Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T01:05:40.083Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Naming Names: The Impact of Supreme Court Opinion Attribution on Citizen Assessment of Policy Outcomes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2024

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The manner in which political institutions convey their policy outcomes can have important implications for how the public views institutions' policy decisions. This paper explores whether the way in which the U.S. Supreme Court communicates its policy decrees affects how favorably members of the public assess its decisions. Specifically, we investigate whether attributing a decision to the nation's High Court or to an individual justice influences the public's agreement with the Court's rulings. Using an experimental design, we find that when a Supreme Court outcome is ascribed to the institution as a whole, rather than to a particular justice, people are more apt to agree with the policy decision. We also find that identifying the gender of the opinion author affects public agreement under certain conditions. Our findings have important implications for how public support for institutional policymaking operates, as well as the dynamics of how the Supreme Court manages to accumulate and maintain public goodwill.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© 2019 Law and Society Association.

The manner in which institutions convey policy choices to the rest of the world has long interested students of politics and public policy and represents a core concern of governance dynamics (e.g., Reference DruckmanDruckman 2001; Reference EastonEaston 1965; Reference EstlundEstlund 2007; Reference MondakMondak 1992). In the case of the U.S. Supreme Court, the American convention of attributing court decisions to justices in signed opinions involves a careful balancing of important and sometimes competing concerns, including judicial independence, accountability, and how the opinions will be perceived by external political actors and the public. Legal scholars have advanced strong opinions on whether the U.S. Supreme Court's approach to transmitting its case decisions is optimal or might be better handled in another manner. Reference FissFiss (1983) argues that the current method of individually signed majority opinions best serves the Court's institutional legitimacy while also promoting justices' accountability for their decisions. On the other hand, some suggest that the Court's institutional credibility and robustness would be better served through the use of anonymous opinions—similar to the practice employed in civil law countries (Reference BozzoBozzo 2015; Reference MarkhamMarkham 2006).

In this paper, we address a puzzle that persists regarding how the U.S. High Court fosters and preserves legitimacy and support among the public: can the manner in which the Court communicates its policy decrees affect whether members of the public react favorably to its legal decisions? Specifically, we are interested in whether citizens' agreement with a decision by the Court is affected by source cues tied to the identity of the majority opinion author. We argue that majority opinions attributed to the U.S. Supreme Court as a whole should enjoy higher levels of agreement than those attributed to particular justices because the Court-attributed opinions will connote neutrality, credibility, and institutional legitimacy (Reference Bartels and MutzBartels and Mutz 2009; Reference Gibson, Lodge and WoodsonGibson et al. 2014; Reference HoekstraHoekstra 1995).

Using an experimental design with approximately 1200 respondents, we find support for our primary thesis that legal decisions attributed to the U.S. Supreme Court—as opposed to a specific justice—enjoy higher levels of agreement. Our results also confirm that this relationship is conditioned on citizens' ideological identity. In extended analyses, we find that environmental cultural influences help determine whether and how the gender of the attributed justice has an impact on citizen agreement with U.S. Supreme Court decisions.

For the U.S. Supreme Court, these considerations are especially significant given that the Court lacks the means possessed by Congress or the executive to enforce and implement its judgments. The Court is an unusually vulnerable policy-making institution that relies on public support to maintain its policy viability and protect it from institutional encroachment (e.g., Reference CaldeiraCaldeira 1986). As Reference GibsonGibson (2012, Reference Gibson, Bornstein and Tomkins2015) and others have noted, “legitimacy is for losers.” In other words, a person's assessment of the legitimacy of an institution and the legitimacy of its policy decisions quite often turns on whether they agree with them. The U.S. Supreme Court typically only has to draw from its “reservoir of good will” when citizens disagree with its verdicts. Hence, making policy decisions in a manner that is more palatable to a wider portion of the public, all else being equal, goes a long way toward an institution being able to maintain long-term diffuse support from the public (i.e., legitimacy) and enjoy its governing benefits (2015: 82–84).

The paper proceeds as follows. Section 1 lays out the theoretical framework for understanding agreement with U.S. Supreme Court decisions, and Section 2 describes our experimental design. In Section 3, we discuss the results from our main models and then in Section 5 test whether our findings on justice attribution are affected by the gender of the opinion author. In the final section of our paper, we discuss the implications of our findings for American politics and legal policymaking and suggest potential paths for future research on this subject.

Theoretical Framework

Agreement and Supreme Court Decisions

Because the U.S. Supreme Court is dependent on public support to maintain its legitimacy and to ensure the implementation of its rulings, scholars have focused a great deal of their attention on its public perception (Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2003; Reference Gibson and CaldeiraGibson and Caldeira 1992; Reference Scheb and LyonsScheb and Lyons 2000). Notable in this literature is the conclusion that the American High Court benefits from a robust sense of legitimacy not enjoyed by other high courts (Reference Gibson, Caldeira and BairdGibson et al. 1998) and that this diffuse support does not turn on affection for its decisions (Reference Gibson and CaldeiraGibson and Caldeira 2009). In recent years, others have argued that assessments of legitimacy are at least in part a function of ideological agreement with the Court, with the implication being that the U.S. Supreme Court's legitimacy may not be so stable after all (Reference Bartels and JohnstonBartels and Johnston 2013; Reference Christenson and GlickChristenson and Glick 2015; but see Reference Gibson and NelsonGibson and Nelson 2015). Central to this debate is the question of how the public actually processes output from the Court (Reference Johnston, Sunshine Hillygus and BartelsJohnston et al. 2014). Given that legitimacy is most important for those who do not agree with the Court's decisions (Reference GibsonGibson 2012; Reference Gibson, Lodge and WoodsonGibson et al. 2014) and can impact implementation (Reference Canon and JohnsonCanon and Johnson 1984), it becomes important to understand what factors influence public agreement with the specific decisions of the Court.

While considerably less attention has been paid to studying citizens' agreement with the U.S. Supreme Court's decisions than citizens' perceptions of the legitimacy of such decisions, the existing research points to the importance of the content of Court opinions (Reference Baas and ThomasBaas and Thomas 1984; Reference MondakMondak 1994; Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009), as well as attributes of the Court (Reference Boddery and YatesBoddery and Yates 2014; Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009) and the framing of the decision by the media (Reference MondakMondak 1994; Reference ZilisZilis 2015). One especially promising avenue for unpacking agreement dynamics focuses on the use of source cues as heuristics in public opinion about the U.S. Supreme Court. Because U.S. Supreme Court rulings are often complicated and can be difficult for a lay person or even journalists (Reference Slotnick and SegalSlotnick and Segal 1994) to understand easily, relying on heuristics can ease the cognitive burden for those trying to understand and evaluate the Court's decision in a case. Such heuristic cue-following allows people to make quick assessments on complex matters in order to help make sense of the world around them in an efficient and largely effective manner (e.g., Reference KahnemanKahneman 2011; Reference Nicholson and HansfordNicholson and Hansford 2014; Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014). In experimental settings, researchers have found that the public is responsive to partisan source cues about the Court (Reference Clark and KastellecClark and Kastellec 2015; Reference Nicholson and HansfordNicholson and Hansford 2014) and that the ideology of the opinion author in particular serves as a source cue that conditions individual agreement with court decisions—even those that run counter to an individual's expressed policy preferences (Reference Boddery and YatesBoddery and Yates 2014). In the section that follows, we build on these insights to lay out an account that describes how the attribution of a U.S. Supreme Court decision on a case affects the public's feelings about that decision.

The Power of Attribution

While opinion writing on the U.S. Supreme Court typically involves contributions and negotiations by other members of the Court, the majority opinion writer represents the agreed upon edict and rational of the collective. Thus, as one scholar observed, “even though a single [j]ustice signs the opinion by name, the text insists throughout on its shared provenance as the voice not just of its author but of all those who have voted to join it” (Reference RayRay 2000: 518). Indeed, Justice Breyer faced a firestorm when he inadvertently used the pronoun “I” in a 1999 majority opinion (Reference BozzoBozzo 2015; Reference MarkhamMarkham 2006; Reference MauroMauro 1999; Reference RayRay 2000).

The tradition of the U.S. Supreme Court issuing a single signed opinion has been credited to Chief Justice John Marshall, who prevailed on the matter in a less famous disagreement with Thomas Jefferson, who favored the use of seriatim opinions (Reference GinsburgGinsburg 1990: 138). But even the Court itself does not always follow this American convention. The U.S. High Court regularly issues unsigned “per curiam” opinions to convey its case decisions, and state high courts and the U.S. Courts of Appeals also follow this practice. While per curiam decrees are traditionally associated with routine low-salience cases, critics of the practice point to numerous U.S. Supreme Court case opinions that have been delivered in this manner that addressed important policy and political issues (e.g., Bush v. Gore 2000). Most recently, in the wake of Justice Scalia's death, the eight-member Roberts Court handed down a per curiam unanimous decision in a controversial case challenging the contraception mandate in President Obama's Affordable Care Act (Zubik v. Burwell 2016).

