Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T02:42:31.416Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The next generation of developmental psychopathology research: Including broader perspectives and becoming more precise

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 February 2024

Kristin Valentino*
Affiliation:
University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN, USA
Katherine Edler
Affiliation:
University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN, USA
*
Corresponding author: Kristin Valentino; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The current Special Issue marks a major milestone in the history of developmental psychopathology; as the final issue edited by Cicchetti, we have an opportunity to reflect on the remarkable progress of the discipline across the last four decades, as well as challenges and future directions for the field. With contemporary issues in mind, including rising rates of psychopathology, health disparities, and international conflict, as well as rapid growth and accessibility of digital and mobile technologies, the discipline of developmental psychopathology is poised to advance multidisciplinary, developmentally- and contextually- informed research, and to make substantial progress in supporting the healthy development of individuals around the world. We highlight key future directions and challenges for the next generation of developmental psychopathology research including further investigation of culture at multiple levels of analysis, incorporation of macro-level influences into developmental psychopathology research, methods advances to address heterogeneity in translational research, precision mental health, and the extension of developmental psychopathology research across the lifespan.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

As famously noted by Dante Cicchetti, special issues “mark significant and noteworthy points in the development and maturation of a scientific discipline” (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984, p.1). Indeed, the 1984 Special Issue of Child Development on the emergence of developmental psychopathology marked the crystallization of developmental psychopathology as a unified scientific discipline. By 1989, Cicchetti edited the inaugural issue of a new journal, Development & Psychopathology, which marked yet another step in the growth of the discipline. The current Special Issue also marks a major milestone in the history of developmental psychopathology; as the final issue edited by Cicchetti, we have an opportunity to reflect on the remarkable progress of the discipline across the last four decades, as well as challenges and future directions for the field.

Developmental psychopathology is an integrative discipline that has transformed our understanding of typical and atypical development. Whereas a full review of the history of developmental psychopathology and its core tenets is beyond the scope of this editorial, and has been well-described elsewhere (e.g., Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti, Rolf, Masten, Cicchetti, Neuchterlein and Weintraub1990; Valentino & Edler, Reference Valentino, Edler, Bornstein and Lambin press), we highlight here a few salient features of the discipline that have been especially influential. First, developmental psychopathology has challenged traditional notions of how to define psychopathology and has illuminated that we cannot separate the study of typical and atypical development from each other (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1990). Indeed, psychopathology is best conceptualized dimensionally, along a continua of symptoms from normative to pathological, rather than categorically (e.g., Achenbach et al., Reference Achenbach, Ivanova, Rescorla, Turner and Althoff2016; Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2003). Additionally, because psychopathology emerges over time in the transactions between developing individuals and their contexts, it is also essential to adopt a developmental and ecological perspective (Bronfenbrenner, Reference Bronfenbrenner1979) to interpret whether behavior is typical or atypical, and to understand the processes contributing to adaptive and maladaptive pathways (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010; Sameroff & Emde, Reference Sameroff and Emde1989.

Furthermore, because development is embedded in multiple ecological systems and is influenced by multiple risk and protective mechanisms operating at each level of ecology and across domains (Bronfenbrenner, Reference Bronfenbrenner1979), development is an incredibly complex and heterogeneous process. Indeed, we can observe multiple developmental pathways to the same outcome (equifinality), as well as multiple possible outcomes originating from the same initial conditions (multifinality) (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996). Moreover, we must integrate transactions across multiple levels of analysis, across contexts, and across development to understand the emergence of adaptation and psychopathology (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2008). As such, multilevel and multidisciplinary investigations have become hallmarks of the developmental psychopathology approach.

Over the last four decades, enormous progress has been made in incorporating these key principles into research. For example, we have observed a significant shift towards dimensional models of psychopathology including the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP; e.g., Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby, Brown, Carpenter, Caspi, Clark, Eaton, Forbes, Forbush, Goldberg, Hasin, Hyman, Ivanova, Lynam, Markon and Zimmerman2017). Similarly, building on the significant progress made in understanding the role of neurobiological, physiological, genetic, and epigenetic processes and transactions in both normative development and psychopathology, additional dimensional models such as the National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC; e.g., Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010) promote a mechanistic and multilevel approach to the understanding of psychopathology, with a heavy emphasis on biological units of analyses. Importantly, however, the constructs of development and context remain underemphasized or altogether missing from these models that currently predominate clinical psychology and psychiatry (Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Jamieson, Glenn and Nock2015; Tackett & Hallquist, Reference Tackett and Hallquist2022). Moreover, developmental psychopathology is not only concerned with the incorporation of biological units of analysis and related disciplines; instead, the developmental psychopathology perspective can be considered a macro-paradigm, or a framework for understanding development from multiple perspectives (Cummings & Valentino, Reference Cummings, Valentino, Overton, Molenaar and Lerner2015). As such, developmental psychopathology is an inclusive science that seeks to bridge all disciplines, perspectives, and levels of analysis (e.g., from genes and cells to broad sociocultural systems) and has room to grow in collaboration with additional disciplines that may bring expertise to the understanding of adaptation and psychopathology.

