Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T01:31:07.509Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal alcohol dependence symptoms, maternal insensitivity to children’s distress, and young children’s blunted emotional reactivity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2024

Debrielle T. Jacques*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
Melissa L. Sturge-Apple
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Patrick T. Davies
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Dante Cicchetti
Affiliation:
Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
*
Corresponding author: D. T. Jacques; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Maternal insensitivity to children’s emotional distress (e.g., expressions of sadness or fearfulness) is one mechanism through which maternal alcohol dependence may increase children’s risk for psychopathology. Although emotion dysregulation is consistently associated with psychopathology, it remains unclear how or why alcohol dependence’s effects on caregiving responses to children’s distress may impact children’s emotion regulation over time, particularly in ways that may engender risks for psychopathology. This study examined longitudinal associations between lifetime maternal alcohol dependence symptoms, mothers’ insensitivity to children’s emotional distress cues, and children’s emotional reactivity among 201 mother-child dyads (Mchild age = 2.14 years; 56% Black; 11% Latino). Structural equation modeling analyses revealed a significant mediational pathway such that maternal alcohol dependence predicted increases in mothers’ insensitivity to children’s emotional distress across a one-year period (β = .16, p = .013), which subsequently predicted decreases in children’s emotional reactivity one year later (β = −.29, p = .009). Results suggest that mothers with alcohol dependence symptoms may struggle to sensitively respond to children’s emotional distress, which may prompt children to suppress or hide their emotions as an adaptive, protective strategy. The potential developmental benefits and consequences of early, protective expressive suppression strategies are discussed via developmental psychopathology frameworks.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Emotion regulation is an important component of everyday life. Successfully modulating or altering when and how we express emotions significantly influences our social lives and how we experience and engage with the world around us. Deficits in emotion regulation often underlie many common psychological conditions, including depression and drug and alcohol use disorders (Beauchaine & Cicchetti, Reference Beauchaine and Cicchetti2019; Cole & Hall, Reference Cole, Hall, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2008; Garland et al., Reference Garland, Bell, Atchley and Froeliger2020), and some suggest that emotion dysregulation perpetuates intergenerational manifestations of psychopathology within families (e.g., Han & Shaffer, Reference Han and Shaffer2013; Han et al., Reference Han, Lei, Qian, Li, Wang and Zhang2016; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Kaliush, Conradt, Terrell, Neff, Allen, Smid, Monk and Crowell2019), particularly among parents with psychiatric conditions that are associated with emotion (e.g. Cheetham et al., Reference Cheetham, Allen, Yücel and Lubman2010). Thus, given its importance in influencing our well-being and its ubiquity in our lives, it is necessary to understand the developmental roots of and processes through which emotion regulation emerges, particularly among families at pronounced risk for emotion-regulation difficulties.

Since early emotion-regulation problems can alter an individual’s mental health trajectory, significant interest has surrounded emotion regulation’s influences on children’s well-being and psychological functioning (e.g., Compas et al., Reference Compas, Jaser, Bettis, Watson, Gruhn, Dunbar and Thigpen2017; McRae et al., Reference McRae, Gross, Weber, Robertson, Sokol-Hessner, Ray, Gabrieli and Ochsner2012; Pitskel et al., Reference Pitskel, Bolling, Kaiser, Crowley and Pelphrey2011). One important and distinct aspect of emotion regulation (Derryberry & Rothbart, Reference Derryberry and Rothbart1988; Putnam & Rothbart, Reference Putnam and Rothbart2006) is emotional reactivity which has been defined as “the extent to which individuals experience emotions, respond to a variety of emotional stimuli, the intensity of the response, and the duration of arousal” (Shapero et al., Reference Shapero, Dale, Archibald, Pedrelli and Levesque2018, p. 2). From a developmental perspective, the development of emotional reactivity is a complex and dynamic process, one that is highly influenced by many multifaceted individual and environmental factors (Braungart-Rieker et al., Reference Braungart-Rieker, Hill-Soderlund and Karrass2010). Emotional reactivity is often characterized as frequent and intense in infancy and early childhood, when children typically lack independent emotion-regulation capacities (Kopp, Reference Kopp1989). Infants and toddlers may be highly responsive to emotional stimuli and therefore may be highly reactive to people, situations, and events in their environments. Infants, for example may display frequent and intense distress and feelings of hunger, sadness, or fear. As they receive greater exposure to and familiarity with emotional stimuli, and as their independent regulatory capacities and coping strategies broaden in early childhood, emotional reactivity may naturally decrease (Somerville & Casey, Reference Somerville, Casey, Gazzaniga and Mangun2014). Importantly, unlike previous developmental periods, middle childhood may bring with it increased abilities to understand, process, express, and respond to complex and nuanced emotions, like shame and empathy specifically since cognitive abilities, which also facilitate and underlie emotions and behaviors, rapidly develop from infancy to middle childhood (Harter, Reference Harter1986; LeDoux, Reference LeDoux1989). Consequently, young children are increasingly able to recognize, process, and understand others’ emotions, emotional stimuli and situations, but also their own emotional repertoire and patterns of responding in and coping with distinct emotional settings (Band & Weisz, Reference Band and Weisz1988; Campos et al., Reference Campos, Campos and Barrett1989; Creasey et al., Reference Creasey, Ottlinger, Devico, Murray, Harvey and Hesson-McInnis1997; Fischer et al., Reference Fischer, Shaver and Carnochan1990). For example, children may recognize feeling heightened levels of fear or nervousness at home (or in the presence of family conflict), and they may employ specific coping strategies to address those feelings in that context, while recognizing feelings of happiness in a different setting.

Importantly, individual differences impact the nature and trajectory of the development of emotional reactivity processes (Mauss et al., Reference Mauss, Levenson, McCarter, Wilhelm and Gross2005; Noroña-Zhou & Tung, Reference Noroña-Zhou and Tung2021). Specifically, there is much variability in the factors that influence the development and trajectory of emotional reactivity (Calkins, Reference Calkins1994; Cole et al., Reference Cole, Zahn-Waxler, Fox, Usher and Welsh1996; Davidson, Reference Davidson1998; Eman et al., Reference Eman, Khalid and Nicolson2019; Panlilio et al., Reference Panlilio, Harring, Harden, Morrison and Duncan2020; Picoito et al., Reference Picoito, Santos and Nunes2021), with age and developmental period and developmental environment being some of the common factors that have been studied (Banerjee, Reference Banerjee1997; Lincoln et al., Reference Lincoln, Russell, Donohue and Racine2017; Picoito et al., Reference Picoito, Santos and Nunes2021; Sanchis-Sanchis et al., Reference Sanchis-Sanchis, Grau, Moliner and Morales-Murillo2020; Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner, Reference Zimmer-Gembeck and Skinner2016). Much heterogeneity and variability also exist within the construct of emotional reactivity, including (A) how, why, and when individuals experience emotions; (B) why the type, intensity, and duration of emotion differs from person to person; and (C) why some individuals – based on their emotional reactivity – might be at heightened risk for psychopathology (e.g., Nock et al., Reference Nock, Wedig, Holmberg and Hooley2008). This work demonstrates the utility in parsing traditional or typical notions of the development of emotional reactivity, by delineating specific predictors, mechanisms, and trajectories or pathways that might influence children’s emotional reactivity.

Early childhood may be a particularly salient developmental period for emotion-regulation development. Young children typically rely on others (primarily caregivers) for emotion-regulation support since they are developmentally incapable of independently and consistently modulating their own emotions (Crockenberg & Leerkes, Reference Crockenberg and Leerkes2004; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Kopp, Reference Kopp1989). Consequently, very young children may have a limited repertoire of independent regulatory strategies available to enact during times of intense of emotional arousal. During early childhood (between ages 2 and 5), children become more adept at understanding, processing, and internalizing contextual norms around emotional expressions – including the social desirability or appropriateness of certain emotions when expressed in certain places or around certain people (e.g., caregivers) (Banerjee, Reference Banerjee1997). These socially and contextually-influenced emotional display norms may confer to children that emotions and emotional expressions function to facilitate social connections and relationship bonds (e.g., children may learn that crying elicits sensitive responses from caregivers, which may help children feels emotionally secure and connected to their parents) (Cole & Jacobs, Reference Cole and Jacobs2018; Gross & Ballif, Reference Gross and Ballif1991). As children gain more experience expressing emotions and navigating emotional contexts, internalization of these emotional display norms may encourage children to independently regulate their emotions and effectively navigate emotional contexts (Cole & Jacobs, Reference Cole and Jacobs2018). This motivation may be particularly strong for young children who experience emotion-related stress or trauma, such as emotional abuse or neglect since harmful or insensitive caregiver responses to their emotional needs may evoke, intensify, or prolong negative emotions like sadness or anger (Leerkes et al., Reference Leerkes, Nayena Blankson and O’Brien2009). For these children, certain patterns of emotional reactivity may reflect intense and/or prolonged dysregulated emotional reactions to environmental stress (Davies & Martin, Reference Davies and Martin2013; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Cummings and Winter2004). In other words, distressing experiences with caregivers or within a family’s emotional climate (e.g., receiving frequent emotional invalidations from parents or receiving messages that your emotions do not matter) may impact children’s emotional reactivity (e.g., Buckholdt et al., Reference Buckholdt, Parra and Jobe-Shields2014; Denham et al., Reference Denham, Mitchell-Copeland, Strandberg, Auerbach and Blair1997; Shenk & Fruzzetti, Reference Shenk and Fruzzetti2011), with some children possibly displaying frequent, intense emotions (e.g., Miu et al., Reference Miu, Szentágotai-Tătar, Balazsi, Nechita, Bunea and Pollak2022) while others may display more muted, controlled, and less intense displays (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Schaller, Fabes, Bustamante, Mathy, Shell and Rhodes1988, Reference Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, Carlo and Miller1991, Reference Eisenberg, Fabes, Carlo, Troyer, Speer, Karbon and Switzer1992; Fabes et al., Reference Fabes, Leonard, Kupanoff and Martin2001). Moreover, during early childhood, theory-of-mind develops and children begin to view themselves as distinct emotional beings from other people, including caregivers (Grolnick et al., Reference Grolnick, McMenamy and & Kurowski1999; Stack & Lewis, Reference Stack and Lewis2008; Stack & Poulin-Dubois, Reference Stack, Poulin-Dubois, Pushkar, Bukowski, Schwartzman, Stack and White1998). When considered together, young children may begin to internalize caregivers as sources of emotional distress who may be unreliable or ineffective external emotion regulators and thereby be more motivated to regulate their own emotions (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998).

Young children (infants and toddlers) may also lack the verbal and linguistic ability to articulate their emotions or (importantly) what they need in order to effectively regulate, process, or address their emotions (e.g., they cannot ask for a hug or tell someone that they are sad or scared) (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Armstrong, Pemberton, Calkins and Bell2010; Roben et al., Reference Roben, Cole and Armstrong2013). Also in early childhood, developmental advancements in other elements of cognition and self-regulation, including executive functioning and inhibitory further enable expressive suppression. Interestingly, even before age 2, children start to gauge the authenticity and congruence of both positive and negative emotional expressions in other children and adults (Serrat et al., Reference Serrat, Amadó, Rostan, Caparrós and Sidera2020; Sidera et al., Reference Sidera, Amadó and Serrat2013; Walle & Campos, Reference Walle and Campos2014). Thus, early on, children may not only start detecting emotional (in)congruence in others’ expressions, but may also begin learning that inauthentic or incongruent expressions may serve a social or behavioral function (Pala & Lewis, Reference Pala and Lewis2021; Walle & Campos, Reference Walle and Campos2014). This builds on prior work that demonstrates that toddlers (e.g., 3- and 4-year-olds) can differentiate discrete emotions (e.g., understand the difference between fear, sadness, anger, etc.) and they may also understand the utility or benefit of using different emotion-regulation strategies when modulating discrete emotions (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Dennis, Smith-Simon and Cohen2009). For example, Berlin and Cassidy (Reference Berlin and Cassidy2003) found that preschool-aged children whose mothers were more controlling and less receptive to their emotional expressions were ultimately less likely to display emotions or share their feelings with their mother; this may further indicate that even before middle childhood, children may modulate or alter their emotional reactions based on information from their surrounding environment, including prior experiences they may have had with caregivers that impacted their sense of safety, security, and freedom to express their emotions (e.g., Davies et al., Reference Davies, Winter and Cicchetti2006). When collectively considered, these factors illustrate that infants and toddlers are capable of concealing emotions, dampening their emotional reactivity, and initiating less cognitively demanding behavioral regulation strategies, like emotional suppression (or masking) compared to older children who are better able to leverage more sophisticated and cognitively involved regulatory strategies.

Family environments and interactions with caregivers play major roles in the development of emotion regulation among young children (Eisenberg & Fabes, Reference Eisenberg, Fabes and Clark1992b; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007). Parenting behaviors, family dynamics, and overall family emotional climate have been shown to impact children’s emotional lives and experiences, and consequently, the way they process, modulate, and express emotions (e.g., (Bariola et al., Reference Bariola, Gullone and Hughes2011; Criss et al., Reference Criss, Morris, Ponce‐Garcia, Cui and Silk2016; Lambie & Lindberg, Reference Lambie and Lindberg2016; Lavi et al., Reference Lavi, Katz, Ozer and Gross2019; Little & Carter, Reference Little and Carter2005; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Criss, Silk and Houltberg2017; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Morris, Heller, Scheeringa, Boris and Smyke2009). Psychopathology within the family, particularly among primary caregivers can directly and indirectly undermine children’s emotion regulation development and emotional well-being in complex and unique ways (Enlow et al., Reference Enlow, Kitts, Blood, Bizarro, Hofmeister and Wright2011; Han et al., Reference Han, Lei, Qian, Li, Wang and Zhang2016; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007; Seddon et al., Reference Seddon, Abdel-Baki, Feige and Thomassin2020; Shadur & Hussong, Reference Shadur and Hussong2020; Shaffer et al., Reference Shaffer, Suveg, Thomassin and Bradbury2012; Suveg et al., Reference Suveg, Shaffer, Morelen and Thomassin2011; van der Pol et al., Reference van der Pol, Groeneveld, Endendijk, van Berkel, Hallers-Haalboom, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Mesman2016). Interestingly, less research has examined distinct elements of emotion regulation – such as emotional reactivity – or the processes through which they manifest among children of mothers with alcohol dependence symptoms. Thus, it remains unclear how parents’ substance use problems may impact emotional reactivity in children from these family environments.

The present study was designed to clarify lingering unknowns about links between maternal alcohol dependence symptoms, parenting, and children’s emotional reactivity. Given known associations between maternal substance use and insensitive caregiving during emotional parent-child situations (e.g., Jacques et al., Reference Jacques, Sturge-Apple, Davies and Cicchetti2021), and given the salience of maternal responsivity to children’s long-term emotional development and well-being (e.g., Leerkes et al, Reference Leerkes, Nayena Blankson and O’Brien2009), maternal insensitivity to children’s emotional distress (expressions of vulnerable emotions like sadness or fear) was examined as a mediator of the relationship between maternal alcohol dependence symptoms and child emotional reactivity. Additionally, several theoretical and conceptual frameworks are discussed in tandem to better unpack how parent alcohol dependence symptoms and family risk factors may influence the developmental processes of emotional reactivity within young children.

Theoretical approaches to studying child emotional reactivity

Several prominent theories on emotion regulation exist and have been used within and across disciplines to bolster our understanding of emotional development and well-being during early childhood. Within the child development field, the main theories are based on familial and contextual influences on the development of emotion regulation and emotional reactivity (e.g., Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007; Thompson & Meyer, Reference Thompson, Meyer and Gross2007). According to emotion socialization perspectives and similar conceptual frameworks (e.g., Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Maccoby, Reference Maccoby, Parke, Ornstein, Rieser and Zahn-Waxler1994; Paley & Hajal, Reference Paley and Hajal2022), children’s regulatory skills are largely molded by parents’ behaviors, specifically through their emotional expressions, responses to children’s emotions, and how they discuss and describe emotions to children (Chaplin et al., Reference Chaplin, Cole and Zahn-Waxler2005; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Raikes, Virmani, Waters and Thompson2014). Specifically, parents’ emotional displays model different ways to express emotions, such as yelling when angry or crying when sad, and they also convey the types of emotional expressions that might be appropriate to display in that situation or setting (Boyum & Parke, Reference Boyum and Parke1995; Cole et al., Reference Cole, Dennis, Smith-Simon and Cohen2009; Denham et al., Reference Denham, Mitchell-Copeland, Strandberg, Auerbach and Blair1997; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998). These parental behaviors, also known as socialization behaviors, help teach children about how emotions function and their role and importance via a broad array of social interactions (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Dennis, Smith-Simon and Cohen2009). Aside from modeling, children may also internalize messages about emotions and their appropriateness from parents’ responses to their emotions (Banerjee, Reference Banerjee1997; Berlin & Cassidy, Reference Berlin and Cassidy2003). Maladaptive or insensitive caregiving behaviors, such as ignoring a child’s emotional needs or punishing a child for expressing emotions may instill harmful messages about emotions and undermine children’s comfort with their emotions and how they express them (Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Halberstadt, Castro, MacCormack and Garrett-Peters2016). Similarly, children whose parents seem less emotionally available or present to sensitively respond to children’s emotions may, over time, demonstrate decreased emotional competence and well-being (Diener et al., Reference Diener, Mangelsdorf, McHale and Frosch2002; Volling et al., Reference Volling, McElwain, Notaro and Herrera2002). This may negatively alter the trajectory of children’s development of emotion regulation and increase their risk of psychological, behavioral, and social problems (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad and Eggum2010).

Importantly, parents with mental health or substance use problems may engage in maladaptive socialization behaviors that ultimately harm children’s emotional well-being and may undermine children’s emotional development (Jacques et al., Reference Jacques, Sturge-Apple, Davies and Cicchetti2021; Seddon et al., Reference Seddon, Abdel-Baki, Feige and Thomassin2020). Substance use interferes with emotional functioning, which puts parents with substance use disorders at high risk to experience significant struggles navigating emotions and contexts in which emotions are elicited, such as times when children are highly emotional and seeking emotional support from caregivers (Frigerio et al., Reference Frigerio, Porreca, Simonelli and Nazzari2019; Goldman Fraser et al., Reference Goldman Fraser, Harris-Britt, Leone Thakkallapalli, Kurtz-Costes and Martin2010; Porreca et al., Reference Porreca, Biringen, Parolin, Saunders, Ballarotto and Simonelli2018; Rossen et al., Reference Rossen, Mattick, Wilson, Clare, Burns, Allsop, Elliott, Jacobs, Olsson and Hutchinson2019). Parents with substance use disorders have been shown to be dismissive, neglectful or disengaged, and harsh during emotional parent-child interactions, such as when responding to children’s emotional needs (e.g., Breaux et al., Reference Breaux, Harvey and Lugo-Candelas2016; Jacques et al., Reference Jacques, Sturge-Apple, Davies and Cicchetti2021; Shadur & Hussong, Reference Shadur and Hussong2020). Others have found that these parents may struggle to accurately identify and/or fully process their own and others’ emotions due to substance-related neurological deficits (e.g., Kim et al., Reference Kim, Iyengar, Mayes, Potenza, Rutherford and Strathearn2017; Rutherford & Mayes, Reference Rutherford and Mayes2017). Collectively considered, these findings imply that parents with symptoms of substance use disorders may struggle to help children’s optimal development of emotion regulation which may negatively affect how children experience and express emotions and thereby increase their risk for future psychosocial problems.