As U.S. Supreme Court scholars have documented, the choice of which justice will be assigned the majority opinion often reflects considerations about the symbolic value of particular justices and their ability to “help make policy more palatable to external actors, including the other political institutions and the public” (Reference Epstein and KnightEpstein and Knight 1998: 127). For instance, Chief Justice Harlan Stone assigned the majority opinion in Korematsu v. United States (1944) to Hugo Black, precisely because of his reputation as a civil libertarian (Reference Epstein and KnightEpstein and Knight 1998: 127). Similarly, in United States v. Virginia (1996), Chief Justice Rehnquist assigned the majority opinion striking down Virginia Military Institute's male-only admissions policy to Justice Ginsburg, as a nod to her expertise (Reference Maltzman and WahlbeckMaltzman and Wahlbeck 1996) and lengthy experience litigating sex discrimination cases prior to becoming a federal judge. These examples suggest that members of the U.S. Supreme Court recognize the importance of opinion assignment and act strategically when selecting opinion authors because they believe it will have an impact on how the policy edict of the Court will be perceived.Footnote 1

Norms about how to attribute judicial decisions can vary a great deal across different court settings, but on the U.S. Supreme Court, the majority opinion author is identified in the case as delivering the “opinion of the Court.” This practice melds together both individual and institutional attributions to a decision, though of course, there are a number of exceptions to this practice throughout the Court's history (see Reference MarkhamMarkham 2006). However, the public generally learns of U.S. Supreme Court decisions not directly from Court opinions, but from media outlets (Reference DavisDavis 2014; Reference Johnston and BartelsJohnston and Bartels 2010), and the way that a ruling is framed has been shown to have an impact on citizen assessments of the Court and its decisions (Reference Baird and GanglBaird and Gangl 2006; Reference Clawson and WaltenburgClawson and Waltenburg 2003; Reference ZilisZilis 2015).Footnote 2 Of course, media framing related to attribution of Court opinions is malleable; the media can make meaningful and consequential discretionary decisions on how it chooses to portray Court decisions. Nonetheless, the U.S. Supreme Court ultimately has the final say on how a decision is attributed.Footnote 3

But less is known about how specific attribution choices made by the U.S. Supreme Court affect public agreement with the Court's decisions. One attribution approach is to characterize the decision as merely coming “from the Supreme Court” without providing additional context about which particular justice authored the opinion. By crediting the institution with the decision this approach would seem to garner greater acceptance of the decision by members of the public by connoting neutrality, credibility, clarity, legality, and even institutional legitimacy (Reference BairdBaird 2001; Reference Bartels and MutzBartels and Mutz 2009; Reference Brigham and MonkkonenBrigham 1987; Reference Gibson, Lodge and WoodsonGibson et al. 2014; Reference HoekstraHoekstra 1995; Reference MondakMondak 1992). This general deference to and reverence of the U.S. High Court (and its actions) has been described as “positivity theory” (e.g., Reference GibsonGibson 2007). In other words, by disentangling the identity of a policy edict from a specific justice (who may be associated with past voting choices or his or her nominating president) and attributing it to the nation's highest legal institution—cloaked in the accouterments of stylized symbols of justice—a verdict that could otherwise appear politically driven is made more palatable, all else being equal. Certainly, the Court has used unattributed per curiam opinions to convey its judgments in particularly volatile cases dealing with politically charged issues involving freedom of the press (New York Times Co. v. United States 1971), capital punishment (Furman v. Georgia 1972) and national elections (Bush v. Gore 2000) among others.

On the other hand, Reference Nicholson and HansfordNicholson and Hansford (2014) cast doubt on this view, as they find minimal evidence that attributing a decision as coming from the U.S. Supreme Court changes how heavily the public relies on partisan cues in its evaluations of a decision. While attributing the outcome of a case to the Court increased public acceptance of the decision in certain instances, the effect was quite small. Assaying a variety of heuristic cues concerning partisan attribution and other political considerations, they found that “public expectations for the Court and its decisions may be no different than expectations for the elected branches of government” (Reference Nicholson and HansfordNicholson and Hansford 2014: 15). Providing additional insight is the work of Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. (2009) on the effect of majority coalition size on citizens' willingness to agree with and find acceptable a U.S. Supreme Court decision. They found that when the justices are unanimous on a case ruling, the public is more likely to agree with the case and find it acceptable than when it is a split majority coalition—even when the Court's decision is at odds with citizens' ideological preferences. This suggests that outcomes that are perceived by the public as not turning on ideological or political lines (i.e., they are agreed to by all justices, regardless of well-known ideological divisions) are more apt to be found satisfactory. In contrast, other studies have found that unanimous votes do not have a positive effect on public views about U.S. Supreme Court decisions (Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2005), and that split decisions may even enhance public views of the Court outcomes under certain conditions (Reference SalamoneSalamone 2014). These findings provide only indirect evidence for the premise that the Court as an institution can act as a heuristic cue because they focus on Court unanimity rather than attribution, but they do prompt us to think about the impact of different ways in which the U.S. Supreme Court presents policy announcements to the public.

We endeavor to address a closely related concern inspired by the scholarly arguments of Justice Ruth Bader Ginsburg, among others, regarding comparative approaches to how legal institutions convey legal policy decisions. As outlined earlier, nations' methods for delivering high court outcomes differ in interesting ways and even within the U.S. Supreme Court we see the use of both signed and unsigned per curiam legal opinions in cases of policy consequence. Our study focuses on how U.S. Supreme Court case outcomes are conveyed to the public—specifically whether the Court's opinion is attributed to the majority opinion writing justice or to the Court as a whole. We theorize that when the identity of the legal decision is tied to the institution, rather than to an identifiable justice, citizens will be more likely to agree with the Court's decision, all else being equal.

The impact of this institutional cue is likely to vary in interesting ways, depending upon the characteristics of the respondent. More precisely, we posit that the positive relationship between attributing the case outcome to the U.S. Supreme Court (rather than a specific justice) and respondent favorability (i.e., agreement) described above will be conditioned on respondents' more general orientations. First, we expect that this relationship will grow stronger as respondents' ideology becomes more conservative. This possibility is consistent with studies suggesting an alignment between conservative values and generalized reverence toward authority, order, and governing institutions (e.g., Reference Arceneaux and NicholsonArceneaux and Nicholson 2012; Reference Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski and SullowayJost et al. 2003, Reference Jost, Napier, Thorisdottir, Gosling, Palfai and Ostafin2007).Footnote 4 Second, this relationship between U.S. Supreme Court attribution and citizens' agreement with Court outcomes should grow stronger as respondents' general feelings regarding the legitimacy of the Court are more favorable—that is, as feelings of legitimacy toward an institution increase, such positivity is parlayed to appreciation of policy edicts that are attributed to the institution (e.g., Reference Boddery and YatesBoddery and Yates 2014).Footnote 5

Finally, we anticipate that another source cue may affect the relationship between attribution and agreement: the gender of the opinion author. As the number of women on the U.S. Supreme Court has increased, it is important to ascertain whether attributing a majority opinion to a female justice will impact agreement with the Court's decision, relative to other attribution options.

The literature on gender stereotypes and implicit bias suggests that male and female justices may not be evaluated similarly. Due to stereotypes that link the law with masculine-associated traits (Reference PiercePierce 1995), studies find that implicit bias appears to undermine assessments of the competence and qualifications of female judges (Reference Gill, Lazos and WatersGill et al. 2011; Reference SenSen 2014) and female attorneys (Reference BogochBogoch 1997; Reference Brown and CampbellBrown and Campbell 1997). Recent scholarship on the U.S. Supreme Court also shows that female justices tend to be interrupted more often than male justices by lawyers and their male colleagues, reflecting ingrained power dynamics (Reference Jacobi and SchweersJacobi and Schweers 2017; Reference Patton and SmithPatton and Smith 2017). More broadly, women are usually not associated with traits considered to be desirable for political leaders (Reference Huddy and TerkildsenHuddy and Terkildsen 1993), and at least one study has found that written work authored by women is viewed as less credible than work authored by men (Reference Armstrong and McAdamsArmstrong and McAdams 2009).

On the other hand, there is some work that suggests that attributing a decision to a female justice might improve agreement or, at the very least, have a similar effect to a male-attributed opinion. Normative arguments about descriptive representation on the U.S. Supreme Court often promote a link between the identity of justices and enhanced institutional legitimacy (Reference MyersMyers 2009; Reference NeffNeff 1981), and there is some evidence supporting the general argument with respect to minority representation (Reference Scherer and CurryScherer and Curry 2010).Footnote 6 That is, apart from the substance of the decision, an opinion attributed to a female justice could send the message that the U.S. Supreme Court reflects the diversity of American society, at least in terms of gender, and is thus more “fair” (National Women's Law Center 2016).

Alternatively, the public may be indifferent to the gender of the authoring justice when evaluating whether they agree with a decision. Reference Schneider and BosSchneider and Bos (2014) speculate that because the public has so little exposure to women in office, they cannot form clear coherent judgments about their stereotypical qualities. If this is the case, it is not unreasonable to assume that the same dynamic could apply to assessments of female judges, given their underrepresentation in more prestigious and high-profile courts such as the U.S. Supreme Court.Footnote 7 Taken as a whole, then, the literature does not provide us with clear expectations for how the public will respond to female-attributed opinions relative to male-attributed or Court-attributed opinions.

However, a number of studies on gender and political culture (Reference HillHill 1981; Reference Norrander and WilcoxNorrander and Wilcox 1998; Reference WindettWindett 2011) suggest that the effect of gender attribution cues may vary in systematic ways, reflecting the prevailing cultural environments in which respondents live. Exposure theories argue that individuals' exposure to experiences and socialization can lead to either more egalitarian views or more traditional views about gender, depending on the content of the experiences and the environment (Reference Davis and GreensteinDavis and Greenstein 2009). These attitudes then translate into assessments of political elites. For instance, the traditionalistic political culture of the South (Reference ElazarElazar 1974) has been linked with more traditional attitudes about gender roles (Reference Powers, Suitor, Guerra, Shackelford, Mecom and GusmanPowers et al. 2003) and poorer electoral prospects for women gubernatorial and legislative candidates (Reference Norrander and WilcoxNorrander and Wilcox 1998; Reference WindettWindett 2011). Beyond region, Reference Moore and VannemanMoore and Vanneman (2003) find that living in a state with a higher proportion of religious fundamentalists is associated with holding more traditionalist attitudes toward gender. Recently, a study using Reference WindettWindett's (2011) measure of female sociopolitical culture (FSC) found that states with higher FSC scores are significantly more likely than low FSC states to elect female attorneys general and to have female candidates for this position in the party primary (Reference GordonGordon 2016).

