby, we evaluated the association of living arrangements at waves 1 and 2 with suicide death, non-suicide death and all-cause mortality during the 14-year follow-up. The directed acyclic graph of the hypothesized associations is shown in Fig. 1. We used random forest imputation (Stekhoven and Bühlmann, Reference Stekhoven and Bühlmann2012) to impute missing data for covariates but not for living arrangements and the number of years followed because they might be caused by the occurrence of events during waves 1 and 2.

Figure 1. A causal directed acyclic graph assumed for the main analysis. Wave 1 was defined as the period from 1995 to 1999, depending on the timing of data collection at each public health centre area, while wave 2 was defined as 5 years after wave 1, from 2000 to 2004.

Our models controlled for time-fixed and time-varying confounders using inverse probability weighting (Hernán and Robins, Reference Hernán, Robins and Boca2020). We calculated stabilized weights for living alone at waves 1 and 2 utilizing separate logistic models for exposures at waves 1 (adjusting for covariates at wave 1) and 2 (adjusting for exposure at wave 1 and covariates at waves 1 and 2). Since the number of regions was small in our data, we used fixed effects models to account for regions, which would safeguard against bias for the estimates (McNeish and Kelley, Reference McNeish and Kelley2019). To address selection bias due to loss to follow-up, we further generated cumulative stabilized weights for censoring, the value of which varied during the follow-up. Each weight was truncated at the 99th percentile (Cole and Hernán, Reference Cole and Hernán2008). The final stabilized weights were obtained as the product of these stabilized weights for censoring up to each follow-up year and the stabilized weights for living arrangements. This weighting produced a pseudo-population where covariates are balanced between groups and between those with and without censoring. A variable for year k (k = 0, 1, …, 14) was created as a continuous variable. The weighted pooled logistic models of the outcome were fit on the exposures and time in years (k), to adjust for changing hazards over time. See Supplementary Appendix 1 for details on our models.

To elucidate the performance of the pooled logistic model in predicting a survival function, we compared the Kaplan–Meier curve and the survival curve from unweighted pooled logistic regression. Next, based on the inverse probability weighted pooled logistic models, we predicted counterfactual hazard at each year and calculated survival probability that would have been observed if everyone had a specific pattern of living arrangements, namely: (1) living with someone at both waves 1 and 2, (2) living with someone at wave 1 but alone at wave 2, (3) living alone at wave 1 but with someone at wave 2 and (4) living alone at both waves 1 and 2. This approach allowed us to obtain cumulative incidence, risk differences (RDs) and risk ratios (RRs) throughout the follow-up period. We calculated RDs and RRs at 7 and 14 years after wave 2 (the midpoint and the end of the follow-up period) with pointwise bootstrap confidence intervals (CIs) using percentiles at each time.

Next, we conducted a sensitivity analysis controlling for additional baseline factors, namely prior mental illness, social support and coping. These factors might have been potential confounders (Kleiman and Liu, Reference Kleiman and Liu2013; Narita et al., Reference Narita, Devylder, Bessaha and Fedina2023, Reference Narita, Wilcox and DeVylder2020b; Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Martínez-Alés, Gratch, Rizk, Galfalvy, Choo and Mann2021) but were not measured at wave 1. Thus, we employed proxies for these factors. For the proxy of prior mental illness, we controlled for self-reported stress level (categorical, low, medium or high) and life enjoyment (categorical, low, medium or high) at wave 1. For the proxy of social support, we controlled for social support (continuous) 5 years prior to wave 1. Social support was evaluated using self-reported items that measured confidant support, esteem support and social isolation; details are available elsewhere (Ikeda et al., Reference Ikeda, Kawachi, Iso, Iwasaki, Inoue and Tsugane2013). Since this information was only available in Cohort II, one of the two cohorts constituting our general population sample, the missing data were handled via random forest imputation. For the proxy of coping, we controlled for approach-oriented coping (continuous) and avoidance-oriented coping (continuous) at wave 2, which were evaluated using self-reported items selected from the validated Japanese version of the Stress and Coping Inventory (Fukunishi et al., Reference Fukunishi, Nakagawa, Nakagawa, Sone, Kaji, Hosaka and Rahe1995); details are available elsewhere (Shikimoto et al., Reference Shikimoto, Nozaki, Sawada, Shimizu, Svensson, Nakagawa, Mimura and Tsugane2022). Our sensitivity analysis adjusted for mental illness and social support proxies at baseline in the wave-1 and -2 exposure models, and for coping variables after baseline only in the wave-2 exposure model. As coping variables were controlled through inverse probability weighting, we partially adjusted for potential confounding due to these variables without conditioning on mediators.