Parental influence on the development of emotional reactivity during early childhood

Parental insensitivity to children’s emotional experiences may be uniquely detrimental to children’s emotional development and well-being, particularly in early childhood (Fabes et al., Reference Fabes, Leonard, Kupanoff and Martin2001; Leerkes et al., Reference Leerkes, Nayena Blankson and O’Brien2009, Reference Leerkes, Weaver and O’Brien2012; Lyons-Ruth et al., Reference Lyons-Ruth, Bureau, Easterbrooks, Obsuth, Hennighausen and Vulliez-Coady2016; Roberts & Strayer, Reference Roberts and Strayer1987). Insensitivity is often all-encompassing construct that includes caregiving behaviors that are inappropriately attuned to their child’s needs, interests, or capabilities (Ainsworth & Wittig, Reference Ainsworth, Wittig and Foss1969; Bretherton, Reference Bretherton2016). Insensitivity to children’s expressions of vulnerable emotions, such as sadness, fear, worry, and others that indicate emotional vulnerability may be particularly hurtful and could erode children's trust, confidence, or emotional security in their caregivers or family environments (Berlin & Cassidy, Reference Berlin and Cassidy2003; Leerkes et al., Reference Leerkes, Nayena Blankson and O’Brien2009). Children may internalize insensitive reactions, such as parental emotional disengagement to mean that their emotions are invalid, unwanted, undeserving of attention or support, which essentially fractures children’s relationship with their emotions and their view of themselves as emotional beings (Lambie & Lindberg, Reference Lambie and Lindberg2016). These reactions may negatively influence children’s emotional development and well-being (Krause et al., Reference Krause, Mendelson and Lynch2003; Shipman et al., Reference Shipman, Schneider, Fitzgerald, Sims, Swisher and Edwards2007). Studies have shown that mothers with mental health challenges may be less responsive, warm, or sensitively attuned to young children’s emotional needs which may subsequently harm their psychosocial well-being (e.g., Dix & Yan, Reference Dix and Yan2014; Garai et al., Reference Garai, Forehand, Colletti, Reeslund, Potts and Compas2009; Jacques et al., Reference Jacques, Sturge-Apple, Davies and Cicchetti2021; Maughan et al., Reference Maughan, Cicchetti, Toth and Rogosch2007; Norcross et al., Reference Norcross, Leerkes and Zhou2017; Velleman & Templeton, Reference Velleman and Templeton2007). Thus, given the high risk for parents with psychological and substance use disorders (including alcohol use disorders) to engage in insensitive caregiving (Jacques et al., Reference Jacques, Sturge-Apple, Davies and Cicchetti2021; Mayes & Truman, Reference Mayes and Truman2002; Velleman & Templeton, Reference Velleman and Templeton2007) and given the negative developmental outcomes associated with this type of caregiving (Lyons-Ruth et al., Reference Lyons-Ruth, Bureau, Easterbrooks, Obsuth, Hennighausen and Vulliez-Coady2016; McElwain & Booth-LaForce, Reference McElwain and Booth-LaForce2006), it remains important to further examine these processes among young children of mothers with histories of alcohol and substance use problems and substance use disorders.

Morris et al. (Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007) and others expound on familial and contextual-based theories of emotion regulation development by emphasizing the broader role family context plays on children’s emotion regulation development. Recent work has recognized the need to better integrate and discuss context since different families have different expectations and working models/standards of emotional expressions, and the benefits and consequences of expressing emotions also vary within and across families (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Sheppes and Gross2015; Aldao, Reference Aldao2013; English et al., Reference English, Lee, John and Gross2017). For example, children may learn that expressing sadness around emotionally insensitive parents may yield backlash or punishment while expressing those emotions around siblings elicits emotional support and comfort (e.g., Grolnick et al., Reference Grolnick, Bridges and Connell1996; Roque et al., Reference Roque, Veríssimo, Fernandes and Rebelo2013). Other work has examined family context as an ecosystem in which emotion regulation develops by focusing on types of family risk that may threaten children’s emotion regulation and emotional expression (e.g., Carreras et al., Reference Carreras, Carter, Heberle, Forbes and Gray2019; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Winter and Cicchetti2006; Grolnick et al., Reference Grolnick, Bridges and Connell1996; Kiel & Kalomiris, Reference Kiel and Kalomiris2015; Lavi et al., Reference Lavi, Katz, Ozer and Gross2019; Shaffer et al., Reference Shaffer, Suveg, Thomassin and Bradbury2012; Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Bitran, Miller, Schaefer, Sheridan and McLaughlin2019). These findings provide clear evidence that maladaptive family environments, such as those featuring child maltreatment, family violence, and parent psychopathology pose significant risks to children’s emotional functioning and have far-reaching implications for children’s risk for psychopathology (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Puzia, Mead, Crowell, McEachern and Beauchaine2017; Lavi et al., Reference Lavi, Katz, Ozer and Gross2019; McLaughlin & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin and Lambert2017; Shields & Cicchetti, Reference Shields and Cicchetti1998; Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Bitran, Miller, Schaefer, Sheridan and McLaughlin2019). Despite these considerations, less attention has been focused on developmental processes of and outcomes associated with distinct aspects of emotion regulation among children of parents with substance use disorders, particularly emotional reactivity.

Theories and frameworks such as emotion socialization are especially important and beneficial when considered within a developmental perspective (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998). Parents play crucial roles in young children’s emotional development largely because young children rely on them for help regulating their emotions since they often lack the emotion regulation skills needed to do so independently (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Kopp, Reference Kopp1989). However, as they age, young children develop more sophisticated and nuanced understanding of emotions and emotional expressions, largely through parental emotion socialization but also through lived experiences (Graziano et al., Reference Graziano, Keane and Calkins2010; Izard & Malatesta, Reference Izard, Malatesta and Osofsky1987). In this regard, children may be able to discern the benefits and consequences of expressing certain discrete emotions (e.g., anger, sadness) around specific people or in specific contexts, and through the development of effortful control and other regulatory skills, may better modulate their emotions reactions to successfully navigate their environment (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Ashana Ramsook and Ram2019; Graziano et al., Reference Graziano, Keane and Calkins2010; Zimmermann & Stansbury, Reference Zimmermann and Stansbury2003). For example, children may reduce or entirely conceal their expressions of sadness if such expressions are consistently ignored or met with parental harshness (e.g., Berlin & Cassidy, Reference Berlin and Cassidy2003). Children may also mask such sadness with disingenuous expressions of positive, “desirable,” or neutral emotions, or those that are more favorably or at least not negatively received by caregivers or others (Malatesta & Haviland, Reference Malatesta and Haviland1982; Zeman & Garber, Reference Zeman and Garber1996).

Theoretical process models of emotion regulation and reactivity

The idea that individuals can alter their emotional displays is the basis of Gross’ highly influential process model of the development of emotion regulation (Gross & Thompson, Reference Gross, Thompson and Gross2007). According to this model, expressive suppression in one way we can modulate our emotions and it involves the behavioral inhibition of emotional expression, or the process of hiding your emotions or altering how you express them (Shapero et al., Reference Shapero, Abramson and Alloy2016). It is important to note that suppression-style strategies do not imply a lack or absence of emotions, only the restriction or concealment of emotional display (e.g., Peters et al., Reference Peters, Overall and Jamieson2014). Many studies have shown that individuals who hide their emotions are just as likely to experience intense and/or frequent emotions as those who do not suppress (Niedenthal & Ric, Reference Niedenthal and Ric2017), however they refrain from displaying or expressing these emotions. Suppression is a response or reaction-focused strategy wherein the individual is trying to manage an emotion they are already feeling or starting to feel (Gross & Thompson, Reference Gross, Thompson and Gross2007). The emotion has already been elicited and experienced but the individual is grappling with whether or how to express their emotions, and this may depend on context-specific and individual characteristics, such as their location, the people around them, and the benefits and consequences of expressing their emotions in a certain way or with a certain intensity (Cutuli, Reference Cutuli2014; Peters et al., Reference Peters, Overall and Jamieson2014; Rogier et al., Reference Rogier, Garofalo and Velotti2019; Sun & Lau, Reference Sun and Lau2018; Troy et al., Reference Troy, Shallcross and Mauss2013). For example, expressing high levels of anger at school may result in more permanent negative consequences such as suspension or expulsion whereas there might be fewer consequences associated with expressing these same emotions at home (Grolnick et al., Reference Grolnick, Bridges and Connell1996).

Hiding or suppressing emotions is typically associated with longstanding negative consequences in both child and older populations (Butler et al., Reference Butler, Egloff, Wlhelm, Smith, Erickson and Gross2003). Suppressive strategies have consistently been linked with emotion dysregulation and subsequent emotional and psychological disorders such as depression (Dryman & Heimberg, Reference Dryman and Heimberg2018), anxiety and stress-related symptoms (Hosogoshi et al., Reference Hosogoshi, Takebayashi, Ito, Fujisato, Kato, Nakajima, Oe, Miyamae, Kanie and Horikoshi2020; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Zoellner and Mollenholt2008; Seligowski et al., Reference Seligowski, Lee, Bardeen and Orcutt2015), and suicide and non-suicidal self-injury (Forkmann et al., Reference Forkmann, Scherer, Böcker, Pawelzik, Gauggel and Glaesmer2014; Turner et al., Reference Turner, Chapman and Layden2012). Hiding or masking emotions has also been linked with impairments in interpersonal functioning and may interfere with an individual’s ability to forge or strengthen emotional or social bonds with others, partially because a core component of these relationships involve empathy, trust, emotional reciprocity, and feelings of connectedness (Howard Sharp et al., Reference Howard Sharp, Cohen, Kitzmann and Parra2016; Jacob et al., Reference Jacob, Suveg and Whitehead2014). Thus, hiding emotions may not only increase children’s risk for mental health challenges but they may also undermine children’s ability to build social network that could provide emotional support especially during times of psychological distress (Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Gill, Johnson and Smith1999). During childhood, this may increase feelings of loneliness, isolation, diminished sense of belonging or worthiness, and/or sense of security or safety in interpersonal relationships (Howard Sharp et al., Reference Howard Sharp, Cohen, Kitzmann and Parra2016; Qualter & Munn, Reference Qualter and Munn2002).

Aside from understanding the outcomes associated with different patterns of emotional reactivity and emotion regulation, it is also important to identify how they develop and how they function in regards to children’s well-being. Studies are increasingly adopting more nuanced approaches that stem from or integrate developmental psychopathology frameworks to more robustly pinpoint relations between emotional reactivity and child functioning across different domains (e.g., Martin & Ochsner, Reference Martin and Ochsner2016; Tan et al., Reference Tan, Oppenheimer, Ladouceur, Butterfield and Silk2020; Ursache et al., Reference Ursache, Blair, Stifter and Voegtline2013). Specifically, it is ostensibly accepted that hiding emotions (or reducing your level of reactivity) can be both adaptive (or beneficial in some ways) and maladaptive or consequential and harmful in other ways (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Jazaieri, Goldin and Gross2014; Cole et al., Reference Cole, Ashana Ramsook and Ram2019; Juang et al., Reference Juang, Moffitt, Kim, Lee, Soto, Hurley and Whitbourne2016; Soto et al., Reference Soto, Perez, Kim, Lee and Minnick2011; Su et al., Reference Su, Lee and Oishi2013). Children from potentially traumatic family environments may use or over-rely on suppressive-style strategies to help them “fly under the radar” in their family environments, circumvent harmful or maladaptive interactions with caregivers (e.g., refraining from crying which may trigger a parent’s aggression), and otherwise reduce the potential for negative interaction that might cause psychological harm or distress (Punamäki et al., Reference Punamäki, Flykt, Belt and Lindblom2021). Growing up in emotionally unstable or aversive family contexts, where the child’s emotional expressions may do more harm than good, might promote emotional suppression or reduced affect displays, thereby turning a traditionally maladaptive strategy into one that is contextually adaptive or beneficial (Troy et al., Reference Troy, Willroth, Shallcross, Giuliani, Gross and Mauss2023). Indeed, children of parents with substance use problems have reported hiding emotions in order to “get by” and survive in their families (e.g., Kroll, Reference Kroll2004; Meulewaeter et al., Reference Meulewaeter, De Pauw and Vanderplasschen2019). However, despite these potential benefits, suppression may be fundamentally counterproductive, since it can prolong or intensify internal emotions (e.g., keeping emotions bottled up without diffusing or addressing them) and individuals may look to other more maladaptive outlets to express their emotions, such as via heightened aggression or substance use (Rogier et al., Reference Rogier, Garofalo and Velotti2019; Stellern et al., Reference Stellern, Xiao, Grennell, Sanches, Gowin and Sloan2022; Velotti et al., Reference Velotti, Garofalo, Bottazzi and Caretti2017). Additionally, bottling up emotions may also force children to overly rely on themselves for emotion regulation and emotional support, which may overwhelm or erode already vulnerable and developing regulatory systems (Suveg & Zeman, Reference Suveg and Zeman2004). From a developmental perspective, it may also rob children of opportunities to develop or strengthen healthy ways to process and express their emotions, which may influence children’s emotional understanding and broader emotional well-being (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Dennis, Smith-Simon and Cohen2009).

Suppressing emotions as an emotion regulation strategy is an understudied topic among child development fields (Gross & Cassidy, Reference Gross and Cassidy2019); most work is done with adolescents and adults. Thus, there may be a unique benefit in better understanding whether, how, and why pre-adolescent children (particularly young children) may engage in emotional masking, and the proximal and distal developmental outcomes that may be associated with masking/suppression (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Liu, Gao, Han, Jiang and Liu2023; Gross & Cassidy, Reference Gross and Cassidy2019; Gullone et al., Reference Gullone, Hughes, King and Tonge2010; McClain et al., Reference McClain, Younginer and Elledge2020; Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Bitran, Miller, Schaefer, Sheridan and McLaughlin2019). Despite this lack of research, some suggest that young children may be more likely to suppress and use reactionary emotional strategies than antecedent-focused strategies like reappraisal (Gullone et al., Reference Gullone, Hughes, King and Tonge2010; Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner, Reference Zimmer-Gembeck and Skinner2016). This notion largely stems from the fact that appraisal is more cognitively demanding and requires more sophisticated, robust, and dynamic neurocognitive skills that young children are yet to possess (e.g., DeCicco et al., Reference DeCicco, Solomon and Dennis2012). Yet, while young children may be unable to effectively engage in cognitive reframing and more advanced regulatory strategies, developments in executive functioning and effortful and inhibitory control across early childhood make it possible for children to effectively engage in emotional suppression and masking (Carlson & Wang, Reference Carlson and Wang2007; Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Murray and Harlan2000; Moriguchi & Hiraki, Reference Moriguchi and Hiraki2013; Williams, Reference Williams2018). The few studies that focus on emotional reactivity and regulatory strategies among children of parents with mental health conditions show that expressive suppression is linked with increased risk of psychopathology and decreased emotional functioning across developmental periods (Miu et al., Reference Miu, Szentágotai-Tătar, Balazsi, Nechita, Bunea and Pollak2022). However, while extant research on childhood adversity and trauma has linked higher levels of emotional reactivity to symptoms of psychopathology (e.g. Dvir et al., Reference Dvir, Ford, Hill and Frazier2014; Shapero et al., Reference Shapero, Farabaugh, Terechina, DeCross, Cheung, Fava and Holt2019; Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Bitran, Miller, Schaefer, Sheridan and McLaughlin2019), future studies may benefit from integrating more process models of emotion regulation with process models of parenting and family functioning and developmental psychopathology to better identify whether and how other forms or patterns of emotion regulation develop and may influence children’s emotional well-being (Gross & Cassidy, Reference Gross and Cassidy2019).

The present study

Parents with substance use disorders may struggle with emotion regulation or emotional availability, thereby interfering with their ability to appropriately respond to and connect with their children’s emotions (Burns et al., Reference Burns, Chethik, Burns and Clark1991; Molitor et al., Reference Molitor, Mayes and Ward2003; Shadur & Hussong, Reference Shadur and Hussong2020). Additionally, children of parents with alcohol and substance use problems often exhibit difficulties regulating emotions, which could undermine their emotional development and increase their risk for psychopathology (Molitor et al., Reference Molitor, Mayes and Ward2003; Punamäki et al., Reference Punamäki, Flykt, Belt and Lindblom2021; Shadur & Hussong, Reference Shadur and Hussong2020). However, few studies focus on how specific elements of emotion regulation such as emotional reactivity may develop among children of mothers with symptoms of alcohol dependence, specifically the role that maladaptive parenting and family context may play on these developmental processes (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Martin and Dennis2004; Cole, Reference Cole2014; Cole et al., Reference Cole, Dennis, Smith-Simon and Cohen2009; Paley & Hajal, Reference Paley and Hajal2022). Moreover, since young children’s independent emotion regulation capacities are still developing, parents’ responses to children’s emotions are an important context through which children internalize emotion-regulation strategies (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Michel and Teti1994); however, the development of regulatory processes is also influenced by the contexts in which parents and children exist. Since some posit that more sophisticated regulatory abilities develop and thus are more salient beginning in middle childhood (Compas et al., Reference Compas, Jaser, Bettis, Watson, Gruhn, Dunbar and Thigpen2017; Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner, Reference Zimmer-Gembeck and Skinner2016), and since younger children are presumed more likely to engage in antecedent-focused, reactive regulatory strategies, it is important to examine whether and how higher-risk developmental contexts (e.g., maternal alcohol dependence) may impact distinct aspects of children’s emotion regulation. To address these gaps, the present sought to examine maternal insensitivity to children’s distress as a mediator in the relationship between maternal alcohol dependence and children’s emotional reactivity. Moreover, recent scholars have emphasized the need for more contextually informed interpretations of associations between maternal psychopathology, maladaptive caregiving, affective developmental settings, and children’s emotional reactivity and regulatory processes (e.g., Cole, Reference Cole2014; Gross & Cassidy, Reference Gross and Cassidy2019; Hajal & Paley, Reference Hajal and Paley2020; Panayiotou et al., Reference Panayiotou, Panteli and Vlemincx2021; Sanders et al., Reference Sanders, Zeman, Poon and Miller2015). Therefore, interpretation of findings will be conducted in line with parenting process models (e.g., Abidin, Reference Abidin1992;) and developmental psychopathology frameworks (e.g., Cicchetti & Aber, Reference Cicchetti and Aber1998; Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996; Frankenhuis & Del Giudice, Reference Frankenhuis and Del Giudice2012) that highlight adaptive and maladaptive functions underlying parents’ and children’s behaviors.

