Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T02:51:29.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Objective and subjective neighbourhood characteristics and suicidality: a multilevel analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 July 2021

Jennifer Dykxhoorn*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, UCL, London, UK Department of Primary Care and Population Health, UCL, London, UK
Joseph Hayes
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, UCL, London, UK
Kavya Ashok
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, UCL, London, UK
Alma Sörberg Wallin
Affiliation:
Department of Global Public Health, Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden
Christina Dalman
Affiliation:
Department of Global Public Health, Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden
*
Author for correspondence: Jennifer Dykxhoorn, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Characteristics of the neighbourhood environment, including population density, social fragmentation, and trust, have been linked to mental health outcomes. Using a longitudinal population-based cohort, we explored the relationship between objective and subjective neighbourhood characteristics and the odds of suicidal thoughts and attempts.

Methods

We conducted a longitudinal study of 20764 participants living in Stockholm County who participated in the Stockholm Public Health Survey. We used multilevel modelling to examine if suicidal thoughts and attempts were associated with neighbourhood characteristics, independent of individual associations. We included objective and subjective measures to explore if there was a different relationship between these measures of the neighbourhood environment and suicidality.

Results

Associations between neighbourhood factors and suicidality were predominantly explained by individual characteristics, with the exception of neighbourhood-level deprivation and average residential trust. Each unit increase of deprivation was linked to increased odds of suicidal thoughts [Odds ratio (OR) 1.04, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.00–1.07] and attempts (OR 1.11, 95% CI 1.06–1.17). Decreasing residential trust was associated with increased odds of suicide attempts (OR 1.09, 95% CI 1.02–1.17). There was no evidence that neighbourhood-level fragmentation or average trust in public and political institutions had an independent effect on suicidality once individual and sociodemographic factors were accounted for.

Conclusions

This study showed that much of the neighbourhood-level variation in suicidal thoughts and attempts could be explained by compositional factors, including sociodemographic clustering within neighbourhoods. The independent effect of neighbourhood-level deprivation and average residential trust provide evidence that the neighbourhood context may exert an independent effect on suicidality beyond the impact of individual characteristics.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Background

Neighbourhood-level factors, including population density, social capital, social fragmentation, and deprivation may explain some of the spatial variation in suicide risk (Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Gunnell, Lewis, Leckie, Dalman and Allebeck2014). The role of contextual factors in explaining suicidality has been of interest since Durkheim's seminal work, where he discussed how a disconnect between the individual and societal norms may result in anomie and increased risk of suicide (Durkheim, Reference Durkheim1897). Ecological associations have suggested that there may be a link between area-level factors and rates of suicidal thought, attempts, and deaths but it has not been clearly shown whether these associations can be explained by individual-level characteristics or if area-level factors exert an independent effect on suicidality (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Walz, Geiser, Imbierowicz, Liedtke and Wegener2009; Hagedoorn, Groenewegen, Roberts, & Helbich, Reference Hagedoorn, Groenewegen, Roberts and Helbich2020; Middleton et al., Reference Middleton, Whitley, Frankel, Dorling, Sterne and Gunnell2004; O'Farrell, Corcoran, & Perry, Reference O'Farrell, Corcoran and Perry2016; Rehkopf, Reference Rehkopf2006; Whitley, Gunnell, Dorling, & Smith, Reference Whitley, Gunnell, Dorling and Smith1999). Multilevel studies attempt to disentangle compositional factors (e.g. individual characteristics which may be clustered within a neighbourhood) and contextual characteristics of the neighbourhood to more explore the independent effect of neighbourhood factors on suicidal thoughts and attempts (Martikainen, Mäki, & Blomgren, Reference Martikainen, Mäki and Blomgren2004; Qin, Agerbo, & Mortensen, Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003). One multilevel study which explored individual- and area-level deprivation in childhood and risk of later death by suicide found that most of the variance was explained by individual-level characteristics, with very little variation attributed to area-level factors (Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Gunnell, Lewis, Leckie, Dalman and Allebeck2014). O'Reilly and colleagues found similar results in Northern Ireland, where compositional factors, like gender, marital status, and socioeconomic disadvantage, accounted for most of the variance in suicide deaths (O'Reilly, Rosato, Connolly, & Cardwell, Reference O'Reilly, Rosato, Connolly and Cardwell2008). A recent study found a cross-sectional association between social cohesion and suicidal thoughts in Korean older adults (Kim & Park, Reference Kim and Park2021). Another study from Seoul found that 2.7% of the variation in rates of suicidal thoughts could be attributed to contextual factors, such as area-level organisational participation (Han & Lee, Reference Han and Lee2013). To date, few studies has used a longitudinal multilevel approach to examine if these results extend to other suicidal outcomes, including suicidal thoughts and attempts.