Contemporary challenges and opportunities

The developmental psychopathology approach will continue to evolve both in rising to the challenges of contemporary society and in progressing forward with advances in technology and methodology. In terms of challenges, rates of psychopathology are at historic highs, especially among youth. In the United States, one in five children face a mental health disorder annually (Office of the Surgeon General, 2021) and those from low-income families and minoritized racial and ethnic backgrounds are disproportionately affected (Alegría et al., Reference Alegría, Vallas and Pumariega2010). Prior to the COVID-19 pandemic, mental health challenges were already the leading cause of disability and poor life outcomes in youth (Perou et al., Reference Perou, Bitsko, Blumberg, Pastor, Ghandour, Gfroerer, Hedden, Crosby, Visser, Schieve, Parks, Hall, Brody, Simile, Thompson, Baio, Avenevoli, Kogan and Huang2013). The pandemic further exacerbated these concerns, and rates of depression and anxiety increased at least two-fold (Racine et al., Reference Racine, McArthur, Cooke, Eirich, Zhu and Madigan2021). Furthermore, individuals belonging to minoritized racial and ethnic groups were disproportionately impacted by the consequences of the pandemic (Alcendor, Reference Alcendor2020), serving as a stark example of the pernicious toll of structural racism on health and development in the United States (Dickinson et al., Reference Dickinson, Roberts, Banacos, Neuberger, Koebele, Blanch-Hartigan and Shanahan2021).

Globally, with significant political violence, conflict, war, and terrorism across the world, there are nearly 30 million refugees, and approximately 40% of refugees are youth (UN Refugee Agency, 2023). Therefore, another global challenge is the mental health of refugees and other forcibly displaced persons (Daar et al., Reference Daar, Chang, Salomon and Singer2018). Refugees are trauma-exposed and especially vulnerable to psychopathology in the context of an uncertain future with limited resources and social support (Byrow et al., Reference Byrow, Pajak, Specker and Nickerson2020; Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Bryant, Silove and Steel2011). The developmental psychopathology perspective has often emphasized that pathological conditions or disruptions can provide unique opportunities to learn more about normative developmental mechanisms (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1990). For example, the study of child maltreatment as an ‘experiment of nature’ (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2003) has allowed researchers to understand more about the role of early adequate caregiving for children’s developmental trajectories. In the same vein, examinations of political violence, war, and forcible displacement may provide an opportunity to learn more about how macro-level processes influence development and psychopathology (Masten & Narayan, Reference Masten and Narayan2012). Such investigations are important and could be leveraged to inform and develop new intervention approaches and policy recommendations to support youth mental health and well-being.

Another key feature of our modern society is the rapid advancement of digital and mobile technologies. In the United States, most two-year-olds use a digital device every day and 95% of adolescents own a smartphone (Anderson & Jiang, Reference Anderson and Jiang2018; Kabali et al., Reference Kabali, Irigoyen, Nunez-Davis, Budacki, Mohanty, Leister and Bonner2015). By 2025, estimates suggest there will be 5.6 billion mobile connections that will be used by nearly three-quarters of the global population (Odea, Reference Odea2020). The accessibility of mobile devices provides new opportunities, including the delivery of healthcare services to individuals throughout the lifespan and across contexts—particularly in places where accessibility to traditional, in-person services are limited including low-income and rural communities, as well as low- and middle-income countries (e.g., McCool et al., Reference McCool, Dobson, Whittaker and Paton2022).

The proliferation of mobile devices has enabled opportunities to conduct more ecologically valid research and to answer more nuanced research questions about within-person change; at the same time, its integration into our everyday lives necessitates more research on the effects of mobile technology itself on development and psychopathology. Regarding the former, smartphones can be leveraged for the collection of ecological momentary assessment data, where participants can report on their moods or behaviors in real time, multiple times per day. These rich, longitudinal, micro-time data allow us to answer questions about intraindividual dynamics. Additional wearable technologies and sensors can allow for real-world measurement of physiology, physical activity, sleep, proximity to others, and natural language exposure (e.g., George et al., Reference George, Russell, Odgers, Centifanti and Williams2017, King et al., Reference King, Querdasi, Humphreys and Gotlib2021, Salo et al., Reference Salo, Pannuto, Hedgecock, Biri, Russo, Piersiak and Humphreys2021) and enable coordinated collection of multilevel data that can be used to study dynamic in-the-moment risk and protective mechanisms associated with emerging mental health problems.

