Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T17:54:26.027Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What do we know about religion and interreligious peace? A review of the quantitative literature

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 September 2023

Julia Köbrich
Affiliation:
German Institute for Global and Area Studies, Hamburg, Germany Ercomer, Interdisciplinary Social Science, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Lisa Hoffmann*
Affiliation:
German Institute for Global and Area Studies, Hamburg, Germany
*
Corresponding author: Lisa Hoffmann; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Interreligious relations remain an important dimension of human coexistence and we currently observe an increase in religiously motivated violence and discrimination. Hence, we need to better understand determinants of interreligious peace. Building on a new concept of interreligious peace which includes but exceeds the absence of interreligious physical violence, we provide a systematic review of 83 quantitative empirical studies examining religious determinants of interreligious physical violence, hostile attitudes, threat perceptions, trust, and cooperation. We find that religious ideas foster or hinder interreligious peace depending on their content. Religious identities have negative effects but must be considered in context. Evidence regarding the role of religious practice is mixed and the role of religious actors and institutions remains understudied. Our results show the need for (1) more conceptual clarity, (2) replications in different contexts, (3) research on dimensions of religion beyond identities, and (4) a better integration of different strands of literature.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Religion and Politics Section of the American Political Science Association

Contrary to secularization theory, the influence of religion on societies worldwide continues to be strong (Toft et al., Reference Toft, Philpott and Shah2011; Fox, Reference Fox2018). In recent decades, there has been an increase in religiously motivated violence—for example, Svensson and Nilsson (Reference Svensson and Nilsson2018) observed an increase in violent conflict over religious issues from only 3% in 1975 to 55% in 2015. These developments make the study of determinants of interreligious peace an important object for research.

Current literature on religion, peace, and conflict, however, is mostly concerned with the study of violent conflict and the role of religion in peacebuilding processes (for an overview, see, e.g., Svensson, Reference Svensson and Yamane2016). Interreligious peace has scarcely been studied, despite a growing body of empirical work investigating interreligious physical violence (Basedau et al., Reference Basedau, Pfeiffer and Vüllers2016; Kim and Choi, Reference Kim and Choi2017). There are three main implications: (1) it remains underspecified what interreligious peace entails; (2) empirical evidence on potential drivers of interreligious peace is currently scattered across different literatures; and (3) quantitative evidence allowing for generalizations beyond case-study approaches is scarce.

It may be argued that examining how to prevent interreligious violent conflict can help us to foster interreligious peace. However, this assumes that interreligious peace entails only the absence of interreligious physical violence. In line with ongoing debates around conceptualizations of peace (e.g., Söderström et al., Reference Söderström, Åkebo and Jarstad2021), we argue that interreligious peace includes but exceeds the absence of interreligious physical violence. To evaluate what we know about religious determinants of interreligious peace, we compile evidence from the literature on the absence of interreligious physical violence, the absence of hostile attitudes, and threat perceptions between religious groups as well as interreligious cooperation and trust.

Our literature review covers quantitative studies investigating the effect of religious factors on elements of interreligious peace published after 2000 as peer-reviewed journal articles. Generally, we find that religious ideas foster or hinder interreligious peace depending on their content. Religious identities mostly have negative effects but must be considered in context, and religious practices may signal trustworthiness, but participation in such practices does not impact a person's attitudes relevant to interreligious peace. The role of religious actors or institutions remains understudied, but they may have positive effects on interreligious peace.

In the following, we, first propose a conceptualization of interreligious peace, enabling empirical investigations of interreligious peace. Second, we compile and evaluate evidence on interreligious peace currently scattered across disciplines. Third, we highlight research gaps and outline ways forward for future research.

Conceptual and theoretical considerations

How do we understand interreligious peace?Footnote 1

We consider interreligious peace to be relational (Söderström et al., Reference Söderström, Åkebo and Jarstad2021), not bound by location and existing in the relationships between religious institutions, groups, and individuals. Within a given space, interreligious peace may exist between some religious groups but not others. For example, a religious group may coexist in a peaceful relationship with one religious group in a country, while it may be simultaneously engaged in an armed conflict with another within the same country. Interreligious peace extends to relationships between major religious groups like Christians and Muslims, but also between denominations like Catholics and Protestants as well as religious and explicitly non-religious groups like atheists or agnostics.

In congruence with debates around general peace, we argue that interreligious peace includes but exceeds the absence of physical violence as this is more instrumental to describe variations in interreligious relations. As others, we contend that peace as the absence of physical violence is blind to nuances of empirical realities (e.g., Goertz et al., Reference Goertz, Diehl and Balas2016; Ossai, Reference Ossai2020). The absence of physical violence is merely the smallest common denominator of what peace entails and many ideas coexist regarding what peace entails beyond the absence of violence (Davenport et al., Reference Davenport, Melander and Regan2018). Our concept builds on previous literature, particularly relational peace concepts (Kasten, Reference Kasten2017; Söderström et al., Reference Söderström, Åkebo and Jarstad2021) and is closely related to discussions around (religious) pluralisms (Joustra, Reference Joustra2020). In contrast to pluralism debates focusing on processes that enable the coexistence in unity within diversity, we focus on defining what the end-state entails. Further, we extend existing concepts of relational peace by reducing the number of constitutive factors and by referring particularly to relations between religions. Overall, we aim to formulate a definition that is concise, recognizes that peace is more than the absence of physical violence, does not prescribe a specific institutional form, and can be operationalized for qualitative and quantitative empirical investigations.

We differentiate between four elements of interreligious peace depicted in Table 1. Negative interreligious peace describes aspects that need to be absent, whereas positive interreligious peace refers to elements that are required in peaceful interreligious relations. Additionally, we maintain that behavioral and attitudinal dimensions inform relationships in line with other concepts of relational peace. Based on these reflections, we define interreligious peace as the absence of physical violence, hostile attitudes, and mutually perceived threat, as well as the presence of trust and cooperation in interreligious relations. Hereby, physical violence refers to physical harm that may take various forms and is perpetrated by adherents of one religious group against adherents of another. Hostile attitudes are hateful, aggressive, or harm-wishing attitudes that can be considered the mental equivalent of physical violence while mutually perceived threats imply that damage or hurt is expected to emanate from the other. On the side of positive interreligious peace, trust entails that actors hold optimistic expectations about the behavior of religious others toward them and their religious group. Cooperation refers to actions undertaken by individuals of different religions to work together toward a common goal.

Table 1. Dimensions of positive and negative interreligious peace

How do we understand religion?

Like peace, we conceive of religion as a multidimensional concept. We understand religion as a set of interrelated ideas in which supernatural factors explain the origin and functioning of the world and provide the meaning of life (Basedau et al., Reference Basedau, Gobien and Prediger2018). Four real-world dimensions of religion seem particularly relevant for the analysis of its role for interreligious peace: religious ideas, religious identity groups, religious practices as well as religious actors and institutions (see Basedau et al., Reference Basedau, Gobien and Prediger2018). Religious ideas comprise, for example, beliefs, values, formal and informal norms, or ad hoc interpretations in relation to desired social conduct. Once people share a belief and differ from others in that regard, they form groups and are hence subject to dynamics between (religious) identity groups. Religious practices refer to specifically religious behavior like worshipping, praying, following specific dietary rules, or making pilgrimages. Religious actors and institutions include individuals like clerics, organizations of individual religious groups or coalitions of several religious groups as well as state or international institutions that regulate relations between religious groups, and between the state and religious groups or faith in general.

Literature review

Our literature review covers quantitative studies investigating the effect of religious factors on elements of interreligious peace published after 2000 as peer-reviewed journal articles.Footnote 2 While these criteria restrict our understanding of the literature, they are necessary to delimit our work. It is unlikely that substantial research is excluded by focusing on research published after 2000 since only nine of the 83 reviewed studies were published prior to 2010, none prior to 2005. Our multidimensional understandings of religion and interreligious peace provided us with a structure for our search for literature and for the presentation of our review below. We searched for and grouped studies according to outcome variables reflecting dimensions of interreligious peace. We initially identified literature through keyword searches in large scientific databases. In a second step, we surveyed literature cited by and literature citing a relevant study. We included empirical journal articles that use statistical methods for analyses, that is, quantitative studies, that examine relations between an independent variable measuring a dimension of religion, that is, religious ideas, identities, practices, actors, and institutions, and a dependent variable measuring an element of interreligious peace, that is, interreligious physical violence, hostile attitudes, threat perceptions, cooperation, and trust.