Drawing from this work, we expect that respondents living in states with unfavorable political environments for women (Reference WindettWindett 2011) should be more apt to find the U.S. Supreme Court's decision unfavorable if it is attributed to a female justice, and that the reverse should be true for respondents from states with more favorable political environments for women.

Experimental Design

To gauge whether opinion attribution affects the level of agreement a U.S. Supreme Court case disposition yields, we constructed a survey experiment in which we randomly populated three groups and presented respondents with one of three possible treatment vignettes that summarized the holding of an actual Court case. (See Supporting Information S1 for a randomization check and Supporting Information S3 for the vignettes' language.) Prior to the vignettes, respondents were given a pretest that asked basic demographic questions, as well as questions to assess their knowledge of and general feelings toward the U.S. Supreme Court. Following the treatment vignettes, a posttest asked respondents to rate their level of agreement with the Court's decision on a 6-point scale, ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree.Footnote 8

Each vignette summarized the U.S. Supreme Court's holding in Hiibel v. Sixth Judicial District of Nevada (2004). Hiibel scored a two out of eight on Reference Collins and CooperCollins and Cooper's (2012) expanded salience index because the case received news coverage in both the New York Times and Washington Post the day after the Court handed down the decision (see Reference GreenhouseGreenhouse 2004; Reference LaneLane 2004). Hiibel is a criminal procedure case; this issue area is particularly useful given our research question because it maps consistently onto a traditional left–right spectrum. Within this issue area, conservative case dispositions rule in favor of the government, supporting law and order concerns, and liberal case outcomes favor individual liberties. Arguments advanced in this issue area by the government often advocate its right to perform some kind of police or surveillance tactic, whereas individuals seek to prevent the government from using those procedures typically invoking the protections of the Bill of Rights.

Hiibel involved Nevada's “stop and identify” law. This statute requires individuals to identify themselves to a police officer if asked. Larry Hiibel was convicted under the Nevada statute for failing to identify himself to a police officer upon request and appealed to the U.S. Supreme Court claiming, among other things, that the law violated his Fourth Amendment right against unreasonable searches. Justice Kennedy's majority opinion held that Nevada's governmental interest in ensuring that its police force is capable of identifying, and potentially clearing, individuals of suspicion was a minor intrusion and was not barred by Hiibel's protection against unreasonable searches. Because this case ruled in favor of the government's advocated position, it is regarded as a conservative decision. The justices voting in Hiibel split along traditional ideological lines.Footnote 9 Chief Justice Rehnquist and Justices O'Connor, Scalia, and Thomas joined Justice Kennedy's majority opinion. Justices Souter and Ginsburg joined a dissent penned by Justice Breyer, and Justice Stevens wrote his own dissenting opinion.

The vignette read by Group 1 credited the case disposition simply to “the Supreme Court” and was accompanied by a photograph of the U.S. Supreme Court building's exterior; such per curiam attribution should trigger the mechanisms posited by positivity theory—namely that the Court is uniquely suited to invoke deferential reactions because of its perception as a learned and hallowed institution (Reference GibsonGibson 2007). In the two other treatment groups, respondents were informed that either a male or female justice authored the Court's decision and were prompted with a corresponding pictureFootnote 10 of the authoring justice as well as appropriate pronoun attribution. Specifically, respondents in Group 2 read a vignette that identified Justice Anthony Kennedy as the author of the majority's opinion, whereas Group 3's vignette identified Justice Sandra Day O'Connor as the majority opinion author.Footnote 11 The substantive content of the Court's holding was held constant among the treatment groups. Thus, the vignettes differed only with respect to the identity of the majority opinion author and the corresponding pronouns and photographs.

Our study entails 1287 respondents obtained via Amazon's Mechanical Turk (AMT) platform. AMT has proven to be a dependable data source for isolating treatment effects in experimental decision-making settings and has been used in a wide variety of political science and policy contexts (e.g., Reference Bishin, Hayes, Incantalupo and SmithBishin et al. 2015; Reference Clifford, Jewell and WaggonerClifford et al. 2015; Reference Grimmer, Messing and WestwoodGrimmer et al. 2012; Reference RyanRyan 2012). We recognize that this platform is not without its limitations, however. AMT offers a nonprobability sample of opt-in respondents who tend to be younger, more liberal, and more educated than the general American public (Reference Berinsky, Huber and LenzBerinsky et al. 2012; Reference Huber, Hill and LenzHuber et al. 2012). But compared to student convenience samples, AMT's respondent pool is more representative (Reference Paolacci, Chandler and IpeirotisPaolacci et al. 2010), and AMT respondents are also more engaged with survey-taking tasks compared to various other respondent pools (Reference Weinberg, Freese and McElhattanWeinberg et al. 2014). In a comparison of AMT with American-based population samples like the American National Election Survey (ANES), Reference Levay, Freese and DruckmanLevay et al. (2016) found that while differences between AMT and ANES respondents did exist, these differences did not stem from immeasurable considerations. They concluded that differences between AMT respondents and population-based respondents could be identified and their effects largely ameliorated by accounting for a number of political and demographic controls. Importantly, a recent exhaustive study performed 20 survey experiments using AMT respondents while simultaneously performing the identical 20 experiments using a nationally representative sample, finding virtually indistinguishable results (Reference Mullinix, Leeper, Druckman and FreeseMullinix et al. 2015: 122).

Still, Reference Mullinix, Leeper, Druckman and FreeseMullinix et al. (2015) and others caution against considering convenience samples such as AMT as unqualified substitutes for population samples (e.g., Reference Huff and TingleyHuff and Tingley 2015; Reference Krupnikov and LevineKrupnikov and Levine 2014). In Supporting Information S1, we address these concerns by providing distribution information for our AMT sample on an array of relevant demographic and political variables. We find that our sample's distribution largely mirrors the patterns in AMT samples identified by Reference Levay, Freese and DruckmanLevay et al. (2016) and others (e.g., Reference Berinsky, Huber and LenzBerensky et al. 2012). Similar to these studies, our AMT sample respondents are generally younger, lower earning, more liberal, and more likely to be white and male than those respondents typically found in population based samples.Footnote 12 Accordingly, we follow the lead of Reference Levay, Freese and DruckmanLevay et al. (2016) and include control variables to help address these differences in our regression models. We hasten to add that in our descriptive analyses (Figures 1 and 2), these control variables are not employed, meaning that agreement levels displayed do not necessarily approximate population levels. Rather, the focus for those analyses is on whether there are significant differences across treatment groups.

Figure 1. Agreement with Court's Decision, by Attribution. Notes: Entries in the top graph (mean agreement with decision) are mean agreement scores based on a 1–6 scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = slightly disagree, 4 = slightly agree, 5 = agree, and 6 = strongly agree). Analysis of variance produced a statistically significant effect (F = 16.84, p < .01) with one-tailed tests. Entries in the bottom graph (proportion agree with decision) are proportions based on a collapsed measure of the agreement scores (0 = disagree, 1 = agree). Difference of proportion analysis produced a statistically significant effect (Z = 3.24, p < .01) with one-tailed tests. For both analyses, N = 1287. The dashed line allows for easier comparison of the effect size across attribution conditions.

Figure 2. Agreement with Court's Decision, by Attribution and Respondent Ideology. Notes: Entries in the top graph (mean agreement with decision) are mean agreement scores based on a 1–6 scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = slightly disagree, 4 = slightly agree, 5 = agree, and 6 = strongly agree). Analysis of variance produced a statistically significant effect for conservative respondents (F = 18.56, p < .01) and for liberal respondents (F = 5.23, p < .01) with one-tailed tests. Entries in the bottom graph (proportion agree with decision) are proportions based on a collapsed measure of the agreement scores (0 = disagree, 1 = agree). Difference of proportion analysis produced a statistically significant effect for conservative respondents (Z = 3.58, p < .01) and for liberal respondents (Z = 1.70, p < .05) with one-tailed tests. For all analyses, N = 1287. The dashed line allows for easier comparison of the effect size across attribution conditions.

Results

Our survey respondents agreed with the case outcome in Hiibel more often than they disagreed—with 62.2 percent of our sample favoring the case outcome and 37.8 percent not agreeing with it. We also find interesting variation among subcategories of agreement–disagreement. Of those who agreed with the decision, 11.5 percent of respondents strongly agreed, 28.3 percent agreed, and 22.5 percent slightly agreed. Of those who disagreed with the decision, 7.9 percent strongly disagreed, 12.4 percent disagreed, and 17.6 percent slightly disagreed. As depicted in Figure 1, we find support for our primary thesis. Case outcomes attributed to the U.S. Supreme Court rather than individual justices are more likely favored, and this holds for both difference of proportion (collapsed agree/disagree) and difference of means (degree of agreement/disagreement) analyses.

Given that the Hiibel case's outcome is generally regarded as conservative, we are interested in how this consideration factors into our sample respondents' propensity to agree with the decision. We find that 66 percent of self-identified conservatives agree with the holding, whereas just 61 percent of self-identified liberals agree with it. While this gap is not extremely large, we do find that there is a statistically significant difference between liberals and conservatives in both difference of proportions and difference of means tests.Footnote 13 This prompts us to question whether ideological considerations may cast an influence upon our primary proposition regarding attribution's effect on how legal outcomes are viewed. Figure 2 provides preliminary evidence that while a relationship exists between case outcome attribution (to the Court) and respondent agreement—both for self-identified liberals and conservatives—the relationship is stronger for conservatives.

We report ordinary least squares (OLS) regression results for our analyses in Table 1. The second column of the table (Model 1) displays the results of a simple bivariate analysis of our primary thesis, which is supported. When the decision is attributed to justices rather than the U.S. Supreme Court—the excluded reference category—respondents are less likely to agree with the outcome. The results in the third column (Model 2) demonstrate that the relationship is robust to the introduction of relevant controls, including the respondent's race, gender, ideology, knowledge of the Court, feelings regarding the Court's overall legitimacy, age, income, and level of education.Footnote 14 (See Supporting Information S2 for variable descriptions.)