As individuals integrate digital technologies into their everyday lives, developmental psychopathologists need to consider how these digital or virtual contexts are influencing (and are influenced by) individuals over time. Investigations into digital media are just now beginning to advance past main effects-type research that has primarily focused on the impact of the duration of screen time on child development, in part because of limitations in tools to assess children’s media use and the family media context (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Kirkorian, Radesky, Coyne, Nichols, Blanchfield, Rusnak, Stockdale, Ribner, Durnez, Epstein, Heimann, Koch, Sundqvist, Birberg-Thornberg, Konrad, Slussareff, Bus, Bellagamba and Fitzpatrick2020). Of course, more nuanced questions can and should be addressed, considering the child’s developmental competencies and context as well as the timing, type, content, and quality of their digital technology use (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Kirkorian, Radesky, Coyne, Nichols, Blanchfield, Rusnak, Stockdale, Ribner, Durnez, Epstein, Heimann, Koch, Sundqvist, Birberg-Thornberg, Konrad, Slussareff, Bus, Bellagamba and Fitzpatrick2020). Moreover, as technology is increasingly embedded in daily interactions, and as the boundaries between virtual and physical realms becomes less clear, it may be important to consider adaptations to ecological systems theory to include virtual microsystems, as suggested by emerging theories such as neo-ecological theory (Navarro & Tudge, Reference Navarro and Tudge2023). Interestingly, individuals always participate in a virtual microsystem while being physically present in another (traditional) microsystem (e.g., at home, in school). Effects of technology on child development may be direct, or indirect through their influences on other microsystem processes. For example, one could imagine that family relationships dynamics may change as children spend more of their time at home interacting with others in virtual contexts. Moving forward, we need a better understanding of mechanisms operating in digital contexts and their influence on development and psychopathology.

Future directions

With the context of contemporary society in mind, including rising rates of psychopathology, health disparities, and international conflict, as well as rapid growth and accessibility of digital and mobile technologies, the discipline of developmental psychopathology is poised to advance multidisciplinary, developmentally- and contextually- informed research, and to make substantial progress in supporting the healthy development of individuals around the world. Below we highlight key future directions and challenges for the next generation of developmental psychopathology research.

Culture & developmental psychopathology

First, it is important to further efforts to study culture and developmental psychopathology. This is not a new idea, but one that bears repeating because the incorporation of culture into developmental psychopathology research continues to lag behind the incorporation of other units of analysis (Causadias & Cicchetti, Reference Causadias and Cicchetti2018; Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). Notably, developmental psychopathology has always emphasized that culture is a context that must be considered to interpret whether a particular behavior is normative or not (e.g., García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Akerman and Cicchetti2000; Hinshaw & Cicchetti, Reference Hinshaw and Cicchetti2000), and similarly, cultural processes have been long included in ecological transactional models of psychopathology (e.g., Cicchetti & Lynch, Reference Cicchetti and Lynch1993; Cicchetti & Valentino, Reference Cicchetti, Valentino, Cicchetti and Cohen2006). However, as noted by Causadias (Reference Causadias2013), most conceptualizations of culture in psychopathology research have tended to be static rather than developmental. As such, an important priority for culture and developmental psychopathology research is to study culture in relation to psychopathology from a developmental and multilevel perspective.

Culture is a system of people, places, and practices, and operates dynamically at multiple levels of ecology rather than being static and distal (Causadias, Reference Causadias2020). Clearly, culture operates at the social level as it is created and shared by communities, and it provides meaning and goals to our societal systems. The social dimensions of culture are also reflected in the concepts of ethnicity and race, which are social concepts used to “represent group categorizations based on real or perceived commonalities, the social hierarchies these taxonomies reflect and enforce, and the biases and stereotypes they engender” (Causadias & Cicchetti, Reference Causadias and Cicchetti2018, p.1550). Furthermore, as described by scholars such as Garíca Coll and Spencer decades ago, culture operates at and influences processes at every level of ecology (García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Crnic, Lamberty and Wasik1996; Spencer, Reference Spencer, Damon and Lerner2006).