These parameters imply that we exclude literature on the role of religious actors in peacebuilding processes like mediation processes (e.g., Johnstone and Svensson, Reference Johnstone and Svensson2013) as well as the role of religion for non-violent protest movements (Svensson, Reference Svensson and Yamane2016). Peacebuilding and non-violent protests are conceptually distinct from interreligious peace as our key dependent variable of interest. Moreover, excellent reviews on terrorism research and radicalization processes exist (e.g., Schuurman, Reference Schuurman2019; Vergani et al., Reference Vergani, Iqbal, Ilbahar and Barton2020) and we only include terrorism studies if they clearly focus on interreligious violence, thereby excluding (1) literature that does not distinguish between the religious identity of perpetrator and target and (2) studies that focus on terrorism more generally (fewer than one-third of all identifiable terrorist attacks have a religious connotation, see Saiya and Scime, Reference Saiya and Scime2015). We do not cover literature on religion and prosociality per se (see Hoffmann, Reference Hoffmann2013, for an overview), but only review studies investigating aspects of prosociality relevant for our interreligious peace definition (i.e., trust and cooperation).

Methodological challenges of studying interreligious peace

In total, we review 83 studies (see Appendix A: Reviewed Literature). Based on the profile of the reviewed literature presented in Table 2, we identify several methodological challenges. First, the literature on (aspects of) interreligious peace is currently scattered across disciplines. Being mostly published in psychology (37), political science (17), and economics journals (13), discourses around the same social phenomenon usually take place in bubbles that do not cross-reference papers beyond their own discipline.

Table 2. Profile—literature on interreligious peacea

a Some articles discuss studies conducted in several countries, on multiple samples or employ different empirical strategies and are considered in two sub-sections of the literature review.

Second, availability of global and large N data remains limited. Only 10 reviewed studies focus on the global level. While there are quite a number of case studies, for example, on Northern Ireland (e.g., Balcells et al., Reference Balcells, Daniels and Escribà-Folch2016; Adida et al., Reference Adida, Brown, McCord and McLachlan2022) or on India (e.g., Field et al., Reference Field, Levinson, Pande and Visaria2008), studies on the global level remain the exception. Comparative designs and replications are important to better understand how context factors matter. Most studies were conducted in the United States (24), primarily on distrust against atheists. A large proportion of studies have been conducted on the Asian continent while particularly research on (aspects of) interreligious peace in South America is very rare.

Third, 66 of the reviewed studies focus their analysis on the individual level. While this is not a problem per se, taking also into consideration that most studies investigate their research question with a student sample, generalizations beyond the case studied become challenging. Many studies do not report how they sample their research participants or rely on a convenience sample of online respondents, which makes generalizations even more difficult.

Fourth, the empirical strategies employed differ substantially across studies and limit comparability: in the experimental literature, behavioral experiments such as the trust game or prisoner's dilemma experiments aim to measure behavioral differences (e.g., Chakravarty et al., Reference Chakravarty, Fonseca, Ghosh and Marjit2016). In contrast, studies employing survey designs often ask about attitudes (e.g., Asadullah, Reference Asadullah2017) while cross-country analyses focus, for example, on violent conflict events (e.g., Basedau et al., Reference Basedau, Pfeiffer and Vüllers2016). Thus, it is likely that studies tap into different aspects of a phenomenon. While converging findings across indicators may highlight their robustness, differences may result from different empirical approaches.

Dimensions of interreligious peace

In the following, we review the literature on each dimension of interreligious peace separately, that is, interreligious physical violence, hostile attitudes, and threat perceptions as well as interreligious trust and cooperation.

Interreligious physical violence

Theories on the influence of religion on conflict argue that religious conflicts are special and seem more intractable vis-à-vis other types of conflicts due to the belief in supernatural rewards and punishments (potentially also in an afterlife) and the indivisibility of religious claims (e.g., Hassner, Reference Hassner2003; Ellingsen, Reference Ellingsen2005). Religion can also be a driver of other types of conflict, such as ethnic conflict, for example, when threat perceptions are prevalent (Fox, Reference Fox2000). However, it is not clear to what extent it is the major cause of such conflict as other differences, such as linguistic differences have been shown to make intrastate conflict more likely as compared to religious differences (Bormann et al., Reference Bormann, Cederman and Vogt2017). In a process of religious outbidding, secular conflicts are sometimes reframed to religious ones (Toft, Reference Toft2013) and conflicts between believers of the same faith emerge to (re)define religious and political authority (Zellman and Brown, Reference Zellman and Brown2022). Similarly, religiosity may matter for conflict initiation (Alexander, Reference Alexander2017). While empirical studies find that religious conflicts last longer as compared to non-religious ones (e.g., Nilsson and Svensson, Reference Nilsson and Svensson2021; Deitch, Reference Deitch2022), results on the impact of religion on conflict termination, re-occurrence, and the level of violence are less clear (e.g., Toft, Reference Toft2021; Deitch, Reference Deitch2022). For this review, we explicitly focus on the link between religion and violent conflict between different religious communities, thereby excluding excellent papers which do not consider interreligious conflict as the dependent variable.

We identify 14 papers seeking to explore the relations between religion and interreligious physical violence. They are published in political science (12) and economics journals (2) and mainly based on secondary data. While some studies focus on the global level (e.g., Basedau et al., Reference Basedau, Fox, Pierskalla, Strüver and Vüllers2017; Kim and Choi, Reference Kim and Choi2017), others investigate case studies such as India (Field et al., Reference Field, Levinson, Pande and Visaria2008) or Northern Ireland (Adida et al., Reference Adida, Brown, McCord and McLachlan2022). Table A1 in the Appendix presents a profile of the papers covered.

What do we know about religion and interreligious physical violence? Existing theories and their empirical support are visualized in Table 3. Only one paper explicitly focuses on the impact of religious ideas on interreligious physical violence: Basedau et al. (Reference Basedau, Pfeiffer and Vüllers2016) theorize that religious elites promote specific ideas and thereby make them theologically justified, beneficial, or even imperative (A1). Considering 130 countries over the period 1990–2010, they find indeed evidence that calls for violence by religious elites are associated with increased interreligious conflict onset. However, the direction of causality is unclear as calls for violence could also result from experiencing previous violence (Isaacs, Reference Isaacs2016). Other, or more specific, religious ideas have not been tested.

Table 3. Interreligious physical violence: theory and empirical support

Studies on the impact of religious identity on interreligious conflict hypothesize that religious demographics affect the opportunity for collective action or intergroup competition (A2). For example, Balcells et al. (Reference Balcells, Daniels and Escribà-Folch2016) expect violence to be more likely when communities are of similar size (polarization) due to increased competition and threat perception. Basedau et al. (Reference Basedau, Strüver, Vüllers and Wegenast2011, Reference Basedau, Pfeiffer and Vüllers2016) expect religious fractionalization to reduce and dominance of one religious group as well as religious polarization to increase collective action and thus, interreligious physical violence. Other authors suggest more complex, for example, curvilinear, relationships between religious demographics and violence (Klašnja and Novta, Reference Klašnja and Novta2016; Adida et al., Reference Adida, Brown, McCord and McLachlan2022).

Many empirical studies find that religious demographics matter for interreligious violence (see A2, Table 3). However, the exact form of this association remains unclear as, for example, Adida et al. (Reference Adida, Brown, McCord and McLachlan2022) find the relationship between diversity and violence to be curvilinear, with the steepest increase when diversity increases from very low-to-medium levels, supporting theories of group threat through exposure. In their example from Northern Ireland, Field et al. (Reference Field, Levinson, Pande and Visaria2008) show that historical conditions (in this case, tenancy rights) can shape segregation and religious demographics. Thus, other context factors (such as political, economic, and historical conditions) seem crucial in shaping how religious identities matter for interreligious violence.

Basedau et al. (Reference Basedau, Strüver, Vüllers and Wegenast2011) find that particularly in environments of pre-existing tensions between or discrimination against religious communities, religious polarization goes along with interreligious physical violence. Theoretically, identities running parallel (“overlapping identities”), for example, religious communities identifying with the same ethnicity (or another relevant identity group) can increase violence toward (non-overlapping) outgroups as they facilitate the opportunity for collective action (Basedau et al., Reference Basedau, Strüver, Vüllers and Wegenast2011; Basedau et al., Reference Basedau, Pfeiffer and Vüllers2016). Empirical evidence supports this hypothesis, emphasizing the importance of religious identities when they run in parallel with other identity groups.