Table 1. OLS Estimates for Degree of Agreement/Disagreement with Supreme Court Decision

Note: Standard errors in parentheses.

** p < .01.

* p < .05 (two-tailed tests).

In the multivariate model, we see that when the decision was attributed to a justice, this yielded a 0.348 point decrease in citizen agreement (again, on a 1–6 scale from “disagree strongly” to “agree strongly”) relative to the situation in which the decision was attributed to the Court as a whole. We find that respondents' race, education, income level, and knowledge of the U.S. Supreme Court have no statistically significant effect on agreement outcomes. On the other hand, women appear to find the decision in Hiibel moderately more palatable than men (a 0.195 point difference), and the results show that conservatism and positive views on the U.S. Supreme Court's legitimacy are also positively associated with agreement with the decision. In contrast, relative youth (i.e., under 35 years of age) was negatively associated with decision agreement. We also assessed the relationship using a logit model with a collapsed dependent variable in which ranges of disagreement are recoded as 0 and the levels of agreement as 1 (see Table A1). Our findings using this approach largely mirror the results we obtained in our OLS estimation model and support our primary hypothesized relationship.Footnote 15

We next revisit the interactive relationship regarding the potential conditioning effect of ideology on attribution of the decision to a justice versus the Court. The interaction results provided in our OLS analysis in Table 1 (Model 3) are depicted graphically in Figure 3 which displays the average marginal effects (Reference WilliamsWilliams 2012) of our primary relationship between decision attribution and respondents' degree of agreement with the case outcome—as conditioned on the respondent's ideology (from extremely liberal [1] to extremely conservative [6]). The negative relationship between justice attribution and agreement attains statistical significance with liberal respondents and grows stronger with increasing levels of self-identified conservatism. In supplemental models using logistic regression, our results are comparable (see Appendix Table A1 and Figure A1). Footnote 16

Figure 3. The Average Marginal Effects of an Opinion Attributed to a Justice Rather than the Court on Case-Specific Agreement as Political Ideology Ranges from Liberal to Conservative. Note: This figure demonstrates an interactive relationship in the linear regression model by plotting the average marginal effect of a justice attributed opinion on the dependent variable, case-level agreement (Hiibel), as political ideology ranges from liberal to conservative.

These results confirm our expectation that respondents with conservative ideological leanings may be more sensitive to the heuristic cue of the nation's highest legal authority, though with some caveats that we will discuss in our concluding remarks. Finally, our statistically significant findings regarding respondents' feelings on the overall legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court prompt us to consider the possibility that such views may also act as a conditioning variable for the influence of case attribution. However, we find that the coefficients for the proposed interactive relationship do not reach conventional levels of statistical significance in either the OLS (Table 1, Model 4) or the logit models (Table A1, Model 4).

Source Cues and Opinion Author Gender

Having found evidence that attribution of the majority opinion to a justice reduces agreement compared to Court-attributed opinions we next turn to the question of whether agreement is affected when the gender of the opinion author can be explicitly identified. As the U.S. Supreme Court has grown from an all-male institution to one with multiple women on the bench, we know very little about what it means for an opinion to be attributed to a female justice versus a male justice. To the best of our knowledge, no systematic study to date has evaluated how opinion author gender affects agreement with decisions in issue areas that lack a salient gender dimension.Footnote 17 As discussed earlier, criminal procedure cases provide a useful lens for assessing agreement with U.S. Supreme Court decisions because they are both common to the Court's docket and salient to the general public. As the majority of U.S. Supreme Court decisions concern issue areas without an explicit gender dimension, the results of this analysis should allow us to identify any general advantage or disadvantage for case agreement that varies by the gender of the opinion author.Footnote 18 Moreover, the four women who have served as justices on the U.S. Supreme Court have been active in writing decisions about a wide range of issues. From 1981 to 2015, female justices penned a total of 549 majority opinions, with nearly one-third of those dealing with criminal procedure.Footnote 19 In this assessment we split our prior “Justice attributed” variable into female justice (denoting O'Connor) and male justice (denoting Kennedy) attributed variables, and once again, the reference is the Court-attributed treatment.

We also wish to assess whether sociopolitical context affects agreement with female-attributed opinions. Accordingly, we utilize Reference WindettWindett's (2011) dynamic measure of FSC, updated through 2012, which captures aspects of gendered political culture, general culture, and gendered social culture.Footnote 20 In our study, high FSC states include Washington, Vermont, and Colorado while low FSC states include Oklahoma, South Carolina, and Louisiana. The former group scores slightly higher in education, but both groups are similar with respect to age. Where respondents live in environments where there are many well-educated, employed women, and women officeholders, this should condition them to hold more egalitarian attitudes about gender roles and thus assess female-attributed opinions more favorably than respondents in low FSC states. We first add the FSC score variable to our existing set of relevant controls (to ascertain its additive effect). We then include it in a set of interactive model specifications.

Another relevant consideration for our investigation is whether male and female respondents will differ systematically in their assessment of female-attributed opinions, compared to male-attributed ones. No studies of which we are aware have directly examined this question, but related research on legitimacy and fairness has produced mixed results (Reference Bartels and JohnstonBartels and Johnston 2013; Reference Cann and YatesCann and Yates 2008; Reference Gibson and NelsonGibson and Nelson 2015). To unpack this in the context of our research question, we include a set of interactions by respondent gender in which we separately assay our tests regarding how female justices are perceived as opinion writers.

Differences in respondent agreement with Hiibel by gender are only slight. When the decision is attributed to a male justice (Kennedy), 58.16 percent of respondents agreed, compared to 60.1 percent agreement when the female justice (O'Connor) was depicted as the opinion author. Moving beyond these descriptive differences, in Table 2, we present the results of the OLS modelsFootnote 21 analyzing the impact of gender attribution on agreement with the Court's decision, employing the set of controls discussed previously, along with FSC, and using Court-attributed opinions as the reference category.Footnote 22

Table 2. OLS Estimates for Agreement with Supreme Court Decision, Including Justice Gender and Interaction with Respondents' Female Sociopolitical Culture (FSC)

Note: Standard errors in parentheses.

** p < .01.

* p < .05 (two-tailed tests).

The results (Model 1) indicate that both male (Justice Kennedy) and female (Justice O'Connor) attributed decisions fare worse than the Court attributed decision—as we might reasonably expect from our prior results. While the coefficient for Male justice is somewhat more negative than Female justice, relative to the Court attribution, the difference between the two was found to not be statistically significant in auxiliary analyses.Footnote 23 Control variables largely mirror our prior findings; however, we do find that there is a negative and statistically significant association between FSC and respondent agreement.

As discussed above, we have reason to believe that gender attribution dynamics could differ for respondents in states with lower levels of FSC, who should be less likely to agree with a female-attributed opinion. The third column of Table 2 displays the interactive results regarding opinion attribution (female and male, relative to Court), conditioned upon FSC. In Figure 4, we depict the average marginal effects of the conditional relationship between Female justice and FSC on agreement. The interactive relationship indicates a positive relationship between female justice attribution and agreement as FSC increases to nearly one s.d. above the mean. In sum, attributing a decision to a female justice in a higher FSC environment may augment public agreement with the decision relative to Court and male justice attribution—under limited conditions.

Figure 4. The Effect of a Female Opinion Author on Case-Specific Agreement as Female Sociopolitical Culture Varies. Note: This figure demonstrates an interactive relationship by plotting the average marginal effects of a female opinion author on the dependent variable, case-level agreement (Hiibel), as female sociopolitical culture varies.

We further investigate the effects of gender with regard to respondents in Model 3. Our interactive term (Female justice × Female respondent) indicates no statistically significant relationship. However, the component terms of our interaction are also informative. When female justice is equal to zero, the statistical significance for Female respondent indicates that women respondents favor attributions to Male justice and Court attribution over Female justice attribution. The statistically significant component term Female justice indicates the effect of this variable (Female justice) when the respondent is male (i.e., Female respondent = 0). Here, we see that the coefficient for this component is negative and significant, suggesting that male respondents are less favorable to U.S. High Court decisions that are attributed to female justices.

Finally, we consider the respondent gender dynamic in the context of different sociopolitical environments by employing a triple interaction term: FSC × Female justice × Female respondent. Model 4 (Table 2) displays the results for this analysis, and the average marginal effects of the interactive relationship are depicted in more detail in Figure 5. As both indicate, for female respondents, there is no statistically significant interactive relationship between Female justice attribution and FSC. For male respondents, we see that while they are generally negatively inclined toward the female-authored decision, agreement levels become more positive as FSC rises, although this effect disappears at the highest levels of FSC.

Figure 5. The Effect of a Female Opinion Author on Case-Specific Agreement as Female Sociopolitical Culture Varies among Male and Female Respondents. Note: This figure demonstrates a triple interactive relationship by plotting the average marginal effects of a female opinion author the dependent variable, case-level agreement, as female sociopolitical culture varies among male and female respondents.

Conclusion

In this paper, we have shown that the manner in which a political institution conveys a policy decision to the public has an important influence on how citizens appraise the policy. More specifically, when a U.S. Supreme Court outcome is ascribed to the institution as a whole, rather than to a particular justice, people are more apt to agree with the policy decision. As a number of prior studies have demonstrated, the agreement dynamic that we uncover has important implications for public legitimacy—both for individual policy outcomes and for the institution more generally (e.g., Reference GibsonGibson 2012, Reference Gibson, Bornstein and Tomkins2015).