Significant progress has been made in identifying key cultural risk and protective factors, both across and within cultural groups. For example, racism, defined as “a system of dominance, power, and privilege based on racial group designations; rooted in the historical oppression of a group defined or perceived by dominant-group members as inferior, deviant or undesirable” (Harrell, Reference Harrell2000, p.43) is a cultural risk factor that adversely affects ethnic minority groups. Racism manifests at multiple ecological levels including the systemic level, as well through individual racialized discrimination and aggression (García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Crnic, Lamberty and Wasik1996; Jones, Reference Jones1972; Williams & Williams-Morris, Reference Williams and Williams-Morris2000). Discrimination increases risk for psychopathology and other poor developmental outcomes (Paradies et al., Reference Paradies, Ben, Denson, Elias, Priest, Pieterse, Gupta, Kelaher and Gee2015; Pascoe & Smart Richman, Reference Pascoe and Smart Richman2009; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Lawrence and Davis2019). These effects are observed directly, as well as indirectly through the effects of racism on other processes. For example, racism proliferates stress, which may increase family conflict and disrupt parenting (Brody et al., Reference Brody, Chen, Kogan, Murry, Logan and Luo2008; Murry et al., Reference Murry, Gonzalez, Hanebutt, Bulgin, Coates, Inniss-Thompson, Debreaux, Wilson, Abel and Cortez2022), which have adverse effects on child development.

Cultural protective factors are related to positive developmental trajectories and decrease risk for psychopathology. For example, cultural values and pride are associated with positive youth adjustment (e.g., Gaylord-Harden et al., Reference Gaylord-Harden, Burrow and Cunningham2012; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Rodriguez, Smith, Johnson, Stevenson and Spicer2006). Moreover, several protective factors have been identified to buffer the pernicious effects of exposure to ethnic-racial discrimination on youth mental health. For example, among Mexican origin adolescents, ethnic identity and social support attenuate the link between discrimination and maladjustment (Park et al., Reference Park, Wang, Williams and Alegría2018). These ethnically homogenous, within-group designs focused on multilevel cultural risk and protective factors are essential for identifying culturally specific mechanistic prevention and intervention targets that will advance and inform culturally appropriate prevention and intervention programs for youth psychopathology. In sum, culture is not only a systemic/macro-level process, and progress in advancing developmental psychopathology must include further evaluation of cultural processes at all levels of analysis.

Incorporation of broader structural levels of analysis

Integration of additional macro-level, structural constructs into our research, and collaboration with social scientists from additional disciplines such as public health, public policy, economics, social work and sociology are critical for gaining a greater understanding of the factors that are contributing to maladaptation, especially as we pursue the goals of promoting adaptive development throughout the lifespan and reducing mental health inequities. There is clear evidence that structural aspects of our contexts and society, including state and federal policies, have influence on development and psychopathology. For example, poverty is strongly associated with psychopathology across the lifespan (e.g., Peverill et al., Reference Peverill, Dirks, Narvaja, Herts, Comer and McLaughlin2021). To combat the adverse effects of poverty on child development, the US government sponsors several anti-poverty programs including cash assistance programs such as Earned Income Tax Credit and Temporary Assistance for Needy Families (TANF). TANF is intended to be work-focused and time-limited, aimed at promoting employment and reducing welfare dependency. States have discretion in the generosity and implementation of these programs, including time limits, family caps, and the severity of sanctions for noncompliance with work requirements (Dunifon et al., Reference Dunifon, Hynes and Peters2006). Increased TANF cash benefits and access are associated with decreased child physical abuse, whereas time limits are associated with increased abuse (Spencer et al., Reference Spencer, Livingston, Komro, Sroczynski, Rentmeester and Woods-Jaeger2021). Moreover, the generosity of state anti-poverty policies may buffer associations between poverty and both youth brain development and psychological functioning (Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Hatzenbuehler, Cikara, Barch and McLaughlin2023). In Weissman et al.s’ (Reference Weissman, Hatzenbuehler, Cikara, Barch and McLaughlin2023) analysis of Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study data of youth across 17 American states, lower income was associated with smaller hippocampal volume and greater internalizing symptoms; however, when youth lived in states with more generous cash benefits, these health disparities were reduced (Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Hatzenbuehler, Cikara, Barch and McLaughlin2023). Altogether, this research implicates state-level macroeconomic factors as influential in the development of psychopathology. Because structural racism drives inequities in poverty and health across racial and ethnic groups in the United States (Bailey et al., Reference Bailey, Krieger, Agénor, Graves, Linos and Bassett2017), more generous anti-poverty programs may be a key way to reduce these health disparities.