We examined the robustness of the estimates to potential unmeasured confounding by calculating E-values (VanderWeele and Ding, Reference VanderWeele and Ding2017). This involved determining the minimum strength of the association that an unmeasured confounder would require to have above and beyond the measured covariates to explain away the estimates. In other words, E-values quantify the degree of influence an unmeasured confounder, not included in our study, would need to have to nullify our findings, thereby offering a measure of the robustness of the results.

Finally, we evaluated age-stratified and gender-stratified associations. Because of the limited number of age-specific and gender-specific suicide incidences, we chose parsimonious models after iteratively fitting pooled logistic regression models with inverse probability weighting for each stratum. The threshold for age was set at 60, as this was the standard retirement age in Japan.

Results

We identified 103,880 participants. Table 1 summarizes the demographic features of the study participants at wave 1, categorized by living arrangements at waves 1 and 2. Data for living arrangements were missing for 16,590 participants (16.0%). The mean age (standard deviation) was 51.7 (7.90). The number of males was 55,352 (53.3%), while females numbered 48,528 (46.7%). Participants who lived alone at both waves 1 and 2 were characterized by older age, a higher likelihood of being female, higher engagement in physical exercise and a higher likelihood of being unemployed or a homemaker compared with those who lived with someone at both time points. We did not observe substantial differences in other characteristics among the groups. Note that the details of missing covariates are also shown in Table 1. Of the identified participants, 86,749 with data for living arrangements at both waves and data for the number of years followed were used for the analysis, while missing data for covariates were handled using random forest imputation. The flow diagram of the study population selection is summarized in Supplementary Fig. S1.

Table 1. Demographic features of study participants at wave 1 by living arrangements at waves 1 and 2

Abbreviation: SD, standard deviation.

Wave 1 was defined as 1995–1999, depending on the timing of the survey conducted at each public health centre area, and wave 2 was defined as 5 years after wave 1, i.e., 2000–2004.

Cumulative incidence over time

During the follow-up period, a total of 17,332 participants died, out of which 306 died by suicide, while 17,026 had non-suicide deaths. The mean (standard deviation) duration of the follow-up period was 12.7 (3.09) years. Supplementary Figure S2 shows the comparison of the Kaplan–Meier curve and the survival curve from unweighted pooled logistic regression. For all types of mortality, these curves appeared to have similar trajectories, indicating the good performance of our pooled logistic model. Figure 2 displays the cumulative incidence curves based on the pooled logistic model. It was observed that individuals living alone at both waves 1 and 2 consistently had a higher incidence rate compared with those who lived with someone over time. On the other hand, the findings were not as substantial for those living alone at either wave 1 or 2, especially in the first half of the follow-up period. While non-suicide death and all-cause mortality displayed comparable trajectories, the difference in cumulative incidence appeared to be particularly remarkable for suicide death at the end of the follow-up period.

Figure 2. The cumulative incidence of suicide death (a), non-suicide death (b) and all-cause mortality (c) estimated using the inverse probability weighted pooled logistic regression, incorporating covariates measured at waves 1 (1995–1999) and 2 (2000–2004) into the weights for time-varying exposures (waves 1 and 2) and censoring during the follow-up period (up to 14 years after wave 2).

Risk at the midpoint and the end of the follow-up period

Table 2 summarizes RDs and RRs at the midpoint and the end of the follow-up period from the inverse probability weighted cumulative incidence curves. Individuals who consistently lived alone exhibited a higher risk of suicide death, with an observed four-fold increase in risk at the end of the follow-up period (RD: 1.1%, 95% CI: 0.3–2.5%; RR: 4.00, 95% CI: 1.83–7.41). While findings were comparable at the midpoint of the follow-up period, the findings at this point should be interpreted with caution due to the limited number of suicide deaths, resulting in wide CIs. The risk in those who consistently lived alone was increased for non-suicide death (RD: 7.8%, 95% CI: 5.2–10.5%; RR: 1.56, 95% CI: 1.38–1.74) and all-cause mortality (RD: 8.7%, 95% CI: 6.2–11.3%; RR: 1.60, 95% CI: 1.42–1.79) at the follow-up period, with narrower CIs due to a greater number of cases. For suicide deaths, the increased risk observed in those who started to live with someone and those who transitioned to living alone was less impactful and did not reach statistical significance. For non-suicide death and all-cause mortality, those who transitioned to living alone showed a small but increased risk (non-suicide death: RD: 3.1%, 95% CI: 1.7–4.8%; RR: 1.22, 95% CI: 1.12–1.34; all-cause mortality: RD: 3.2%, 95% CI: 1.7–4.9%; RR: 1.22, 95% CI: 1.12–1.34), whereas those who started to live with someone showed non-significant results.