We could not identify literature examining the impact of religious practice on interreligious violence. When it comes to religious actors and institutions it is assumed that a politicization of religion makes violent conflict more likely due to power imbalances and/or a culture of intolerance (A4). At the same time, felt discrimination and marginalization are expected to increase the motivation for violent collective action (A5). Empirical results support these theories (but come from case studies only): for example, in their study on Nigeria, Bunte and Vinson (Reference Bunte and Vinson2016) find evidence that power-sharing of different religious groups reduces the number of interreligious violent events. In their case study on India, Dhattiwala and Biggs (Reference Dhattiwala and Biggs2012) find a strong association between electoral competition and interreligious violence between Hindus and Muslims. Grievances over discrimination are found to be (weakly) associated with interreligious conflict onset in one study (Basedau et al., Reference Basedau, Pfeiffer and Vüllers2016), but not in another (Basedau et al., Reference Basedau, Fox, Pierskalla, Strüver and Vüllers2017). In his model on competition over religious adherents where religious organizations are treated as rational firms, Isaacs (Reference Isaacs2017) expects religion to be more salient in conflicts when groups' religious organizations are fragmented (A6). He finds support for his hypothesis (see also Zellman and Brown, Reference Zellman and Brown2022).

Finally, the level of religious restrictions may matter (A6), see also Saiya (Reference Saiya2018) as well as Saiya and Manchanda (Reference Saiya and Manchanda2020): in their study on autocracies, Kim and Choi (Reference Kim and Choi2017) hypothesize that the probability of interreligious violence is particularly high when rulers enforce moderate restrictions on religion. They find evidence for their hypothesis as there is no possibility for violence when restrictions are tight and no reason when they are loose, supporting the idea of a non-linear relationship.

Taken together, these findings suggest that (1) religious ideas may affect interreligious violence both positively and negatively but empirical evidence is limited; (2) religious demographics impact interreligious violence, especially in interaction with other unfavorable conditions; (3) there is a lack of evidence on the association between religious practices and interreligious violence; and (4) the existence of religious institutions creating equality and power-sharing between religious groups; competition between religious groups; as well as religious restrictions matter for interreligious physical violence.

Hostile attitudes and threat perceptions between religious groups

Next, we review the literature on hostile attitudes and threat perceptions between religious groups. We include literature that aligns with our understanding of hostile attitudes as unfavorable, mostly emotionally charged attitudes toward another party such as aggression, the wish or intention to harm someone, excluding broader literature on discrimination and prejudice. We identify 22 papers investigating hostile attitudes (13) and threat perceptions (12) as dependent variables. The majority of them are published in psychology journals (12) (see Table A2 in the Appendix). All except for one study focus on the individual level and most papers investigate one specific country context. The reviewed studies mostly use survey designs (19), while three studies employ experimental approaches. Thus, statements about causality are—in many cases—not possible.

Table 4 summarizes some of the theories tested and their empirical support. Fundamentalist religious ideas are assumed to increase hostile attitudes toward other religious groups as they usually emphasize an exclusive truth-claim of religion (B1). This idea is supported across studies (see B1, Table 4) hinting at a certain robustness of this finding. We could not identify literature focusing on more inclusive religious ideas.

Table 4. Hostile attitudes and threat perceptions: theory and empirical support

Two studies test if religious identity impacts hostile attitudes: they find an association between the two if violent-prone ideas are prevalent and/or believers feel threatened by others, see B2 in Table 4. Furthermore, ingroup identification seems to increase hostile attitudes and threat perceptions (B3) due to intergroup biases as shown by several studies (see B3, Table 4). Drawing on case studies from France (Badea et al., Reference Badea, Bender and Korda2020) and the United States (Gerteis et al., Reference Gerteis, Hartmann and Edgell2020), it becomes evident that threat perceptions are particularly high if people believe that their national/political identity is tied to religion (B4). Studies by Kanas et al. (Reference Kanas, Scheepers and Sterkens2015, Reference Kanas, Scheepers and Sterkens2017) as well as Obaidi et al. (Reference Obaidi, Kunst, Kteily, Thomsen and Sidanius2018, Reference Obaidi, Kunst, Ozer and Kimel2022) demonstrate how threat perception can lead to hostile attitudes, for example, through fears of replacement of the own religion by another one (B5).

Contact between religious groups is thought to decrease hostile attitudes and threat perceptions under some circumstances (B6), for example, when it is institutionalized and structured. Empirical evidence shows that contact can indeed reduce hostile attitudes and threat perceptions (see B6 in Table 4). However, reverse causation may also be at play as higher perceptions of threat seem to reduce the quantity and quality of contact (Kanas et al., Reference Kanas, Scheepers and Sterkens2016).

Theoretically, religious practice can impact hostile attitudes in differing ways (B7a and B7b)Footnote 3: attending service can decrease hostile attitudes if it has beneficial effects on prosocial values, for example, when service contents transmit prosocial ideas but also when common activities such as volunteering play a role. Yet, it could also increase hostile attitudes as the ingroup identity is emphasized through service contents and common rituals, which could enhance outgroup derogation. Furthermore, service attendance could be interpreted as commitment to conservative and authoritative values. Empirical evidence is mixed: while two studies find a negative association between service attendance and hostile attitudes and threat perceptions, two find a positive association and three studies do not find any significant association (see B7a and B7b, Table 4). These differing results could be due to differences in operationalizing service attendance (which is often subsumed under “religiosity”) as well as different study contexts (e.g., Bangladesh, Kenya, Malaysia, Switzerland, the United States, or a subset of different countries).

Only one identified study empirically tests the connection between religious actors and institutions and hostile attitudes. Neuberg et al. (Reference Neuberg, Warner, Mistler, Berlin, Hill, Johnson, Filip-Crawford, Millsap, Thomas, Winkelman, Broome, Taylor and Schober2014) find support for their theory that when religion is an important part of public life, hostile attitudes increase if imbalances between groups regarding power and resources exist (B8).

To sum up, empirical evidence points at (1) the importance of religious ideas for hostile attitudes toward religious others. (2) Religious identity impacts hostile attitudes and threat perceptions, particularly in interaction with other factors, for example, the prevalence of fundamentalist religious ideas or power imbalances. (3) Religious practice is not robustly connected with attitudes toward others. (4) Religious actors and institutions seem to be important for hostile attitudes, but empirical evidence is scarce.

Interreligious trust

Turning to interreligious trust, the 42 articles reviewed investigate trust in the relation between members of different religious groups but also between religious and non-religious individuals. With few exceptions, studies on trust between religious and non-religious are conducted in the United States whereas studies on trust between religious groups are mostly set in Asian contexts with many studies focusing on China, Bangladesh, and India. All studies considering relations between denominations were conducted in Northern Ireland. Most studies are based on psychological or economics research, or the intersection between the two. Measures of trust depend on the empirical strategies employed which include behavioral experiments, survey experiments, and surveys (see Table A3 in the Appendix).

Table 5 provides an overview of theories on religious determinants of interreligious trust and their empirical support. Regarding religious ideas, a prominent theory is that belief in a (monitoring) God increases distrust in atheists (C1). The idea is that others believe religious individuals to be acting under the impression of being monitored by a rewarding and punishing God, which is thought to make them more prosocially (Gervais et al., Reference Gervais, Shariff and Norenzayan2011; see Gervais, Reference Gervais2013). Particularly individuals who believe in a (monitoring) God should, hence, distrust atheists, because atheists do not believe in the existence of God and consequentially are expected to lack prosocial behavior. Empirical evidence supports this theory (see C1, Table 5). Alternative mechanisms suggest that effective secular institutions may replace the effect of a moralizing God (Norenzayan and Gervais, Reference Norenzayan and Gervais2015) and that religious identities are associated with specific reproductive strategies, which signal trustworthiness (Moon et al., Reference Moon, Krems and Cohen2018; Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, Hart and Kinrade2023).