An additional challenge is that there are many ways to measure neighbourhood characteristics, including objective measures, such as neighbourhood deprivation, fragmentation, and physical infrastructure; and subjective factors, such as how residents feel about their neighbourhood, if they trust their neighbours, or if they feel connected to the local area. Much of the existing literature has focused on the relationship between suicidality and objectively measured neighbourhood characteristics like deprivation (Dupéré, Leventhal, & Lacourse, Reference Dupéré, Leventhal and Lacourse2009; Rehkopf, Reference Rehkopf2006) and social fragmentation (Middleton, Gunnell, Frankel, Whitley, & Dorling, Reference Middleton, Gunnell, Frankel, Whitley and Dorling2003). Less research has been conducted on the possible impact of subjectively reported neighbourhood characteristics on suicidal thoughts and attempts (Allen & Goldman-Mellor, Reference Allen and Goldman-Mellor2018). Previous research on adolescents in California found that subjective perceptions of neighbourhood safety and social cohesion were associated with suicidality, but objective measures of neighbourhood quality were unrelated to adolescent suicidal outcomes (Allen & Goldman-Mellor, Reference Allen and Goldman-Mellor2018). At the individual level, subjectively reported levels of trust in neighbours, institutions, and government have been linked to mental health outcomes (Lindstrom & Mohseni, Reference Lindstrom and Mohseni2009). There is some evidence that aggregated level of trust is linked to health outcomes (Engström, Mattsson, Järleborg, & Hallqvist, Reference Engström, Mattsson, Järleborg and Hallqvist2008).

To date, most studies have included a limited number of objective or subjective neighbourhood characteristics, often focusing on a single aspect of the neighbourhood environment. This has not allowed the comparison of the relative importance of subjective or objective neighbourhood characteristics.

Aims of the study

The current study aims to further the existing literature by examining the association between multiple objective and subjective neighbourhood characteristics, and subsequent suicidal thoughts and attempts. We included objectively measured neighbourhood deprivation and fragmentation. We also explored two subjectively rated trust measures: average trust in those living in the neighbourhood and average trust in public and political institutions, as these measures may capture distinct differences; the former estimates the trust, reciprocity, and bonds between neighbours, while the later estimates the confidence in official structures and institutions.

We expected that objective and subjective factors consistent with lower neighbourhood quality and cohesion, including low levels of trust, high levels of deprivation, and high fragmentation, would be linked to increased odds of both suicidal thoughts and suicide attempts. As there was little prior evidence to indicate if objective or subjective factors were more strongly linked to suicidality, we did not develop an a priori hypothesis regarding the relative magnitude of effect between these ways of estimating neighbourhood characteristics.

Methods

Study design and population

The study population included adults (age 18–64 at baseline) or older who participated in the Stockholm Public Health Cohort (SPHC) in 2002 and who participated in at least one follow-up survey in 2006, 2010, and or 2014. We excluded individuals who did not have record of their neighbourhood at baseline, did not answer questions on trust in 2002, or did not respond to the suicide questionnaire in at least one of the follow-up waves of SPHC. This study was covered under the ethical permissions from the Regional Ethical Review Committee in Stockholm (Dnr 2010/1185-31/5).

Outcomes

Our primary outcomes were suicidal thoughts and attempts, assessed using a self-report questionnaire administered in 2002 (baseline), 2006, 2010, and 2014. Participants were asked [translated] ‘Have you ever been in the situation that you seriously considered taking your own life, maybe even planned how you would do it?’ and ‘Have you ever made an attempt to take your life?’ Individuals who responded ‘Yes, in the last week’ or ‘Yes, in the last year’ were coded as having suicidal thoughts or suicide attempts during follow-up in order to ensure the suicidal thought and suicide attempts occurred after the baseline measure. Those who reported suicidal thoughts or attempts at baseline were excluded (n = 771).

Individual-level variables

We used variables from the SPHC questionnaire and linked registers to estimate a range of individual-level exposures. To estimate an individual's subjective view of their neighbourhood, we calculated two exposures: trust in residential area and average trust in public and political institutions. Trust in residential area was measured by the question ‘You can trust most of the people living in this neighbourhood’ with four possible response options from ‘completely agree’ to ‘completely disagree.’ Participants were also asked to rate their level of trust in public and political institutions, including the health care system, police, and politicians at local and national levels. Participants could rank their level of trust in each from (1) ‘no trust whatsoever’ to (5) ‘considerable trust.’ ‘No opinion’ was considered an equivocal response and coded as the middle response option. We summed the scores from these nine questions and rescaled them to create an ‘average trust in public and political institutions’ score between 1 and 45 (with higher value indicating higher levels of trust).