Our analysis also sheds light on debates over the relative suitability of the U.S. High Court's (and lower courts') present use of a “hybrid” approach for communicating policy decisions (i.e., generally attributing Court opinions of the majority coalition to a specific justice, while allowing other justices to write dissenting or concurring opinions at their discretion). Our findings suggest that this method of policy conveyance may work to undermine potential agreement with legal edicts, notwithstanding legal scholars' arguments to the contrary (e.g., Reference RobbinsRobbins 2012). In contrast, the U.S. Supreme Court's use of unsigned per curiam opinions may actually serve to help maintain the public's “reservoir of good will” for both specific decisions and the Court more generally. This finding lends credence to calls by some legal academics to do away with individually signed opinions—which they believe lead to unhealthy “judicial individualism” and may ultimately be damaging to the U.S. Supreme Court's standing (Reference Lerner and LundLerner and Lund 2010). In light of trends related to mass polarization and declining confidence in the U.S. Supreme Court, it could be advantageous for the Court's legitimacy to utilize more per curiam opinions in order to dampen ideological cues associated with particular justices. It is conceivable that there may be an upper limit to the use of per curiam opinions—at least in relation to the degree to which they can be used to encourage public agreement. If so, the Court might strategically employ per curiam opinions only for its most salient and potentially politically volatile decisions. Of course, we recognize the importance of replicating our findings in other issue domains to understand more fully the impact on legitimacy.

Furthermore, our findings run counter to prior studies showing that attribution of legal policy decisions to the U.S. Supreme Court has little influence on how the public assesses them (Reference Nicholson and HansfordNicholson and Hansford 2014). It remains to be seen whether our findings will apply in similar fashion to issue areas other than criminal procedure. However, given that this issue area occupies a significant portion of the Court's docket (relative to any other single issue area), the phenomenon we have revealed involves a sizeable share of the U.S. Supreme Court's policy output. It is also possible that such institutional cue-taking dynamics might not be applicable to lower federal or state courts in which the personalities and ideological proclivities of the judicial actors may not be as well-known as those of the justices of the U.S. High Court. Future research might explore this intriguing question as well as the possibility that similar institution-oriented heuristics might affect public views on the policy edicts of other branches of government.

Our paper also breaks new ground in identifying the role that gender plays in how the U.S. Supreme Court's policy decisions are interpreted by the public. In the overall analysis, we find that attributing a decision to the institution is associated with more agreement than attributing it to either a male or a female justice. However, the social, political, and economic environment for women in a state also affects how citizens assess gender cues in the attribution of a decision. In states where female candidates face better odds in winning elections, respondents afford more deference to female-attributed opinions relative to Court-attributed decisions, preferring both over those attributed to a male author. But in states with low levels of FSC, female-attributed opinions are viewed less favorably relative to those attributed to the U.S. Supreme Court. Our findings regarding female sociopolitical context appear to be driven primarily by male respondents; they are most affected by their environment, at least in this regard.

To the best of our knowledge, no existing study has uncovered a gender effect for authorship of U.S. Supreme Court decisions before, and it has important implications for the efficacy of the Court in garnering agreement with its policy pronouncements. While this provides some support for the perspective that having a U.S. Supreme Court that “looks like America” may improve public perceptions of the Court's decisions, it is clear that this legitimizing effect is not uniform. From the Court's perspective, our findings suggest that it may be advantageous to utilize per curiam decisions more often, so as to mitigate differences in public reactions to the gender of the opinion author.

The effects isolated in this study are noteworthy but, like all experimental effects, call for replication. Future research should explore whether opinion attribution effects also appear in more gender-salient issue areas, such as reproductive rights and employment discrimination. Furthermore, it may be advantageous to see if our findings persist for U.S. Supreme Court policy outcomes that are traditionally thought of as liberal in orientation. In the context of our conservative policy vignette, we find that that the Court attribution effect is stronger as respondent ideology grows more conservative. It remains to be seen whether this same conditioning effect (presumably based on conservative deference to authoritarian institutions) holds true for conservatives' favorability toward liberal U.S. Supreme Court outcomes or, conversely, if there is a mirrored effect for more liberal respondents (suggesting an alternative path of such conditioned influence).Footnote 24 Finally, our primary finding regarding gender and political environment is driven to some degree by male respondents. This finding may be a product of the issue area (criminal procedure) that we utilize or may be a more general phenomenon. Either possibility is intriguing and prompts us to consider the contextual nature of citizen contemplation of U.S. Supreme Court verdicts. Along these lines, additional research should explore the negative relationship between FSC and agreement, even after controlling for ideology. This too could be a function of the criminal context of our experiment, although we cannot say for certain.

Although AMT samples have consistently been shown to mirror results from studies based on probability samples (Reference Mullinix, Leeper, Druckman and FreeseMullinix et al. 2015), future studies should also survey a representative sample of individuals in real time, as the U.S. Supreme Court releases its written opinions. Lastly, a natural extension of this study could employ content analysis to examine how media reports treat per curiam opinions compared to attributed opinions handed down by the Court. Given the recent spotlight political actors displeased with certain legal holdings have placed on individual U.S. federal judges (Reference LiptakLiptak 2016; Reference WangWang 2017), employing per curiam opinions to a greater extent is a reasonable and straightforward way judges may avoid individualized scrutiny while simultaneously protecting the public support that is so crucial to a judiciary's longevity.

APPENDIX

Table A1 Logit Estimates for Agreement with Supreme Court Decision

Note: Standard errors in parentheses.

** p < .01.

* p < .05 (two-tailed tests).

Figure A1 The Average Marginal Effects of an Opinion Attributed to a Justice Rather than the Court on Case-Specific Agreement as Political Ideology Ranges from Liberal to Conservative. Note: This figure demonstrates an interactive relationship in the logistic regression model by plotting the average marginal effect of a justice attributed opinion on the dependent variable, case-level agreement (Hiibel), as political ideology ranges from liberal to conservative.

Table A2 OLS Estimates for Agreement with Supreme Court Decision. Interaction Between Justice Attributed Opinion and Respondent Gender

Note: Standard errors in parentheses.

** p < .01.

* p < .05 (two-tailed tests).

Table A3 OLS Estimates for Agreement with Supreme Court Decision. Head-to-Head Comparison of Female and Male Attributed Opinions

Note: Standard errors in parentheses.

** p < .01.

* p < .05 (two-tailed tests). Observations for court attributed opinions are not included in this model, so the reference category is male attributed opinions.

Footnotes

We are grateful to Jamie Druckman, Susan Haire, Andrew O'Geen, Steven Brooke, David Buckley, Jamie Carson, Jim Gibson, and the reviewers and editors of Law & Society Review for their helpful critiques on earlier drafts of this article.

Note: Standard errors in parentheses.

** p < .01.

* p < .05 (two-tailed tests).

Note: Standard errors in parentheses.

** p < .01.

* p < .05 (two-tailed tests).

Note: Standard errors in parentheses.

** p < .01.

* p < .05 (two-tailed tests). Observations for court attributed opinions are not included in this model, so the reference category is male attributed opinions.

1 Assignment decisions may also reflect a desire for particular expertise (e.g., Reference NashNash 2015), in addition to or instead of a concern for agreement. We thank an anonymous reviewer for raising this point.

2 A related body of literature examines the quality, tone, and substance of media coverage of the Supreme Court and the American judicial system (e.g., Reference Slotnick and SegalSlotnick and Segal 1998; Reference Spill and OxleySpill and Oxley 2003).

3 As we noted previously, the U.S. Supreme Court can choose to present opinions as a “united front” through the use of unsigned per curiam opinions. Indeed, Reference Epstein, Segal and SpaethEpstein et al. (2001) demonstrated that there have been important changes over time in how the Court chooses to convey its opinions to the public. They employ the papers of Chief Justice Waite (Reference Epstein, Segal and SpaethEpstein et al. 2001: 1874–88) to show that strong consensus on the Court during this period was not due to “easy cases”—the conference vote records demonstrate significant preliminary dissensus among the justices. During this period and well into the twentieth century, the Court chose to mask these differences from the public in its conveyance of the formal opinion, ostensibly for institution enhancing reasons by delivering primarily unanimous (although signed) opinions (2001: 364–65). The Court can and does utilize a similar tactic through the use of per curiam opinions today—it need only expand its existing practice to exert more control on how its decisions are portrayed by the media in the public realm.

4 This relationship could emanate from more than one pathway for respondents. As noted, it could be steeped in conservative respondents' orientation toward formal authority and authoritative institutions such as the U.S. Supreme Court. Alternatively, it could be the situation that attribution of a case outcome that most would consider conservative (see discussion of vignette and experimental treatment in Supporting Information S2 and S3) to the Court generally (as opposed to a moderate Republican appointed justice) allows a conservative respondent to imagine that the “real” opinion author (or winning coalition) could be from one of the most conservative members of the Court.

5 As noted above, our questionnaire poses a number of questions regarding respondents' feelings regarding the legitimacy of the U.S. High Court. These questions are used to create an index of a respondent's overall positive feelings regarding Court legitimacy. We acknowledge that the relationship dynamics between U.S. Supreme Court decisions and perceptions of Court legitimacy are complex. While we, along with a robust literature, are inclined to think that citizens' perceptions of Court legitimacy are augmented (in relative terms) when they agree with a legal outcome from the institution (e.g., Reference Gibson, Bornstein and TomkinsGibson 2015), we also posit that a citizen holding strong generalized feelings that the Court is legitimate is more apt to agree with one of its legal decisions, all else being equal. Thus, there is the possibility of a circular relationship whereby citizens are more predisposed to agree with a Court they believe to be legitimate and, in turn, are also more inclined to have positive feelings regarding the legitimacy of the legal institution when they agree with its policy outcomes.

6 Indeed, President Obama took this view and stated his support for appointing a judiciary that “looks like America” (Reference Goldman, Slotnick and SchiavoniGoldman et al. 2013).

7 While the numbers of women serving as judges in the United States has steadily increased since the 1970s, the occupation of judging is still heavily male-dominated as a whole and exhibits a strong positive relationship with court prestige (Reference Bratton and SpillBratton and Spill 2002).

8 In our study discussion, we employ certain synonyms to denote agreement, including “favorable” and “palatable” to promote readability and to avoid repetitiveness. To clarify this matter up front, we are always referring to the same 6-point scale of agreement degrees that we outline here.