Table 5. Interreligious trust: theory and empirical support

Most studies investigate the impact of religious identities on interreligious trust. The intergroup bias hypothesis (C2a) builds on the idea that a common social identity like a religious identity suggests familiarity, similarity, connectedness, or closeness which result in greater trust in the ingroup and lower trust in the outgroup. Empirical evidence is mixed (C2a, Table 5): some studies report significant intergroup bias, while others do not. Four reasons could explain these diverging findings. First, design differences must be considered. To illustrate, Chuah et al. (Reference Chuah, Gächter, Hoffmann and Tan2016) report that religious people believe that other religious people are more trustworthy than non-religious individuals, but they do not act on this belief in their trust game (see also Johansson-Stenman et al., Reference Johansson-Stenman, Mahmud and Martinsson2009). Thus, religious ingroup favoritism seems to show less in behavior than in attitudes. It may further matter how religious identities are conveyed as this is not uniform across studies (Timming and Perrett, Reference Timming and Perrett2016; Galen et al., Reference Galen, Kurby and Fles2022; Totton et al., Reference Totton, Rios and Shogren2023). Second, only established identity groups show ingroup favoritism. Although Christians rather consistently show ingroup favoritism, groups subsumed under “non-Christian,” “non-religious,” or “people believing in karma” do not trust those falling under the same category more (e.g., Xu et al., Reference Xu, Liu and Liang2018; Grove et al., Reference Grove, Rubenstein and Terrell2020; Ong et al., Reference Ong, Evans, Nelissen and van Beest2022). Moreover, studies must specifically match religious identities of targets to respondents as, for example, only atheists may favor atheists but not all respondents that are categorized as non-religious. Indeed, diversity within the non-religious should be considered in future studies (e.g., Thunström, Reference Thunström2020; Van Tongeren et al., Reference Van Tongeren, DeWall and Van Cappellen2023). Third, the intersection of religious with other identities must be considered. Intersecting identities like nationality (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Cohen, Meyer, Varley and Brewer2015) or student identities (Delavande and Zafar, Reference Delavande and Zafar2015; Galen et al., Reference Galen, Kurby and Fles2022) could bridge religious differences but also be more salient in determining (dis-)trust rendering common religious identities irrelevant (Howard, Reference Howard2022).

Fourth, context matters beyond identity salience and intersectionality. Particularly, demographic constellations and minority–majority relations impact interreligious trust (Gervais, Reference Gervais2011; Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Mahmud, Maitra, Mitra and Neelim2018). Further, interreligious trust in the relation between Protestants and Catholics in Northern Ireland was found to be improved by experiences of positive interreligious interactions (C4, Table 5). Higher levels of desegregation and larger minority groups increase the frequency of (positive) interreligious interactions (Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Christ, Pettigrew, Stellmacher and Wolf2006; Kenworthy et al., Reference Kenworthy, Voci, Al Ramiah, Tausch, Hughes and Hewstone2016; Laurence et al., Reference Laurence, Schmid and Hewstone2018). Therefore, demographic constellations, resulting majority–minority relations and opportunities for positive interreligious contact should be considered when studying interreligious trust.

Further, religious identities could be associated with interreligious trust if higher religiosity implies stronger ingroup identification and enhances biases in interreligious trust (C2b). Studies that find significant effects report ingroup trust rather than outgroup distrust to be amplified. Yet, mixed results (C2b, Table 5) remain difficult to evaluate as religiosity is operationalized in various ways—not always clearly measuring strength of ingroup identification—and theorizing remains limited (for a discussion on religiosity and generalized trust, see Badaoui, Reference Badaoui2023). Moreover, cross-national studies suggest differences depending on region and religion (Kollar and Fleischmann, Reference Kollar and Fleischmann2022; Badaoui, Reference Badaoui2023). Only a measure of religiosity that taps into exclusivist ideas rather than strength of identification tends to be negatively related to outgroup trust (LaBouff and Ledoux, Reference LaBouff and Ledoux2016; Kollar and Fleischmann, Reference Kollar and Fleischmann2022; Masood et al., Reference Masood, Xiang, Skoric and Ahmed2022; Badaoui, Reference Badaoui2023).

Finally, religious identities may signal trustworthiness by signaling a belief in a monitoring God (cf. C1, Table 5) to religious and non-religious individuals alike as well as across religious divides (C3, Table 5). Mixed evidence suggests that effects of generalized trust into religious people (C3) superimpose with intergroup bias effects (C2a). For example, Chuah et al. (Reference Chuah, Gächter, Hoffmann and Tan2016) find that religious and non-religious alike believe that the more religious are more trustworthy while—at the same time—ingroups are favored. Similarly, Edgell et al. (Reference Edgell, Gerteis and Hartmann2006) report that although atheists show distrust against atheists, they do so less than religious people. Further investigating the interaction between the two effects could advance the study of the effect of religious identities on interreligious trust.

A fruitful way forward may be costly signaling theory (Ruffle and Sosis, Reference Ruffle and Sosis2007), which suggests that participation in religious practices signals trustworthiness as it shows commitment to the group and its traditions as well as acknowledgment of accountability (C4). There is evidence that individuals participating in religious rites are more trusted than those who do not (Purzycki and Arakchaa, Reference Purzycki and Arakchaa2013), even across religious divides (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Cohen, Meyer, Varley and Brewer2015, experiment 4). Yet, replication of these results in different contexts is warranted. Studying effects of participation on religious practices seems less relevant as frequencies of praying did not affect interreligious trust in previous studies (e.g., Johansson-Stenman et al., Reference Johansson-Stenman, Mahmud and Martinsson2009; Shaver et al., Reference Shaver, Lang, Krátký, Klocová, Kundt and Xygalatas2018).

The role of religious actors and institutions in interreligious trust is not considered in the reviewed literature. Yet, a study by Kenworthy et al. (Reference Kenworthy, Voci, Al Ramiah, Tausch, Hughes and Hewstone2016) examined perceptions of equality in power of Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland and found a positive relation with outgroup trust. Future studies should investigate if religious actors and institutions could establish perceptions of equality between religious groups and whether these findings hold in other contexts.

Overall, the reviewed literature indicates that (1) belief in a (monitoring) God has negative effects for trust between religious and non-religious. (2) Established religious groups favor their ingroup but intersecting identities and context factors must be considered. (3) Participation in religious practices seems to signal trustworthiness—potentially across religious divides. (4) The role of religious actors and institutions in interreligious trust warrants research.

Interreligious cooperation

We reviewed eight studies on religious determinants of interreligious cooperation. Most of these studies are based on economics or psychological research, study cooperation at the individual level with students, and employ economic games or survey experiments (see Table A4 in the Appendix). One exception is a study on interreligious networks in developing countries at the country level (Vüllers et al., Reference Vüllers, Pfeiffer and Basedau2015). Conducting research outside artificial laboratory settings, beyond student populations, and in more geographical locations could generate greater insights on the generalizability and real-life applicability of existing research.

Table 6 shows an overview of theories on religious determinants of interreligious cooperation and their empirical support. Concerning religious ideas, evidence is mixed whether fundamentalist convictions decrease interreligious cooperation (D1; Chuah et al., Reference Chuah, Hoffmann, Ramasamy and Tan2014; but Xia et al., Reference Xia, Guo, Luo, Ye, Chen, Chen and Xia2021). Theory remains rather unspecific expecting fundamentalist convictions to affect adherent's social interactions generally and, hence, also interreligious cooperation specifically (Chuah et al., Reference Chuah, Hoffmann, Ramasamy and Tan2014). Developing hypotheses detailing why fundamentalist convictions reduce interreligious cooperation may allow for more specific theory testing. The impact of religious ideas may depend on their content (D2). Indeed, Preston and Ritter (Reference Preston and Ritter2013, experiment 3) report evidence for their hypothesis that concepts of God highlight outgroup prosociality norms and, hence, increase interreligious cooperation. They also find that being primed with the concept of religion decreases interreligious cooperation potentially because it highlights ingroup favoring norms. This suggests that the specific content and potential imperatives of religious ideas determine their effect on interreligious cooperation.

Table 6. Interreligious cooperation: theory and empirical support

Having different religious identities does not per se have a negative impact on cooperation. While there is evidence that cooperation with religious others is lower than religious ingroup cooperation (D3a, Table 6), interreligious cooperation does not seem to differ from cooperation with unknown others (D3b, Table 6). Hence, religious identities lead to ingroup favoritism (ingroup cooperation > cooperation with others), but not outgroup derogation (outgroup cooperation < cooperation with unknown others).Footnote 4 Moreover, Keuschnigg and Schikora (Reference Keuschnigg and Schikora2014) find that participants do not contribute less to mixed than homogenous groups in a public goods game. Chakravarty et al. (Reference Chakravarty, Fonseca, Ghosh and Marjit2016) only find evidence of ingroup favoritism in contexts with a certain level of religious fragmentation and argue that ingroup favoritism only manifests if religious ingroups are salient. Similar to the discussion on biases in interreligious trust, studies on biases in interreligious cooperation must consider context factors, specifically demography, and the salience of identities studied.