Our study had several aims and hypotheses. One major aim was to expand on recent research showing that maternal psychopathology and substance use (including alcohol use) problems may negatively impact caregiving responses to children’s emotional cues (e.g., Jacques et al., Reference Jacques, Sturge-Apple, Davies and Cicchetti2021; Karl, Reference Karl1995; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Teti and Cole2012, Reference Kim, Iyengar, Mayes, Potenza, Rutherford and Strathearn2017; Lowell et al., Reference Lowell, Maupin, Landi, Potenza, Mayes and Rutherford2020; Romanowicz et al., Reference Romanowicz, Vande Voort, Shekunov, Oesterle, Thusius, Rummans, Croarkin, Karpyak, Lynch and Schak2019). Our second aim was to evaluate how mothers’ responses to children’s emotional distress may impact children’s emotional development and reactivity over time (e.g., Jacques et al., Reference Jacques, Sturge-Apple, Davies and Cicchetti2021). We also had two main hypotheses. First, since maternal substance use problems have been previously linked with decreased sensitivity to children’s emotional cues (e.g., Jacques et al., Reference Jacques, Sturge-Apple, Davies and Cicchetti2021; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Iyengar, Mayes, Potenza, Rutherford and Strathearn2017; Lowell et al., Reference Lowell, Maupin, Landi, Potenza, Mayes and Rutherford2020; Romanowicz et al., Reference Romanowicz, Vande Voort, Shekunov, Oesterle, Thusius, Rummans, Croarkin, Karpyak, Lynch and Schak2019); we predicted a positive association between maternal alcohol dependence symptoms and mothers’ insensitive responses to children’s emotional distress such that greater endorsement of alcohol dependence symptoms at Wave 1 (M child age = 2 years old) would predict increases in maternal insensitivity to children’s distress over time (from Wave 1 to Wave 2, when children were approximately 3 years old). Additionally, although the literature has focused on identifying how and why risk factors within the parent and family environment (e.g., parent substance use problems) may contribute to children’s emotional over-reactivity (e.g., highly emotionally reactive children who may struggle to effectively regulate their emotions or adapt to change in their environments) (e.g., Davis et al., Reference Davis, Alto, Oshri, Rogosch, Cicchetti and Toth2020; Fabes et al., Reference Fabes, Martin, Hanish, Anders and Madden-Derdich2003), we hypothesized that children may suppress their emotions (or become less emotionally reactive) over time if they encounter caregiving responses that are less sensitive to their emotional cues. We based this hypothesis on prior work showing that parental insensitivity or caregiving responses that are not appropriately attuned or calibrated to children’s emotional needs or expressions (especially negative emotions like sadness) may be aversive, emotionally harmful contexts for children, especially younger children who still developing and honing their emotion-regulation skills and experiences as emotional beings in a broader familial context (Eisenberg & Fabes, Reference Eisenberg and Fabes1994; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Fabes and Murphy1996; Field et al., Reference Field, Vega-Lahr, Scafidi and Goldstein1986; Leerkes et al., Reference Leerkes, Nayena Blankson and O’Brien2009). Consequently, over time, some children may perceive and internalize emotionally dismissive, inattentive, and invalidating family environments as unsafe spaces to openly express or experience their emotions, and therefore, may show less emotion in those environments (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Hu, Lu, Ma and Zheng2022; Eisenberg & Fabes, Reference Eisenberg and Fabes1994; Gross, Reference Gross1999; Lindblom et al., Reference Lindblom, Punamäki, Flykt, Vänskä, Nummi, Sinkkonen, Tiitinen and Tulppala2016; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Criss, Silk and Houltberg2017). In other words, children may ultimately conceal their emotions as an adaptive, protective strategy to cope with increasingly maladaptive and emotionally challenging contexts. Thus, since we expected that maternal alcohol dependence symptoms would predict increases insensitivity to children’s emotional distress, we also predicted that this association would subsequently predict decreases in children’s emotional reactivity from Wave 2 (M child age = 3.08 years (37.05 months); SD = 1.917 months) to Wave 3 (M child age = 4.04 years (48.57 months); SD = 2.112 months). When considered together, we believed both study aims would help us better identify whether and how maternal alcohol dependence symptoms influence caregiving responses to children’s emotional distress and how said responses may impact children’s later emotional functioning.

Methods

Participants

201 mother-child dyads were enrolled at baseline. Child participants (44% female) were approximately 26 months old (M = 2.14 years old, SD = 1.69) at Wave 1, 37 months old (M = 3.08 years old) at Wave 2 (one year after data collection at Wave 1), and 48 months old (M = 4.04 years old) at Wave 3, one year after Wave 2. Mothers’ average age at Wave 1 was 26.24 years old (SD = 5.784 years). Most families (more than 95%) were living below the poverty line and were receiving public assistance. A majority (67%) self-identified as Black or African American (56%) or Hispanic or Latino/a/e (11%) with fewer participants self-identifying as white or European American (23%), multiracial or multiethnic (7%), and “other” (3%).

A multiphase recruitment process was used to help us better identify and subsequently enroll dyads who might be at higher risk to experience various types of adversity or risk. First, our research team recruited mother-child dyads from local agencies serving families in need and families who may experiencing various types of acute and chronic adversity, such as poverty and family conflict and domestic violence. One such organization was a consortium of 17 local agencies dedicated to families impacted by child maltreatment or family adversity, or those living in lower income, under-resourced environments. The next step involved recruitment from rosters of families receiving Temporary Assistance for Needy Families obtained from the local county office of the Department of Human and Health Services. Enrollment was contingent upon (a) the adult woman participant reported that they were the target child’s biological mother and primary caregiver, (b) mothers reporting that the target child had not been diagnosed with significant developmental or cognitive disabilities that would hinder study participation, and (c) the target child was 24-months old (+/− 7 months) at the time of data collection at Wave 1. Families not meeting these criteria, and families wherein the target child had been adopted, placed in foster care, or had known cognitive, developmental, or motor impairments were deemed ineligible and consequently excluded from participation. To maximize the feasibility of our recruitment strategy, mothers with children as young as 18 months were enrolled for subsequent visits when the child turned 24-months old.

Procedure

Data were collected in three waves, each spaced one year apart. Collection began when the focal child was approximately two-years-old (M W1childage = 2.14 years, SD = 1.69). For each wave of the of data collection, mother-child dyads visited a university laboratory, at which time mother and child participants completed standardized questionnaires and assessments, and dyadic interactions were observed, with select dyadic tasks being recorded for later coding by two trained experimenters. Children’s emotional distress and mothers’ responses to children’s distress were assessed via the Strange Situation (Ainsworth & Wittig, Reference Ainsworth, Wittig and Foss1969). Both reunions (the point in the procedure when the mother returns to the child) were coded for the nature, intensity, and frequency of children’s expressions of emotional distress as well as the nature of mothers’ responses to said distress. Although historically used to assess children’s attachment styles, and while acknowledging heterogeneity in the levels of distress that children display throughout the Strange Situation (e.g., Kochanska & Coy, Reference Kochanska and Coy2002; Shiller et al., Reference Shiller, Izard and Hembree1986), the reunions were selected for the child distress paradigm since the task often successfully elicits emotional distress among child participants and mothers are primed to respond to their child’s distress upon returning to the room (Ainsworth et al., Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall2015; Ainsworth & Wittig, Reference Ainsworth, Wittig and Foss1969). Thus, it presents an optimal paradigm through which child distress and parental responses to distress can be examined. Moreover, recent studies have used the Strange Situation in novel ways to assess non-attachment constructs and associations, including but not limited to assessments of mothers’ responses to children’s distress (e.g., Sturge-Apple et al., Reference Sturge-Apple, Skibo, Rogosch, Ignjatovic and Heinzelman2011), children’s reactivity during stressful familial contexts (e.g., Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Allen and Laurent2018), long-term developmental and psychological outcomes associated with maternal insensitivity or lack of responsiveness to children’s distress (e.g., Behrens et al., Reference Behrens, Parker and Haltigan2011; Köhler-Dauner et al., Reference Köhler-Dauner, Roder, Krause, Buchheim, Gündel, Fegert, Ziegenhain and Waller2019), and as indicators of risky, dysregulated, and maladaptive parent-child dynamics (e.g., Kochanska & Coy, Reference Kochanska and Coy2002; Lyons-Ruth et al., Reference Lyons-Ruth, Connell, Zoll and Stahl1987), some of which have been linked to increased risk for child psychopathology (e.g., Guo et al., Reference Guo, Spieker and Borelli2021; Hollenstein et al., Reference Hollenstein, Granic, Stoolmiller and Snyder2004). Operationalizations of both child emotional distress and mothers’ responses to child distress are described below.

In addition to the two trained experimenters who coded the Strange Situation’s reunions, two different trained coders provided ratings of children’s emotional reactivity at Wave 2 (M Child Age = 3.08 years) and Wave 3 (M Child Age = 4.04 years) using the Caregiver- Teacher Report Form (C-TRF for Ages 1 ½-5)’s Emotional Reactivity subscale. These two experimenters were either (a) postbaccalaureate research assistants with educational and professional backgrounds in either child development, child mental health, family processes, and/or family intervention implementation or (b) doctoral students in developmental psychology with research training in child psychopathology. All experimenters who were rating children’s emotional reactivity received structured and monitored training on how to identify, interpret, and code commonly known symptoms or indicators of childhood internalizing problems. As part of their training, all research staff working with child participants watched videos that depicted various manifestations of child internalizing symptomatology (e.g., irritability, withdrawn behaviors) and were instructed to use this training when evaluating and rating children’s behaviors and emotional expressions. Following training, each experimenter kept detailed written notes and records of multiple domains of children’s functioning, including the nature and extent of their emotional reactions and the context in which children expressed various emotions (e.g., noting that the child would get upset if they were yelled at or noting that the child would become irritable if left alone with an unfamiliar adult). These records and the resultant ratings were based on cumulative observation and interaction with children at both Wave 2 (7.43 hours) and Wave 3 (6.08 hours), including observations during laboratory visits, transitions between study tasks, and transportation to and from various laboratory rooms. Both raters maintained such records at Wave 2 when children were approximately three years old, and Wave 3 when children were approximately four years old, and provided their rating on the child Emotional Reactivity subscale after each dyad completed their assessment battery for each respective wave of data collection.

All study procedures were standardized across all study participants, and the study protocol was reviewed and approved by the University’s Institutional Review Board.

Measures

Maternal alcohol dependence and maternal psychopathology

Mothers’ alcohol dependence symptoms and depressive, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress symptoms were assessed at Wave 1 using the revised fourth edition of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS-IV; Robins et al., Reference Robins, Cottler, Bucholz and Compton1995). Each of the aforementioned psychiatric conditions is represented by their own respective index which includes the diagnostic criteria featured in the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Participants respond “yes” or “no” when asked whether they have ever experienced the symptom in question, thus higher scores on respective indices reflect more severe psychiatric presentations. The DIS-IV is a full structured interview designed for use by non-clinicians and non-clinically trained interviewers and can be used to assess major psychiatric conditions (including major depression, generalized anxiety, post-traumatic stress disorder, and drug and alcohol dependence symptoms) within socioeconomically and racially and ethnically diverse community samples (Breslau et al., Reference Breslau, Kessler, Chilcoat, Schultz, Davis and Andreski1998; Buu et al., Reference Buu, Dipiazza, Wang, Puttler, Fitzgerald and Zucker2009; Hwa-Froelich et al., Reference Hwa-Froelich, Loveland Cook and Flick2008; Segal, Reference Segal2010; Tabet et al., Reference Tabet, Flick, Cook, Xian and Chang2016).

Symptoms of alcohol dependence were recorded via the seven-item Alcohol Dependence index which encompasses DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for alcohol dependence. The subscale’s items are: (1) “alcohol tolerance,” (2) “alcohol withdrawal,” (3) “alcohol is often taken in larger amounts or over longer period than intended,” (4) “persistent desire or unsuccessful efforts to cut down or control alcohol use,” (5) “great deal of time spent in activities to obtain, use, or recover from alcohol”; (6) “important activities are given up or reduced because of alcohol”; and (7) “alcohol use is continued despite persistent physical or psychological problem that is caused or exacerbated by alcohol.” The mean of the inter-item correlations (M = .292) suggested good reliability (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1995), and internal consistency analyses (seven items, α = .742, ω = .783) suggested good reliability in our sample (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1994; McNeish, Reference McNeish2018). While this is a lifetime assessment of mothers’ alcohol dependence symptoms, we could not isolate mothers’ use or dependence symptoms to examine symptoms that emerged or changed during pregnancy, or subsequently examine any developmental effects of prenatal substance use or dependence.

Symptoms of Major Depressive Disorder were captured using the Major Depression subscale which included DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for said disorder. Symptoms featured on this subscale are: (1) feeling sad, empty, or depressed; (2) lost interest; (3) appetite problems; (4) sleep problems; (5) feeling slow or restless; (6) feeling tired of lacking energy; (7) feeling worthless or guilty; (8) having trouble thinking; (9) having thoughts about death; and (10) feeling “uninterested in everything” even if they did not endorse (2) lost interest. The average of the inter-item correlations (.566) suggested good to excellent reliability (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1995). Reliability analyses revealed excellent internal consistency for this subscale (10 items, α = .942, ω = .954) (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1994; McNeish, Reference McNeish2018).

Lifetime symptoms of Generalized Anxiety Disorder were measured using the Anxiety Disorder Subscale. Symptoms included on this subscale were: (1) feeling restless, keyed up, or on edge; (2) feeling easily fatigued; (3) experiencing difficulty concentrating or your mind going blank; (4) experiencing irritability; (5) experiencing muscle tension; (6) experiencing difficulty controlling worry; and (7) experiencing sleep disturbances. The average of the inter-item correlations (.746) indicated excellent reliability (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1995). Further analyses revealed excellent internal consistency (seven items, α = .946, ω = .949) (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1994; McNeish, Reference McNeish2018).

Lastly, the PTSD subscale captured lifetime symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder. Symptoms assessed on this scale were: (1) difficulty falling or staying asleep; (2) irritability or outburst of anger; (3) difficulty concentrating; (4) hypervigilance; and (5) exaggerated startle response. The average of the inter-item correlations (.610) indicated good reliability (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1995). Internal consistency (five items, α = .887, ω = .889) revealed excellent reliability (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1994; McNeish, Reference McNeish2018).

Each psychopathology subscale – measuring depression, anxiety, and PTSD, respectively – were standardized and combined into a composite variable of “maternal psychopathology” which was used in all subsequent analyses.

Maternal insensitivity to children’s emotional distress

Mothers’ responses to children’s emotional distress were assessed using the Iowa Family Interaction Rating Systems’ Insensitive/Parent-Centered scale (IFIRS; Melby & Conger, Reference Melby, Conger, Kerig and Lindahl2001). This scale is conceptualized and has been used to capture broad and variable forms of parental insensitivity, specifically including any and all manifestations of caregiving that are parent-focused (meaning that they are more focused on or aligned with the parents’ needs, interests, goals, etc.) and inappropriately or maladaptively attuned to the child’s needs, interests, and/or capabilities (e.g., Sturge-Apple et al., Reference Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti and Manning2012). Maternal behaviors fitting this description may reflect a mothers’ lack of awareness of her child’s needs, interests, or abilities. Consequently, mothers’ behaviors may not be well-paced, well-timed, or otherwise calibrated to the child’s mood, needs, or desires. Also included are parents’ missed attempts to interact with or support children in both proactive (e.g., initiating contact) or reactive ways (e.g., a mother not picking a child up after a child reaches out for them, not responding to children’s questions, etc.). Mothers may also enforce rules, regulations, or restrictions upon the child without consideration for the child’s needs or autonomy. Importantly, insensitivity is recognized to possible manifest as both verbal (e.g., yelling or insulting a child) and/or nonverbal behavior (e.g., ignoring a child; walking away from children, etc.).

Two trained independent coders rated mothers’ insensitive responses on a 9-point Likert scale ranging from (1) “not at all characteristic,” meaning the mother displays no evidence of insensitive or parent-centered behavior, to (9) “mainly characteristic,” meaning the mother is consistently and typically insensitive and self-focused. Intraclass correlation coefficients produced in two-way mixed effects models ranged from .858 to .901 across 25% of the Strange Situation reunions from Waves 1 and 2, indicating excellent inter-rater agreement (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1994). Raters’ scores were averaged and combined for subsequent analyses.

Child distress

Two independent coders rated the intensity and frequency of distress children exhibited throughout both reunions of the Strange Situation that was administered at Wave 1. We operationalized child distress as behavior that communicates emotional vulnerability such as anxiety, fear, nervousness, and sadness. This includes physical expressions (e.g., slumping, fidgeting), verbalizations (e.g., crying, whining), and actions (e.g., hiding from caregivers) since young children often express distress via both verbalizations and nonverbal behaviors (Kopp, Reference Kopp1989). All ratings were made along a 9-point Likert scale ranging from (1) “not at all characteristic” to (9) “mainly characteristic.” Intraclass correlation coefficients produced from two-way mixed effects models ranged from .92 to .94 across the Strange Situation’s two reunions, indicating excellent inter-rater agreement (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1994). An average of both coders’ ratings from reunion 1 and reunion 2 of Wave 1’s Strange Situation was used in the final analyses.

Child emotional reactivity as a measure of expressive suppression

Children’s emotional reactivity was rated by two trained experimenters via the Emotionally Reactive Syndrome Scale from the Caregiver-Teacher Report Form for ages 1 ½ to 5 (C-TRF 1 ½-5; Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2000) at Waves 2 and 3. Behaviors on this subscale are meant to reflect the nature, intensity, and frequency of children’s emotional arousal. The scale’s items include: (1) being disturbed by change in routine; (2) nervous movements or twitching; (3) sudden changes in mood or feelings; (4) sulks a lot; (5) upset by new people or situations; (6) whining; and (7) worries. For each item, responses are recorded as (1) “not true (as far as you know)”; (2) “somewhat or sometimes true”; and (3) “very true or often true.” Prior work has used this scale as an indicator of children’s emotional functioning and as a predictor of children’s risk to experience later internalizing problems (e.g., Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Izard and Hyde2014). It has also been used and validated in studies featuring socioeconomically and racially and ethnically diverse samples and children of parents with psychopathologies (e.g., Cai et al., Reference Cai, Kaiser and Hancock2004; Gregl et al., Reference Gregl, Kirigin, Sućeska Ligutić and Bilać2014). Average inter-item correlations (across Waves 2 and 3 = .118) reflected good reliability. Two-way mixed effects models (ICCs across Waves 2 and 3 = .765) suggested very good to excellent inter-rater reliability among coders. Due to a lack of distinct observational assessments of early childhood expressive suppression (Gross & Cassidy, Reference Gross and Cassidy2019), children’s emotional reactivity served as a proxy measure of expressive suppression, with decreases across waves being interpreted as possible evidence for expressive suppression.