Table 2. Association of time-varying living arrangements at waves 1 and 2 with suicide death, non-suicide death and all-cause mortality

Abbreviations: RD, risk difference; RR, risk ratio; CI, confidence interval; NA, not applicable.

Wave 1 was defined as the period from 1995 to 1999, depending on the timing of data collection at each public health centre area, while wave 2 was defined as 5 years after wave 1, from 2000 to 2004.

The RD and RR were calculated based on cumulative incidence functions estimated by the inverse probability of exposure and censoring weighted pooled logistic regression accounting for covariates measured at waves 1 and 2 in the weights.

Age, gender, body mass index, smoking status, alcohol consumption, physical activity, employment status, sleep duration, history of cancer, history of cerebrovascular or cardiovascular disease, vegetable consumption, fruit consumption, fish consumption, meat consumption and region at wave 1 were controlled for. Also, smoking status, employment status, sleep duration, history of cancer, history of cerebrovascular or cardiovascular disease, vegetable consumption, fruit consumption, fish consumption and meat consumption at wave 2 were controlled for because these factors may be affected by living arrangements at wave 1 and also confound the association of living arrangements at wave 2 with suicide death, non-suicide death and all-cause mortality.

Sensitivity analysis

Next, we conducted a sensitivity analysis to control for proxies of prior mental illness, social support and coping. Supplementary Table S1 summarizes the results of this analysis. In short, the findings remained robust, with the RD and RR at both the midpoint and the end of the follow-up period showing no substantial changes when these potential confounders were incorporated into the models. The findings were consistent across all types of mortality.

Robustness to unmeasured confounders

The calculated E-values showed that some observed associations between living arrangements and suicide death, non-suicide death and all-cause mortality were reasonably robust to unmeasured confounders (see Table 3). For example, for the association of living alone at both waves 1 and 2 with suicide death at the end of the follow-up period, an unmeasured confounder would need to be associated with both of them above and beyond the adjusted covariates by an RR of 7.46 to fully explain away the observed association and 3.06 to shift the CI to include the null value.

Table 3. Robustness to unmeasured confounding of the association of living arrangements at waves 1 and 2 with suicide death, non-suicide death and all-cause mortality

Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval.

E-values for point estimates are the minimum strength of association on the RR that unmeasured confounding would need to have above and beyond the adjusted covariates to explain away the estimates. E-values for the limit of CI are the minimum strength of association on the RR that unmeasured confounding would need to have above and beyond the adjusted covariates to shift the 95% CI and include the null value.

Age-stratified and gender-stratified associations

The age-stratified associations (60 or older vs. 59 or younger) are presented in Supplementary Tables S2 and S3. The results for participants aged 60 or older were inconsistent, likely due to the limited number of cases, whereas the findings for individuals aged 59 or younger were similar to those of the main analyses. Supplementary Tables S4 and S5 present the gender-stratified associations. Overall, the results appeared inconsistent. The limited number of suicide deaths warrants caution in the interpretation of these findings.

Discussion

In this cohort study comprising a general population sample, the inverse probability weighted pooled logistic regression and the cumulative incidence curve allowed us to evaluate the impact of time-varying living arrangements over time, controlling for time-varying confounders. The performance of our pooled logistic model was shown by comparing the survival curve from the unweighted model with the Kaplan–Meier curve. While the cumulative incidence curve showed the dynamics of the impact of living arrangements, those who lived alone at both waves 1 and 2 consistently showed a higher cumulative incidence rate than those who persistently lived with someone throughout the study period. The computation of risk at the midpoint and the end of the follow-up period showed that persistently living alone at both waves 1 and 2 was associated with an increased risk of suicide death compared to those who persistently lived with someone (0.9–1.1% absolute risk increase and more than a four-fold relative risk increase). On the other hand, this association was weaker and non-significant for those who changed living arrangements (up to 0.6% absolute risk increase and up to a 2.6-fold relative risk increase). Those who persistently lived alone showed an increased risk of non-suicide death and all-cause mortality (3.2–8.7% absolute risk increase and more than a 1.5-fold relative risk increase), suggesting that living alone is a pervasive risk factor for various causes of death. The robustness of the findings was supported via the sensitivity analysis controlling for the proxies of prior mental illness, social support and coping. The robustness was further verified by calculating E-values. While previous research has shown an association between living arrangements and suicide death (Olfson et al., Reference Olfson, Cosgrove, Altekruse, Wall and Blanco2022; Poudel-Tandukar et al., Reference Poudel-Tandukar, Nanri, Mizoue, Matsushita, Takahashi, Noda, Inoue and Tsugane2011; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Cullen, Graham, Lyall, Mackay, Okolie, Pearsall, Ward, John and Smith2021), this study is the first to examine living arrangements as a time-varying variable, controlling for time-fixed and time-varying confounders, and to evaluate the changing association over time.