Religiosity is sometimes understood as an indicator of an individual's strength of identification with a religious group. Strong identifiers are expected to have a greater motivation to uphold a positive group identity through outgroup derogation which would decrease interreligious cooperation (D4; Chuah et al., Reference Chuah, Hoffmann, Ramasamy and Tan2014). There is no evidence supporting this hypothesis (D4, Table 6). However, religiosity measures used in these studies vary and may not always be indicators of strength of identification with or commitment to the ingroup.

Another hypothesis suggests that religious individuals may be intuitively cooperative regardless of the cooperation partner's religious identity (D5; Isler et al., Reference Isler, Yilmaz and John Maule2021). In contrast to this claim, Isler et al. (Reference Isler, Yilmaz and John Maule2021) find that Christian participants cooperate more with fellow Christians than atheists (ingroup favoritism). They further report intuition to decrease interreligious cooperation while deliberation increased cooperation also across the religious–non-religious divide.

The effects of religious actors and organizations as well as of religious practices on interreligious cooperation are not studied except for an initial study investigating the impact of religious leadership (D6; Keuschnigg and Schikora, Reference Keuschnigg and Schikora2014). Opposite to expectations, religious leadership did not enhance interreligious cooperation. Keuschnigg and Schikora explain these findings with “bad leadership” (leaders contributed less than average contribution to the no-leadership control group). Through this study, we do not learn about potential effects of “good leadership.” Moreover, in this experiment religious leadership was operationalized as one participant contributing prior to the others which may not reflect the role of religious leaders in real life.

All in all, the scarce literature on the effect of religious factors on interreligious cooperation suggests that (1) the role of religious ideas for cooperation depends on their content. (2) Interreligious cooperation is smaller than ingroup cooperation but there is no evidence of outgroup derogation, that is, that interreligious cooperation is less prevalent than cooperation with unknown others. (3) Rather than religious individuals being intuitively cooperative, deliberative thinking potentially enhances interreligious cooperation. (4) The role of religious practices as well as of religious actors and institutions in interreligious cooperation remains understudied.

Discussion and outlook for further research

After reviewing literature on each dimension of interreligious peace, what do we know about religious determinants of interreligious peace? Table 7 summarizes our main results. We find that religious ideas foster or hinder interreligious peace depending on their content. The current literature on interreligious physical violence, hostile attitudes as well as interreligious trust mainly tests religious ideas that hinder interreligious peace. In contrast, literature on interreligious cooperation reports evidence for religious ideas fostering peace. In fact, calls for violence by religious leaders, fundamentalist convictions, or belief in a (monitoring) God hinder interreligious peace while reminders of norms making outgroup prosociality imperative seem to foster interreligious peace. Theoretical works assume some ideas, like forgiveness, to be key to positive interreligious relations (e.g., Appleby, Reference Appleby2000; Auerbach, Reference Auerbach2005), yet their impact on interreligious peace has not been tested. Thus, while it seems crucial to promote inclusive religious ideas and at the same time counteract more problematic, exclusive ideas, studies testing ideas with potentially positive effects are needed as they may not always have expected positive effects (Hoffmann, Reference Hoffmann2022).

Table 7. Multidimensional impact of religion on interreligious peace

Religious identities are the focus of much of the reviewed literature. Our review finds that “dangerous demographics” as well as strongly identifying with the own religious group can negatively impact intergroup biases, and thereby matter for interreligious peace. However, contextual factors (such as political, economic, and historical conditions) seem crucial in shaping how religious identities matter for interreligious relations. Further, research on interreligious trust and cooperation highlights that preferences for the ingroup do not necessarily go together with outgroup derogation and that salience of identities as well as intersectionality must be considered. Moreover, demography affects opportunities for positive interreligious contact which has positive effects on interreligious relations. Thus, future studies should disentangle under what conditions religious identities pose a risk to peace in a more nuanced way, for example, by combining cross-country data with comparative case-study approaches. For example, theoretical claims that changing religious demography could cause unrest because threat perceptions and motives for acts of violence may arise especially on the part of the shrinking group (e.g., Seul, Reference Seul1999), remain untested.

Results on the influence of religious practices on interreligious peace are mixed. In light of inconclusive results and insignificant effects of individuals' participation in religious practices on their attitudes or behaviors pertaining to interreligious peace, it does not seem promising to further test these associations. Rather, participation in religious practices may function as a signal to others, for example, of trustworthiness (Ruffle and Sosis, Reference Ruffle and Sosis2007; Hall et al., Reference Hall, Cohen, Meyer, Varley and Brewer2015), but also of a strong ingroup identity or exclusive values (Rink and Sharma, Reference Rink and Sharma2018). Additional research must investigate the generalizability of existing findings by replicating these studies in more contexts and evaluate under what conditions religious practices enhance or counteract interreligious peace. A recent qualitative study on Ethiopia, for example, suggests that spatial components of religious practices can play a role in dynamics of competition between religious groups (Østebø, Reference Østebø2023).

Studies on the impact of religious actors and institutions on interreligious peace find that religious institutions ensuring power sharing or equal treatment of religions may prevent interreligious violence whereas religious competition may fuel religious conflict. However, quantitative evidence is scarce. Research focusing on the role of religious actors and institutions in interreligious physical violence could be broadened to investigate further elements of interreligious peace. Additionally, dynamic debates on which types of institutions may best serve diverse societies can inspire further research. For instance, religion–state relations governed by institutions guaranteeing freedom of religion or belief (FORB) are theoretically considered important to enable interreligious peace (Joustra, Reference Joustra2020; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Seiple and Hoover2020). Indeed, a qualitative study on Myanmar found that violent mobs in Myanmar used claims of mosques being “illegal” as justifications to attack places of worship (Kyaw, Reference Kyaw2021). However, the relation between FORB and interreligious peace still warrants quantitative investigations. Moreover, the way religious institutions are organized could affect interreligious peace. For example, strictly hierarchical religious organizations may be less prone to interreligious violence because they have more control over their believers and lower-ranking clergy (cf. Basedau, Reference Basedau2016). Regarding the role of religious leaders for interreligious peace, examining theoretical claims regarding relations between religious leaders and state institutions could further debates (De Juan, Reference De Juan2008). Similarly, empirically studying the assumed effectiveness of religious leaders' rhetoric in promoting interreligious peace (cf. Sharma, Reference Sharma2017) could inform research on interreligious peace.

Our conclusions are limited by the selection criteria for literature included in this review. While criteria are necessary to delimit our search, they restrict our understanding of what is known about interreligious peace. The main limitation is that our review focuses on quantitative literature which implies that evidence from relevant qualitative research is not considered (e.g., Østebø, Reference Østebø2023). Further, we restrict our review to religious determinants of interreligious peace. Yet, there are other important factors like economic welfare (e.g., Mousseau, Reference Mousseau2011) affecting interreligious peace. Additionally, we focus on literature published after 2000. Yet, the small number of reviewed studies published prior to 2010 suggests that it is unlikely that we exclude substantial research on this criterium. Finally, we do not review studies focusing on the impact of religion on conflict and peace more generally. Thus, our review is a first step in a greater endeavor to learn about the impact of religion on relational peace more generally.

We would like to emphasize several aspects that should be considered for further research on the link between religion and interreligious peace. First, there is a need for conceptual clarity. For example, “religion” was often not specified and, if specified, sometimes unfittingly—and many times inconsistently—operationalized (see also Basedau et al., Reference Basedau, Gobien and Prediger2018). In many studies, religiosity is used as an explanatory variable. However, religiosity is sometimes measured by accounting for theological, ritual, experiential, and consequential dimensions (e.g., Chuah et al., Reference Chuah, Hoffmann, Ramasamy and Tan2014) and other times as the strength of identification with a religion (e.g., Xia et al., Reference Xia, Guo, Luo, Ye, Chen, Chen and Xia2021). In the absence of differentiated hypotheses on the effect of various aspects of religiosity on interreligious peace, a lack of consistency of definitions makes comparisons across studies difficult.