Child self-control

The Social Skills Rating System-Parent Form (SSRS-P; Gresham & Elliott, Reference Gresham and Elliott1990) is a robust parent-reported assessment of children’s social behavior across several domains of functioning. The 10-item Self-Control subscale of the Social Skills Scale was completed by the child’s mother at Wave 2 when the child was approximately three-years-old (MW1child age = 3.08 years) and was used to measure children’s ability to regulate and control their impulses, negative emotions, and behaviors in social settings. Exemplary items include “controls temper in conflict situations with you”; “ends disagreements with you calmly”; “controls temper when arguing with other children”; and “responds appropriately to teasing from friends or relatives of his or her own age”. For each item, mothers indicated how often their child displayed or engaged in the described behavior on a scale from (0) “never” to (2) “very often.” Test-retest reliability ranged from .65 to .93, internal consistency results (10 items, α = .831, ω = .830) suggested excellent reliability (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1994; McNeish, Reference McNeish2018), and the average inter-item correlation (.331) also indicated good reliability (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1995).

Parent and family sociodemographic variables

At Wave 1, mothers completed a demographics interview (Cicchetti & Carlson, Reference Cicchetti and Carlson1989) which included information regarding her and her child’s racial and ethnic identity, ages, and gender; total annual family income; and her highest level of education attained. This demographic measure was developed for and has been consistently used in research with lower income, higher-risk samples (e.g., Cicchetti et al., Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Toth2006; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Sturge-Apple, Cicchetti, Manning and Zale2009)

Data analysis plan

SPSS Version 29 (IBM Corp. Released, 2022) was used to obtain descriptive statistics, zero-order correlations between study variables, and to assess patterns of missing data. Little’s missing data test (1988) was selected to assess whether data were missing completely at random (MCAR), and to help us make an informed selection of the estimation method within subsequent structural equation modeling (SEM) analyses. AMOS Version 29 (IBM Corp. Released, 2022) was used for all SEM analyses. Latent difference score (LDS) analyses were modeled within SEM to parameterize change over time in mothers’ insensitivity to child distress and children’s emotional reactivity. See Figure 1 for a parameterization of the LDS model for change in maternal insensitivity to children’s distress from Wave 1 to Wave 2. The LDS parameterization for change in children’s emotional reactivity from Wave 2 to Wave 3 is presented in Figure 2. Several covariates were entered to better estimate pathways linking maternal alcohol dependence, maternal insensitivity to child distress, and children’s emotional reactivity. Specifically, family income and maternal age were included as covariates given their known influence on caregiving, mother-child interactions, and children’s developmental outcomes (Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Lee, Rosales-Rueda and Kalil2018; Highlander et al., Reference Highlander, Zachary, Jenkins, Loiselle, McCall, Youngstrom and Jones2021). Additionally, noting the literature’s lack of consensus regarding the role child gender plays in parents’ emotion socialization practices and early emotional development (see Chaplin & Aldao, Reference Chaplin and Aldao2013 for a deeper review), and the lack of rigorous examination of gender differences in young children’s use of suppressive emotion-regulation strategies (Gross & Cassidy, Reference Gross and Cassidy2019), child gender was entered as a covariate. Moreover, psychological distress and mental health conditions such as depression and post-traumatic stress are not only prevalent among Black and Latina populations (Alim et al., Reference Alim, Charney and Mellman2006; Himle et al., Reference Himle, Baser, Taylor, Campbell and Jackson2009; Williams, Reference Williams2018) but these disorders also often co-occur with substance dependence (Debell et al., Reference Debell, Fear, Head, Batt-Rawden, Greenberg, Wessely and Goodwin2014; Hasin et al., Reference Hasin, Sarvet, Meyers, Saha, Ruan, Stohl and Grant2018) and are known to directly and indirectly impact caregiving, family processes, and children’s psychological development (Leijdesdorff et al., Reference Leijdesdorff, van Doesum, Popma, Klaassen and van Amelsvoort2017; van der Pol et al., Reference van der Pol, Groeneveld, Endendijk, van Berkel, Hallers-Haalboom, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Mesman2016). Thus, we included these disorders, represented as a standardized composite termed “maternal psychopathology” as a covariate. This composite, created to preserve degrees of freedom and construct the most parsimonious model was built by standardizing each disorder’s respective DIS-IV subscale and summing said standardized results.

Figure 1. Parameterization of latent difference score modeling for change in maternal insensitivity to children’s emotional distress from wave 1 to wave 2.

Figure 2. Parameterization of latent difference score modeling of change in children’s emotional reactivity from wave 2 to wave 3.

Furthermore, since we wanted to use mothers’ caregiving responses and children’s distress levels from both of the Strange Situation reunions, we averaged mothers’ insensitivity and children’s distress ratings from Reunion 1 and Reunion 2 at each wave and used this score in subsequent analyses. Lastly, many emphasize that emotional reactivity is bidirectionally associated with an individual’s broader emotion and self-regulation abilities (Derryberry & Rothbart, Reference Derryberry and Rothbart1997; Fox, Reference Fox1989; Gross, Reference Gross1998; Mirabile et al., Reference Mirabile, Scaramella, Sohr-Preston and Robison2009). Specifically, an individual’s emotional expressions are partially dictated by their ability to monitor, process, and modulate their internal emotional experiences (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad and Eggum2010; Shapero et al., Reference Shapero, Abramson and Alloy2016; Sheppes et al., Reference Sheppes, Suri and Gross2015). For example, children with poor or impaired emotion-regulation skills may exhibit intense and prolonged emotional reactions when emotionally aroused, while a child with better emotion-regulation skills may show fewer or less intense emotions (Eisenberg & Fabes, Reference Eisenberg and Fabes1992a). Thus, since emotional reactivity is often dynamically influenced by emotion regulation more broadly (e.g., Ursache et al., Reference Ursache, Blair, Stifter and Voegtline2013), children’s social self-control at Wave 2 was entered as a covariate predicting subsequent change in children’s emotional reactivity.

Results

Since non-significant results on Little’s (Reference Little1988) MCAR test indicate that data are missing completely at random, our findings from Little’s MCAR test revealed that the data were MCAR, χ 2 (64) = 57.289, p = 7.11. Given families’ high mobility and relocation rates over time, combined with the sample’s frequent experiences of familial and socioeconomic adversity, not all families were able to contribute data at every timepoint throughout the study; however, retention remained relatively high, with 86% families returning across three annual data collection waves. Families who participated at baseline but were unable to participate a year later (Wave 2) were allowed to contribute data at Wave 3. Further examination into patterns of missing data on all study variables (including covariates) did not reveal evidence of selective attrition or significant differences between mother-child dyads who dropped out and those who remained in the study.

A detailed breakdown of frequencies for each of the lifetime alcohol dependence symptoms as reported by mothers at baseline are shown in Table 1. 75 mothers (37% of the sample) in total endorsed at least one symptom or indicator of lifetime alcohol dependence; 30 mothers (15% of the sample) endorsed one symptom of alcohol dependence, 22 mothers (11% of the sample) endorsed two symptoms of alcohol dependence, and 23 (11.5% of the sample) endorsed three or more symptoms of alcohol dependence. Moreover, of the 75 mothers reporting symptoms of alcohol dependence, 19 (25% of this subset of mothers) indicated that they had experienced symptoms of alcohol dependence within 12 months of Wave 1. 35 (46.7% of mothers reporting symptoms of alcohol dependence) reported that they were experiencing symptoms of alcohol dependence at Wave 1. Most mothers (81%) reported that they experienced their first symptom of alcohol dependence in adolescence or early adulthood, with the youngest age at first symptom appearance being 12 and the oldest being 37. Eight mothers (4% of the total sample) reported ever receiving treatment for alcohol dependence. While these statistics may seem low, they are aligned with prior work investigating alcohol use disorders and alcohol dependence in community samples with racially, ethnically, and socioeconomically underrepresented populations (e.g., Mulia & Bensley, Reference Mulia and Bensley2020). Prior to proceeding to SEM to examine the study’s main research questions, Maximum Likelihood Robust estimation was chosen as the estimator given its ability to handle non-normally distributed and missing data (Allison, Reference Allison2003; Enders & Bandalos, Reference Enders and Bandalos2001; Li & Lomax, Reference Li and Lomax2017). Descriptive statistics and correlations among all study’s variables are displayed in Table 2.

Table 1. Frequency distribution of mothers’ responses on the DIS-IV alcohol dependence symptom index

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations for study’s primary variables

Note. W1 = Wave 1; W2 = Wave 2; W3 = Wave 3. Ch. = child; Mat. = maternal; Alc. Dep. = alcohol dependence symptoms; Psychop. = psychopathology symptoms. Avg. = average; Ch. Dist. = child distress; Insens. = maternal insensitivity; Self-Cont. = self-control; Emot. React. = emotional reactivity. * p < .05. ** p < .01.

Findings from the process-oriented structural equation model are depicted in Figure 3. An evaluation of overall model and practical fit indices indicated excellent model fit, χ 2(4) = 3.28, p = .51; CFI = 1.0; RSMEA = .000, according to cutoff criteria outlined by West et al. (Reference West, Taylor and Wu2012). For our primary pathways of interest, the model revealed that over and above maternal age, family income, mothers’ non-substance-related psychopathology, and children’s baseline emotional distress, maternal alcohol dependence symptoms at Wave 1 predicted increased insensitivity to children’s distress from Wave 1 to Wave 2, β = .16, SE = .09, p = .013. Moreover, this increased insensitivity to children’s distress from Wave 1 to Wave 2 predicted decreases in children’s emotional reactivity from Wave 2 to Wave 3, β = −.29, SE = .12, p = .009. To test whether this mediational path linking maternal alcohol dependence to children’s emotional reactivity through maternal insensitivity was statistically significant, we computed Monte Carlo confidence intervals using Selig and Preacher’s (Reference Selig and Preacher2008) method to test for significant mediation since bootstrapped confidence intervals cannot be computed in AMOS for samples with missing data. The 95% Monte Carlo confidence intervals (computed with 2,000 repetitions) did not contain zero [−.173, −0.001], thereby indicating that maternal insensitivity to child distress is a significant mediator of the relationship between maternal alcohol dependence symptoms and children’s emotional reactivity.

Figure 3. Structural equation model examining longitudinal associations between sociodemographic variables, maternal psychopathology symptoms, maternal alcohol dependence symptoms, change in maternal insensitivity to children’s distress, and change in children’s emotional reactivity. Dashed lines indicate non-significant pathways. W1 = wave 1; W2 = wave 2; W3 = wave 3. * p < .05.

Additionally, several covariates were significant or marginally significant predictors of maternal insensitivity to child distress or child emotional reactivity. First, child gender predicted mothers’ insensitivity to child distress such that mothers became increasingly sensitive to girls’ distress over time, β = −.13, SE = .23, p = .03. Lastly, children’s social self-control at Wave 2 was marginally predictive of children’s emotional reactivity from Wave 2 to Wave 3 such that higher levels of self-control when children were approximately 3 years old predicted decreases in emotional reactivity between ages 3 and 4 years old, β = −.13, SE = .05, p = .08

Discussion

Children of parents with alcohol and substance use problems are at heightened risk of developing psychological problems, though it remains unclear how or why said symptoms develop, particularly during early childhood. Recent research posits that emotion dysregulation is a salient risk factor for the development of psychopathology, namely internalizing disorders such as mood disorders that are characterized by dysfunctional or maladaptive emotional processes (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Gee, De Los Reyes and Seager2016; Cole & Hall, Reference Cole, Hall, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2008; Sheppes et al., Reference Sheppes, Suri and Gross2015, among others). While much research has been done on children’s emotion regulatory processes as they relate to child and adolescent psychopathology (e.g., Cole & Hall, Reference Cole, Hall, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2008; Keenan, Reference Keenan2000; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, Mennin and Nolen-Hoeksema2011), many studies overlook distinct aspects or types of regulatory processes and how they might operate in challenging and complex developmental environments to influence children’s psychological functioning in both adaptive and maladaptive ways. For example, the seemingly dominant narrative in the developmental literature emphasizes emotional under-control, or children’s inability to effectively regulate their emotions as a key component underlying early psychological and behavioral problems such as conduct disorder symptom and aggression (see Zeman et al., Reference Zeman, Cassano, Perry-Parrish and Stegall2006 for review). Thus, seemingly less attention is paid to the developmental benefits, consequences, and other outcomes potentially associated with emotion over-control, or the over-regulation of emotional expression. This present study’s findings show that maternal alcohol dependence symptoms may interfere with mothers’ ability to respond to children’s emotional distress in healthy, supportive ways. In turn, these insensitive responses received from their mothers may lead to a decrease in the frequency and intensity of children’s emotional expressions over time. Findings also suggest that mothers’ responses to children’s emotional distress may differ based on the child’s gender, with mothers in this sample responding more insensitively to boys’ emotional distress over time.

Although we did not use a direct or explicit measure of child expressive suppression, we believe our main findings are conceptually consistent with the emotion regulation concept of expressive suppression, meaning that over time, children of mothers with alcohol dependence symptoms may be suppressing or hiding their emotions, potentially as adaptive strategy or coping mechanism that helps them navigate emotionally impoverished family environments. Several factors support this notion. First, children’s self-control was included as a key covariate since prior work shows that emotion dysregulation is not defined as a lack or absence of regulatory control (meaning that one cannot control their emotions) but is rather represented as regulation in dysfunctional or maladaptive ways (e.g., Cole et al., Reference Cole, Michel and Teti1994). Gross and John (Reference Gross and John2003) emphasize that emotional suppression requires intact and well-developed self-control capabilities since the individual must not only identify the emotion that needs regulating but also actively engage in hiding or masking their emotions, in part by expressing an emotion that may differ from what they actually feel in that moment (e.g., stifling a cry, smiling instead of frowning, etc.). Higher levels of self-control were marginally associated with children’s emotional reactivity in conceptually consistent ways (e.g., Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad and Eggum2010; Fox & Calkins, Reference Fox and Calkins2003; Nelson & Perry, Reference Nelson and Perry2015; Paschke et al., Reference Paschke, Dörfel, Steimke, Trempler, Magrabi, Ludwig, Schubert, Stelzel and Walter2016; Thompson, Reference Thompson1991), thereby implying that children may be overcontrolling or over-regulating emotions in these familial environments.

The second key element bolstering our belief that children in this study may be concealing their emotions is the fact that this outcome was uniquely predicted by mothers’ insensitivity to children’s emotional expressions, thereby supporting prior research that links emotionally insensitive parenting and developmental environments to child emotional overcontrol and suppression (e.g., Balan et al., Reference Balan, Dobrean, Roman and Balazsi2017; Kokkinos et al., Reference Kokkinos, Algiovanoglou and Voulgaridou2019). Thus, although somewhat contrary to the dominant narrative in the literature, we posit that the children in our study may be engaging in emotional overcontrol or expressive suppression, potentially as an adaptive coping strategy given the socioemotional dynamics present in their family and caregiving environments.

Although the majority of mothers in our sample did not display high levels of insensitive responses to children’s emotional distress, it is important to question what mechanisms might explain why maternal alcohol dependence symptoms may interfere with mothers’ ability to sensitively and appropriately respond to their child’s emotional distress. Broadly speaking, mothers’ insensitive responses may reflect an inability for mothers to emotionally connect to their children when children are highly distressed (e.g., Jacques et al., Reference Jacques, Sturge-Apple, Davies and Cicchetti2021). Specifically, several key mechanisms and processes might be contributing to this caregiving disruption. One may be distress intolerance, or the inability to withstand feeling emotional distress yourself and/or witnessing distress in others. Distress intolerance is a widely studied component of substance use disorders and is often identified as a potential target of intervention in both substance use disorder treatment (McHugh et al., Reference McHugh, Reynolds, Leyro and Otto2013; Özdel & Ekinci, Reference Özdel and Ekinci2014) and more recently, parenting programs (e.g., Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, Booth, Luyten, Bridgett and Mayes2015). One way that distress intolerance may imbrue mother’s ability to sensitively respond to children’s emotional distress is that mothers may find children’s distress cues (e.g., crying) aversive, and consequently may react abrasively – such as yelling, ignoring, or walking away – instead of processing, addressing, and regulating the distress they feel when encountering such cues. As a result, mothers may be unable to work through their own distress in a way that allows them to identify their child’s emotional needs, identify an appropriate caregiving response, and effectively implement said response. Interestingly, maternal distress intolerance may create or perpetuate a maladaptive dyadic cycle in which her inability to quell her own distress prolongs the child’s distress, and consequently her own distress. In sum, a decreased tolerance for feeling or encountering distressing emotions may undermine mothers’ ability to sensitively respond to children’s distress cues.

Neurocognitive and executive functioning deficits associated with alcohol and substance use problems may also be disrupting mothers’ provision of sensitive caregiving (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Iyengar, Mayes, Potenza, Rutherford and Strathearn2017; Landi et al., Reference Landi, Montoya, Kober, Rutherford, Mencl, Worhunsky and Mayes2011; Lowell et al., Reference Lowell, Maupin, Landi, Potenza, Mayes and Rutherford2020; Rogers & Robbins, Reference Rogers and Robbins2001). Prior work using imaging methods has shown decreased neural activation in the brain regions responsible for motivation, empathy, and emotional processing, particularly when encountering highly salient child distress cues, such as crying (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Iyengar, Mayes, Potenza, Rutherford and Strathearn2017, Lowell et al., Reference Lowell, Maupin, Landi, Potenza, Mayes and Rutherford2020, Rutherford & Mayes, Reference Rutherford and Mayes2017). Thus, mothers with symptoms of alcohol dependence may struggle to accurately identify their child’s emotions and/or their emotional needs, and consequently, may not select caregiving responses that are appropriately calibrated to the nature, urgency, or intensity of their child’s emotional experiences. Additionally, children’s distress cues may also place additional regulatory demands on mothers’ already taxed or dysregulated emotional processing capabilities (Cataldo et al., Reference Cataldo, Azhari, Coppola, Bornstein and Esposito2019). Thus, it should be noted that most mothers may feel an urge or desire to appropriately respond to their child’s emotional needs, but rather, for several reasons, neurocognitive impairments may prevent these mothers from effectively implementing these caregiving responses (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Iyengar, Mayes, Potenza, Rutherford and Strathearn2017; Landi et al., Reference Landi, Montoya, Kober, Rutherford, Mencl, Worhunsky and Mayes2011; (Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, Yip, Worhunsky, Kim, Strathearn, Potenza and Mayes2020).