In this study, 3,507 participants changed their living arrangements from wave 1 to wave 2. Such participants did not show an increased risk of suicide death in the first half of the follow-up period. Even at the end of the follow-up period, the impact was not as substantial as for those who persistently lived alone. Thus, living arrangements may change, and this variability indeed impacts the risk of suicide death. A similar discussion applies to non-suicide death and all-cause mortality. These findings hold significant importance, particularly considering that living alone represents an intervenable factor that can be effectively addressed through approaches in public health and social work (Nestadt, Reference Nestadt2022).

Previous research examining suicide death using survival analysis has reported a single average HR over the follow-up period. However, the causal interpretation of this single HR may potentially be misleading due to its variation over time (Hernán, Reference Hernán2010; Prentice et al., Reference Prentice, Pettinger and Anderson2005). Consequently, it is crucial to account for time-dependent HRs. Nonetheless, presenting only a single time-dependent HR can also be misleading due to built-in selection bias, which arises from the selection of participants at higher risk of events earlier in the study (Hernán, Reference Hernán2010; Prentice et al., Reference Prentice, Pettinger and Anderson2005). To address these issues, we employed a discrete-time hazard model (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Caniglia and Petito2021), showing the cumulative incidence over time and calculating RD and RR at multiple time points. Our cumulative incidence curve provides insights into the dynamics of the association between living arrangements and suicide death throughout the follow-up period.

The heterogeneous suicide rates across ages (World Health Organization, 2021b) compelled us to explore the age-stratified associations. While the results for participants aged 59 or younger were similar to those of the main analyses, the limited number of cases led to inconsistent results in those aged 60 or older. In addition, the same issue prevented the inclusion of the product term of the year and living arrangements variables, hindering the analysis of time-dependent HR. Taken together, these findings should be regarded as exploratory results, and caution should be exercised when interpreting and comparing the results. The same discussion applies to the gender-stratified associations. Past research has suggested an association between living arrangements and suicide death, particularly in men (Poudel-Tandukar et al., Reference Poudel-Tandukar, Nanri, Mizoue, Matsushita, Takahashi, Noda, Inoue and Tsugane2011; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Cullen, Graham, Lyall, Mackay, Okolie, Pearsall, Ward, John and Smith2021). While effect heterogeneity across gender is possible, the limited number of cases again impeded obtaining reliable results. Future studies should include an adequate number of cases to incorporate the product term of the year and living arrangements variables, accounting for time-dependent HR.

Two mechanisms may be proposed to explain the association between living alone and suicide death. The interpersonal theory of suicide suggests that the risk of suicide is influenced by various social factors, including thwarted belongingness, perceived burdensomeness and hopelessness (Van Orden et al., Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner2010). Living alone may increase the likelihood of social isolation and loneliness, which can correlate with a sense of thwarted belongingness. Indeed, social isolation and loneliness are associated with detrimental mental health and suicide-related outcomes (Narita et al., Reference Narita, Banawa, Zhou, DeVylder, Koyanagi and Oh2021, Reference Narita, Stickley and DeVylder2020c; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Veronese, Galvano, Favaro, Ostinelli, Noventa, Favaretto, Tudor, Finessi, Shin, Smith, Koyanagi, Cester, Bolzetta, Cotroneo, Maggi, Demurtas, De Leo and Trabucchi2020; Stickley and Koyanagi, Reference Stickley and Koyanagi2016). The integrated motivational–volitional theory distinguishes between the premotivational, motivational and volitional phases in the progression of suicide development (O’Connor and Kirtley, Reference O’Connor and Kirtley2018). Among these phases, the volitional phase is characterized by the transition of thoughts about suicidal intent into actual actions (O’Connor and Kirtley, Reference O’Connor and Kirtley2018). We did not conduct a mediation analysis in this study because mediation using a discrete-time hazard model may require additional development. However, employing such an analysis in a different framework could enhance scientific understanding.