Second, research findings should be replicated in different contexts to further understand their generalizability. The majority of studies reviewed focus on a single country. Hence, we do not know whether theories also hold in other contexts. Furthermore, especially the literature on interreligious physical violence, hostile attitudes, and threat perceptions relies on cross-country and survey designs. For these literatures, we identify a need for causal studies going beyond correlational designs.

Third, religion impacts interreligious peace through more than religious identities. Even though religion is multidimensional, current research mainly focuses on the role of religious identities. While existing studies highlight aspects of religious identities that hinder peace, conditions under which religious groups live peacefully side-by-side without identities becoming contentious need further study. Moreover, the role of religious actors and institutions may foreground ways religion fosters interreligious peace. Similarly, different religious ideas should be more vigorously investigated to learn about the ambiguity of their effect on interreligious peace.

Finally, it remains a challenge to better integrate different strands of literature and piece together a more cohesive literature. Making claims about interreligious peace requires drawing on various literatures to reflect its multidimensionality. Hereby, it will be important to investigate the interrelation of its dimensions. Existing research suggests a web of linkages between the dimensions of interreligious peace proposed in our definition. Threat perceptions may be an important determinant of hostile attitudes (Kanas et al., Reference Kanas, Scheepers and Sterkens2015), while trust seems to increase cooperation (Balliet and Van Lange, Reference Balliet and Van Lange2013; Preston and Ritter, Reference Preston and Ritter2013). In fact, previous violence and interreligious tensions may foment a perceived need for more cooperation and exchange between religious groups (Vüllers et al., Reference Vüllers, Pfeiffer and Basedau2015). While this is initial evidence for the interrelation of dimensions of interreligious peace, a more in-depth investigation is warranted.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755048323000238.

Acknowledgments

We have profited from helpful comments provided by Matthias Basedau and Eric Stollenwerk. We also thank Sarah Wenzel and Jordy Kiel for their support. Moreover, we appreciate the helpful comments and feedback from the three anonymous reviewers which helped us to strengthen our analyses.

Financial support

This research was conducted as part of the project “Religion for peace: identifying conditions and mechanisms of interfaith peace” funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) (BA-3515/5-1).

Competing interests

None.

Julia Köbrich is a research fellow at the German Institute for Global and Area Studies (GIGA) in Hamburg and a PhD candidate at the European Research Center on Migration and Ethnic Relations (ERCOMER) at Utrecht University. Her main research interests lie at the intersection of peace research and social psychology particularly dynamics of intergroup relations and peacebuilding interventions.

Lisa Hoffmann is a research fellow at the GIGA Institute for African Affairs. Her research interests include peace and conflict studies, social cohesion, and experimental economics. She has conducted research in Ghana, Kenya, Liberia, Sierra Leone, Tanzania, and Togo.

Footnotes

1. The concept of interreligious peace was developed by the authors together with Matthias Basedau and Eric Stollenwerk (see Hoffmann et al., Reference Hoffmann, Köbrich, Stollenwerk and Basedau2021).

2. We included articles published in journals on the social sciences citation index, science citation index expanded, or the arts and humanities citation information index.

3. Some studies use composite indicators to measure “religiosity” which, however, are usually based on service attendance and frequency of prayers.

4. For a theoretical discussion on the difference between ingroup favoritism and outgroup derogation see Brewer (Reference Brewer2001).