We used data from the Integrated Database for Health Insurance and Labour Market Studies (LISA) to calculate individual-level fragmentation and deprivation. Individual-level fragmentation was measured using relationship status, moving in the past year, and lone dwelling. Individual-level deprivation was measured using income quintile, education level, and employment status.

We had complete data on both individual-level objective measures (individual-level fragmentation and deprivation) for all those who had a valid address at cohort entry, however, we excluded individuals who did not have subjective measures at baseline (n = 115).

Neighbourhood-level variables

We obtained Small Area Market Statistics (SAMS) from Statistics Sweden to estimate neighbourhood exposures. SAMS use administrative boundaries to delineate socioeconomically homogenous areas containing between 1000 and 2000 individuals (Statistika Centralbån, 2016). We merged any SAMS area with less than 50 inhabitants with the adjacent SAMS (Dykxhoorn, Lewis, Hollander, Kirkbride, & Dalman, Reference Dykxhoorn, Lewis, Hollander, Kirkbride and Dalman2020), resulting in 889 neighbourhood areas in our cohort. Those individuals did not have a valid address at baseline and thus could not be assigned a neighbourhood were excluded (n = 18).

For subjective neighbourhood factors, we calculated the mean scores based on individual responses from residents of each neighbourhood and calculated the average trust in the residential area and average trust in public and political institutions based on all individual responses from the neighbourhood. We then used quintiles to divide these mean scores into fifths of neighbourhood-level trust in residential area and neighbourhood-level trust in public and political institutions, from quintile 1 (very low trust) to quintile 5 (very high trust).

Objective neighbourhood characteristics were generated for each neighbourhood that contained at least one SPHC participant at baseline. These were obtained through record linkage with the LISA and the Total Population Register (RTB).

Using methods similar to previous research (Allardyce et al., Reference Allardyce, Gilmour, Atkinson, Rapson, Bishop and McCreadie2005; Congdon, Reference Congdon1996) we generated a neighbourhood-level fragmentation index using three measures: neighbourhood residential mobility (proportion of people who moved in the previous year), proportion of lone dwellers, and proportion of individuals who were not married/in registered partnerships (see supplement). These indicators were z-standardised and summed to create a continuous index of fragmentation. We also used quintiles to divide neighbourhood-level fragmentation into fifths, from quintile 1 (very low fragmentation) to quintile 5 (very high fragmentation). We estimated neighbourhood-level deprivation in a similar fashion, using four measures from the LISA, including the proportion of people who were unemployed, receiving social welfare, had an income below the national median, or who had a criminal conviction. These measures were standardised and summed to create a continuous measure of neighbourhood-level deprivation. We used quintiles to divide neighbourhood-level deprivation into fifths.

Covariates

We controlled for a range of confounders from the SPHC questionnaire including age, sex, relationship status, lone dwelling, employment, general health (GHQ), occupational classification. We included educational attainment and residential moves using information from the LISA; and region of birth, income, and population density from the RTB.

Statistical analysis

We explored missingness within the cohort to determine if complete case analysis was appropriate. We then generated descriptive statistics of the sample. Next, using multilevel logistic regression, we investigated the association between neighbourhood characteristics at baseline and suicidality during follow-up. Individuals (level 1) were nested within neighbourhoods (level 2). We fitted a random intercept at the neighbourhood level to account for this higher-level clustering. We fitted random intercept Weibull models with normally distributed random effects to allow the baseline odds to vary across neighbourhoods. The null model, without fixed effects, was fitted to quantify the variation in the baseline odds for suicidality attributable to neighbourhood random effects. We then ran unadjusted and adjusted models and reported odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (95% CI).

Results

Study sample

There were 20 764 individuals, from 889 neighbourhoods in Stockholm County (Table 1). The median number of participants in each neighbourhood was 44 (min: 1, max: 406 (IQR: 22–92). Individual levels of trust in the residential area was high, with 51.7% reporting ‘pretty much’ and 38.1% reporting ‘very much’ trust in the residential area. 14.6% of the sample had suicidal thoughts and 3.7% had attempted suicide during follow-up. Table 1 shows additional cohort characteristics.

Table 1. Baseline characteristics from the Stockholm public health cohort

We examined the full cohort and determined that the proportion of missing data was low across study variables. The highest proportion was 5.3% missing in occupational position, with all other variables having between 0.0% and 2.0% missing (Fig. 1, online Supplementary Table S1). Due to low proportions of missingness, we used complete cases analysis.

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow chart.