Research on exosystem and macrosystem factors must then be incorporated into multilevel prevention and intervention efforts, Developmental psychopathologists often aim to translate research into individual- or family-level programs; however, not all conditions can be addressed, or should be addressed, with individual- or family-level treatment approaches (e.g., Alvarez et al., Reference Alvarez, Cervantes, Nelson, Seag, Horwitz and Hoagwood2022; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Simon, Yadeta, Patterson and Anderson2023). When we have evidence that factors from broader ecological levels are contributing to psychopathology, we must not only create programs that help individuals and their families cope with those factors; we must also advocate for structural and policy-level changes. For example, with regard to structural racism as a macro-level risk factor, Jones et al. (Reference Jones, Simon, Yadeta, Patterson and Anderson2023) consider how current conceptualizations of resilience among Black youth fall short. In particular, Jones et al., urge the field to not be complacent in helping Black youth successfully cope with racism, but move “toward collective efforts to transform our approach, pushing back against the perniciousness of racism” (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Simon, Yadeta, Patterson and Anderson2023, p.1). Moreover, taking a public health approach, Alvarez et al. (Reference Alvarez, Cervantes, Nelson, Seag, Horwitz and Hoagwood2022) provide several recommendations both within and across the mental health and policy-level systems to attend to racism and mental health equity and to catalyze broader systems transformation. These recommendations include continued research on important individual- and family-level interventions aimed at enhancing ethnic-racial socialization (e.g., EMBRace; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Jones, Navarro, McKenny, Mehta and Stevenson2018) and integrating racial socialization into trauma-focused cognitive behavior therapy (Metzger et al., Reference Metzger, Anderson, Are and Ritchwood2021) while also attending to systems-level efforts. Just as developmental psychopathology has facilitated collaboration with genetics, biological science, immunology and physiology to transform our understanding of developmental trajectories towards adaptation and maladaptation, so too can enhancing our collaborative efforts with fields dedicated to broader, social and structural levels of analysis.

Methods advances to embrace heterogeneity in translational research

A hallmark of the developmental psychopathology approach is its focus on heterogeneity in both manifestations of and developmental trajectories underlying typical and atypical development. Increasingly, there is evidence that prevention and intervention programs are not equally effective for all people, at all times, and in all contexts (e.g., Bryan et al., Reference Bryan, Tipton and Yeager2021; Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Volkmann, Volkmann, Karyotaki, Cuijpers and Brakemeier2022); even more, there is acknowledgement that much more research is needed to understand the nuances of how treatments work (Mulder et al., Reference Mulder, Murray and Rucklidge2017; Zilcha-Mano, Reference Zilcha-Mano2021). In light of clear equifinality and multifinality in development more broadly and treatment research specifically, one key future direction for developmental psychopathology is the advancement of and uptake in use of methods that embrace heterogeneity; this is especially needed in the context of translational research.

Historically, psychological research has relied heavily on variable-centered, between-person analyses. The variable is the main unit of analysis in a variable-centered approach (Bergman & Magnusson, Reference Bergman and Magnusson1997; Laursen & Hoff, Reference Laursen and Hoff2006); analyses could include examining the association between parental emotion dysregulation and harsh parenting behaviors, cross-sectionally or longitudinally. When an association is found between two variables at the between-person level, it indicates the extent to which people who are above average in one variable also tend to be above (or below) average on the other (Wang & Maxwell, Reference Wang and Maxwell2015). For example, variable-centered, between-person analyses suggest that parents who are above average in emotion dysregulation also tend to be above average in negative parenting behaviors (for a review, see Edler & Valentino, Reference Edler and Valentinoin press). These analyses are useful in that they demonstrate the presence of an association at the level of a sample. They can also teach us about what is happening on average in a given group, illuminating potential mechanisms for future translational research to target.

Yet, variable-centered, between-person analyses gloss over individual differences and the time-varying nature of many psychological processes, and can only be generalized to the individual under specific conditions that are difficult to meet in practice (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Medaglia and Jeronimus2018; Molenaar & Campbell, Reference Molenaar and Campbell2009). They are also limited in their capacity to capture heterogeneity, given their reliance on the assumption that the population is homogeneous with respect to how one variable acts on another (see Laursen & Hoff, Reference Laursen and Hoff2006 for a review). Even more, the presence of a between-person association does not mean that an association between the same variables exists in the same magnitude and or even in the same direction at the within-person level (e.g., Curran & Bauer, Reference Curran and Bauer2011; Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015; Wang & Maxwell, Reference Wang and Maxwell2015; Zilcha-Mano, Reference Zilcha-Mano2021). Thus, while variable-centered, between-person analyses are valuable, we must be cautious in considering how they may be applied to the development of intervention and prevention programs, and whether they may guide decisions at the individual level.