References

Adida, CL, Brown, JM, McCord, GC and McLachlan, P (2022) Diversity and violence during conflict migration: the troubles in Northern Ireland. Political Science Research and Methods 11(3), 117.Google Scholar
Alexander, KJ (2017) Religiosity and bellicosity: the impact of religious commitment on patterns of interstate conflict. Journal of Global Security Studies 2(4), 271287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Appleby, RS (2000) The Ambivalence of the Sacred: Religion, Violence, and Reconciliation. Carnegie Commission on Preventing Deadly Conflict Series. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.Google Scholar
Asadullah, MN (2017) Who trusts others? Community and individual determinants of social capital in a low-income country. Cambridge Journal of Economics 41(2), 515544.Google Scholar
Auerbach, Y (2005) Forgiveness and reconciliation: the religious dimension. Terrorism and Political Violence 17(3), 469485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badaoui, E (2023) Which dimensions of religiosity matter for trust? New insights from the MENA region. World Development 167(July), 106215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badea, C, Bender, M and Korda, H (2020) Threat to national identity continuity: when affirmation procedures increase the acceptance of Muslim immigrants. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, Viewing intercultural adaptation and social inclusion through constructs of national identity 78(September), 6572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balcells, L, Daniels, L-A and Escribà-Folch, A (2016) The determinants of low-intensity intergroup violence: the case of Northern Ireland. Journal of Peace Research 53(1), 3348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balliet, D and Van Lange, PAM (2013) Trust, conflict, and cooperation: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 139(5), 10901112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basedau, M (2016) Ideen, Identitäten und Institutionen: Explikation Eines Multidimensionalen Religionsbegriffs in der Friedens- und Konfliktforschung. In Religion in Der Friedens- Und Konfliktforschung, 235–65. Nomos Verlagsgesellschaft mbH & Co. KG. https://doi.org/10.5771/9783845266145-235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basedau, M, Fox, J, Pierskalla, JH, Strüver, G and Vüllers, J (2017) Does discrimination breed grievances—and do grievances breed violence? New evidence from an analysis of religious minorities in developing Countries. Conflict Management and Peace Science 34(3), 217239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basedau, M, Gobien, S and Prediger, S (2018) The multidimensional effects of religion on socioeconomic development: a review of the empirical literature. Journal of Economic Surveys 32(4), 11061133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basedau, M, Pfeiffer, B and Vüllers, J (2016) Bad religion? Religion, collective action, and the onset of armed conflict in developing Countries. Journal of Conflict Resolution 60(2), 226255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basedau, M, Strüver, G, Vüllers, J and Wegenast, T (2011) Do religious factors impact armed conflict? Empirical evidence from sub-Saharan Africa. Terrorism and Political Violence 23(5), 752779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauchamp, AL and Rios, K (2020) Secularism in science: the role of religious affiliation in assessments of scientists’ trustworthiness. Public Understanding of Science 29(2), 194210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bormann, N-C, Cederman, L-E and Vogt, M (2017) Language, religion, and ethnic civil war. Journal of Conflict Resolution 61(4), 744771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, MB (2001) Ingroup identification and intergroup conflict: when does ingroup love become outgroup hate? Social Identity, Intergroup Conflict, and Conflict Reduction 3, 1741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bunte, JB and Vinson, LT (2016) Local power-sharing institutions and interreligious violence in Nigeria. Journal of Peace Research 53(1), 4965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chakravarty, S, Fonseca, MA, Ghosh, S and Marjit, S (2016) Religious fragmentation, social identity and cooperation: evidence from an artefactual field experiment in India. European Economic Review 90(November), 265279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chuah, S-H, Fahoum, R and Hoffmann, R (2013) Fractionalization and trust in India: a field-experiment. Economics Letters 119(2), 191194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chuah, S-H, Gächter, S, Hoffmann, R and Tan, JHW (2016) Religion, discrimination and trust across three cultures. European Economic Review, Social identity and discrimination 90(November), 280301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chuah, S-H, Hoffmann, R, Ramasamy, B and Tan, JHW (2014) Religion, ethnicity and cooperation: an experimental study. Journal of Economic Psychology 45(December), 3343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, S and Gaskins, B (2016) Trust me, I believe in God: candidate religiousness as a signal of trustworthiness. American Politics Research 44(6), 10661097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davenport, C, Melander, E and Regan, PM (2018) The Peace Continuum: What It Is and How to Study It. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deitch, M (2022) Is religion a barrier to peace? Religious influence on violent intrastate conflict termination. Terrorism and Political Violence 34(7), 14541470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Juan, A (2008) A pact with the devil? Elite alliances as bases of violent religious conflicts. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism 31(12), 11201135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delavande, A and Zafar, B (2015) Stereotypes and madrassas: experimental evidence from Pakistan. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, Economic Experiments in Developing Countries 118(October), 247267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhattiwala, R and Biggs, M (2012) The political logic of ethnic violence: the anti-Muslim pogrom in Gujarat, 2002. Politics & Society 40(4), 483516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edgell, P, Gerteis, J and Hartmann, D (2006) Atheists As ‘Other’: Moral Boundaries and Cultural Membership in American Society. American Sociological Review 71(2), 211234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellingsen, T (2005) Toward a revival of religion and religious clashes? Terrorism and Political Violence 17(3), 305332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fair, CC and Patel, P (2022) Support for domestic Islamist terrorism in Bangladesh: insights from a novel survey. Politics and Religion 15(4), 673699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, E, Levinson, M, Pande, R and Visaria, S (2008) Segregation, rent control, and riots: the economics of religious conflict in an Indian city. The American Economic Review 98(2), 505510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, J (2000) The ethnic–religious nexus: the impact of religion on ethnic conflict. Civil Wars 3(3), 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, J (2018) An Introduction to Religion and Politics: Theory and Practice, 2nd Edn. Routledge Studies in Religion and Politics. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franks, AS and Scherr, KC (2014) A sociofunctional approach to prejudice at the polls: are atheists more politically disadvantaged than gays and Blacks? Journal of Applied Social Psychology 44(10), 681691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franks, AS, Scherr, KC and Gibson, B (2019) Godless by association: deficits in trust mediate antiatheist stigma-by-association. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 25, 303316.Google ScholarPubMed
Galen, LW, Kurby, CA and Fles, EH (2022) Religiosity, shared identity, trust, and punishment of norm violations: No evidence of generalized prosociality. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 14(2), 260272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerteis, J, Hartmann, D and Edgell, P (2020) Racial, religious, and civic dimensions of anti-Muslim sentiment in America. Social Problems 67(4), 719740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervais, WM (2011) Finding the faithless: perceived atheist prevalence reduces anti-atheist prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(4), 543556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervais, WM (2013) In godlessness we distrust: using social psychology to solve the puzzle of anti-atheist prejudice. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 7(6), 366377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervais, WM, Shariff, AF and Norenzayan, A (2011) Do you believe in atheists? Distrust is central to anti-atheist prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101(6), 11891206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giddings, L and Dunn, TJ (2016) The robustness of anti-atheist prejudice as measured by way of cognitive errors. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 26(2), 124135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goertz, G, Diehl, PF and Balas, A (2016) The Puzzle of Peace: The Evolution of Peace in the International System. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grove, RC, Rubenstein, A and Terrell, HK (2020) Distrust persists after subverting atheist stereotypes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 23(7), 11031124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullickson, A and Ahmed, S (2021) The role of education, religiosity and development on support for violent practices among Muslims in thirty-five countries. PLoS ONE 16(11), e0260429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, G, Mahmud, M, Maitra, P, Mitra, S and Neelim, A (2018) Religion, minority status, and trust: evidence from a field experiment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 146(February), 180205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, DL, Cohen, AB, Meyer, KK, Varley, AH and Brewer, GA (2015) Costly signaling increases trust, even across religious affiliations. Psychological Science 26(9), 13681376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hassner, RE (2003) “To halve and to hold”: conflicts over sacred space and the problem of indivisibility. Security Studies 12(4), 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffmann, R (2013) The experimental economics of religion. Journal of Economic Surveys 27(5), 813845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffmann, L (2022) Cooperation in the name of God? Experimental evidence from Ghana and Tanzania. Journal of Economic Psychology 93, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffmann, L, Köbrich, J, Stollenwerk, E and Basedau, M (2021) Please, make peace! The religious determinants of positive and negative interreligious peace. Unpublished manuscript. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association in Seattle.Google Scholar
Howard, S (2022) Exploring white individuals’ perceptions of Black atheists’ trustworthiness, racial identity, and feelings of warmth. International Journal of Psychology 57(5), 652659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isaacs, M (2016) Sacred violence or strategic faith? Disentangling the relationship between religion and violence in armed conflict. Journal of Peace Research 53(2), 211225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isaacs, M (2017) Faith in contention: explaining the salience of religion in ethnic conflict. Comparative Political Studies 50(2), 200231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isler, O, Yilmaz, O and John Maule, A (2021) Religion, parochialism and intuitive cooperation. Nature Human Behaviour 5, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansson-Stenman, O, Mahmud, M and Martinsson, P (2009) Trust and religion: experimental evidence from rural Bangladesh. Economica 76(303), 462485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnstone, N and Svensson, I (2013) Belligerents and believers: exploring faith-based mediation in internal armed conflicts. Politics, Religion & Ideology 14(4), 557579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joustra, RJ (2020) The coordinates of covenantal pluralism: mapping pluralist theory in the 21st Century. The Review of Faith & International Affairs 18(4), 1834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanas, A, Scheepers, P and Sterkens, C (2015) Interreligious contact, perceived group threat, and perceived discrimination: predicting negative attitudes among religious minorities and majorities in Indonesia. Social Psychology Quarterly 78(2), 102126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanas, A, Scheepers, P and Sterkens, C (2016) Religious identification and interreligious contact in Indonesia and the Philippines: testing the mediating roles of perceived group threat and social dominance orientation and the moderating role of context. European Journal of Social Psychology 46(6), 700715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanas, A, Scheepers, P and Sterkens, C (2017) Positive and negative contact and attitudes towards the religious out-group: testing the contact hypothesis in conflict and non-conflict regions of Indonesia and the Philippines. Social Science Research 63(March), 95110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasten, L (2017) When less is more: constructing a parsimonious concept of interstate peace for quantitative analysis. International Studies Review 19(1), 2852.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenworthy, JB, Voci, A, Al Ramiah, A, Tausch, N, Hughes, J and Hewstone, M (2016) Building trust in a postconflict society: an integrative model of cross-group friendship and intergroup emotions. Journal of Conflict Resolution 60(6), 10411070.