Suicidal thoughts

Individual-level factors and suicidality

At the individual level, both objective and subjective factors were associated with increased odds of suicidal thoughts at follow-up. In the adjusted analysis, those with lower individual-level trust in the residential area had elevated odds of reporting suicidal thoughts: some trust (OR 1.20, 95% CI 1.10–1.32), low trust (OR 1.69, 95% CI 1.46–1.95), and very low trust (OR 1.62, 95% CI 1.25–2.10) compared to those with high levels of trust (Fig. 2). Each unit decrease in trust corresponded to a 24% increase in odds of reporting suicidal thoughts (95% CI 16–31). A similar pattern was found for the average trust in public and political institutions, with each unit decrease in an individual's score on the average trust in public and political institutions corresponding to increased odds of suicidal thoughts (OR 1.18, 95% CI 1.15–1.22). Participants with higher individual-level disconnection and fragmentation from their local area (including recent residential moves, lone dwelling, and unmarried/divorced/widowed relationship status) had increased odds of suicidal thoughts. Compared to those with low fragmentation scores, individuals with higher scores had increased odds of suicidal thoughts: medium (OR 1.12, 95% CI 1.01–1.25), high (OR 1.26, 95% CI 1.11–1.42), or very high (OR 1.54, 95% CI 1.18–2.03). Individuals with high levels of deprivation, including low income, low educational attainment, and were unemployed had elevated odds of suicidal thoughts when compared to low individual-level deprivation: medium (OR 1.37, 95% CI 1.25–1.51), and high (OR 2.05, 95% CI 1.62–2.60).

Fig. 2. Individual-level factors and suicidal thoughtsa aAdjusted for sex, age, migrant status, relationship status, lone dwelling, residential moves, population density

Suicide attempts

There was some evidence for similar patterns of increasing odds of suicide attempts by individual-level factors. There was evidence for increased odds of suicide attempts with decreasing individual-level trust in residential area (unit decrease: OR 1.20, 95% CI 1.14–1.28) and average trust in public and political institutions (unit decrease OR 1.20, 95% CI 1.14–1.28) (online Supplementary Table S3). Individual-level fragmentation and deprivation scores were also associated with suicide attempts. For each unit increase in fragmentation, the odds of suicide attempts were 1.18 (95% CI 1.07–1.31), following adjustment for covariates. Compared to the lowest level of individual deprivation, individuals experiencing medium or high deprivation had elevated odds of suicide attempts (OR 1.81, 95% CI 1.53–2.16; OR 3.11, 95% CI 2.29–4.44, respectively) Fig. 3.

Fig. 3. Individual-level factors and suicide attemptsb bAdjusted for sex, age, migrant status, relationship status, lone dwelling, residential moves, population density.

Neighbourhood-level factors and suicidality

At the neighbourhood level, the unadjusted estimate for trust in residential area suggested a 13% increase in the odds of suicidal thoughts with each unit decrease (95% CI 10–17) (online Supplementary Table S4), but once fully adjusted for individual-level trust and sociodemographic factors, the association was null (OR 1.01, 95% CI 0.97–1.05) (Fig. 4). A weaker association was found between neighbourhood mean score on average trust in public and political institutions and suicidal thoughts in the unadjusted analysis (OR 1.03, 95% CI 1.00–1.06), which was also fully attenuated upon adjustment (OR 1.03, 95% CI 0.98–1.04).

Fig. 4. Neighbourhood-level factors and suicidal thoughtsc cAdjusted for sex, age, migrant status, relationship status, lone dwelling, residential moves, education, income, employment, population density; Q = quintile; ref = reference category.

In contrast, objectively measured neighbourhood deprivation was found to be associated with an association with suicidal thoughts following individual adjustment (unit increase: OR 1.06, 95% CI 1.03–1.09) and when fully adjusted for individual-level factors and sociodemographic factors (OR 1.04, 95% CI 1.00–1.07). Neighbourhood fragmentation suggested an association between higher levels of fragmentation and suicidal thoughts in the unadjusted estimate (OR 1.11, 95% CI 1.08–1.14), which persisted when adjusted for individual measures of fragmentation (OR 1.06, 95% CI 1.03–1.09), but attenuated once sociodemographic factors were taken into account (OR 1.02, 95% CI 0.98–1.06).

These patterns were broadly consistent with those for suicide attempts. Lower neighbourhood-level mean trust in residential areas corresponded to increased odds of suicide attempts, even following adjustment (unit decrease: OR 1.09, 95% CI 1.02–1.17), driven by high odds in the neighbourhoods with the lowest scores (very low trust: OR 1.44, 95% CI 1.05–1.95). The fully adjusted estimates did not show an association between neighbourhood-level average trust in public and political institutions score and suicide attempts, echoing what was found for suicidal thoughts.