9 In our vignettes, we chose to present the Court's decision as split rather than unanimous or not mentioning the vote. We acknowledge that the literature on unanimity versus split vote (in relation to public views) is mixed (Reference Gibson, Caldeira and SpenceGibson et al. 2005; Reference Zink, Spriggs and ScottZink et al. 2009), but we wanted to mirror the actual vote in the case (which was split). We also believe that presenting it as a split case gave respondents a more free choice to agree or disagree, since elite legal minds could differ on the outcome. Lastly, we wanted to signal that the case was in no way a pro forma error correction of a lower court—rather, it was a contentious Court decision.

10 We utilized the justices' official color portraits from the U.S. Supreme Court's web site—originally accessed in June 2015. They are shown here in black and white.

11 We selected Justices Kennedy and O'Connor for several reasons. First, Justice Kennedy did, in fact, author the majority opinion, and news agencies attributed the majority's holding to him (Reference GreenhouseGreenhouse 2004; Reference LaneLane 2004). Second, Justice O'Connor was the only female member of Hiibel's majority bloc. Third, and most importantly, widely used measures of justice ideology (e.g., Reference BaileyBailey 2007; Reference Martin and QuinnMartin and Quinn 2002) place Justice O'Connor as ideologically proximate to Justice Kennedy. Justice O'Connor is ideologically the closest member on the Court to Kennedy (Reference Martin and QuinnMartin and Quinn 2002). This relationship allows us to make Groups 2 and 3 as similar as possible in every aspect while varying only the photographs, justice names, and pronouns used.

12 Just over 6 percent of our respondents identified themselves as black or mixed race (where black was one of the races chosen). In the U.S. population, the Census Bureau estimates that about 12 percent of the population is African American.

13 The gap between self-identified liberal and conservative (collapsed measure) respondents in favoring the Hiibel decision is smaller than we anticipated. We believe that this may be due, in part, to the inherent bluntness of a one-dimensional (liberal-conservative) measurement for ideology. On the other hand, when we compare views of the law-and-order-oriented Hiibel outcome at the far ends of the self-identified spectrum (i.e., “extremely liberal” and “extremely conservative”), we see a more substantial gap. We find that only 48 percent of self-identified “extremely liberal” respondents agree with the decision, whereas 69 percent of those identifying as “extremely conservative” agree with it. In our sample, a total of 158 respondents identified as “extremely liberal” and 32 identified as “extremely conservative.”

14 Because the use of controls in experimental data is open to some debate (e.g., Reference MutzMutz 2011), we include results for models both with and without control variables. Our control variables inevitably tap into concerns that potentially hinge on both generalized feelings toward the Court as well as feelings toward a decision that favors law enforcement over civil liberties. Our ideology variable touches on both rationales: conservatives generally favor law and order and at the same time are more deferential toward formal authoritarian institutions, whereas liberals favor civil liberties and question institutional authority (e.g., Reference Jost, Napier, Thorisdottir, Gosling, Palfai and OstafinJost et al. 2007). With respect to the demographic variables of race and sex, agreement with the Hiibel decision may turn on a combination of attitudes toward legal institutions (Reference Gibson, Bornstein and TomkinsGibson 2015), feelings on the importance of personal legal compliance and the rule of law (Reference Cann and YatesCann and Yates 2016), as well as trust in enforcement entities and relative concern over the problem of crime and personal safety (University of Albany 2016). Our other control variables touch more on general feelings toward the U.S. Supreme Court as an institution. General feelings regarding U.S. Supreme Court legitimacy should make one more amenable to the Court's verdicts; high knowledge of the U.S. Supreme Court is likely to also be associated with higher regard for the Court and its actions (Reference Gibson, Bornstein and TomkinsGibson 2015).

15 Our primary findings are also confirmed by an ordered logit analysis (not shown) on respondents' degree of agreement.

16 In constructing, interpreting, and graphically presenting our interactive terms, we follow Ai and Reference Ai and NortonNorton (2003), Reference Brambor, Clark and GolderBrambor et al. (2006), and Reference WilliamsWilliams (2012). The interaction results are confirmed by auxiliary logit and ordered logit analyses in which our control variables (other than ideology) are not included.

17 Reference NelsonNelson (2015) examines how gender stereotypes affect assessments of U.S. Courts of Appeals judges in a gender salient issue area (sex discrimination cases).

18 See Reference Childs and KrookChilds and Krook (2006) for a discussion of the debate on how to conceptualize whether an issue is gender salient or not.

19 These figures were drawn from the Spaeth Supreme Court Database and include only orally argued cases with a majority opinion. Of these, O'Connor wrote 289, Ginsburg 180, Sotomayor 45, and Kagan 35. The justices penned between 25 and 30 percent of their opinions in criminal procedure cases.

20 Specifically, the Windett measure is a composite of the following: the percentage of female elected officials (statewide, state senate, state house, and Congress), Elazar's political culture measures, women's presence in the workforce, female college graduates, and ERA ratification. We also estimated our models using a measure of the “Best and Worst States for Women,” and the results were substantively identical (data and explanation of the alternative measure can be found here: https://wallethub.com/edu/best-and-worst-states-for-women/10728/).

21 Note that in the tables, the models employing the FSC measure in interactions have three fewer observations than all other models. This is due to the fact that our survey incorporated residents of Washington, D.C., whereas Washington, D.C. was not a part of Windett's measure.

22 Auxiliary analyses of models with no control variables confirm the findings in Table A2.

23 In Table A3, we provide a head-to-head analysis of the female versus male justice attribution in which the observations for the Court attribution are dropped from the analysis and the female justice attribution is directly compared against the missing reference category of male justice attribution. We also include a similarly constructed model that addresses this matter in the context of the FSC interaction.

24 As noted previously, attribution of a conservative case outcome to the entire Court (as opposed to an ideological swing justice such as Kennedy or O'Connor) may allow a more conservative respondent to conceive that the actual opinion author (or, alternatively, the winning coalition) is one of the Court's most conservative justices (e.g., Scalia or Thomas).