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keuschnigg, M and Schikora, J (2014) The dark side of leadership: an experiment on religious heterogeneity and cooperation in India. The Journal of Socio-Economics 48(February), 1926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, D and Choi, HJ (2017) Autocracy, religious restriction, and religious civil war. Politics and Religion 10(2), 311338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klašnja, M and Novta, N (2016) Segregation, polarization, and ethnic conflict. Journal of Conflict Resolution 60(5), 927955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kollar, R and Fleischmann, F (2022) Does religion foster prejudice among adherents of all world religions? A comparison across religions. Review of Religious Research 64(4), 627653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koopmans, R and Rebers, S (2009) Collective action in culturally similar and dissimilar groups: an experiment on parochialism, conditional cooperation, and their linkages. Evolution and Human Behavior 30(3), 201211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunst, JR, Thomsen, L and Sam, DL (2014) Late Abrahamic reunion? Religious fundamentalism negatively predicts dual Abrahamic group categorization among Muslims and Christians. European Journal of Social Psychology 44(4), 337348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyaw, NN (2021) The excuse of (il)legality in discriminating and persecuting religious minorities: anti-mosque legal violence in Myanmar. Asian Journal of Law and Society 8(1), 108131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaBouff, JP and Ledoux, AM (2016) Imagining atheists: reducing fundamental distrust in atheist intergroup attitudes. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 8(4), 330340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lam, DYP, Koh, KS, Gan, SW and Sow, JTY (2023) Religiosity and the perception of interreligious threats: the suppressing effect of negative emotions towards god. Religions 14(3), 366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, JT, Hart, W and Kinrade, C (2023) Testing a perceived uncommitted mating strategy account for atheist distrust and marriage disapproval. Current Psychology 42(24), 20916–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laurence, J, Schmid, K and Hewstone, M (2018) Ethnic diversity, inter-group attitudes and countervailing pathways of positive and negative inter-group contact: an analysis across workplaces and neighbourhoods. Social Indicators Research 136(2), 719749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masood, M, Xiang, M, Skoric, MM and Ahmed, S (2022) Trust in religious others: a three-way interaction model of religious bias, informational use of digital media, and education. International Journal of Communication 16(0), 20.Google Scholar
Moon, JW, Krems, JA and Cohen, AB (2018) Religious people are trusted because they are viewed as slow life-history strategists. Psychological Science 29(6), 947960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mousseau, M (2011) Urban poverty and support for Islamist terror: survey results of Muslims in fourteen Countries. Journal of Peace Research 48(1), 3547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neuberg, SL, Warner, CM, Mistler, SA, Berlin, A, Hill, ED, Johnson, JD, Filip-Crawford, G, Millsap, RE, Thomas, G, Winkelman, M, Broome, B, Taylor, TJ and Schober, J (2014) Religion and intergroup conflict: findings from the global group relations project. Psychological Science 25(1), 198206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nilsson, D and Svensson, I (2021) The intractability of Islamist insurgencies: Islamist rebels and the recurrence of civil war. International Studies Quarterly 65(3), 620632.10.1093/isq/sqab064CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norenzayan, A and Gervais, WM (2015) Secular rule of law erodes believers’ political intolerance of atheists. Religion, Brain & Behavior 5(1), 314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obaidi, M, Kunst, JR, Kteily, N, Thomsen, L and Sidanius, J (2018) Living under threat: mutual threat perception drives anti-Muslim and anti-western hostility in the age of terrorism. European Journal of Social Psychology 48(5), 567584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obaidi, M, Kunst, J, Ozer, S and Kimel, SY (2022) The “Great replacement” conspiracy: how the perceived ousting of whites can evoke violent extremism and Islamophobia. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 25(7), 16751695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ong, HH, Evans, AM, Nelissen, RMA and van Beest, I (2022) Belief in karma is associated with perceived (but not actual) trustworthiness. Judgment and Decision Making 17(2), 362377.10.1017/S1930297500009141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ossai, EC (2020) “We aren't killing each other, but we bear grudges that could be sparked”: how interreligious “peace” and non-peace coexist in a West African city. Islam and Christian–Muslim Relations 31(3), 307323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Østebø, T (2023) Religious dynamics and conflicts in contemporary Ethiopia: expansion, protection, and reclaiming space. African Studies Review April, 124. https://doi.org/10.1017/asr.2023.11Google Scholar
Paterson, JL, Turner, RN and Hodson, G (2019) Receptivity to dating and marriage across the religious divide in Northern Ireland: the role of intergroup contact. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 49(9), 575584.10.1111/jasp.12617CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preston, JL and Ritter, RS (2013) Different effects of religion and god on prosociality with the ingroup and outgroup. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 39(11), 14711483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purzycki, BG and Arakchaa, T (2013) Ritual behavior and trust in the Tyva Republic. Current Anthropology 54(3), 381388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putra, IE, Mashuri, A, Nurhamida, Y and Halperin, E (2022) Beliefs about human nature moderate the association between religious fundamentalism and hate: the case of Muslims in Indonesia. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 53(4), 329340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reimer, NK, Hughes, J, Blaylock, D, Donnelly, C, Wölfer, R and Hewstone, M (2022) Shared education as a contact-based intervention to improve intergroup relations among adolescents in postconflict Northern Ireland. Developmental Psychology 58(1), 193208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rink, A and Sharma, K (2018) The determinants of religious radicalization: evidence from Kenya. Journal of Conflict Resolution 62(6), 12291261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruffle, BJ and Sosis, R (2007) Does it pay to pray? Costly ritual and cooperation. The B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy 7(1), 135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiya, N (2018) Weapon of Peace: How Religious Liberty Combats Terrorism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiya, N and Manchanda, S (2020) Do burqa bans make us safer? Veil prohibitions and terrorism in Europe. Journal of European Public Policy 27(12), 17811800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiya, N and Scime, A (2015) Explaining religious terrorism: a data-mined analysis. Conflict Management and Peace Science 32(5), 487512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, K, Tausch, N, Hewstone, M, Hughes, J and Cairns, E (2008) The effects of living in segregated vs. mixed areas in Northern Ireland: a simultaneous analysis of contact and threat effects in the context of micro-level neighbourhoods. International Journal of Conflict and Violence (IJCV) 2(1), 5671.Google Scholar
Schuurman, B (2019) Topics in terrorism research: reviewing trends and gaps, 2007–2016. Critical Studies on Terrorism 12(3), 463480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seul, JR (1999) “Ours is the way of god”: religion, identity, and intergroup conflict. Journal of Peace Research 36(5), 553569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharma, K (2017) Clerical Persuasion and Religious Extremism: In Elite Persuasion and Religious Extremism: A Study Among Sunni and Shia Muslims in Northern India (PhD dissertation). Columbia University ProQuest Dissertations Publishing. 10637608.Google Scholar
Shaver, JH, Lang, M, Krátký, J, Klocová, EK, Kundt, R and Xygalatas, D (2018) The boundaries of trust: cross-religious and cross-ethnic field experiments in Mauritius. Evolutionary Psychology 16(4), 14747049188176441474704918817644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Söderström, J, Åkebo, M and Jarstad, AK (2021) Friends, fellows, and foes: a new framework for studying relational peace. International Studies Review 23(3), 484508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, WC, Seiple, C and Hoover, DR (2020) Toward a global covenant of peaceable neighborhood: introducing the philosophy of covenantal pluralism. The Review of Faith & International Affairs 18(4), 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svensson, I (2016) Conflict and peace. In Yamane, D (ed), Handbook of Religion and Society. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 467484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svensson, I and Nilsson, D (2018) Disputes over the divine: introducing the religion and armed conflict (RELAC) Data, 1975 to 2015. Journal of Conflict Resolution 62(5), 11271148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tam, T, Hewstone, M, Kenworthy, J and Cairns, E (2009) Intergroup trust in Northern Ireland. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 35(1), 4559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, JHW and Vogel, C (2008) Religion and trust: an experimental study. Journal of Economic Psychology 29(6), 832848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tausch, N, Hewstone, M, Schmid, K, Hughes, J and Cairns, E (2011) Extended contact effects as a function of closeness of relationship with ingroup contacts. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 14(2), 239254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tausch, N, Tam, T, Hewstone, M, Kenworthy, J and Cairns, E (2007) Individual-level and group-level mediators of contact effects in Northern Ireland: the moderating role of social identification. British Journal of Social Psychology 46(3), 541556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thunström, L (2020) Thoughts and prayers—do they crowd out charity donations? Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 60(1), 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timming, AR and Perrett, D (2016) Trust and mixed signals: a study of religion, tattoos and cognitive dissonance. Personality and Individual Differences 97, 234238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toft, MD (2013) The politics of religious outbidding. The Review of Faith & International Affairs 11(3), 1019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toft, MD (2021) Getting religion right in civil wars. Journal of Conflict Resolution 65(9), 0022002721997895.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toft, MD, Philpott, D and Shah, TS (2011) God's Century: Resurgent Religion and Global Politics. New York: WW Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Totton, RR, Rios, K and Shogren, N (2023) Distrusted disclosures: deception drives anti-transgender but not anti-atheist prejudice. Frontiers in Psychology 13(1006107), 115. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1006107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Tongeren, DR, DeWall, CN and Van Cappellen, P (2023) A sheep in wolf's clothing? Toward an understanding of the religious dones. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 152(1), 98119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velasco González, K, Verkuyten, M, Weesie, J and Poppe, E (2008) Prejudice towards Muslims in The Netherlands: testing integrated threat theory. British Journal of Social Psychology 47(4), 667685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vergani, M, Iqbal, M, Ilbahar, E and Barton, G (2020) The three PS of radicalization: push, pull and personal. A systematic scoping review of the scientific evidence about radicalization into violent extremism. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism 43(10), 854854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vüllers, J, Pfeiffer, B and Basedau, M (2015) Measuring the ambivalence of religion: introducing the religion and conflict in developing countries (RCDC) dataset. International Interactions 41(5), 857881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, U, Christ, O, Pettigrew, TF, Stellmacher, J and Wolf, C (2006) Prejudice and minority proportion: contact instead of threat effects. Social Psychology Quarterly 69(4), 380390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xia, W, Guo, X, Luo, J, Ye, H, Chen, Y, Chen, S and Xia, W (2021) Religious identity, between-group effects and prosocial behavior: evidence from a field experiment in China. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics 91(April), 101665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, H, Liu, Y and Liang, J (2018) How religion impacts deception and trust behavior: evidence from a lab-in-the-field experiment in China. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, & Economics 11(4), 239248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yendell, A and Huber, S (2020) The relevance of the centrality and content of religiosity for explaining Islamophobia in Switzerland. Religions 11(3), 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zellman, A and Brown, D (2022) Uneasy lies the crown: external threats to religious legitimacy and interstate dispute militarization. Security Studies 31(1), 152182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Dimensions of positive and negative interreligious peace

Figure 1

Table 2. Profile—literature on interreligious peacea

Figure 2

Table 3. Interreligious physical violence: theory and empirical support

Figure 3

Table 4. Hostile attitudes and threat perceptions: theory and empirical support

Figure 4

Table 5. Interreligious trust: theory and empirical support

Figure 5

Table 6. Interreligious cooperation: theory and empirical support

Figure 6

Table 7. Multidimensional impact of religion on interreligious peace

Supplementary material: File

Köbrich and Hoffmann supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 208.8 KB