Advances in statistical methods and designs allow researchers to evaluate processes at the person-level. A researcher could use a person-centered analysis, in which the individual is the main analytical unit and individuals are clustered together based on their levels of multiple variables (Bergman & Magnusson, Reference Bergman and Magnusson1997; Laursen & Hoff, Reference Laursen and Hoff2006); for example, researchers could seek to identify groupings of parents who engage in different levels of multiple emotion regulation strategies, and then examine whether these groupings are associated with harsh parenting behaviors. Or, researchers could take advantage of the opportunities that digital technologies provide to gather repeated measures of real-time data from participants with their smartphones, such as tracking emotion regulation and parenting multiple times per day. Disaggregation of between- and within-person components of a model using repeated measures data (Curran & Bauer, Reference Curran and Bauer2011; Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015; Wang & Maxwell, Reference Wang and Maxwell2015) can allow a researcher to examine an association of interest at within-person level, such as whether parents engage in more harsh parenting behaviors when they are experiencing higher levels of emotion dysregulation. Finally, repeated measures data could enable researchers to conduct an N-of-1 study on a single parent to gain insight into unique, intraindividual processes (Kazdin, Reference Kazdin and Kazdin2016; Schork, Reference Schork2015). Apart from applied behavior analysis research, however, person-level methods are still relatively rarely integrated into program efficacy research, including randomized clinical trials (though see Deisenhofer et al., Reference Deisenhofer, Barkham, Beierl, Schwartz, Aafjes-van Doorn, Beevers, Berwian, Blackwell, Bockting, Brakemeier, Brown, Buckman, Castonguay, Cusack, Dalgleish, de Jong, Delgadillo, DeRubeis, Driessen and Cohen2024 for a discussion of person-level methods in the treatment of psychopathology, and see Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Bosley, Fernandez, Reeves, Soyster, Diamond and Barkin2019 for an empirical example of using data from individuals to inform personalized treatment).

When appropriate distinctions are not made between group and individual levels, within- and between-person associations, and variable- and person-centered analyses, seemingly conflicting findings can emerge, and prevention and intervention programs may be designed based on imprecise evidence. Take for example the literature on adverse childhood experiences (ACEs). Evidence suggests that people who are above average in number of exposures to childhood adversities also tend to be above average in psychopathology symptoms and physical health difficulties throughout the lifespan (e.g., Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards, Koss and Marks1998). In the decades since this seminal ACE study, universal efforts to screen youth for their ACE scores as a way to determine who may benefit from trauma-informed care have surged. However, although ACE scores can predict mean group differences in some health outcomes, they have poor accuracy in predicting an individual’s risk for poor health outcomes (Baldwin et al., Reference Baldwin, Caspi, Meehan, Ambler, Arseneault, Fisher, Harrington, Matthews, Odgers, Poulton, Ramrakha, Moffitt and Danese2021), leading some to caution against universal trauma screening and using ACE scores to guide clinical decisions (e.g., Anda et al., Reference Anda, Porter and Brown2020). Altogether, these findings highlight the need for increased attention to different study designs and levels of analysis, and what they can and cannot tell us about individuals, groups, and who is likely to benefit from experimental manipulation of a mechanism (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Medaglia and Jeronimus2018; Zilcha-Mano, Reference Zilcha-Mano2021).

More broadly, consideration is needed regarding how developmental psychopathologists can make thoughtful methodological choices that apply the concepts of equifinality and multifinality to prevention and intervention design and evaluation. Calls have been made to move prevention and intervention research beyond group-level, main effects evaluations (Bryan et al., Reference Bryan, Tipton and Yeager2021; Mulder et al., Reference Mulder, Murray and Rucklidge2017), and we reiterate that call here. As Bryan et al. (Reference Bryan, Tipton and Yeager2021) have noted, “we must expect, study, and capitalize on the heterogeneity that characterize most effects in science” (p. 986). Developmental psychopathology fundamentally expects individual differences in constellations of risk and protective mechanisms and developmental trajectories. It is not surprising, then, that there is heterogeneity in pathways to psychopathology manifestation (equifinality) and pathways following treatment (multifinality). Moving forward, it is essential that both our theorizing and methods together align with the reality that there are many distinct pathways that can precede and follow an individual’s involvement in a prevention or intervention program. Just as the developmental psychopathology approach pushed the field away from simple main effects to evaluate complex transactions over time and in context, our methods and translational research designs must now expand above and beyond group comparisons that assume homogeneity to mechanistic, idiographic research that can benefit individuals.

Developmental psychopathology & precision mental health

Acknowledging the heterogeneity in treatment response both between individuals and within individuals over time (e.g., Bryan et al., Reference Bryan, Tipton and Yeager2021; Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Volkmann, Volkmann, Karyotaki, Cuijpers and Brakemeier2022; Murphy, Reference Murphy2005), an important priority for the field is to identify methods to evaluate interventions in more personalized ways that can match individuals to the treatments that may be most effective for them. This personalized approach to health care is often referred to as precision or personalized medicine (e.g., Collins & Varmus, Reference Collins and Varmus2015; Schork, Reference Schork2015). Precision medicine is sometimes narrowly focused on applications of genetic testing to guide treatment decisions; however, it can refer more broadly to evidence-based treatment recommendations based on individual features (DeRubeis, Reference DeRubeis2019). Personalized recommendations can include treatment selection, dosage, delivery mode, and timing, and can be informed by characteristics at multiple levels of analysis including an individual’s biological, psychological, and contextual characteristics and circumstances (e.g., Collins & Varmus, Reference Collins and Varmus2015; Deisenhofer et al., Reference Deisenhofer, Barkham, Beierl, Schwartz, Aafjes-van Doorn, Beevers, Berwian, Blackwell, Bockting, Brakemeier, Brown, Buckman, Castonguay, Cusack, Dalgleish, de Jong, Delgadillo, DeRubeis, Driessen and Cohen2024; Ng & Weisz, Reference Ng and Weisz2016; Wright & Woods, Reference Wright and Woods2020; Zilcha-Mano, Reference Zilcha-Mano2021).