Objectively measured neighbourhood-level deprivation was found to be related to increased suicide attempts following adjustment for individual-level deprivation and sociodemographic factors (OR 1.11, 95% CI 1.06–1.17). Neighbourhood-level fragmentation was associated with increased odds of suicide attempts in the unadjusted (OR 1.19, 95% CI 1.12–1.26) and in the individually adjusted estimate (OR 1.12, 95% CI 1.05–1.19), but these elevated odds were attenuated when fully adjusted (OR 1.07, 95% CI 0.99–1.16) (online Supplementary Table S5) Fig. 5.

Fig. 5. Neighbourhood-level factors and suicide attemptsd dAdjusted for sex, age, migrant status, relationship status, lone dwelling, residential moves, education, income, employment, population density; Q = quintile; ref = reference category.

Discussion

Principal findings

At the individual level, both objective (high deprivation and fragmentation) and subjective (low trust in residential area and trust in public and political institutions) were associated with increased odds of suicidal thoughts and attempts.

At the neighbourhood level, higher levels of deprivation were associated with increased odds of suicidal thoughts and attempts. Decreasing average trust in residential area was associated with increased odds of suicide attempts, although this was not found for suicidal thoughts. Neighbourhood-level fragmentation and average trust in public and political institutions were not associated with increased odds of suicidality once individual characteristics were considered.

Limitations and strengths

There are several limitations that must be noted, including cohort response rates and limited information for calculating social fragmentation. Among those selected for the survey, the response rates in 2002, 2006, and 2010 were 62, 61, and 56%, respectively (Svensson et al., Reference Svensson, Fredlund, Laflamme, Hallqvist, Alfredsson and Ekbom2013). This study may have been impacted by selection bias, as the response rate was lower among men, migrant populations, and those with lower levels of income and education (Svensson et al., Reference Svensson, Fredlund, Laflamme, Hallqvist, Alfredsson and Ekbom2013). It is plausible that the response rate may have been lower for those experiencing suicidal thoughts and attempts as well as those living in more deprived areas. There may also be differential losses to follow-up over the cohort period, as those experiencing mental health problems or socioeconomic difficulties may be more likely to drop out from subsequent waves of the survey.

Although we had access to detailed information on individuals and neighbourhoods in the population registers and cohort data, we were limited to what was available in these sources, which restricted the construction of our social fragmentation indicator. In previous literature, the Congdon anomie score has been frequently used to estimate social fragmentation. This score includes four aspects: (1) % of persons who have moved in the last year, (2) % unmarried persons, (3) % single person households, and (4) % of persons in private rented accommodations (Congdon, Reference Congdon1996). We did not have access to information about housing tenure, so we only included three measures in our social fragmentation score. This deviation from the Congdon score may mean that these results are not directly comparable to other studies of social fragmentation.

A key strength of this analysis was the rich data available in the Stockholm Public Health Cohort and linked Swedish registers. We were able to combine self-report responses from the cohort study with register information to generate detailed estimates of exposures, outcomes, and confounders. Further, we were able to calculate area-level exposures on small, demographically homogenous areas (SAMS), which reduced the risk of exposure misclassification. Previous research on the appropriate scale for area-based analysis must balance size and power, as smaller areas tend to be more homogeneous but often lack the power to detect an effect. This has been demonstrated in a previous study, which found that the association between suicide rates and area deprivation disappeared when moving from smaller areas (wards) to larger areas (local authorities) (Rezaeian, Dunn, St. Leger, & Appleby, Reference Rezaeian, Dunn, St. Leger and Appleby2006). We measured exposures at both the individual and neighbourhood levels and used a multilevel design to clarify the relative importance of the factors at each level. This allowed us to isolate the added effect of neighbourhood characteristics, above the individual factors. We used a longitudinal design to reduce the risk of differential recall, as those experiencing suicidal thoughts or attempts may have a tendency to report lower levels of neighbourhood trust (Engström et al., Reference Engström, Mattsson, Järleborg and Hallqvist2008).