The present study has implications for public health and social work, as well as for the fields of psychiatry and psychology. Our study showed that persistently living alone had a substantial impact on suicide death, emphasizing its importance in the context of prevention. The identification of individuals at higher risk of suicide enables targeted prevention efforts (Eaton, Reference Eaton2012). Living alone is a prevalent condition, exceeding 30% in Japan (Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare, 2022). This widespread distribution would make targeted prevention impactful. Living alone is an intervenable factor (Nestadt, Reference Nestadt2022), and the present study indeed showed that transitions in living arrangements can substantially change the risk of suicide death, providing warranted practical evidence (Olfson et al., Reference Olfson, Cosgrove, Altekruse, Wall and Blanco2022). While addressing the challenges of living alone at the national level is beyond the scope of this paper, social programmes that provide a sense of community and improved access to mental health services might be beneficial (Kleiman and Liu, Reference Kleiman and Liu2013; Tadmon and Bearman, Reference Tadmon and Bearman2023). For clinicians, our findings highlight the need to consider patients’ living situations as a factor that may contribute to suicide risk. Psychologists, too, can incorporate these insights into therapeutic approaches, potentially targeting feelings of isolation or developing strategies to foster social connection for individuals living alone. Further, our findings may help guide future studies investigating social factors that can be intervened upon to mitigate the impact of living alone, including causal mediation analysis.

Limitations

Our study has several limitations. First, measurement bias is conceivable not only in self-reported living arrangements but also in suicide deaths obtained from death certificates (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Harron, Pescarini, Pinto Junior, Allik, Siroky, Campbell, Dundas, Ichihara, Leyland, Barreto and Katikireddi2022). Second, certain potential confounders were not accounted for due to the lack of available information, e.g., childhood maltreatment (Stickley et al., Reference Stickley, Waldman, Sumiyoshi, Narita, Shirama, Shin and Oh2021, Reference Stickley, Waldman, Ueda, Koyanagi, Sumiyoshi, Narita, Inoue, DeVylder and Oh2020). For socio-economic status (Rehkopf and Buka, Reference Rehkopf and Buka2006), we used employment status as a surrogate (Bartley and Owen, Reference Bartley and Owen1996). For prior mental illness, social support, and coping (Kleiman and Liu, Reference Kleiman and Liu2013; Narita et al., Reference Narita, Devylder, Bessaha and Fedina2023, Reference Narita, Wilcox and DeVylder2020b; Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Martínez-Alés, Gratch, Rizk, Galfalvy, Choo and Mann2021), we showed the robustness of the findings via a sensitivity analysis incorporating the proxies of these potential confounders at wave 1. Also, E-values suggested the robustness of the findings against unmeasured potential confounders. Taken together, although the influence of unmeasured confounders is conceivable, it is unlikely to substantially change the conclusion. Third, the limited number of cases hindered our ability to provide comprehensive information on certain results. This limitation was particularly relevant when analysing suicide death in those who changed living arrangements, which resulted in small statistical power and wide CIs. Similarly, the age-stratified and gender-stratified analyses provided inconsistent results, likely due to this limitation. Also, we were unable to construct a model that examines how the impact may vary depending on other factors, including social support and coping strategies (Kleiman and Liu, Reference Kleiman and Liu2013; Narita et al., Reference Narita, Devylder, Bessaha and Fedina2023; Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Martínez-Alés, Gratch, Rizk, Galfalvy, Choo and Mann2021). Fourth, we maintained the same 5-year interval for evaluating living arrangements and the 14-year follow-up for every public health centre area, but the initiation of data collection varied between areas. While this variability was controlled for in the model, it did not address the variations in suicide trends towards the end of the follow-up period (i.e., 2014–2018). In Japan, the number of suicides in 2014 was 25,427 with a rate of 20.0 per 100,000 individuals; by 2018, they dropped to 20,840 and 16.5 per 100,000 individuals, respectively (National Police Agency, 2023), which might have influenced the results. Fifth, we handled missing covariate data using random forest imputation but chose not to impute missing exposure data due to potential events occurring between waves 1 and 2. While the demographics did not show substantial differences between the analysed and missing data, the selection of participants might have somewhat influenced the results. Sixth, we evaluated living arrangements as time-varying variables at waves 1 and 2. However, it is possible that some participants could have changed living arrangements multiple times between these waves, which was not fully captured. Finally, this study is population-based but only includes individuals aged 40–69 years at baseline, limiting the generalizability of the results to other age groups.

Conclusions

Individuals who persistently live alone have an increased risk of suicide death, non-suicide death and all-cause mortality, whereas this impact is weaker for those who change their living arrangements.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796024000325.

Availability of data and materials

For information on how to apply to gain access to JPHC data, follow the instructions at https://epi.ncc.go.jp/en/jphc/805/8155.html.