Nonetheless, children growing up in environments where their emotional needs go unmet may internalize important messages about such settings early on, which can have lasting implications for how they navigate these environments and interact with others (Kopp, Reference Kopp1989). In fact, several theories explicate this notion, namely Emotional Security Theory (EST; Davies & Cummings, Reference Davies and Cummings1994) and emotional socialization frameworks (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998). From an EST viewpoint, mothers’ insensitivity to children’s emotional distress across early childhood may erode children’s sense of security in their emotional connection with their mothers, leaving children lacking confidence, faith, or assurance that their emotions matter or are valid. Additionally, children may suppress their emotions to circumvent or avoid eliciting harmful caregiving responses such as yelling (Koss et al., Reference Koss, George, Bergman, Cummings, Davies and Cicchetti2011). Essentially, suppression may reflect children’s attempts to “fly under the radar” and protect themselves from experiencing negative reactions from parents. In this way, emotional suppression may be adaptive; children may consciously engage in this strategy to effectively navigate potentially harmful and maladaptive situations or interactions. Over time, children may increasingly look to expressive suppression as a useful self-preservation mechanism, especially if they frequently encounter or experience emotionally insensitive contexts (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Levine, Lench and Quas2010; Ponzetti et al., Reference Ponzetti, Spinelli, Coppola, Lionetti, D’Urso, Shah and Aureli2023).

Engagement in expressive suppression strategies may be only one explanation for longitudinal decreases in children’s emotional reactivity. Often implicated as a factor involved in post-traumatic stress disorder (e.g., Litz et al., Reference Litz, Schlenger, Weathers, Caddell, Fairbank and LaVange1997), emotional blunting, numbing, and reduced affect display are labels often used to describe a general reduction in emotional expression and it is possible that mothers’ prolonged and increasingly insensitive responses to their emotional needs over time may be emotionally and psychological traumatizing for children. The harm this trauma inflicts may be exacerbated if children continually internalize negative messages linked to their mothers’ insensitivity, such as feeling worthless, that their feelings are unimportant, or that expressing emotions is meaningless endeavor. This explanation may support recent work that shows that children who have endured various types of traumas may struggle processing complex, vulnerable emotions and feelings like anxiety post-trauma (e.g., Dvir et al., Reference Dvir, Ford, Hill and Frazier2014; Fujiwara et al., Reference Fujiwara, Mizuki, Miki and Chemtob2015) and as a result, these children may gradually shut down emotionally in a process some refer to as “posttraumatic self-dysregulation” (Frewen & Lanius, Reference Frewen and Lanius2006). As others have linked emotional numbing and reduced affect display to various negative behavioral and psychological outcomes such as PTSD (Armsworth & Holaday, Reference Armsworth and Holaday1993; Dvir et al., Reference Dvir, Ford, Hill and Frazier2014; Kerig et al., Reference Kerig, Bennett, Thompson and Becker2012), and given the increased prevalence of psychiatric conditions among children of parents with substance use disorders (Velleman & Templeton, Reference Velleman and Templeton2007), it may be especially beneficial for future research to further investigate whether emotional numbing (or reduced affect display) is a present and potential target of intervention for children from these families.

Interestingly, decreased emotional reactivity among children remains a relatively underexplored research area. Thus, one important question remains: why is it important to study these concepts among children, and how might reduced emotional expressions impact children’s long-term development? Emotional suppression may be both adaptive and maladaptive and may positively and negatively impact children. On one hand, children may be trying to survive in or navigate emotionally harmful family environments. They may also use this approach in other higher-risk environments, or any environment in which it is beneficial to mask or hide one’s emotions (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Levine, Lench and Quas2010). On the other hand, at least among adults, expressive suppression is widely associated with emotional and interpersonal problems, such as impairments in emotion processing and trouble empathizing with others (Butler et al., Reference Butler, Egloff, Wlhelm, Smith, Erickson and Gross2003; Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Hempel and Lynch2013). From a developmental perspective, young children who rely on this strategy may never develop a sense of safety or comfort expressing emotions, and thus may struggle to elicit emotional support from others or build emotional intimacy into interpersonal relationships, including friendships, future romantic relationships, and relationships with their own children. They may continue to bottle their emotions and rely on themselves as their sole source of emotion regulation and support, a decision that might overwhelm and therefore undermine their emotion regulation development. Thus, while it is understandable that children would use and even come to lean on suppression as a useful protective strategy, doing so may ultimately prove more harmful than helpful in the long run.

Therefore, we posit that this study may help motivate child researchers to broaden their conceptualizations and operationalizations of early indicators of psychopathology and dysregulation, given the transdiagnostic nature of emotional functioning. Emotional outbursts and aggression are classic signs of early regulatory and psychological problems, yet the reticent child, the one not showing much emotion, and the one who infrequently engages with peers or relatives may also be displaying early warning signs. While our study did not explicitly link children’s reduced emotional reactivity to later psychopathology, our findings imply that children may be psychological and emotionally responding to traumatic or adverse caregiving contexts in adaptive ways that could have maladaptive consequences later. More work is needed to further identify longitudinal processes that could link reduced emotional reactivity among children and developmental psychopathology.

Strengths, limitations, and suggestions for future research

We believe the present study has several strengths. First, it contributes to a growing interest in young children’s development of emotion regulation, particularly among children from adverse family contexts, such as families struggling with parental substance use disorders. Specifically, our work offers a contextual, process-oriented approach to understanding how and why different emotion regulatory strategies may operate in distinct caregiving environments. As such, our paper may motivate future research in this area to incorporate more racial, ethnic, socioeconomic, and familial diversity, including more diversity in how studies are developed, conducted, analyzed, and interpreted.

Another strength resides in our main findings’ potential clinical implications. Although overall rates of maternal insensitivity were low (with the majority of mothers not displaying significant increases in insensitivity to children’s emotional distress), the mediational path linking maternal alcohol dependence symptoms, insensitivity to children’s emotional distress, and children’s decreased emotional reactivity identifies key targets of intervention for both mothers with symptoms of alcohol dependence and their children. For mothers, interventions that help mothers accurately identify and process children’s emotions and then engage in sensitive, child-oriented caregiving behaviors may significantly improve their caregiving. This may also promote stronger emotional bonds, trust, and intimacy in their mother-child relationships. Regarding children, our work may encourage those working with children to increasingly consider emotion overcontrol, suppressive strategies, and underexplored regulatory processes in their research or applied work. Many children may not outwardly express sadness, anger, or other discrete negative emotions despite feeling these emotions internally, and many may not feel they have a safe place to express and work through their emotional experiences. Therefore, we may be missing and consequently not meeting the needs of children with these lived experiences. Early identification of harmful or maladaptive developmental processes is key in intervening and steering children toward more positive outcomes. Moreover, far more research on the developmental origins and processes of expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal is needed, particularly among young children from racially and ethnically diverse, lower-income, and higher-risk backgrounds (see Gross and Cassidy, Reference Gross and Cassidy2019 for a review of future suggestions in child expressive suppression research).

Our longitudinal use of observational paradigms for both maternal insensitivity to children’s emotional distress and children’s emotional reactivity are also strengths of the study. Discussions around discrepant responding via multi-informant reports of parenting behaviors persist (Curhan et al., Reference Curhan, Rabinowitz, Pas and Bradshaw2020; Sessa et al., Reference Sessa, Avenevoli, Steinberg and Morris2001; and see De Los Reyes & Epkins, Reference De Los Reyes and Epkins2023 for a recent overview of informant discrepancies). Specifically, parent reports may be heavily influenced by the closeness of their relationship with their child (Lohaus, et al., Reference Lohaus, Rueth and Vierhaus2020). Younger children may struggle to provide in-depth, comprehensive, and specific reports of caregiving behaviors when asked using current assessments (Shelton et al., Reference Shelton, Frick and Wootton1996; Taber, Reference Taber2010), and external reports of parenting (e.g., from partners, other family members, or teachers) may also present inaccuracies in examining parenting behaviors since these reporters may be absent and unaware of the parents’ parenting behaviors in various settings or situations (including the contexts or situations being studied); may be influenced by external factors such as self-serving or other biases; and may still be susceptible to socially desirable reporting (Sedikides et al., Reference Sedikides, Campbell, Reeder and Elliot1998; Lohaus et al., 2020; Marchand-Reilly & Yaure, Reference Marchand-Reilly and Yaure2019; Treutler & Epkins, Reference Treutler and Epkins2003). Additionally, parent-report of parenting behaviors, children’s behaviors and child outcomes, while important and beneficial in many instances may also present challenges. Parents may either intentionally or unintentionally (a) over-report socially desirable behaviors that promote desirable perceptions of them as parents, like responding warmly, attentively, and consistently to children’s emotions and downplay or under-report less desirable behaviors like intrusiveness or harshness, and similarly (b) under-report symptoms of children’s emotional or psychological problems, such as irritability, high levels of emotional reactivity, or aggression (Bornstein et al., Reference Bornstein, Putnick, Lansford, Pastorelli, Skinner, Sorbring, Tapanya, Uribe Tirado, Zelli, Alampay, Al-Hassan, Bacchini, Bombi, Chang, Deater-Deckard, Di Giunta, Dodge, Malone and Oburu2015; Chi & Hinshaw, Reference Chi and Hinshaw2002; Lohaus et al., 2020; Merydith et al., Reference Merydith, Prout and Blaha2003; Moens et al., Reference Moens, Weeland, Van der Giessen, Chhangur and Overbeek2018; Paulhus, Reference Paulhus, Braun, Jackson and Wiley2002, Stifter et al., Reference Stifter, Willoughby and Towe-Goodman2008; Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Warner, John, Prusoff, Merikangas and Gammon1987). Importantly, parents, particularly those who may be experiencing psychopathologies or deficits in emotion regulation may inaccurately report (either exaggerating or underreporting) children’s emotions and internal states (Bingham et al., Reference Bingham, Loukas, Fitzgerald and Zucker2003; Kroes et al., Reference Kroes, Veerman and De Bruyn2003; Ordway, Reference Ordway2011; Treutler & Epkins, Reference Treutler and Epkins2003; Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Warner, John, Prusoff, Merikangas and Gammon1987). Observational paradigms, while harboring their own limitations, may present novel and insightful ways of studying: the specific ways that young children of mothers with alcohol or substance use problems express emotional distress; how these mothers respond to said distress; and how children may regulate their emotions or differentially react as a result of their mothers’ responses to their emotional cues. Moreover, such paradigms pave the way for new questions that can be uniquely addressed through more rigorous assessment or analytical approaches. For example, implementing ecological momentary assessments or dyadic/co-regulation approaches may reveal new insights into how these mothers and their children navigate emotionally stressful interactions, or regulate emotions based on evolving situational, contextual, and/or interpersonal factors (e.g., how a parent’s reactions changes based on how children’s emotions change). Depending on a study’s procedure, one could also examine whether or how children may use distinct strategies across parent-child contexts, and whether the function of these behaviors predicts different developmental outcomes. In sum, our observational approach allowed us to specifically examine the nature and extent of children’s behavioral emotional distress, mothers’ behavioral responses to said distress, and children’s emotional reactivity in ways that may uniquely contribute to how the literature understands associations between these constructs.

Nonetheless, this study’s strengths are not without limitations. First, this study used a global operationalization of maternal insensitivity to children’s emotional distress, thus, it is unclear whether distinct types of maternal insensitivity (e.g., harshness vs. intrusiveness vs. emotional or psychological disengagement) differentially or uniquely influence children’s emotional reactivity. Recent work indicates that certain insensitive responses to children’s emotional distress may pose unique threats to children’s psychological functioning (Jacques et al., Reference Jacques, Sturge-Apple, Davies and Cicchetti2021). Therefore, specific manifestations of insensitivity may be the driving force behind children’s reduced emotional expression. Subsequent studies may parse this global construct to examine more distinct pathways linking discrete insensitive parental responses to children’s expressive suppression specifically and emotion-regulation strategies more broadly. Moreover, although behavioral observations of parent-child interactions may present strengths and advantages over relative- or partner-report (e.g., (Cross et al., Reference Cross, Overall, Low and Henderson2021) or parent-report measures of parenting (e.g., Moens et al., Reference Moens, Weeland, Van der Giessen, Chhangur and Overbeek2018; Morsbach & Prinz, Reference Morsbach and Prinz2006) – particularly reports completed by parents from socioeconomically challenging backgrounds (Herbers et al., Reference Herbers, Garcia and Obradović2017), parents experiencing pronounced psychological distress, psychopathologies, family adversity, or substance use problems - a single assessment of mothers’ sensitive responses to children’s emotional distress may not fully capture as much heterogeneity in mothers’ responses or the ways in which mothers may alter their responsiveness across emotional parent-child contexts. In expanding our work, future research may benefit or yield more robust results from including multi-assessment or multi-informant reports of mothers’ sensitivity to children’s emotions (e.g., Groh & Roisman, Reference Groh and Roisman2009; Leerkes et al., Reference Leerkes, Weaver and O’Brien2012; Parent et al., Reference Parent, Forehand, Dunbar, Watson, Reising, Seehuus and Compas2014).

It is also important to note that while lifetime alcohol dependence symptoms predicted longitudinal increases in maternal insensitivity to children’s distress, the overwhelming majority of mothers displayed low (and decreasing) levels of insensitivity across study waves. Said differently, most mothers were responding sensitively to their children during dyadic observations. While it is important to examine how psychological challenges, such as symptoms of psychopathology (including alcohol and substance use disorders) may engender risks to parenting behaviors and children’s development, it is equally important to recognize the positive implications and patterns gleaned from the data, such as declining incidences and consistently low levels of insensitive caregiving, behaviors which might pose risks to children’s well-being and emotional development. Future research should intentional include more person-centered, holistic, and contextually-oriented frameworks, and continue examining this and similar work from strengths- and assets-based perspectives, for both parenting (e.g., what parents are doing well despite economic, psychological, and socio-structural challenges and how we can support extant parenting behaviors) and for children’s psychological development and well-being.

Another limitation is that we only examined two elements or manifestations of children’s emotion regulation: reactivity and self-control, with the former used as an indicator and operationalization of children’s emotional suppression. It is possible that other regulatory components or processes may explain, influence or expand on associations between key study variables, such as children’s psychophysiological regulation or reactions to stress, or cognitive reappraisal (or reappraisal-based strategies). Importantly, the lack of theoretically-based, evidence-based, and culturally responsive assessments of expressive suppression and suppression-like strategies among preschool-aged and younger children complicates our ability to study and therefore understand the manifestation, function, and adaptiveness of these strategies during and across early development (Gross & Cassidy, Reference Gross and Cassidy2019). Consequently, despite prior work broadly linking early emotion-regulation deficits and lack of familial or parental support for emotional expression as risk factors for psychopathology and behavioral problems (Compas et al., Reference Compas, Jaser, Bettis, Watson, Gruhn, Dunbar and Thigpen2017; Miu et al., Reference Miu, Szentágotai-Tătar, Balazsi, Nechita, Bunea and Pollak2022; Sheppes et al., Reference Sheppes, Suri and Gross2015; Suveg & Zeman, 2010), a lack of a “go-to,” widely embraced assessment or paradigm (especially an observational paradigm) to specifically study expressive suppression can further interfere with our ability to more definitively link early affect-based regulation strategies to either risk for or buffers against psychopathology in young children (Troy et al., Reference Troy, Willroth, Shallcross, Giuliani, Gross and Mauss2023). Additional or supplemental assessments, such as assessments of children’s socio-cognitive functioning or internal indicators of emotional arousal may provide more robust or conclusive evidence as to the presence or use of expressive suppression or suppression-like regulatory strategies. Lastly, as a secondary data analytic project, a lack of targeted recruitment in the primary study may have inadvertently produced various limitations for our work. First, rates of mothers’ endorsement of various alcohol dependence symptoms were generally low, and therefore, results should be interpreted with caution. Specifically, the nature and extent of mothers’ endorsements of alcohol dependence symptoms were infrequent and not diverse (e.g., few mothers endorsing many different symptoms) and thus, it is unclear whether or how results may generalize to other maternal samples. Therefore, we encourage future researchers to further explore our study’s constructs in samples of mothers with (a) diagnosed, (b) more severe and/or (c) more diverse endorsement of symptoms of alcohol dependence. Second, our study procedure and assessment of mothers’ alcohol dependence symptoms did not include prenatal alcohol use or dependence symptoms during pregnancy, and consequently could not speak to any possible effects of prenatal alcohol exposure. Additionally, due to low statistical power and recruitment effects, we were unable to examine the presence or potential influence of pre- or postnatal polysubstance use or multiple substance use disorders on mothers’ responses to children’s emotional distress or children’s emotional reactivity. Subsequent research that includes these assessments may be better able to identify and unpack nuances and specificity in associations between maternal drug and alcohol use disorders, parental responses within and across emotional parent-child contexts, and children’s emotion regulation.

Future work may aim to include more diverse measures, conceptualizations, and operationalizations of children’s emotion-regulation strategies, including assessments of cognitive reappraisal in addition to explicit measures of expressive suppression to compare and contrast how they develop in very young children from adverse developmental backgrounds (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Gee, De Los Reyes and Seager2016; Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015; Berking & Wupperman, Reference Berking and Wupperman2012; McRae et al., Reference McRae, Gross, Weber, Robertson, Sokol-Hessner, Ray, Gabrieli and Ochsner2012; Todd et al., Reference Todd, Cunningham, Anderson and Thompson2012). These suggestions coincide with recent calls (e.g., Gross & Cassidy, Reference Gross and Cassidy2019) to identify and better integrate developmental models and assessments of expressive suppressive to further our understanding of how and why expressive suppression develops, appears, or functions within and across early developmental periods (e.g., infancy) and within and across different types of family risk or adversity. Future research that incorporates longitudinal designs may further delineate how, why, when, and in what contexts (familial or otherwise) children’s emotion regulation and reactivity strategies may be either adaptive or maladaptive (e.g., Bridges et al., Reference Bridges, Denham and Ganiban2004; Diamond & Aspinwall, Reference Diamond and Aspinwall2003; Izard et al., Reference Izard, Stark, Trentacosta and Schultz2008; Kovacs et al., Reference Kovacs, Joormann and Gotlib2008; Miu et al., Reference Miu, Szentágotai-Tătar, Balazsi, Nechita, Bunea and Pollak2022).