Comparisons with previous literature

The findings at the individual level were consistent with previous research which has shown an association between suicidal thoughts or attempts and individual sociodemographic characteristics, including income (Wetherall, Daly, Robb, Wood, & O'Connor, Reference Wetherall, Daly, Robb, Wood and O'Connor2015), employment (Gunnell, Harbord, Singleton, Jenkins, & Lewis, Reference Gunnell, Harbord, Singleton, Jenkins and Lewis2004), education, relationship status (Gunnell et al., Reference Gunnell, Harbord, Singleton, Jenkins and Lewis2004), lone dwelling (Gisle & Van Oyen, Reference Gisle and Van Oyen2013), and residential moves (Forman-Hoffman, Glasheen, & Ridenour, Reference Forman-Hoffman, Glasheen and Ridenour2018). There has been less research on the association of neighbourhood-level factors on suicidality. Like our study, previous research has shown different associations between deprivation and fragmentation on suicidality. Importantly, much of the existing literature uses ecological designs to explore the relationship between suicide mortality and neighbourhood characteristics, which may exhibit different patterns of association to suicidal thoughts and attempts. Previous studies have showed a relationship between neighbourhood deprivation and suicidal thoughts and attempts (Cairns, Graham, & Bambra, Reference Cairns, Graham and Bambra2017; O'Farrell et al., Reference O'Farrell, Corcoran and Perry2016). One study using national mortality statistics found that suicide deaths were predicted by neighbourhood deprivation, but that fragmentation was only associated with suicide risk in specified population sub-groups (O'Farrell et al., Reference O'Farrell, Corcoran and Perry2016). This contrasts with earlier research which found at an ecological level that suicide was more strongly associated with fragmentation than deprivation (Whitley et al., Reference Whitley, Gunnell, Dorling and Smith1999). To our knowledge, association between neighbourhood-level trust and suicidal outcomes have not been explored in previous research.

When comparing objective (deprivation and fragmentation) with subjective (residential trust and trust in public and political institutions) at the neighbourhood-level, we found differing patterns of association with suicidal thoughts and attempts. One objective measure, neighbourhood-level deprivation, was linked to both suicidal thoughts and attempts, but the other objective measure, neighbourhood-level fragmentation, was not associated with suicidality. Similarly, one subjective measure, neighbourhood-level residential trust, was found to be associated with suicide attempts, but the other subjective measure (neighbourhood level trust in public and political institutions) was not associated with suicidal thoughts or attempts. This varies from a previous cross-sectional study of adolescents, which found an association between perceived neighbourhood characteristics and suicidal thoughts and behaviours, but no evidence for a link between objective measures and suicidality (Allen & Goldman-Mellor, Reference Allen and Goldman-Mellor2018). However, the relative import of objective and subjective factors remains equivocal and requires further exploration in longitudinal population cohorts.

Meaning of findings

The unadjusted models suggested that some of the variance in odds of suicidal thoughts and attempts may be explained by neighbourhood factors, including neighbourhood-level residential trust, trust in public and political institutions, fragmentation, and deprivation. However, once individual scores and sociodemographic characteristics were considered only neighbourhood-level deprivation and residential trust remained associated with suicidality. Specifically, increasing neighbourhood-level deprivation was associated with increased odds of suicidal thoughts and attempts, while decreasing residential trust was associated with increased odds of suicide attempts.

The complete attenuation of the association with other neighbourhood characteristics, including neighbourhood-level fragmentation and trust in public and political institutions supports a composition rather than contextual explanation for the results. The multilevel design of this study has enabled us to begin to disentangle the contextual and compositional effects, and these findings suggest that observed neighbourhood variation in suicidality may be largely attributed to the compositional characteristics of the people living within the neighbourhood. Residential segregation by socioeconomic status is persistent in many cities, leading to clustering of individuals with similar socioeconomic status in neighbourhood areas (Pickett & Wilkinson, Reference Pickett and Wilkinson2008). Thus, once the individual characteristics are taken into account, the additional effect of neighbourhood factors was marginal, suggesting that observed associations between most area-level factors and rates of suicidal thoughts and attempts can be attributed to the characteristics of the residents. However, it is possible that other neighbourhood characteristics, like availability of green or blue spaces, transit connectivity, or the density of alcohol outlets might have a larger contribution to rates of suicidality, which could be explored in future studies.

In contrast, the associations between neighbourhood-level deprivation and average residential trust following adjustment for individual and demographic factors, provide evidence for contextual effects, where area-level factors conferred additional risk beyond individual and compositional explanations. Neighbourhood-level deprivation was linked to both increased suicidal thoughts and attempts, while residential trust remained associated with elevated odds of suicide attempts, following adjustment for individual and demographic characteristics.

Living in neighbourhoods characterised by high levels of deprivation and distrust of neighbours may be linked to suicidal thoughts and attempts through entrapment or defeat, consistent with the arrested flight (Gilbert & Allan, Reference Gilbert and Allan1998), cry of pain (Johnson, Gooding, & Tarrier, Reference Johnson, Gooding and Tarrier2008), Schematic Appraisal Model of Suicide (Hagedoorn et al., Reference Hagedoorn, Groenewegen, Roberts and Helbich2020), and the Integrated Motivational-Volitional Model of suicidal behaviour (O'Connor & Kirtley, Reference O'Connor and Kirtley2018; O'Connor & Nock, Reference O'Connor and Nock2014). These models suggest that individuals who feel trapped by external circumstances may exhibit suicidal thoughts as they fail to find a way to escape their problems. Individuals living in deprived neighbourhoods may be more likely to feel trapped and without options for escape (Turney, Kissane, & Edin, Reference Turney, Kissane and Edin2013).