Acknowledgements

This study was conducted under the framework of the Collaborative Cohort Research Network Project to the Six National Centers for Advanced and Specialized Medical Care.

Author contributions

Research questions were devised by ZN. Analyses were conducted by ZN and supervised by TS, AG and NS. The initial draft was written by ZN and revised by all authors with substantial contributions.

Financial support

This study was supported by a Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (22K14858), the National Cancer Center Research and Development Fund (since 2011) and a Grant-Aid for Cancer Research from the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare of Japan (from1989 to 2010).

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical standards

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the National Cancer Center and the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent.

References

Amiri, S and Behnezhad, S (2018) Body mass index and risk of suicide: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders 238, 615625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartley, M and Owen, C (1996) Relation between socioeconomic status, employment, and health during economic change, 1973-93. BMJ 313, 445449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, JW, Liang, J, Krause, N, Akiyama, H, Sugisawa, H and Fukaya, T (2002) Transitions in living arrangements among elders in Japan: Does health make a difference? The Journals of Gerontology Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 57, S20920.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, SS, Lyness, JM and Conwell, Y (2007) Do cerebrovascular risk factors confer risk for suicide in later life? A case-control study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry: Official Journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry 15, 541544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, SR and Hernán, MA (2008) Constructing inverse probability weights for marginal structural models. American Journal of Epidemiology 168, 656664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolsen, EA, Prather, AA, Lamers, F and Penninx, BWJH (2021) Suicidal ideation and suicide attempts: Associations with sleep duration, insomnia, and inflammation. Psychological Medicine 51, 20942103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eaton, WW (ed.) (2012) Public Mental Health. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fedina, L, Mushonga, DR, Bessaha, ML, Jun, H-J, Narita, Z and DeVylder, J (2021) Moderating effects of perceived neighborhood factors on intimate partner violence, psychological distress, and suicide risk. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 36, 1054610563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fukunishi, I, Nakagawa, T, Nakagawa, H, Sone, Y, Kaji, N, Hosaka, T and Rahe, RH (1995) Validity and reliability of the Japanese version of the Stress and Coping Inventory. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 49, 195199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, R, Munafò, MR, Davey Smith, G and Wootton, RE (2020) Examining the effect of smoking on suicidal ideation and attempts: Triangulation of epidemiological approaches. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science 217, 701707.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernán, MA (2010) The hazards of hazard ratios. Epidemiology 21, 1315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernán, M, and Robins, J (2020) Causal Inference: What If, Boca, R (ed.). FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC, 257275.Google Scholar
Ikeda, A, Kawachi, I, Iso, H, Iwasaki, M, Inoue, M and Tsugane, S (2013) Social support and cancer incidence and mortality: The JPHC study cohort II. Cancer Causes and Control 24, 847860.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isaacs, JY, Smith, MM, Sherry, SB, Seno, M, Moore, ML and Stewart, SH (2022) Alcohol use and death by suicide: A meta-analysis of 33 studies. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior 52, 600614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleiman, EM and Liu, RT (2013) Social support as a protective factor in suicide: Findings from two nationally representative samples. Journal of Affective Disorders 150, 540545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeish, D and Kelley, K (2019) Fixed effects models versus mixed effects models for clustered data: Reviewing the approaches, disentangling the differences, and making recommendations. Psychological Methods 24, 2035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare (2022) Comprehensive survey of living conditions. https://www.mhlw.go.jp/toukei/saikin/hw/k-tyosa/k-tyosa22/dl/02.pdf (Japanese) (accessed 15 May 2024).Google Scholar
Molendijk, M, Molero, P, Ortuño Sánchez-Pedreño, F, Van der Does, W and Angel Martínez-González, M (2018) Diet quality and depression risk: A systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective studies. Journal of Affective Disorders 226, 346354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, EJ, Caniglia, EC and Petito, LC (2021) Causal survival analysis: A guide to estimating intention-to-treat and per-protocol effects from randomized clinical trials with non-adherence. Research Methods in Medicine & Health Sciences 2, 3949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nanri, A, Mizoue, T, Poudel-Tandukar, K, Noda, M, Kato, M, Kurotani, K, Goto, A, Oba, S, Inoue, M, Tsugane, S and Japan Public Health Center-based Prospective Study Group (2013) Dietary patterns and suicide in Japanese adults: The Japan Public Health Center-based Prospective Study. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science 203, 422427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narita, Z, Banawa, R, Zhou, S, DeVylder, J, Koyanagi, A and Oh, H (2021) Loneliness and psychotic experiences among US university students: Findings from the Healthy Minds Study 2020. Psychiatry Research 308, .Google ScholarPubMed