Lastly, learning more about longitudinal links between parental psychopathology, substance use disorders, parenting responses, and children’s emotional functioning may advance translational efforts. Children’s externalizing problems are ostensibly easier to identify, categorize, and diagnose relative to internalizing problems, and even when early childhood onset of internalizing disorders is diagnosed, children who are not displaying typical, well-known, distinctive symptoms of or risk factors for psychopathology (e.g., high emotional reactivity, anhedonia, etc.) may go undiagnosed and therefore untreated, thereby allowing symptoms to persist and possibly worsen over time (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Luby and Sullivan2008; Mäntymaa et al., Reference Mäntymaa, Puura, Luoma, Latva, Salmelin and Tamminen2012). Our findings when combined with emerging research (e.g., Afek et al., Reference Afek, Ben-Avraham, Davidov, Berezin Cohen, Ben Yehuda, Gilboa and Nahum2021; De France et al., Reference De France, Evans, Brody and Doan2022; Denckla et al., Reference Denckla, Cicchetti, Kubzansky, Seedat, Teicher, Williams and Koenen2020; Thompson & Goodman, Reference Thompson, Goodman, Kring and Sloan2010; Trudel-Fitzgerald & Ouellet-Morin, Reference Trudel-Fitzgerald and Ouellet-Morin2022) suggest that clinicians, practitioners, and others working in child and family mental health services may unintentionally overlook subsets of children whose emotional functioning might place them at unique risk for psychopathology and the under-identification of needs for treatment services.

Conclusion

Alcohol dependence problems are known to undermine parents’ ability to sensitively respond to their children’s emotional needs. Consequently, children of parents with symptoms of alcohol dependence are at increased risk for socioemotional problems. Emotion regulation challenges are a key factor underlying the development of more pervasive and severe psychological disorders, yet it remains unclear how these challenges may manifest among children of mothers with alcohol dependence symptoms. Findings show that alcohol dependence symptoms may promote mothers’ insensitive responses to children’s vulnerable emotions which may encourage children to suppress or hide their emotions. Although children may do this to protect themselves from emotional harm, suppression may undermine their immediate and long-term psychological, emotional, and social well-being. Such evidence further demonstrates the multifaceted, pervasive impacts parental substance use problems have on parents and their children. Future research may build on these findings by more specifically unpacking links between parental substance use disorders, caregiving, children’s emotion regulation, and the impact regulatory processes have on children’s later well-being.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the amazing children and families who contributed to this project, as well as our community-based research partners and clinical and research staff who collaborated on this project. We also appreciate the reviewers for their helpful comments in revising this manuscript.

Funding statement

Funding for the primary research project were provided via the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH071256) awarded to Patrick T. Davies and Dante Cicchetti. Partial funding for secondary data analyses and manuscript completion were provided by an American Psychological Foundation/Council of Graduate Departments of Psychology Graduate Research Scholarship awarded to Debrielle T. Jacques.

Competing interests

None.