References

References

Ai, Chunrong & Norton, Edward C. (2003) “Interaction Terms in Logit and Probit Models,” 80 Economics Letters 123–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arceneaux, Kevin & Nicholson, Stephen P. (2012) “Who Wants to Have a Tea Party? The Who, What, and Why of the Tea Party Movement,” 45 PS: Political Science and Politics 700–10.Google Scholar
Armstrong, Cory L. & McAdams, Melinda J. (2009) “Blogs of Information: How Gender Cues and Individual Motivations Influence Perceptions of Credibility,” 14 J. of Computer-Mediated Communication 435–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baas, Larry R. & Thomas, Dan (1984) “The Supreme Court and Policy Legitimation: Experimental Tests,” 12 American Politics Research 335–60.Google Scholar
Bailey, Michael (2007) “Comparable Preference Estimates across Time and Institutions for the Court, Congress, and Presidency,” 51 American J. of Political Science 433–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baird, Vanessa A. (2001) “Building Institutional Legitimacy: The Role of Procedural Justice,” 54 Political Research Q. 333–54.Google Scholar
Baird, Vanessa A. & Gangl, Amy (2006) “Shattering the Myth of Legality: The Impact of the Media's Framing of Supreme Court Procedures on Perceptions of Fairness,” 27 Political Psychology 597614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartels, Brandon L. & Mutz, Diana C. (2009) “Explaining Processes of Institutional Opinion Leadership,” 71 J. of Politics 249–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartels, Brandon L. & Johnston, Christopher D. (2013) “On the Ideological Foundations of Supreme Court Legitimacy in the American Public,” 57 American J. of Political Science 184–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berinsky, Adam J., Huber, Gregory A., & Lenz, Gabriel S. (2012) “Evaluating Online Labor Markets for Experimental Research: Amazon.com's Mechanical Turk,” 20 Political Analysis 351–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishin, Benjamin G., Hayes, Thomas J., Incantalupo, Matthew B., & Smith, Charles A. (2015) “Opinion Backlash and Public Attitudes: Are Political Advances in Gay Rights Counterproductive?60 American J. of Political Science 625–48.Google Scholar
Boddery, Scott S. & Yates, Jeff (2014) “Do Policy Messengers Matter? Majority Opinion Writers as Policy Cues in Public Agreement with Supreme Court Decisions,” 67 Political Research Q. 851–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogoch, Bryna (1997) “Gendered Lawyering: Difference and Dominance in Lawyer-Client Interaction,” 31 Law and Society Rev. 677712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bozzo, Peter (2015) “The Jurisprudence of ‘As Though’: Democratic Dialogue and the Signed Supreme Court Opinion,” 26 Yale J. of Law & the Humanities 269300.Google Scholar
Brambor, Thomas, Clark, William R., & Golder, Matt (2006) “Understanding Interaction Models: Improving Empirical Analyses,” 14 Political Analysis 6382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bratton, Kathleen A. & Spill, Rorie L. (2002) “Existing Diversity and Judicial Selection: The Role of the Appointment Method in Establishing Gender Diversity in State Supreme Courts,” 83 Social Science Q. 504–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brigham, John (1987) “Right, Rage, and Remedy: Forms of Law in Political Discourse,” in Monkkonen, E., ed., Studies in American Political Development, Vol. 2. New Haven, CT: Yale Univ. Press. 303.Google Scholar
Brown, Robert L. & Campbell, Shelia (1997) “How the Public Views Female and Black Attorneys,” 33 Arkansas Law Rev. 22–8.Google Scholar
Caldeira, Gregory A. (1986) “Neither the Purse Nor the Sword: Dynamics of Public Confidence in the Supreme Court,” 80 American Political Science Rev. 1209–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cann, Damon & Yates, Jeff (2016) These Estimable Courts: Understanding Public Perceptions of State Judicial Institutions and Legal Policy-Making. New York: Oxford Univ. Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cann, Damon & Yates, Jeff (2008) “Homegrown Institutional Legitimacy: Assessing Citizens' Diffuse Support for State Courts,” 36 American Politics Research 297329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canon, Bradley C. & Johnson, Charles A. (1984) Judicial Policies: Implementation and Impact. Washington, D.C.: CQ Press.Google Scholar
Childs, Sarah & Krook, Mona L. (2006) “Should Feminists Give Up on Critical Mass? A Contingent Yes,” 2 Politics & Gender 522–30.Google Scholar
Christenson, Dino P. & Glick, David M. (2015) “Chief Justice Roberts's Health Care Decision Disrobed: The Microfoundations of the Supreme Court's Legitimacy,” 59 American J. of Political Science 403–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Tom D. & Kastellec, Jonathan P. (2015) “Source Cues and Public Support for the Supreme Court,” 43 American Politics Research 504–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clawson, Rosalee A. & Waltenburg, Eric N. (2003) “Support for a Supreme Court Affirmative Action Decision: A Story in Black and White,” 31 American Politics Research 251–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, Scott, Jewell, Ryan M., & Waggoner, Phillip D. (2015) “Are Samples Drawn from Mechanical Turk Valid for Research on Political Ideology?,” 2 Research and Politics 19 (October).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, Todd A. & Cooper, Christopher A. (2012) “Case Salience and Media Coverage of Supreme Court Decisions Toward a New Measure,” 65 Political Research Q. 396407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, Richard (2014) “Political and Media Factors in the Evolution of the Media's Role in US Supreme Court Nominations,” 4 Oñati Socio-Legal Series 652–84.Google Scholar
Davis, Shannon N. & Greenstein, Theodore N. (2009) “Gender Ideology: Components, Predictors, and Consequences,” 35 Annual Rev. of Sociology 87105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James N. (2001) “On the Limits of Framing Effects: Who Can Frame?63 J. of Politics 1041–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easton, David (1965) A Systems Analysis of Political Life. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Elazar, Daniel (1974) American Federalism: A View from the States. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Epstein, Lee, Segal, Jeffrey, & Spaeth, Harold (2001) “The Norm of Consensus on the U.S. Supreme Court,” 45 American J. of Political Science 362–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, Lee & Knight, Jack (1998) The Choices Justices Make. Washington, DC: CQ Press.Google Scholar
Estlund, David M. (2007) Democratic Authority: A Philosophical Framework. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Fiss, Owen M. (1983) “The Bureaucratization of the Judiciary,” 92 The Yale Law J. 1442–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. & Caldeira, Gregory A. (1992) “Blacks and the United States Supreme Court: Models of Diffuse Support,” J. of Politics 1120–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., Caldeira, Gregory A., & Baird, Vanessa A. (1998) “On the Legitimacy of National High Courts,” 92 American Political Science Rev. 343–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., Caldeira, Gregory A., & Spence, Lester K. (2003) “Measuring Attitudes toward the United States Supreme Court,” 47 American J. of Political Science 867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., Caldeira, Gregory A., & Spence, Lester K. (2005) “Why Do People Accept Public Policies They Oppose? Testing Legitimacy Theory with a Survey-Based Experiment,” 58 Political Research Q. 187201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. (2007) “The Legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court in a Polarized Policy,” 4 J. of Empirical Legal Studies 507–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. & Caldeira, Gregory A. (2009) “Confirmation Politics and the Legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court: Institutional Loyalty, Positivity Bias, and the Alito Nomination,” 53 American J. of Political Science 139–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. (2012) Can the Supreme Court Survive a Health-Care Decision? Pacific Standard. Available at: https://psmag.com/can-the-supreme-court-survive-a-health-care-decision-4347fa5fc65f#.agcr8jrwp (accessed 6 October 2018).Google Scholar
Gibson, James L., Lodge, Milton, & Woodson, Benjamin (2014) “Losing, but Accepting: Legitimacy, Positivity Theory, and the Symbols of Judicial Authority,” 48 Law & Society Rev. 837–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. (2015) “Legitimacy Is for Losers: The Interconnections of Institutional Legitimacy, Performance Evaluations, and the Symbols of Judicial Authority,” in Bornstein, B. & Tomkins, A., eds., Motivating Cooperation and Compliance with Authority: The Role of Institutional Trust. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Gibson, James L. & Nelson, Michael J. (2015, 59) “Is the U.S. Supreme Court's Legitimacy Grounded in Performance Satisfaction and Ideology?American J. of Political Science 162–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gill, Rebecca D., Lazos, Sylvia R., & Waters, Mallory M. (2011) “Are Judicial Performance Evaluations Fair to Women and Minorities? A Cautionary Tale from Clark County, Nevada,” 45 Law and Society Rev. 731–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginsburg, Ruth B. (1990) “Remarks on Writing Separately,” 90 Washington Law Rev. 133–50.Google Scholar
Goldman, Sheldon, Slotnick, Elliot, & Schiavoni, Sara (2013) “Obama's First Term Judiciary: Picking Judges in the Minefield of Obstructionism,” 97 Judicature 747.Google Scholar
Gordon, Elizabeth E. (2016) “Female Candidates, Sociopolitical Subculture, and State Attorney General Elections,” 37 Justice System J. 6371 (January).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenhouse, Linda (2004) “Justices Uphold a Nevada Law Requiring Citizens to Identify Themselves to the Police,” New York Times, 22 June 22, sec. A, p. 16.Google Scholar
Grimmer, Justin, Messing, Solomon, & Westwood, Sean J. (2012) “How Words and Money Cultivate a Personal Vote: The Effect of Legislator Credit Claiming on Constituent Credit Allocation,” 106 American Political Science Rev. 706–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, David (1981) “Political Culture and Female Political Representation,” 43 J. of Politics 159–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoekstra, Valerie J. (1995) “The Supreme Court and Opinion Change: An Experimental Study of the Court's Ability to Change Opinion,” 23 American Politics Research 109–29.Google Scholar
Huber, Gregory, Hill, Seth, & Lenz, Gabriel (2012) “Sources of Bias in Retrospective Decision Making: Experimental Evidence on Voters' Limitations in Controlling Incumbents,” 106 American Political Science Rev. 720–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddy, Leonie & Terkildsen, Nayda (1993) “The Consequences of Gender Stereotypes for Women Candidates at Different Levels and Types of Offices,” 46 Political Research Q. 503–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huff, Connor, & Tingley, Dustin (2015) “‘Who Are These People?’ Evaluating the Demographic Characteristics and Political Preferences of MTurk Survey Respondents,” 2 Research and Politics 112 (July).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobi, Tonja & Schweers, Dylan (2017) “Justice, Interrupted: The Effect of Gender, Ideology, and Seniority at Supreme Court Oral Arguments,” 103 Virginia Law Rev. 1379–496.Google Scholar
Johnston, Christopher D. & Bartels, Brandon L. (2010) “Sensationalism and Sobriety: Differential Media Exposure and Attitudes toward American Courts,” 74 Public Opinion Q. 260–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, Christopher D., Sunshine Hillygus, D., & Bartels, Brandon L. (2014) “Ideology, the Affordable Care Act Ruling, and Supreme Court Legitimacy,” 78 Public Opinion Q. 963–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, John T., Napier, Jaime L., Thorisdottir, Hulda, Gosling, Samuel D., Palfai, Tibor P., & Ostafin, Brian (2007) “Are Needs to Manage Uncertainty and Threat Associated with Political Conservatism or Ideological Extremity?33 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 9891007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jost, John T., Glaser, Jack, Kruglanski, Arie W., & Sulloway, Frank J. (2003) “Political Conservatism as Motivated Social Cognition,” 129 Psychological Bulletin 339–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, Daniel (2011) Thinking Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Krupnikov, Yanna & Levine, Adam S. (2014) “Cross-Sample Comparisons and External Validity,” 1 J. of Experimental Political Science 5980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, Charles (2004) “Refusing to Give Name a Crime,” Washington Post, 22 June, sec. A, p. 6.Google Scholar
Lerner, Craig S. & Lund, Nelson (2010) “Judicial Duty and the Supreme Court's Cult of Celebrity,” 78 George Washington Law Rev. 1255–99.Google Scholar
Levay, Kevin E., Freese, Jeremy, & Druckman, James N. (2016) “The Demographic and Political Composition of Mechanical Turk Samples,” 6 SAGE Open 117 (January).Google Scholar
Liptak, Adam (2016) “When a Senator Passes Judgment on a Chief Justice,” New York Times, 19 April, sec. A, p. 17.Google Scholar
Maltzman, Forrest & Wahlbeck, Paul J. (1996, 58) “Inside the U.S. Supreme Court: The Reliability of the Justices' Conference Records,” J. of Politics 528–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markham, James (2006) “Against Individually Signed Judicial Opinions,” 56 Duke Law J. 923–51.Google Scholar
Martin, Andrew D. & Quinn, Kevin M. (2002) “Dynamic Ideal Point Estimation via Markov Chain Monte Carlo for the U.S. Supreme Court, 1953–1999,” 10 Political Analysis 134–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauro, Tony (1999) “Justice's Supreme Use of ‘I’ Sparks a Legal Frenzy.” USA Today, 2 April 2, sec. A, p. 11.Google Scholar
McDermott, Monika L. (1998) “Race and Gender Cues in Low-Information Elections,” 51 Political Research Q. 895918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mondak, Jeffrey J. (1992) “Institutional Legitimacy, Policy Legitimacy, and the Supreme Court,” 20 American Politics Q. 457–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mondak, Jeffrey J. (1994) “Policy Legitimacy and the Supreme Court: The Sources and Contexts of Legitimation,” 47 Political Research Q. 675–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, Laura M. & Vanneman, Reeve (2003) “Context Matters: Effects of the Proportion of Fundamentalists on Gender Attitudes,” 82 Social Forces 115–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullinix, Kevin J., Leeper, Thomas J., Druckman, James N., & Freese, Jeremy (2015) “The Generalizability of Survey Experiments,” 2 J. of Experimental Political Science 109–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mutz, Diana C. (2011) Population-Based Survey Experiments. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Myers, Dee Dee (2009) Why the Supreme Court Needs a ‘Critical Mass’ of Women. Vanity Fair (July 13). Available at: http://www.vanityfair.com/news/2009/07/why-the-supreme-court-needs-a-critical-mass-of-women (accessed 6 October 2018).Google Scholar
Nash, Jonathan R. (2015) “Expertise and Opinion Assignment on the Courts of Appeals: A Preliminary Investigation,” 66 Florida Law Rev. 1599–684.Google Scholar
National Women's Law Center (2016) Judges and the Courts: Women in the Federal Judiciary: Still a Long Way to Go. National Women's Law Center. Available at: http://nwlc.org/resources/women-federal-judiciary-still-long-way-go/ (accessed 6 October 2018).Google Scholar
Neff, Allen (1981) The United States District Judge Nominating Commissions: Their Members, Procedures and Candidates. Chicago, IL: American Judicature Society.Google Scholar
Nelson, Kjersten (2015) “Double-Bind on the Bench: Citizen Perceptions of Judge Gender and the Court,” 11 Politics & Gender 235–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholson, Stephen P. & Hansford, Thomas G. (2014) “Partisans in Robes: Party Cues and Public Acceptance of Supreme Court Decisions,” 58 American J. of Political Science 620–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norrander, Barbara & Wilcox, Clyde (1998) “Public Opinion and Policymaking in the States: The Case of Post-Roe Abortion Policy,” 27 Policy Studies J. 707–22.Google Scholar
Paolacci, Gabriele, Chandler, Jesse, & Ipeirotis, Panagiotis G. (2010) “Running Experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk,” 5 Judgment and Decision making 411–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patton, Dana & Smith, Joseph L. (2017) “Lawyer, Interrupted: Gender Bias in Oral Arguments at the US Supreme Court,” 5 J. of Law and Courts 337–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierce, Jennifer L. (1995) Gender Trials: Emotional Lives in Contemporary Law Firms. Berkeley: Univ. of California Press.Google Scholar
Powers, Rebecca S., Suitor, J. Jill, Guerra, Susana, Shackelford, Monisa, Mecom, Dorothy, & Gusman, Kim (2003) “Regional differences in Gender—Role Attitudes: Variations by Gender and Race,” 21 Gender Issues 4054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ray, Laura K. (2000) “The Road to Bush v. Gore: The History of the Supreme Court's Use of the per Curiam Opinion,” 79 Nebraska Law Rev. 517–76.Google Scholar
Robbins, Ira (2012) “Hiding behind the Cloak of Invisibility: The Supreme Court and per Curiam Opinions,” 86 Tulane Law Rev. 1197–242.Google Scholar
Ryan, Timothy J. (2012) “What Makes Us Click? Demonstrating Incentives for Angry Discourse with Digital-Age Field Experiments,” 74 J. of Politics 1138–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salamone, Michael F. (2014) “Judicial Consensus and Public Opinion: Conditional Response to Supreme Court Majority Size,” 62 Political Research Q. 320–34.Google Scholar
Schneider, Monica C. & Bos, Angela L. (2014) “Measuring Stereotypes of Female Politicians,” 35 Political Psychology 245–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slotnick, Elliot E. & Segal, Jennifer A. (1994) “‘Supreme Court Decided Today’…Or Did It?78 Judicature 8995.Google Scholar
Slotnick, Elliot E. & Segal, Jennifer A. (1998) Television News and the Supreme Court. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheb, John M. II & Lyons, William (2000) “The Myth of Legality and Public Evaluation of the Supreme Court,” 81 Social Science Q. 928–40.Google Scholar
Scherer, Nancy & Curry, Brett (2010) “Does Descriptive Race Representation Enhance Institutional Legitimacy? The Case of the U.S. Courts,” 72 J. of Politics 90104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sen, Maya (2014) “How Judicial Qualification Ratings May Disadvantage Minority and Female Candidates,” 2 J. of Law and Courts 3365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spill, Rorie & Oxley, Zoe (2003) “Philosopher Kings or Political Actors: How the Media Portray the Supreme Court,” 87 Judicature 22–9.Google Scholar
University of Albany (2016) Hindelang Criminal Justice Research Center: Sourcebook of Criminal Justice Statistics. Available at: http://www.albany.edu/sourcebook/about.html (accessed 6 October 2018).Google Scholar
Wang, Amy B. (2017) “Trump Lashes Out at ‘So-Called Judge’ Who Temporarily Blocked Travel Ban,” Washington Post, 4 February. Available at: https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/the-fix/wp/2017/02/04/trump-lashes-out-at-federal-judge-who-temporarily-blocked-travel-ban/?utm_term=.ae9d93b38678 (accessed 6 October 2018).Google Scholar
Weinberg, Jill D., Freese, Jeremy, & McElhattan, David (2014) “Comparing Data Characteristics and Results of an Online Factorial Survey between a Population-Based and a Crowdsource-Recruited Sample,” 1 Sociological Science 292310 (August).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, Richard (2012) “Using the Margins Command to Estimate and Interpret Adjusted Predictions and Marginal Effects,” 12 Stata J. 308–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Windett, Jason H. (2011) “State Effects and the Emergence and Success of Female Gubernatorial Candidates,” 11 State Politics & Policy Q. 460–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zilis, Michael A. (2015) The Limits of Legitimacy: Dissenting Opinions, Media Coverage, and Public Responses to Supreme Court Decisions. Ann Arbor: Univ. of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zink, James R., Spriggs, James F., & Scott, John T. (2009) “Courting the Public: The Influence of Decision Attributes on Individuals' Views of Court Opinions,” 71 J. of Politics 909–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Cases Cited