These results suggest that multifaceted suicide prevention activities which target high-risk individuals clustering within neighbourhoods as well as high-risk neighbourhoods, characterised by high deprivation and distrust could be useful in reducing suicidal thoughts and attempts. Before such public mental health interventions could be implemented, further understanding of the underlying mechanisms which drive the contextual effects are warranted, as is further evidence for effective area-based suicide prevention interventions.

Conclusions

Our findings show that while much of the neighbourhood variation in suicidal thoughts and attempts can be attributed to compositional characteristics of the area, neighbourhood-level deprivation and residential trust may confer additional risk. These findings support both compositional and contextual explanations for area-level variation in suicidality. These results suggest that multifaceted suicide prevention programmes which target both high-risk individuals and high-risk neighbourhoods could be beneficial in reducing rates of suicidal thoughts and attempts.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721002579.

Data

The data used in this study are available from the Stockholm Public Health Cohort. Request to access the data can be made to the Stockholm County through the Steering Committee: http://folkhalsoguiden.se/halsa-stockholm/halsa-stockholm---for-forskare/. Interested researchers can apply to access these data in the same manner as the authors.

Acknowledgements

JD, JH, KA were supported by the NIHR University College London Hospital, Biomedical Research Centre. JH was supported by the Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Career Development Fellowship. ASW received support from the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare, grant 2017-00173. CD received support from Psychiatry Sweden grant 523-2010-1052. JD received support from the NIHR School for Public Health Research. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Allardyce, J., Gilmour, H., Atkinson, J., Rapson, T., Bishop, J., & McCreadie, R. G. (2005). Social fragmentation, deprivation and urbanicity: Relation to first-admission rates for psychoses. British Journal of Psychiatry, 187, 401406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, K., & Goldman-Mellor, S. (2018). Neighborhood characteristics and adolescent suicidal behavior: Evidence from a population-based study. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 48, 677689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cairns, J. M., Graham, E., & Bambra, C. (2017). Area-level socioeconomic disadvantage and suicidal behaviour in Europe: A systematic review. Elsevier Ltd Social Science and Medicine, 192, 102111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Congdon, P. (1996). Suicide and parasuicide in London: A small-area study. Urban Studies, 33, 137158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conrad, R., Walz, F., Geiser, F., Imbierowicz, K., Liedtke, R., & Wegener, I. (2009). Temperament and character personality profile in relation to suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in major depressed patients. Psychiatry Research, 170, 212217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dupéré, V., Leventhal, T., & Lacourse, E. (2009). Neighborhood poverty and suicidal thoughts and attempts in late adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 39, 12951306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durkheim, E. (1897). On suicide (2006th ed.). London; New York: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Dykxhoorn, J., Lewis, G., Hollander, A.-C., Kirkbride, J. B., & Dalman, C. (2020). Association of neighbourhood migrant density and risk of non-affective psychosis: A national, longitudinal cohort study. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 7, 327336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engström, K., Mattsson, F., Järleborg, A., & Hallqvist, J. (2008). Contextual social capital as a risk factor for poor self-rated health: A multilevel analysis. Social Science and Medicine, 66, 22682280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forman-Hoffman, V. L., Glasheen, C., & Ridenour, T. A. (2018). Residential transience and substance use disorder are independently associated with suicidal thoughts, plans, and attempts in a nationally representative sample of U.S. Adults. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 48, 401412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P., & Allan, S. (1998). The role of defeat and entrapment (arrested flight) in depression: An exploration of an evolutionary view. Psychological Medicine, 28, 585598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gisle, L., & Van Oyen, H. (2013). Household composition and suicidal behaviour in the adult population of Belgium. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 48, 11151124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnell, D., Harbord, R., Singleton, N., Jenkins, R., & Lewis, G. (2004). Factors influencing the development and amelioration of suicidal thoughts in the general population: Cohort study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 185, 385393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagedoorn, P., Groenewegen, P. P., Roberts, H., & Helbich, M. (2020). Is suicide mortality associated with neighbourhood social fragmentation and deprivation? A Dutch register-based case-control study using individualised neighbourhoods. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 74, 197202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, S., & Lee, H. S. (2013). Factors associated with suicidal ideation: The role of context. Oxford Academic Journal of Public Health (United Kingdom), 35, 228236.Google ScholarPubMed