References

Abidin, R. R. (1992). The determinants of parenting behavior. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21(4), 407412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles, (vol. 30). University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Afek, A., Ben-Avraham, R., Davidov, A., Berezin Cohen, N., Ben Yehuda, A., Gilboa, Y., & Nahum, M. (2021). Psychological resilience, mental health, and inhibitory control among youth and young adults under stress. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 608588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. N. (2015). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Psychology press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Wittig, B. A. (1969). Attachment and exploratory behaviour of one-year-olds in a strange situation. In Foss, B. M. (Ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour. (vol. 4), Methuen.Google Scholar
Aldao, A. (2013). The future of emotion regulation research: Capturing context. Perspectives On Psychological Science, 8(2), 155172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldao, A., Gee, D. G., De Los Reyes, A., & Seager, I. (2016). Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic factor in the development of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology: Current and future directions. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4pt1), 927946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldao, A., Jazaieri, H., Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies: Interactive effects during CBT for social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(4), 382389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.03.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldao, A., Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation flexibility. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(3), 263278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alim, T. N., Charney, D. S., & Mellman, T. A. (2006). An overview of posttraumatic stress disorder in African Americans. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(7), 801813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allison, P. D. (2003). Missing data techniques for structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(4), 545557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. American Psychiatric Association, American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Armsworth, M. W., & Holaday, M. (1993). The effects of psychological trauma on children and adolescents. Journal of Counseling & Development, 72(1), 4956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balan, R., Dobrean, A., Roman, G. D., & Balazsi, R. (2017). Indirect effects of parenting practices on internalizing problems among adolescents: The role of expressive suppression. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(1), 4047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Band, E. B., & Weisz, J. R. (1988). How to feel better when it feels bad: Children’s perspectives on coping with everyday stress. Developmental Psychology, 24(2), 247253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.2.247 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banerjee, M. (1997). Hidden emotions: Preschoolers’ knowledge of appearance-reality and emotion display rules. Social Cognition, 15(2), 107132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bariola, E., Gullone, E., & Hughes, E. K. (2011). Child and adolescent emotion regulation: The role of parental emotion regulation and expression. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 198212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P. (2015). Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 44(5), 875896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., & Cicchetti, D. (2019). Emotion dysregulation and emerging psychopathology: A transdiagnostic, transdisciplinary perspective. Development andPsychopathology, 31(3), 799804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behrens, K. Y., Parker, A. C., & Haltigan, J. D. (2011). Maternal sensitivity assessed during the strange situation procedure predicts child’s attachment quality and reunion behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(2), 378381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Berking, M., & Wupperman, P. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55(2), 8396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berking, M., & Wupperman, P. (2012). Emotion regulation and mental health: recent findings, current challenges, and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25(2), 128134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlin, L. J., & Cassidy, J. (2003). Mothers’ self‐reported control of their preschool children’s emotional expressiveness: A longitudinal study of associations with infant–mother attachment and children’s emotion regulation. Social Development, 12(4), 477495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bingham, C. R., Loukas, A., Fitzgerald, H. E., & Zucker, R. A. (2003). Parental ratings of son’s behavior problems in high-risk families: Convergent validity, internal structure, and interparent agreement. Journal of Personality Assessment, 80(3), 237251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Lansford, J. E., Pastorelli, C., Skinner, A. T., Sorbring, E., Tapanya, S., Uribe Tirado, L. M., Zelli, A., Alampay, L. P., Al-Hassan, S. M., Bacchini, D., Bombi, A. S., Chang, L., Deater-Deckard, K., Di Giunta, L., Dodge, K. A., Malone, P. S., & Oburu, P. (2015). Mother and father socially desirable responding in nine countries: Two kinds of agreement and relations to parenting self-reports. International Journal of Psychology : Journal International De Psychologie, 50(3), 174185. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12084 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyum, L. A., & Parke, R. D. (1995). The role of family emotional expressiveness in the development of children’s social competence. Journal of Marriage and Family, 57(3), 593608. https://doi.org/10.2307/353915 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., & Karrass, J. (2010). Fear and anger reactivity trajectories from 4 to 16 months: The roles of temperament, regulation, and maternal sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 46(4), 791804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breaux, R. P., Harvey, E. A., & Lugo-Candelas, C. I. (2016). The role of parent psychopathology in emotion socialization. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(4), 731743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N., Kessler, R. C., Chilcoat, H. D., Schultz, L. R., Davis, G. C., & Andreski, P. (1998). Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community: The 1996 Detroit area survey of trauma. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(7), 626632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bretherton, I. (2016). Revisiting Mary Ainsworth’s conceptualization and assessments of maternal sensitivity-insensitivity. In Maternal sensitivity (pp. 1842). Routledge.Google Scholar
Bridges, L. J., Denham, S. A., & Ganiban, J. M. (2004). Definitional issues in emotion regulation research. Child Development, 75(2), 340345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckholdt, K. E., Parra, G. R., & Jobe-Shields, L. (2014). Intergenerational transmission of emotion dysregulation through parental invalidation of emotions: Implications for adolescent internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23(2), 324332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, K., Chethik, L., Burns, W. J., & Clark, R. (1991). Dyadic disturbances in cocaine-abusing mothers and their infants. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47(2), 316319.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wlhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression. Emotion, 3(1), 4867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buu, A., Dipiazza, C., Wang, J., Puttler, L. I., Fitzgerald, H. E., & Zucker, R. A. (2009). Parent, family, and neighborhood effects on the development of child substance use and other psychopathology from preschool to the start of adulthood. Journal of Studies On Alcohol and Drugs, 70(4), 489498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cai, X., Kaiser, A. P., & Hancock, T. B. (2004). Parent and teacher agreement on child behavior checklist items in a sample of preschoolers from low-income and predominantly African American families. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(2), 303312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calkins, S. D. (1994). Origins and outcomes of individual differences in emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2-3), 5372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, S. D., Gill, K. L., Johnson, M. C., & Smith, C. L. (1999). Emotional reactivity and emotional regulation strategies as predictors of social behavior with peers during toddlerhood. Social Development, 8(3), 310334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1989). Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 394402. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.394 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Wang, T. S. (2007). Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in preschool children. Cognitive Development, 22(4), 489510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carreras, J., Carter, A. S., Heberle, A., Forbes, D., & Gray, S. A. (2019). Emotion regulation and parent distress: Getting at the heart of sensitive parenting among parents of preschool children experiencing high sociodemographic risk. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28(11), 29532962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cataldo, I., Azhari, A., Coppola, A., Bornstein, M. H., & Esposito, G. (2019). The influences of drug abuse on mother-infant interaction through the lens of the biopsychosocial model of health and illness: A review. Frontiers in Public Health, 7, 45. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2019.00045 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaplin, T. M., & Aldao, A. (2013). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 735765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaplin, T. M., Cole, P. M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2005). Parental socialization of emotion expression: Gender differences and relations to child adjustment. Emotion, 5(1), 8088. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.80 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheetham, A., Allen, N. B., Yücel, M., & Lubman, D. I. (2010). The role of affective dysregulation in drug addiction. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(6), 621634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J., Hu, Z., Lu, A. Y., Ma, T., & Zheng, J. How do Family Factors Impact Children’s Emotional Regulation? In: 2022 3rd International Conference on Mental Health, Education and Human Development (MHEHD 2022), Atlantis Press, 2022, 270275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chi, T. C., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2002). Mother-child relationships of children with ADHD: The role of maternal depressive symptoms and depression-related distortions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30(4), 387400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J. L. (1998). Contextualism and developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 10(2), 137141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Carlson, V. (1989). Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(4), 597600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2006). Fostering secure attachment in infants in maltreating families through preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 18(3), 623649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed andstandardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological Assessment, 6(4), 284290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 309319. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.309 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, P., Ashana Ramsook, K., & Ram, N. (2019). Emotion dysregulation as a dynamic process. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 11911201. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000695 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M. (2014). Moving ahead in the study of the development of emotion regulation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(2), 203207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, P. M., Armstrong, L. M., & Pemberton, C. K. (2010). The role of language in the development of emotion regulation. In Calkins, S. D., & Bell, M. A. (Ed.), Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 5977). American Psychological Association, https://doi.org/10.1037/12059-004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, P. M., Dennis, T. A., Smith-Simon, K. E., & Cohen, L. H. (2009). Preschoolers’ emotion regulation strategy understanding: Relations with emotion socialization and child self-regulation. Social Development, 18(2), 324352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, P. M., & Hall, S. E. (2008). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for psychopathology. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Hinshaw, S. P. (Ed.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (pp. 265298). John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google Scholar
Cole, P. M., & Jacobs, A. E. (2018). From children’s expressive control to emotion regulation: Looking back, looking ahead. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(6), 658677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M., Luby, J., & Sullivan, M. W. (2008). Emotions and the development of childhood depression: Bridging the gap. Child Development Perspectives, 2(3), 141148. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00056.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75(2), 317333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M., Michel, M. K., & Teti, L. O. D. (1994). The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. In Monographs of the society for research in child development 59(2/3), 73–100. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166139.Google Scholar
Cole, P. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., Fox, N. A., Usher, B. A., & Welsh, J. D. (1996). Individual differences in emotion regulation and behavior problems in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105(4), 518529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compas, B. E., Jaser, S. S., Bettis, A. H., Watson, K. H., Gruhn, M. A., Dunbar, J. P., & Thigpen, J. C. (2017). Coping, emotion regulation, and psychopathology in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143(9), 939991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creasey, G., Ottlinger, K., Devico, K., Murray, T., Harvey, A., & Hesson-McInnis, M. (1997). Children’s affective responses, cognitive appraisals, and coping strategies in response to the negative affect of parents and peers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67(1), 3956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Criss, M. M., Morris, A. S., Ponce‐Garcia, E., Cui, L., & Silk, J. S. (2016). Pathways to adaptive emotion regulation among adolescents from low‐income families. Family Relations, 65(3), 517529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockenberg, S. C., & Leerkes, E. M. (2004). Infant and maternal behaviors regulate infant reactivity to novelty at 6 months. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 11231132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cross, E. J., Overall, N. C., Low, R. S., & Henderson, A. M. (2021). Relationship problems, agreement and bias in perceptions of partners’ parental responsiveness, and family functioning. Journal of Family Psychology, 35(4), 510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curhan, A. L., Rabinowitz, J. A., Pas, E. T., & Bradshaw, C. P. (2020). Informant discrepancies in internalizing and externalizing symptoms in an at-risk sample: The role of parenting and school engagement. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49(1), 311322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutuli, D. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression strategies role in the emotion regulation: An overview on their modulatory effects and neural correlates. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 175. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00175 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Cognition & Emotion, 12(3), 307330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1994). Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 116(3), 387411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, P. T., Cummings, E. M., & Winter, M. A. (2004). Pathways between profiles of family functioning, child security in the interparental subsystem, and child psychological problems. Development and Psychopathology, 16(3), 525550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, P. T., & Martin, M. J. (2013). The reformulation of emotional security theory: The role of children’s social defense in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4pt2), 14351454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, P. T., Sturge-Apple, M. L., Cicchetti, D., Manning, L. G., & Zale, E. (2009). Children’s patterns of emotional reactivity to conflict as explanatory mechanisms in links between interpartner aggression and child physiological functioning. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 50(11), 13841391. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02154.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, P. T., Winter, M. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2006). The implications of emotional security theory for understanding and treating childhood psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 18(3), 707735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. L., Levine, L. J., Lench, H. C., & Quas, J. A. (2010). Metacognitive emotion regulation: Children’s awareness that changing thoughts and goals can alleviate negative emotions. Emotion, 10(4), 498510. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018428 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, J. A., Alto, M. E., Oshri, A., Rogosch, F., Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2020). The effect of maternal depression on mental representations and child negative affect. Journal of Affective Disorders, 261, 920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De France, K., Evans, G. W., Brody, G. H., & Doan, S. N. (2022). Cost of resilience: Childhood poverty, mental health, and chronic physiological stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 144, 105872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105872 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Los Reyes, A., & Epkins, C. C. (2023). Introduction to the special issue. A dozen years of demonstrating that informant discrepancies are more than measurement error: Toward guidelines for integrating data from multi-informant assessments of youth mental health. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 52(1), 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Debell, F., Fear, N. T., Head, M., Batt-Rawden, S., Greenberg, N., Wessely, S., & Goodwin, L. (2014). A systematic review of the comorbidity between PTSD and alcohol misuse. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49(9), 14011425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCicco, J. M., Solomon, B., & Dennis, T. A. (2012). Neural correlates of cognitive reappraisal in children: An ERP study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(1), 7080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, C. A., Cicchetti, D., Kubzansky, L. D., Seedat, S., Teicher, M. H., Williams, D. R., & Koenen, K. C. (2020). Psychological resilience: An update on definitions, a critical appraisal, and research recommendations. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 11(1), 1822064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denham, S. A., Mitchell-Copeland, J., Strandberg, K., Auerbach, S., & Blair, K. (1997). Parental contributions to preschoolers’ emotional competence: Direct and indirect effects. Motivation and Emotion, 21(1), 6586. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024426431247 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Arousal, affect, and attention as components of temperament. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(6), 958966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9(4), 633652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, L. M., & Aspinwall, L. G. (2003). Emotion regulation across the life span: An integrative perspective emphasizing self-regulation, positive affect, and dyadic processes. Motivation and Emotion, 27(2), 125156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diener, M. L., Mangelsdorf, S. C., McHale, J. L., & Frosch, C. A. (2002). Infants’ behavioral strategies for emotion regulation with fathers and mothers: Associations with emotional expressions and attachment quality. Infancy, 3(2), 153174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dix, T., & Yan, N. (2014). Mothers’ depressive symptoms and infant negative emotionality in the prediction of child adjustment at age 3: Testing the maternal reactivity and child vulnerability hypotheses. Development and Psychopathology, 26(1), 111124. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000898 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dryman, M. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2018). Emotion regulation in social anxiety and depression: A systematic review of expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Clinical Psychology Review, 65, 1742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, G. J., Lee, K., Rosales-Rueda, M., & Kalil, A. (2018). Maternal age and child development. Demography, 55(6), 22292255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dvir, Y., Ford, J. D., Hill, M., & Frazier, J. A. (2014). Childhood maltreatment, emotional dysregulation, and psychiatric comorbidities. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 22(3), 149161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1994). Mothers’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s temperament and anger behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40(1), 138156.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Carlo, G., Troyer, D., Speer, A. L., Karbon, M., & Switzer, G. (1992). The relations of maternal practices and characteristics to children’s vicarious emotional responsiveness. Child Development, 63(3), 583602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development, 67(5), 22272247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., Carlo, G., & Miller, P. A. (1991). The relations of parental characteristics and practices to children’s vicarious emotional responding. Child Development, 62(6), 13931408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Schaller, M., Fabes, R. A., Bustamante, D., Mathy, R. M., Shell, R., & Rhodes, K. (1988). Differentiation of personal distress and sympathy in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 24(6), 766775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6(1), 495525. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N. E., & Fabes, R. A. (1992a). Emotion and its regulation in early development. Jossey-Bass/Pfeiffer.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N. E., & Fabes, R. A. (1992b). Emotion, regulation, and the development of social competence. In Clark, M. S. (Ed.), Emotion and social behavior (pp. 119150). Sage Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
Eman, S., Khalid, A., & Nicolson, R. I. (2019). A review of heterogeneous interpretations of emotional reactivity. International Journal of Emotional Education, 11(2), 7190.Google Scholar
Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 8(3), 430457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
English, T., Lee, I. A., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2017). Emotion regulation strategy selection in daily life: The role of social context and goals. Motivation and Emotion, 41(2), 230242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enlow, M. B., Kitts, R. L., Blood, E., Bizarro, A., Hofmeister, M., & Wright, R. J. (2011). Maternal posttraumatic stress symptoms and infant emotional reactivity and emotion regulation. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(4), 487503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabes, R. A., Leonard, S. A., Kupanoff, K., & Martin, C. L. (2001). Parental coping with children’s negative emotions: Relations with children’s emotional and social responding. Child Development, 72(3), 907920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabes, R. A., Martin, C. L., Hanish, L. D., Anders, M. C., & Madden-Derdich, D. A. (2003). Early school competence: the roles of sex-segregated play and effortful control. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, X., Liu, W., Gao, C., Han, Y., Jiang, Z., & Liu, F. (2023). Expressive suppression downregulates negative emotion for children: An ERP study. Open Journal of Social Sciences, 11(03), 194203. https://doi.org/10.4236/jss.2023.113013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T., Vega-Lahr, N., Scafidi, F., & Goldstein, S. (1986). Effects of maternal unavailability on mother-infant interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 9(4), 473478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W., Shaver, P. R., & Carnochan, P. (1990). How emotions develop and how they organise development. Cognition and Emotion, 4(2), 81127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forkmann, T., Scherer, A., Böcker, M., Pawelzik, M., Gauggel, S., & Glaesmer, H. (2014). The relation of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression to suicidal ideation and suicidal desire. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 44(5), 524536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, N. A. (1989). Psychophysiological correlates of emotional reactivity during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 364372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2003). The development of self-control of emotion: Intrinsic and extrinsic influences. Motivation and Emotion, 27(1), 726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankenhuis, W. E., & Del Giudice, M. (2012). When do adaptive developmental mechanisms yield maladaptive outcomes? Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 628642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frewen, P. A., & Lanius, R. A. (2006). Toward a psychobiology of posttraumatic self-dysregulation: Reexperiencing, hyperarousal, dissociation, and emotional numbing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1071(1), 110124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frigerio, A., Porreca, A., Simonelli, A., & Nazzari, S. (2019). Emotional availability in samples of mothers at high risk for depression and with substance use disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujiwara, T., Mizuki, R., Miki, T., & Chemtob, C. (2015). Association between facial expression and PTSD symptoms among young children exposed to the great East Japan earthquake: A pilot study. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garai, E. P., Forehand, R. L., Colletti, C. J., Reeslund, K., Potts, J., & Compas, B. (2009). The relation of maternal sensitivity to children’s internalizing and externalizing problems within the context of maternal depressive symptoms. Behavior Modification, 33(5), 559582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Bell, S., Atchley, R., & Froeliger, B. (2020). Emotion dysregulation in addiction. In Oxford University Press: Oxford handbook of emotion dysregulation (pp. 313326).Google Scholar
Goldman Fraser, J., Harris-Britt, A., Leone Thakkallapalli, E., Kurtz-Costes, B., & Martin, S. (2010). Emotional availability and psychosocial correlates among mothers in substance-abuse treatment and their young infants. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of The World Association for Infant Mental Health, 31(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziano, P. A., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2010). Maternal behaviour and children’s early emotion regulation skills differentially predict development of children’s reactive control and later effortful control. Infant and Child Development, 19(4), 333353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregl, A., Kirigin, M., Sućeska Ligutić, R., & Bilać, S. (2014). Emotional competence of mothers and psychopathology in preschool children with specific language impairment (SLI). Psychiatria Danubina, 26(3), 0270.Google ScholarPubMed
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skills rating system: Manual. American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Groh, A. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2009). Adults’ autonomic and subjective emotional responses to infant vocalizations: The role of secure base script knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grolnick, W. S., Bridges, L. J., & Connell, J. P. (1996). Emotion regulation in two-year-olds: Strategies and emotional expression in four contexts. Child Development, 67(3), 928941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grolnick, W. S., McMenamy, J. M., & Kurowski, C. O. (1999). Emotional and self-regulation in infancy and toddlerhood. In L. Balter & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child Psychology: A Handbook of Contemporary Issues (pp. 3–22). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Gross, A. L., & Ballif, B. (1991). Children’s understanding of emotion from facial expressions and situations: A review. Developmental Review, 11(4), 368398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 224237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion regulation: Past, present, and future. Cognition and Emotion, 13(5), 551573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gross, J. T., & Cassidy, J. (2019). Expressive suppression of negative emotions in children and adolescents: Theory, data, and a guide for future research. Developmental Psychology, 55(9), 19381950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullone, E., Hughes, E. K., King, N. J., & Tonge, B. (2010). The normative development of emotion regulation strategy use in children and adolescents: A 2-year follow-up study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(5), 567574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, Y., Spieker, S. J., & Borelli, J. L. (2021). Emotion co-regulation among mother-preschooler dyads completing the strange situation: Relations to internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 30(3), 699710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hajal, N. J., & Paley, B. (2020). Parental emotion and emotion regulation: A critical target of study for research and intervention to promote child emotion socialization. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 403417. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000864 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, Z. R., Lei, X., Qian, J., Li, P., Wang, H., & Zhang, X. (2016). Parent and child psychopathological symptoms: The mediating role of parental emotion dysregulation. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 21(3), 161168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, Z. R., & Shaffer, A. (2013). The relation of parental emotion dysregulation to children’s psychopathology symptoms: The moderating role of child emotion dysregulation. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 44(5), 591601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harter, S. (1986). Cognitive-developmental processes in the integration of concepts about emotions and the self. Social Cognition, 4(2), 119151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasin, D. S., Sarvet, A. L., Meyers, J. L., Saha, T. D., Ruan, W. J., Stohl, M., & Grant, B. F. (2018). Epidemiology of adult DSM-5 major depressive disorder and its specifiers in the United States. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(4), 336346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbers, J. E., Garcia, E. B., & Obradović, J. (2017). Parenting assessed by observation versus parent-report: Moderation by parent distress and family socioeconomic status. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26, 33393350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Highlander, A., Zachary, C., Jenkins, K., Loiselle, R., McCall, M., Youngstrom, J., & Jones, D. J. (2021). Clinical presentation and treatment of early-onset behavior disorders: The role of parent emotion regulation, emotion socialization, and family income. Behavior Modification, 46, 10471074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Himle, J. A., Baser, R. E., Taylor, R. J., Campbell, R. D., & Jackson, J. S. (2009). Anxiety disorders among African Americans, blacks of Caribbean descent, and non-hispanic whites in the United States. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(5), 578590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollenstein, T., Granic, I., Stoolmiller, M., & Snyder, J. (2004). Rigidity in parent—child interactions and the development of externalizing and internalizing behavior in early childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32(6), 595607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosogoshi, H., Takebayashi, Y., Ito, M., Fujisato, H., Kato, N., Nakajima, S., Oe, Y., Miyamae, M., Kanie, A., & Horikoshi, M. (2020). Expressive suppression of emotion is a moderator of anxiety in a unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of anxiety and depressive disorders: A secondary analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 277, 14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howard Sharp, K. M., Cohen, R., Kitzmann, K. M., & Parra, G. R. (2016). Mechanisms mediating children’s perceived maternal nonsupportive reactions to sadness and children’s social and emotional functioning. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25(2), 367380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hwa-Froelich, D. A., Loveland Cook, C. A., & Flick, L. H. (2008). Maternal sensitivity and communication styles: Mothers with depression. Journal of Early Intervention, 31(1), 4466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
IBM Corp. Released (2022). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, Version 29.0. IBM Corp.Google Scholar
Izard, C., Stark, K., Trentacosta, C., & Schultz, D. (2008). Beyond emotion regulation: Emotion utilization and adaptive functioning. Child Development Perspectives, 2(3), 156163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1987). Perspectives on emotional development I: Differential emotions theory of early emotional development. In Osofsky, J. D. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nded.). Wiley Interscience.Google Scholar
Jacob, M. L., Suveg, C., & Whitehead, M. R. (2014). Relations between emotional and social functioning in children with anxiety disorders. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 45(5), 519532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacques, D. T., Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., & Cicchetti, D. (2021). Parsing alcohol-dependent mothers’ insensitivity to child distress: Longitudinal links with children’s affective and anxiety problems. Developmental Psychology, 57(6), 900912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juang, L. P., Moffitt, U., Kim, S. Y., Lee, R. M., Soto, J. A., Hurley, E., … & Whitbourne, S. K. (2016). Cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression: Links to racial-ethnic discrimination and adjustment among Latino/a and Asian-Heritage college students. Journal of Adolescence, 53, 2133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karl, D. (1995). Maternal responsiveness of socially high-risk mothers to the elicitation cues of their 7-month-old infants. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 10(4), 254263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, E. A., Puzia, M. E., Mead, H. K., Crowell, S. E., McEachern, A., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2017). Children’s emotion regulation difficulties mediate the association between maternal borderline and antisocial symptoms and youth behavior problems over 1 year. Journal of Personality Disorders, 31(2), 170192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keenan, K. (2000). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for child psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7(4), 418434.Google Scholar