A multivariable, multilevel approach is likely critical for making significant progress in the identification of precision treatment rules that can guide treatment personalization. When focused on individual moderators of treatment efficacy, no single variable has emerged as powerful enough be clinically meaningful or to guide treatment decisions, however, multivariable approaches appear to be a promising method for advancing personalized, precision treatment (DeRubeis, Reference DeRubeis2019; Lorenzo-Luaces et al., Reference Lorenzo-Luaces, Peipert, De Jesus Romero, Rutter and Rodriguez-Quintana2021). A recent meta-analysis of personalized psychological interventions versus standardized psychological interventions for adults reported a small but statistically significant effect, such that personalized treatments were more effective than standardized treatments; this effect remained statistically significant even after removing studies that had a high risk of bias (Nye et al., Reference Nye, Delgadillo and Barkham2023). As this important work moves forward it will be important to further integrate aspects of developmental timing and context into these personalization endeavors. Moreover, the extension of these methods to the development of personalized prevention programs is an area ripe for innovation that has potential for high public health impact (August & Gewirtz, Reference August and Gewirtz2019; Supplee & Duggan, Reference Supplee and Duggan2019).

Another promising approach for the optimization of individual outcomes is to consider sequential interventions. Given limitations in any single treatment approach, multiple interventions (or intervention components) may be used in sequence to improve individual outcomes; however, traditional randomized clinical trial designs often focus on the efficacy of one treatment at a time. An alternate RCT approach is the sequential multiple assignment randomized trial (SMART; Lavori & Dawson, Reference Lavori and Dawson2004; Murphy, Reference Murphy2005) which can be used to rigorously evaluate tailored sequential interventions, otherwise known as adaptive interventions (Collins et al., Reference Collins, Murphy and Stretcher2008). This type of design may be especially useful in evaluating the effectiveness of tiered adaptive intervention approaches which begin with lower intensity treatments, assess for treatment response to the initial intervention, and then provide additional, more intensive treatment components to nonresponders. Adaptive, multicomponent programs that can flexibly provide treatments that match the needs of individuals is a promising approach to intervention and prevention. The Building Healthy Children program (Demeusy et al., Reference Demeusy, Handley, Manly, Sturm and Toth2021; Paradis et al., Reference Paradis, Sandler, Manly and Valentine2013) is an example of an adaptive model of home-visitation prevention services that includes a variety of treatment components including supporting maternal depression, maternal trauma, mother-child attachment, parenting skills, and families’ concrete needs; it has been shown to have positive short and long term benefits for mothers and their young children (Demeusy et al., Reference Demeusy, Handley, Manly, Sturm and Toth2021). Although adaptive, multicomponent prevention programs may initially require a greater investment in training and evaluation, they have the potential for ultimately providing communities with effective and efficient solutions (Supplee & Duggan, Reference Supplee and Duggan2019). Altogether, both precision medicine and sequential interventions have great promise for preventing and intervening to support psychological well-being and should be prioritized in developmental psychopathology research.

The extension of DP to understand lifespan development & psychopathology

A final challenge and future direction for the field of developmental psychopathology is the expansion of research across the lifespan to better characterize the development of psychopathology among adults. Although the onset of psychopathology often occurs during adolescence and emerging adulthood (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Ambler, Danese, Elliott, Hariri, Harrington, Hogan, Poulton, Ramrakha, Rasmussen, Reuben, Richmond-Rakerd, Sugden, Wertz, Williams and Moffitt2020), risk for psychopathology continues across the lifespan into middle and late adulthood (e.g., Durak et al., Reference Durak, Karakose and Yow2023). Of course, developmental psychopathology has long defined itself by its commitment to understanding trajectories of typical and typical development across the lifespan. Perspectives on developmental psychopathology and young adulthood have been advanced (e.g., Burt & Paysnick, Reference Burt and Paysnick2012; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti, Crockett, Carlo and Schulenberg2023), previously mentioned dimensional models of psychopathology focus on adulthood (e.g., Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby, Brown, Carpenter, Caspi, Clark, Eaton, Forbes, Forbush, Goldberg, Hasin, Hyman, Ivanova, Lynam, Markon and Zimmerman2017), and there is a rich literature on the psychology of aging (e.g., Greve & Staudinger, Reference Greve, Staudinger, Cicchetti and Cohen2006; Ong et al., Reference Ong, Bergeman and Boker2009; Rothermund et al., Reference Rothermund, Englert and Gerstorf2023). Nevertheless, the field of developmental psychopathology can do more to explicate stage-salient developmental tasks across adulthood, particularly middle and late adulthood. There is also a need for greater knowledge of risk and protective mechanisms of psychopathology that may be unique to middle and late adulthood (e.g., Kitayama et al., Reference Kitayama, Berg and Chopik2020; Stroebe et al., Reference Stroebe, Schut and Stroebe2007; Teo et al., Reference Teo, Cheng, Cheng, Lau and Lau2023; Yon et al., Reference Yon, Mikton, Gassoumis and Wilber2017). In sum, a more coherent framework for the development of psychopathology across adulthood is needed.