Johnson, J., Gooding, P., & Tarrier, N. (2008). Suicide risk in schizophrenia: Explanatory models and clinical implications, The Schematic Appraisal Model of Suicide (SAMS). Psychology and Psychotherapy, 81, 5577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J., & Park, M. J. (2021). Multilevel effect of neighborhood social cohesion and characteristics on suicidal ideation among Korean older adults. Community Mental Health Journal, 57, 522528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindstrom, M., & Mohseni, M. (2009). Social capital, political trust and self-reported psychological health: A population-based study. Social Science and Medicine, 68, 436443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martikainen, P., Mäki, N., & Blomgren, J. (2004). The effects of area and individual social characteristics on suicide risk: A multilevel study of relative contribution and effect modification. European Journal of Population, 20, 323350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Middleton, N., Gunnell, D., Frankel, S., Whitley, E., & Dorling, D. (2003). Urban–rural differences in suicide trends in young adults: England and Wales, 1981–1998. Social Science & Medicine, 57, 11831194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Middleton, N., Whitley, E., Frankel, S., Dorling, D., Sterne, J., & Gunnell, D. (2004). Suicide risk in small areas in England and Wales, 1991–1993. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 39, 4552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, R. C., & Kirtley, O. J. (2018). The integrated motivational-volitional model of suicidal behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(20170268), 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, R. C., & Nock, M. K. (2014). The psychology of suicidal behaviour. The Lancet Psychiatry, 1, 7385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Farrell, I. B., Corcoran, P., & Perry, I. J. (2016). The area level association between suicide, deprivation, social fragmentation and population density in the Republic of Ireland: A national study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 51, 839847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Reilly, D. O., Rosato, M., Connolly, S., & Cardwell, C. (2008). Area factors and suicide: 5-year follow-up of the northern Ireland population. The British Journal of Psychiatry : The Journal of Mental Science, 192, 106111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickett, K. E., & Wilkinson, R. G. (2008). People like us: Ethnic group density effects on health. Ethnicity & Health, 13, 321334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qin, P., Agerbo, E., & Mortensen, P. B. (2003). Suicide risk in relation to socioeconomic, demographic, psychiatric, and familial factors: A national register-based study of all suicides in Denmark, 1981–1997. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 765772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rehkopf, D. B. S. (2006). The association between suicide and the socio-economic characteristics of geographical areas: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 36, 145157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rezaeian, M., Dunn, G., St. Leger, S., & Appleby, L. (2006). Ecological association between suicide rates and indices of deprivation in the north west region of England: The importance of the size of the administrative unit. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 60, 956961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistika Centralbån (2016) Statistics Sweden's market profiles. Available at: https://www.scb.se/contentassets/2ce41d60bac84a4c9ba1d86f8dfb242a/product-catalogue-2017.pdf.Google Scholar
Svensson, A. C., Fredlund, P., Laflamme, L., Hallqvist, J., Alfredsson, L., Ekbom, A., ... Magnusson, C. (2013). Cohort profile: The Stockholm Public Health Cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 12631272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turney, K., Kissane, R., & Edin, K. (2013). After moving to opportunity: How moving to a low-poverty neighborhood improves mental health among African American women. Society and Mental Health, 3, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wetherall, K., Daly, M., Robb, K. A., Wood, A. M., & O'Connor, R. C. (2015). Explaining the income and suicidality relationship: Income rank is more strongly associated with suicidal thoughts and attempts than income. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 50, 929937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitley, E., Gunnell, D., Dorling, D., & Smith, G. D. (1999). Ecological study of social fragmentation, poverty, and suicide. British Medical Journal, 319, 10341037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zammit, S., Gunnell, D., Lewis, G., Leckie, G., Dalman, C., & Allebeck, P. (2014). Individual- and area-level influence on suicide risk: A multilevel longitudinal study of Swedish schoolchildren. Psychological Medicine, 44, 267277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline characteristics from the Stockholm public health cohort

Figure 1

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow chart.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Individual-level factors and suicidal thoughtsa aAdjusted for sex, age, migrant status, relationship status, lone dwelling, residential moves, population density

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Individual-level factors and suicide attemptsb bAdjusted for sex, age, migrant status, relationship status, lone dwelling, residential moves, population density.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Neighbourhood-level factors and suicidal thoughtsccAdjusted for sex, age, migrant status, relationship status, lone dwelling, residential moves, education, income, employment, population density; Q = quintile; ref = reference category.

Figure 5

Fig. 5. Neighbourhood-level factors and suicide attemptsddAdjusted for sex, age, migrant status, relationship status, lone dwelling, residential moves, education, income, employment, population density; Q = quintile; ref = reference category.

Supplementary material: File

Dykxhoorn et al. supplementary material

Dykxhoorn et al. supplementary material

Download Dykxhoorn et al. supplementary material(File)
File 71.3 KB