Kerig, P. K., Bennett, D. C., Thompson, M., & Becker, S. P. (2012). Nothing really matters”: Emotional numbing as a link between trauma exposure and callousness in delinquent youth. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 25(3), 272279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiel, E. J., & Kalomiris, A. E. (2015). Current themes in understanding children’s emotion regulation as developing from within the parent-child relationship. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 1116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, B. R., Teti, D. M., & Cole, P. M. (2012). Mothers’ affect dysregulation, depressive symptoms, and emotional availability during mother-infant interaction. Infant Mental Health Journal, 33(5), 469476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S., Iyengar, U., Mayes, L. C., Potenza, M. N., Rutherford, H. J., & Strathearn, L. (2017). Mothers with substance addictions show reduced reward responses when viewing their own infant’s face. Human Brain Mapping, 38(11), 54215439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., & Coy, K. C. (2002). Child emotionality and maternal responsiveness as predictors of reunion behaviors in the strange situation: Links mediated and unmediated by separation distress. Child Development, 73(1), 228240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köhler-Dauner, F., Roder, E., Krause, S., Buchheim, A., Gündel, H., Fegert, J. M., Ziegenhain, U., & Waller, C. (2019). Reduced caregiving quality measured during the strange situation procedure increases child’s autonomic nervous system stress response. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 13(1), 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kokkinos, C. M., Algiovanoglou, I., & Voulgaridou, I. (2019). Emotion regulation and relational aggression in adolescents: Parental attachment as moderator. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28(11), 31463160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 343354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.343 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koss, K. J., George, M. R., Bergman, K. N., Cummings, E. M., Davies, P. T., & Cicchetti, D. (2011). Understanding children’s emotional processes and behavioral strategies in the context of marital conflict. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(3), 336352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovacs, M., Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Emotion (dys) regulation and links to depressive disorders. Child Development Perspectives, 2(3), 149155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krause, E. D., Mendelson, T., & Lynch, T. R. (2003). Childhood emotional invalidation and adult psychological distress: The mediating role of emotional inhibition. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(2), 199213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroes, G., Veerman, J. W., & De Bruyn, E. E. J. (2003). Bias in parental reports? Maternal psychopathology and the reporting of problem behavior in clinic-referred children. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 19(3), 195203. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.19.3.195 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, B. (2004). Living with an elephant: Growing up with parental substance misuse. Child & Family Social Work, 9(2), 129140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambie, J. A., & Lindberg, A. (2016). The role of maternal emotional validation and invalidation on children’s emotional awareness. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (1982-), 62(2), 129157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landi, N., Montoya, J., Kober, H., Rutherford, H. J., Mencl, W. E., Worhunsky, P. D., & Mayes, L. C. (2011). Maternal neural responses to infant cries and faces: Relationships with substance use. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2, 32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavi, I., Katz, L. F., Ozer, E. J., & Gross, J. J. (2019). Emotion reactivity and regulation in maltreated children: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 90(5), 15031524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (1989). Cognitive-emotional interactions in the brain. Cognition & Emotion, 3(4), 267289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leerkes, E. M., Nayena Blankson, A., & O’Brien, M. (2009). Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning. Child Development, 80(3), 762775. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01296.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leerkes, E. M., Weaver, J. M., & O’Brien, M. (2012). Differentiating maternal sensitivity to infant distress and non-distress. Parenting, 12(2-3), 175184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leijdesdorff, S., van Doesum, K., Popma, A., Klaassen, R., & van Amelsvoort, T. (2017). Prevalence of psychopathology in children of parents with mental illness and/or addiction: An up to date narrative review. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 30(4), 312317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, J., & Lomax, R. G. (2017). Effects of missing data methods in SEM under conditions of incomplete and nonnormal data. The Journal of Experimental Education, 85(2), 231258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, B., Kaliush, P. R., Conradt, E., Terrell, S., Neff, D., Allen, A. K., Smid, M. C., Monk, C., & Crowell, S. E. (2019). Intergenerational transmission of emotion dysregulation: Part I. Psychopathology, self-injury, and parasympathetic responsivity among pregnant women. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 817831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lincoln, C. R., Russell, B. S., Donohue, E. B., & Racine, L. E. (2017). Mother-child interactions and preschoolers’ emotion regulation outcomes: Nurturing autonomous emotion regulation. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(2), 559573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindblom, J., Punamäki, R.-L., Flykt, M., Vänskä, M., Nummi, T., Sinkkonen, J., Tiitinen, A., & Tulppala, M. (2016). Early family relationships predict children’s emotion regulation and defense mechanisms. Sage Open, 6(4), 2158244016681393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, C., & Carter, A. S. (2005). Negative emotional reactivity and regulation in 12-month-olds following emotional challenge: Contributions of maternal-infant emotional availability in a low-income sample. Infant Mental Health Journal, 26(4), 354368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, R. J. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 11981202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litz, B. T., Schlenger, W. E., Weathers, F. W., Caddell, J. M., Fairbank, J. A., & LaVange, L. M. (1997). Predictors of emotional numbing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 10(4), 607618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lohaus, A., Rueth, J. E., & Vierhaus, M. (2020). Cross-Informant discrepancies and their association with maternal depression, maternal parenting stress, and mother-child relationship. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 29, 867879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowell, A. F., Maupin, A. N., Landi, N., Potenza, M. N., Mayes, L. C., & Rutherford, H. J. (2020). Substance use and mothers’ neural responses to infant cues. Infant Mental Health Journal, 41(2), 264277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons-Ruth, K., Bureau, J. F., Easterbrooks, M. A., Obsuth, I., Hennighausen, K., & Vulliez-Coady, L. (2016). Parsing the construct of maternal insensitivity: Distinct longitudinal pathways associated with early maternal withdrawal. In Maternal sensitivity (1st ed. pp.120140). Routledge.Google Scholar
Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D. B., Zoll, D., & Stahl, J. (1987). Infants at social risk: Relations among infant maltreatment, maternal behavior, and infant attachment behavior. Developmental Psychology, 23(2), 223232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.2.223 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (1994). The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview. In Parke, R. D., Ornstein, P. A., Rieser, J. J., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (Ed.), A century of developmental psychology (pp. 589615). American Psychological Association, https://doi.org/10.1037/10155-021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malatesta, C. Z., & Haviland, J. M. (1982). Learning display rules: The socialization of emotion expression in infancy. Child Development, 53(4), 9911003. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129139 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mäntymaa, M., Puura, K., Luoma, I., Latva, R., Salmelin, R. K., & Tamminen, T. (2012). Predicting internalizing and externalizing problems at five years by child andparental factors in infancy and toddlerhood. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 43(2), 153170. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-011-0255-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchand-Reilly, J. F., & Yaure, R. G. (2019). The role of parents’ relationship quality in children’s behavior problems. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28(8), 21992208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. E., & Ochsner, K. N. (2016). The neuroscience of emotion regulation development: Implications for education. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 142148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maughan, A., Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L., & Rogosch, F. A. (2007). Early-occurring maternal depression and maternal negativity in predicting young children’s emotion regulation and socioemotional difficulties. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(5), 685703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mauss, I. B., Levenson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion, 5(2), 175190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayes, L., & Truman, S. (2002). Substance abuse and parenting. Handbook of Parenting, 4(2), 329360.Google Scholar
McClain, C. M., Younginer, S. T., & Elledge, L. C. (2020). Social risk and internalizing distress in middle childhood: The moderating role of emotion regulation processes. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 29(1), 167181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McElwain, N. L., & Booth-LaForce, C. (2006). Maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress as predictors of infant-mother attachment security. Journal of Family Psychology, 20(2), 247255. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.20.2.247 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McHugh, R. K., Reynolds, E. K., Leyro, T. M., & Otto, M. W. (2013). An examination of the association of distress intolerance and emotion regulation with avoidance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37(2), 363367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Mennin, D. S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Emotion dysregulation and adolescent psychopathology: A prospective study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(9), 544554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., & Lambert, H. K. (2017). Child trauma exposure and psychopathology: Mechanisms of risk and resilience. Current Opinion in Psychology, 14, 2934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeish, D. (2018). Thanks coefficient alpha, we’ll take it from here. Psychological Methods, 23(3), 412433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McRae, K., Gross, J. J., Weber, J., Robertson, E. R., Sokol-Hessner, P., Ray, R. D., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The development of emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal in children, adolescents and young adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 1122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melby, J. N., & Conger, R. D. (2001). The Iowa family interaction rating scales: Instrument summary. In Kerig, P. K., & Lindahl, K. M. (Ed.), Family observation coding systems (pp. 3358). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Merydith, S. P., Prout, H. T., & Blaha, J. (2003). Social desirability and behavior rating scales: An exploratory study with the child behavior checklist/4-18. Psychology in the Schools, 40(2), 225235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meulewaeter, F., De Pauw, S. S., & Vanderplasschen, W. (2019). Mothering, substance use disorders and intergenerational trauma transmission: An attachment-based perspective. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, S., Raikes, H. A., Virmani, E. A., Waters, S., & Thompson, R. A. (2014). Parent emotion representations and the socialization of emotion regulation in the family. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(2), 164173. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413519014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirabile, S. P., Scaramella, L. V., Sohr-Preston, S. L., & Robison, S. D. (2009). Mothers’ socialization of emotion regulation: The moderating role of children’s negative emotional reactivity. In Child & youth care forum. (vol. 38, pp. 1937). Springer US.Google Scholar
Miu, A. C., Szentágotai-Tătar, A., Balazsi, R., Nechita, D., Bunea, I., & Pollak, S. D. (2022). Emotion regulation as mediator between childhood adversity and psychopathology: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 93, 102141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moens, M. A., Weeland, J., Van der Giessen, D., Chhangur, R. R., & Overbeek, G. (2018). In the eye of the beholder? Parent-observer discrepancies in parenting and child disruptive behavior assessments. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(6), 11471159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molitor, A., Mayes, L., & Ward, A. (2003). Emotion regulation behavior during a separation procedure in 18-month-old children of mothers using cocaine and other drugs. Development and Psychopathology, 15(1), 3954. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579403000038 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, S. A., Zoellner, L. A., & Mollenholt, N. (2008). Are expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal associated with stress-related symptoms? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(9), 9931000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, J. K., Izard, C. E., & Hyde, C. (2014). Emotional reactivity and regulation in head start children: Links to ecologically valid behaviors and internalizing problems. Social Development, 23(2), 250266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moriguchi, Y., & Hiraki, K. (2013). Prefrontal cortex and executive function in young children: A review of NIRS studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, A. S., Criss, M. M., Silk, J. S., & Houltberg, B. J. (2017). The impact of parenting on emotion regulation during childhood and adolescence. Child Development Perspectives, 11(4), 233238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 361388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morsbach, S. K., & Prinz, R. J. (2006). Understanding and improving the validity of self-report of parenting. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 9, 121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulia, N., & Bensley, K. M. (2020). Alcohol-related disparities among women: Evidence and potential explanations. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews, 40(2), 09. https://doi.org/10.35946/arcr.v40.2.09 Google ScholarPubMed
Nelson, B. W., Allen, N. B., & Laurent, H. (2018). Infant HPA axis as a potential mechanism linking maternal mental health and infant telomere length. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 88, 3846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, J. A., & Perry, N. B. (2015). Emotional reactivity, self-control and children’s hostile attributions over middle childhood. Cognition and Emotion, 29(4), 592603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niedenthal, P. M., & Ric, F. (2017). Psychology of emotion. Psychology Press, https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315276229 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nock, M. K., Wedig, M. M., Holmberg, E. B., & Hooley, J. M. (2008). The emotion reactivity scale: Development, evaluation, and relation to self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Behavior Therapy, 39(2), 107116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norcross, P. L., Leerkes, E. M., & Zhou, N. (2017). Examining pathways linking maternal depressive symptoms in infancy to children’s behavior problems: The role of maternal unresponsiveness and negative behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 49, 238247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noroña-Zhou, A. N., & Tung, I. (2021). Developmental patterns of emotion regulation in toddlerhood: Examining predictors of change and long-term resilience. Infant Mental Health Journal, 42(1), 520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ordway, M. R. (2011). Depressed mothers as informants on child behavior: Methodological issues. Research in Nursing & Health, 34(6), 520532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Özdel, K., & Ekinci, S. (2014). Distress intolerance in substance dependent patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55(4), 960965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pala, F. C., & Lewis, C. (2021). Do preschoolers grasp the importance of regulating emotional expression? European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18(4), 494519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paley, B., & Hajal, N. J. (2022). Conceptualizing emotion regulation and coregulation as family-level phenomena. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 25(1), 1943. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-022-00378-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panayiotou, G., Panteli, M., & Vlemincx, E. (2021). Adaptive and maladaptive emotion processing and regulation, and the case of alexithymia. Cognition and Emotion, 35(3), 488499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panlilio, C. C., Harring, J. R., Harden, B. J., Morrison, C. I., & Duncan, A. D. (2020). Heterogeneity in the dynamic arousal and modulation of fear in young foster children. Children and Youth Services Review, 116, 105199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parent, J., Forehand, R., Dunbar, J. P., Watson, K. H., Reising, M. M., Seehuus, M., & Compas, B. E. (2014). Parent and adolescent reports of parenting when a parent has a history of depression: Associations with observations of parenting. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 173183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paschke, L. M., Dörfel, D., Steimke, R., Trempler, I., Magrabi, A., Ludwig, V. U., Schubert, T., Stelzel, C., & Walter, H. (2016). Individual differences in self-reported self-control predict successful emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(8), 11931204. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw036 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulhus, D. L. (2002). Socially desirable responding: The evolution of a construct. In Braun, H. I., Jackson, D. N., & Wiley, D. E. (Ed.), The role of constructs in psychological and educational measurement (pp. 4969). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Peters, B. J., Overall, N. C., & Jamieson, J. P. (2014). Physiological and cognitive consequences of suppressing and expressing emotion in dyadic interactions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 94(1), 100107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picoito, J., Santos, C., & Nunes, C. (2021). Heterogeneity and heterotypic continuity of emotional and behavioural profiles across development. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 56(5), 807819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitskel, N. B., Bolling, D. Z., Kaiser, M. D., Crowley, M. J., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2011). How grossed out are you? The neural bases of emotion regulation from childhood to adolescence. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 324337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ponzetti, S., Spinelli, M., Coppola, G., Lionetti, F., D’Urso, G., Shah, P., & Aureli, T. (2023). Emotion regulation in toddlerhood: Regulatory strategies in anger and fear eliciting contexts at 24 and 30 months. Children, 10(5), 878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porreca, A., Biringen, Z., Parolin, M., Saunders, H., Ballarotto, G., & Simonelli, A. (2018). Emotional availability, neuropsychological functioning, and psychopathology: The context of parental substance use disorder. BioMed Research International, 2018, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Punamäki, R. L., Flykt, M., Belt, R., & Lindblom, J. (2021). Maternal substance use disorder predicting children’s emotion regulation in middle childhood: The role of early mother-infant interaction. Heliyon, 7(4), e06728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Development of short and very short forms of the children’s behavior questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87(1), 102112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qualter, P., & Munn, P. (2002). The separateness of social and emotional loneliness in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(2), 233244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roben, C. K. P., Cole, P. M., & Armstrong, L. M. (2013). Longitudinal relations among language skills, anger expression, and regulatory strategies in early childhood. Child Development, 84(3), 891905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, W. L., & Strayer, J. (1987). Parents’ responses to the emotional distress of their children: Relations with children’s competence. Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 415422. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.3.415 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robins, L., Cottler, L., Bucholz, K., & Compton, W. (1995). Diagnostic interview schedule for DSM-IV (DIS-IV). Washington University School of Medicine.Google Scholar
Robinson, L. R., Morris, A. S., Heller, S. S., Scheeringa, M. S., Boris, N. W., & Smyke, A. T. (2009). Relations between emotion regulation, parenting, and psychopathology in young maltreated children in out of home care. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18(4), 421434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, M. L., Halberstadt, A. G., Castro, V. L., MacCormack, J. K., & Garrett-Peters, P. (2016). Maternal emotion socialization differentially predicts third-grade children’s emotion regulation and lability. Emotion, 16(2), 280291. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000142 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, R. D., & Robbins, T. W. (2001). Investigating the neurocognitive deficits associated with chronic drug misuse. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11(2), 250257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogier, G., Garofalo, C., & Velotti, P. (2019). Is emotional suppression always bad? A matter of flexibility and gender differences. Current Psychology, 38(2), 411420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romanowicz, M., Vande Voort, J. L., Shekunov, J., Oesterle, T. S., Thusius, N. J., Rummans, T. A., Croarkin, P. E., Karpyak, V. M., Lynch, B. A., & Schak, K. M. (2019). The effects of parental opioid use on the parent-child relationship and children’s developmental and behavioral outcomes: A systematic review of published reports. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 13(1), 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roque, L., Veríssimo, M., Fernandes, M., & Rebelo, A. (2013). Emotion regulation and attachment: Relationships with children’s secure base, during different situational and social contexts in naturalistic settings. Infant Behavior and Development, 36(3), 298306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossen, L., Mattick, R. P., Wilson, J., Clare, P. J., Burns, L., Allsop, S., Elliott, E. J., Jacobs, S., Olsson, C. A., & Hutchinson, D. (2019). Mother-infant bonding and emotional availability at 12-months of age: The role of early postnatal bonding, maternal substance use and mental health. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 23(12), 16861698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutherford, H. J., Booth, C. R., Luyten, P., Bridgett, D. J., & Mayes, L. C. (2015). Investigating the association between parental reflective functioning and distress tolerance in motherhood. Infant Behavior and Development, 40, 5463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutherford, H. J., & Mayes, L. C. (2017). Parenting and addiction: Neurobiological insights. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 5560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutherford, H. J., Yip, S. W., Worhunsky, P. D., Kim, S., Strathearn, L., Potenza, M. N., & Mayes, L. C. (2020). Differential responses to infant faces in relation to maternal substance use: An exploratory study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 207, 107805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchis-Sanchis, A., Grau, M. D., Moliner, A. R., & Morales-Murillo, C. P. (2020). Effects of age and gender in emotion regulation of children and adolescents. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 946. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00946 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanders, W., Zeman, J., Poon, J., & Miller, R. (2015). Child regulation of negative emotions and depressive symptoms: The moderating role of parental emotion socialization. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(2), 402415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, K. G., Hempel, R. J., & Lynch, T. R. (2013). That, poker face, just might lose you the game! the impact of expressive suppression and mimicry on sensitivity to facial expressions of emotion. Emotion, 13(5), 852866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seddon, J. A., Abdel-Baki, R., Feige, S., & Thomassin, K. (2020). The cascade effect of parent dysfunction: An emotion socialization transmission framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 579519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K., Reeder, G. D., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). The self-serving bias in relational context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 378386. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.378 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segal, D. L. (2010). Diagnostic interview schedule for DSM-IV (DIS-IV). In The corsini encyclopedia of psychology (pp. 12). Wiley.Google Scholar
Selig, J. P., & Preacher, K. J.Monte Carlo method for assessing mediation: An interactive tool for creating confidence intervals for indirect effects [Computer software] 2008).Google Scholar
Seligowski, A. V., Lee, D. J., Bardeen, J. R., & Orcutt, H. K. (2015). Emotion regulation and posttraumatic stress symptoms: A meta-analysis. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 44(2), 87102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serrat, E., Amadó, A., Rostan, C., Caparrós, B., & Sidera, F. (2020). Identifying emotional expressions: Children’s reasoning about pretend emotions of sadness and anger. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 602385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sessa, F. M., Avenevoli, S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2001). Correspondence among informants on parenting: Preschool children, mothers, and observers. Journal of Family Psychology, 15(1), 5368. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.15.1.53 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shadur, J. M., & Hussong, A. M. (2020). Maternal substance use and child emotion regulation: The mediating role of parent emotion socialization. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 29(6), 15891603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaffer, A., Suveg, C., Thomassin, K., & Bradbury, L. L. (2012). Emotion socialization in the context of family risks: Links to child emotion regulation. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 21(6), 917924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapero, B. G., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Emotional reactivity and internalizing symptoms: Moderating role of emotion regulation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40(30), 328340. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9722-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapero, B. G., Dale, C. F., Archibald, A. R., & Pedrelli, P. (2018). Emotional reactivity. In Levesque, R. J. R. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of adolescence. Springer.Google Scholar
Shapero, B. G., Farabaugh, A., Terechina, O., DeCross, S., Cheung, J. C., Fava, M., & Holt, D. J. (2019). Understanding the effects of emotional reactivity on depression and suicidal thoughts and behaviors: Moderating effects of childhood adversity and resilience. Journal of Affective Disorders, 245, 419427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shelton, K. K., Frick, P. J., & Wootton, J. (1996). Assessment of parenting practices in families of elementary school-age children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25(3), 317329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shenk, C. E., & Fruzzetti, A. E. (2011). The impact of validating and invalidating responses on emotional reactivity. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30(2), 163183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheppes, G., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11(1), 379405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among maltreated children: The contributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27(4), 381395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiller, V. M., Izard, C. E., & Hembree, E. A. (1986). Patterns of emotion expression during separation in the strange-situation procedure. Developmental Psychology, 22(3), 378382. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.3.378 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shipman, K. L., Schneider, R., Fitzgerald, M. M., Sims, C., Swisher, L., & Edwards, A. (2007). Maternal emotion socialization in maltreating and non-maltreating families: Implications for children’s emotion regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 268285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidera, F., Amadó, A., & Serrat, E. (2013). Are you really happy? Children’s understanding of real vs. pretend emotions. Current Psychology, 32(1), 1831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Somerville, L. H., & Casey, B. J. (2014). Emotional reactivity and regulation across development. In Gazzaniga, M. S., & Mangun, G. R. (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 731739). Boston Review. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9504.003.0081 Google Scholar
Soto, J. A., Perez, C. R., Kim, Y. H., Lee, E. A., & Minnick, M. R. (2011). Is expressive suppression always associated with poorer psychological functioning? A cross-cultural comparison between european Americans and Hong Kong chinese. Emotion, 11(6), 14501455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stack, D. M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (1998). Socioemotional and cognitive competence in infancy. In Pushkar, D., Bukowski, W. M., Schwartzman, A. E., & Stack, D. M., White, D. R. (Ed.), Improving competence across the lifespan: Building interventions based on theory and research (pp. 3757). Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Stack, J., & Lewis, C. (2008). Steering towards a developmental account of infant social understanding. Human Development, 51(4), 229234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stellern, J., Xiao, K. B., Grennell, E., Sanches, M., Gowin, J. L., & Sloan, M. E. (2022). Emotion regulation in substance use disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Addiction.Google ScholarPubMed
Stifter, C. A., Willoughby, M. T., & Towe-Goodman, N. (2008). Agree or agree to disagree? Assessing the convergence between parents and observers on infant temperament. Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 17(4), 407426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., Cicchetti, D., & Manning, L. G. (2012). Interparental violence, maternal emotional unavailability and children’s cortisol functioning in family contexts. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 237249. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025419 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Skibo, M. A., Rogosch, F. A., Ignjatovic, Z., & Heinzelman, W. (2011). The impact of allostatic load on maternal sympathovagal functioning in stressful child contexts: Implications for problematic parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 23(3), 831844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, J. C., Lee, R. M., & Oishi, S. (2013). The role of culture and self-construal in the link between expressive suppression and depressive symptoms. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44(2), 316331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, M., & Lau, A. S. (2018). Exploring cultural differences in expressive suppression and emotion recognition. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 49(4), 664672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suveg, C., Shaffer, A., Morelen, D., & Thomassin, K. (2011). Links between maternal and child psychopathology symptoms: Mediation through child emotion regulation and moderation through maternal behavior. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 42(5), 507520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suveg, C., & Zeman, J. (2004). Emotion regulation in children with anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(4), 7507504 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taber, S. M. (2010). The veridicality of children’s reports of parenting: A review of factors contributing to parent-child discrepancies. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(8), 9991010. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.06.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabet, M., Flick, L. H., Cook, C. A., Xian, H., & Chang, J. J. (2016). Age at first birth and psychiatric disorders in low-income pregnant women. Journal of Women’s Health, 25(8), 810817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, P. Z., Oppenheimer, C. W., Ladouceur, C. D., Butterfield, R. D., & Silk, J. S. (2020). A review of associations between parental emotion socialization behaviors and the neural substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in youth. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 516527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. A. (1991). Emotional regulation and emotional development. Educational Psychology Review, 3(4), 269307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. A., & Goodman, M. (2010). Development of emotion regulation: More than meets the eye. In Kring, A. M., & Sloan, D. M. (Ed.), Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment (pp. 3858). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A., & Meyer, S. (2007). Socialization of emotion regulation in the family. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 249268). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Todd, R. M., Cunningham, W. A., Anderson, A. K., & Thompson, E. (2012). Affect-biased attention as emotion regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(7), 365372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treutler, C. M., & Epkins, C. C. (2003). Are discrepancies among child, mother, and father reports on children’s behavior related to parents’ psychological symptoms and aspects of parent-child relationships? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(1), 1327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troy, A. S., Shallcross, A. J., & Mauss, I. B. (2013). A person-by-situation approach to emotion regulation: Cognitive reappraisal can either help or hurt, depending on the context. Psychological Science, 24(12), 25052514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troy, A. S., Willroth, E. C., Shallcross, A. J., Giuliani, N. R., Gross, J. J., & Mauss, I. B. (2023). Psychological resilience: An affect-regulation framework. Annual Review of Psychology, 74(1), 547576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trudel-Fitzgerald, C., & Ouellet-Morin, I. (2022). The cost of resilience: How allostatic load may jeopardize health through repeated demands for (successful) adaptation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 144, 105874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105874 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, B. J., Chapman, A. L., & Layden, B. K. (2012). Intrapersonal and interpersonal functions of non suicidal self-injury: Associations with emotional and social functioning. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 42(1), 3655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ursache, A., Blair, C., Stifter, C., & Voegtline, K. (2013). Emotional reactivity and regulation in infancy interact to predict executive functioning in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 127137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Pol, L. D., Groeneveld, M. G., Endendijk, J. J., van Berkel, S. R., Hallers-Haalboom, E. T., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Mesman, J. (2016). Associations between fathers’ and mothers’ psychopathology symptoms, parental emotion socialization, and preschoolers’ social-emotional development. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25(11), 33673380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velleman, R., & Templeton, L. (2007). Understanding and modifying the impact of parents’ substance misuse on children. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 13(2), 7989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velotti, P., Garofalo, C., Bottazzi, F., & Caretti, V. (2017). Faces of shame: Implications for self-esteem, emotion regulation, aggression, and well-being. The Journal of Psychology, 151(2), 171184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. L., Notaro, P. C., & Herrera, C. (2002). Parents’ emotional availability and infant emotional competence: Predictors of parent-infant attachment and emerging self-regulation. Journal of Family Psychology, 16(4), 447465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walle, E. A., & Campos, J. J. (2014). The development of infant detection of inauthentic emotion. Emotion, 14(3), 488503. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035305 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, D. G., Bitran, D., Miller, A. B., Schaefer, J. D., Sheridan, M. A., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2019). Difficulties with emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic mechanism linking child maltreatment with the emergence of psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 899915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, M. M., Wickramaratne, P., Warner, V., John, K., Prusoff, B. A., Merikangas, K. R., & Gammon, G. D. (1987). Assessing psychiatric disorders in children: Discrepancies between mothers’ and children’s reports. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44(8), 747753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, S. G., Taylor, A. B., & Wu, W. (2012). Model fit and model selection in structural equation modeling. Handbook of Structural Equation Modeling, 1, 209231.Google Scholar
Williams, D. R. (2018). Stress and the mental health of populations of color: Advancing our understanding of race-related stressors. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 59(4), 466485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeman, J., Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Stegall, S. (2006). Emotion regulation in children and adolescents. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), 155168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeman, J., & Garber, J. (1996). Display rules for anger, sadness, and pain: It depends on who is watching. Child Development, 67(3), 957973.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2016). The development of coping: Implications for psychopathology and resilience. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Developmental Psychopathology. Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmermann, L. K., & Stansbury, K. (2003). The influence of temperamental reactivity and situational context on the emotion-regulatory abilities of 3-year-old children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164(4), 389409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Parameterization of latent difference score modeling for change in maternal insensitivity to children’s emotional distress from wave 1 to wave 2.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Parameterization of latent difference score modeling of change in children’s emotional reactivity from wave 2 to wave 3.

Figure 2

Table 1. Frequency distribution of mothers’ responses on the DIS-IV alcohol dependence symptom index

Figure 3

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations for study’s primary variables

Figure 4

Figure 3. Structural equation model examining longitudinal associations between sociodemographic variables, maternal psychopathology symptoms, maternal alcohol dependence symptoms, change in maternal insensitivity to children’s distress, and change in children’s emotional reactivity. Dashed lines indicate non-significant pathways. W1 = wave 1; W2 = wave 2; W3 = wave 3. * p < .05.