Narita, Z, Devylder, J, Bessaha, M and Fedina, L (2023) Associations of self-isolation, social support and coping strategies with depression and suicidal ideation in U.S. young adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing 32, 929937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narita, Z, Knowles, K, Fedina, L, Oh, H, Stickley, A, Kelleher, I and DeVylder, J (2020a) Neighborhood change and psychotic experiences in a general population sample. Schizophrenia Research 216, 316321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narita, Z, Nozaki, S, Shikimoto, R, Hori, H, Kim, Y, Mimura, M, Tsugane, S and Sawada, N (2022) Association between vegetable, fruit, and flavonoid-rich fruit consumption in midlife and major depressive disorder in later life: The JPHC Saku Mental Health Study. Translational Psychiatry 12, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narita, Z, Stickley, A and DeVylder, J (2020c) Loneliness and psychotic experiences in a general population sample. Schizophrenia Research 218, 146150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narita, Z, Wilcox, HC and DeVylder, J (2020b) Psychotic experiences and suicidal outcomes in a general population sample. Schizophrenia Research 215, 223228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Police Agency (2023) The number of suicide deaths. https://www.npa.go.jp/publications/statistics/safetylife/jisatsu.html (Japanese) (accessed 15 May 2024).Google Scholar
Nestadt, PS (2022) Suicide and the solitary life: Differential risks of living alone across sociodemographic groups. American Journal of Public Health 112, 17021704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, RC and Kirtley, OJ (2018) The integrated motivational–volitional model of suicidal behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences 373, .Google ScholarPubMed
Olfson, M, Cosgrove, CM, Altekruse, SF, Wall, MM and Blanco, C (2022) Living alone and suicide risk in the United States, 2008‒2019. American Journal of Public Health 112, 17741782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poudel-Tandukar, K, Nanri, A, Mizoue, T, Matsushita, Y, Takahashi, Y, Noda, M, Inoue, M, Tsugane, S and Japan Public Health Center-based Prospective Study Group (2011) Differences in suicide risk according to living arrangements in Japanese men and women – The Japan Public Health Center-based (JPHC) prospective study. Journal of Affective Disorders 131, 113119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prentice, RL, Pettinger, M and Anderson, GL (2005) Statistical issues arising in the Women’s Health Initiative. Biometrics 61, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rehkopf, DH and Buka, SL (2006) The association between suicide and the socio-economic characteristics of geographical areas: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine 36, 145157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sawada, N, Nakaya, T, Kashima, S, Yorifuji, T, Hanibuchi, T, Charvat, H, Yamaji, T, Iwasaki, M, Inoue, M, Iso, H and Tsugane, S (2022) Long-term exposure to fine particle matter and all-cause mortality and cause-specific mortality in Japan: The JPHC Study. BMC Public Health 22, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, B, Grebner, K, Schnabel, A, Hampel, H, Georgi, K and Seidler, A (2011) Impact of employment status and work-related factors on risk of completed suicide. A case-control psychological autopsy study. Psychiatry Research 190, 265270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, RJ (2022) Living alone and suicide risk: A complex problem requiring a whole population approach. American Journal of Public Health 112, 16991701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, RJ, Cullen, B, Graham, N, Lyall, DM, Mackay, D, Okolie, C, Pearsall, R, Ward, J, John, A and Smith, DJ (2021) Living alone, loneliness and lack of emotional support as predictors of suicide and self-harm: A nine-year follow up of the UK Biobank cohort. Journal of Affective Disorders 279, 316323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, RJ, Harron, KL, Pescarini, JM, Pinto Junior, EP, Allik, M, Siroky, AN, Campbell, D, Dundas, R, Ichihara, MY, Leyland, AH, Barreto, ML and Katikireddi, SV (2022) Biases arising from linked administrative data for epidemiological research: A conceptual framework from registration to analyses. European Journal of Epidemiology 37, 12151224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shikimoto, R, Nozaki, S, Sawada, N, Shimizu, Y, Svensson, T, Nakagawa, A, Mimura, M and Tsugane, S (2022) Coping in mid- to late life and risk of mild cognitive impairment subtypes and dementia: A JPHC Saku Mental Health Study. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 90, 10851101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solmi, M, Veronese, N, Galvano, D, Favaro, A, Ostinelli, EG, Noventa, V, Favaretto, E, Tudor, F, Finessi, M, Shin, JI, Smith, L, Koyanagi, A, Cester, A, Bolzetta, F, Cotroneo, A, Maggi, S, Demurtas, J, De Leo, D and Trabucchi, M (2020) Factors associated with loneliness: An umbrella review of observational studies. Journal of Affective Disorders 271, 131138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanley, B, Martínez-Alés, G, Gratch, I, Rizk, M, Galfalvy, H, Choo, T-H and Mann, JJ (2021) Coping strategies that reduce suicidal ideation: An ecological momentary assessment study. Journal of Psychiatric Research 133, 3237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stekhoven, DJ and Bühlmann, P (2012) MissForest—Non-parametric missing value imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics 28, 112118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stickley, A and Koyanagi, A (2016) Loneliness, common mental disorders and suicidal behavior: Findings from a general population survey. Journal of Affective Disorders 197, 8187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stickley, A, Waldman, K, Sumiyoshi, T, Narita, Z, Shirama, A, Shin, JI and Oh, H (2021) Childhood physical neglect and psychotic experiences: Findings from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 15, 256262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stickley, A, Waldman, K, Ueda, M, Koyanagi, A, Sumiyoshi, T, Narita, Z, Inoue, Y, DeVylder, JE and Oh, H (2020) Childhood neglect and suicidal behavior: Findings from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Child Abuse and Neglect 103, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tadmon, D and Bearman, PS (2023) Differential spatial-social accessibility to mental health care and suicide. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 120, .Google ScholarPubMed
Tsugane, S and Sawada, N (2014) The JPHC study: Design and some findings on the typical Japanese diet. Japanese Journal of Clinical Oncology 44, 777782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
U.S. Census Bureau, (2020) Older adults who are foreign born less likely to live alone than native born. https://www.census.gov/library/stories/2020/08/young-adults-most-likely-to-change-living-arrangements.html (accessed 15 May 2024).Google Scholar
Vancampfort, D, Hallgren, M, Firth, J, Rosenbaum, S, Schuch, FB, Mugisha, J, Probst, M, Van Damme, T, Carvalho, AF and Stubbs, B (2018) Physical activity and suicidal ideation: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders 225, 438448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderWeele, TJ (2019) Principles of confounder selection. European Journal of Epidemiology 34, 211219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderWeele, TJ and Ding, P (2017) Sensitivity analysis in observational research: Introducing the E-value. Annals of Internal Medicine. 167, 268274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Orden, KA, Witte, TK, Cukrowicz, KC, Braithwaite, SR, Selby, EA and Joiner, TE (2010) The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychological Review 117, 575600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willett, WC, Howe, GR and Kushi, LH (1997) Adjustment for total energy intake in epidemiologic studies. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 65, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (1990) International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 1st edn. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
World Health Organization, (2021a) Suicide. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/suicide (accessed 15 May 2024).Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2021b). Suicide rates. https://www.who.int/data/gho/data/themes/mental-health/suicide-rates (accessed 18 April 2024).Google Scholar
Wu, VC-C, Chang, S-H, Kuo, C-F, Liu, J-R, Chen, S-W, Yeh, Y-H, Luo, S-F and See, L-C (2018) Suicide death rates in patients with cardiovascular diseases – A 15-year nationwide cohort study in Taiwan. Journal of Affective Disorders 238, 187193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoshioka, E, Hanley, SJB, Sato, Y and Saijo, Y (2021) Geography of suicide in Japan: Spatial patterning and rural–urban differences. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology 56, 731746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaorsky, NG, Zhang, Y, Tuanquin, L, Bluethmann, SM, Park, HS and Chinchilli, VM (2019) Suicide among cancer patients. Nature Communications 10, .Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. A causal directed acyclic graph assumed for the main analysis. Wave 1 was defined as the period from 1995 to 1999, depending on the timing of data collection at each public health centre area, while wave 2 was defined as 5 years after wave 1, from 2000 to 2004.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic features of study participants at wave 1 by living arrangements at waves 1 and 2

Figure 2

Figure 2. The cumulative incidence of suicide death (a), non-suicide death (b) and all-cause mortality (c) estimated using the inverse probability weighted pooled logistic regression, incorporating covariates measured at waves 1 (1995–1999) and 2 (2000–2004) into the weights for time-varying exposures (waves 1 and 2) and censoring during the follow-up period (up to 14 years after wave 2).

Figure 3

Table 2. Association of time-varying living arrangements at waves 1 and 2 with suicide death, non-suicide death and all-cause mortality

Figure 4

Table 3. Robustness to unmeasured confounding of the association of living arrangements at waves 1 and 2 with suicide death, non-suicide death and all-cause mortality

Supplementary material: File

Narita et al. supplementary material

Narita et al. supplementary material
Download Narita et al. supplementary material(File)
File 134.6 KB