Bush v. Gore, 531 U.S. 98 (2000).Google Scholar
Furman v. Georgia, 408 U.S. 238 (1972).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiibel v. Sixth Judicial District of Nevada, 542 U.S. 177 (2004).Google Scholar
Korematsu v. United States, 323 U.S. 214 (1944).Google Scholar
New York Times Co. v. United States, 403 U.S. 713 (1971).Google Scholar
United States v. Virginia, 518 U.S. 515 (1996).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zubik v. Burwell, 578 U.S. ___ (2016).Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Agreement with Court's Decision, by Attribution. Notes: Entries in the top graph (mean agreement with decision) are mean agreement scores based on a 1–6 scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = slightly disagree, 4 = slightly agree, 5 = agree, and 6 = strongly agree). Analysis of variance produced a statistically significant effect (F = 16.84, p < .01) with one-tailed tests. Entries in the bottom graph (proportion agree with decision) are proportions based on a collapsed measure of the agreement scores (0 = disagree, 1 = agree). Difference of proportion analysis produced a statistically significant effect (Z = 3.24, p < .01) with one-tailed tests. For both analyses, N = 1287. The dashed line allows for easier comparison of the effect size across attribution conditions.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Agreement with Court's Decision, by Attribution and Respondent Ideology. Notes: Entries in the top graph (mean agreement with decision) are mean agreement scores based on a 1–6 scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = slightly disagree, 4 = slightly agree, 5 = agree, and 6 = strongly agree). Analysis of variance produced a statistically significant effect for conservative respondents (F = 18.56, p < .01) and for liberal respondents (F = 5.23, p < .01) with one-tailed tests. Entries in the bottom graph (proportion agree with decision) are proportions based on a collapsed measure of the agreement scores (0 = disagree, 1 = agree). Difference of proportion analysis produced a statistically significant effect for conservative respondents (Z = 3.58, p < .01) and for liberal respondents (Z = 1.70, p < .05) with one-tailed tests. For all analyses, N = 1287. The dashed line allows for easier comparison of the effect size across attribution conditions.

Figure 2

Table 1. OLS Estimates for Degree of Agreement/Disagreement with Supreme Court Decision

Figure 3

Figure 3. The Average Marginal Effects of an Opinion Attributed to a Justice Rather than the Court on Case-Specific Agreement as Political Ideology Ranges from Liberal to Conservative. Note: This figure demonstrates an interactive relationship in the linear regression model by plotting the average marginal effect of a justice attributed opinion on the dependent variable, case-level agreement (Hiibel), as political ideology ranges from liberal to conservative.

Figure 4

Table 2. OLS Estimates for Agreement with Supreme Court Decision, Including Justice Gender and Interaction with Respondents' Female Sociopolitical Culture (FSC)

Figure 5

Figure 4. The Effect of a Female Opinion Author on Case-Specific Agreement as Female Sociopolitical Culture Varies. Note: This figure demonstrates an interactive relationship by plotting the average marginal effects of a female opinion author on the dependent variable, case-level agreement (Hiibel), as female sociopolitical culture varies.

Figure 6

Figure 5. The Effect of a Female Opinion Author on Case-Specific Agreement as Female Sociopolitical Culture Varies among Male and Female Respondents. Note: This figure demonstrates a triple interactive relationship by plotting the average marginal effects of a female opinion author the dependent variable, case-level agreement, as female sociopolitical culture varies among male and female respondents.

Figure 7

Table A1 Logit Estimates for Agreement with Supreme Court Decision

Figure 8

Figure A1 The Average Marginal Effects of an Opinion Attributed to a Justice Rather than the Court on Case-Specific Agreement as Political Ideology Ranges from Liberal to Conservative. Note: This figure demonstrates an interactive relationship in the logistic regression model by plotting the average marginal effect of a justice attributed opinion on the dependent variable, case-level agreement (Hiibel), as political ideology ranges from liberal to conservative.

Figure 9

Table A2 OLS Estimates for Agreement with Supreme Court Decision. Interaction Between Justice Attributed Opinion and Respondent Gender

Figure 10

Table A3 OLS Estimates for Agreement with Supreme Court Decision. Head-to-Head Comparison of Female and Male Attributed Opinions

Supplementary material: File

Boddery et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 190 KB