Infancy, childhood, and adolescence have been the focus of much of developmental psychopathology research. One reason for this is evidence for sensitive periods (see Gee & Cohodes, Reference Gee and Cohodes2021; Sisk & Gee, Reference Sisk and Gee2022 for reviews) and stage-salient developmental tasks (see Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1993; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1979 for reviews) during these developmental periods. Given that early experiences set the stage for development across the lifespan in an organizational, probabilistic manner, it is logical to focus basic and translational research on infancy, childhood, and adolescence, as many developmental cascades initiate based on success or failure resolving stage-salient tasks. However, into and across adulthood, there are additional developmental milestones and tasks. During the developmental period of emerging adulthood, for example, individuals may begin to live apart from their families, pursue vocational training or advanced education, enter the workforce, initiate and maintain romantic relationships, and start their own families (e.g., Arnett et al., Reference Arnett, Žukauskienė and Sugimura2014; Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Masten, Coatsworth and Tellegen2004). Across middle and late adulthood, adults may navigate familial transitions (e.g., adult children moving out of the house, becoming a grandparent, bereavement), career changes and retirement, age-related declines in functioning and corresponding changes in independence, and concerns about generativity, religiosity, mortality, and life purpose. Developmental milestones earlier in the lifespan (e.g., attachment, self-regulation) generally appear to be experienced during relatively short spans of time that are consistent across individuals. On the other hand, developmental tasks of emerging, middle, and late adulthood are unique in that, for some of them, whether and when they occur is variable and affected by cultural expectations and individual choices. Nevertheless, the extent to which any of these developmental tasks are successfully resolved could have an influence on psychological well-being across adulthood.

Take for example the transition to parenthood. Burgeoning evidence demonstrates the notable changes that occur at multiple levels of analysis and ecology as individuals become parents (e.g., Barrett & Fleming, Reference Barrett and Fleming2011; Gettler et al., Reference Gettler, McDade, Feranil and Kuzawa2011; Hoekzema et al., Reference Hoekzema, Barba-Müller, Pozzobon, Picado, Lucco, García-García and Vilarroya2017; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Strathearn and Swain2016; Saxbe et al., Reference Saxbe, Rossin-Slater and Goldenberg2018, Reference Saxbe, Martínez-Garcia, Cardenas, Waizman and Carmona2023). As noted by Saxbe et al. (Reference Saxbe, Rossin-Slater and Goldenberg2018), the transition to parenthood is characterized by such neurobiological, psychosocial, and behavioral changes that it represents a key window for determining trajectories of mental and physical health (see also Howland, Reference Howland2023). Still, while parenthood is a life-changing developmental milestone for some adults, not all adults choose or are able to become parents; our study of adulthood must expand beyond simply considering adults in their roles as parents.

Indeed, the field of developmental psychopathology seems to lose track of adults outside of their role as parents. Once the transition to parenthood begins, parents tend to be viewed in relation to their children’s developmental trajectories, such that the infant replaces the parent(s) at the center of the family’s ecology (Valentino & Edler, Reference Valentino, Edler, Bornstein and Lambin press). Even when parental psychopathology is the explicit subject of investigation, the motivating reason for understanding and intervening to address it is often its potential intergenerational, negative consequences on youth. There is great reason for attention to the downstream effects of parental psychopathology on youth, and this research should continue. At the same time, adult psychopathology research would benefit from the incorporation of developmental psychopathology principles.

Moving forward, the field of developmental psychopathology should advance research on the specific risk and protective factors involved in trajectories toward psychopathology and from the treatment of psychopathology within and across emerging, middle, and late adulthood. For adults who are parents, this should include attention to transactional dynamics, such as child-driven effects on parents (e.g., Paschall & Mastergeorge, Reference Paschall and Mastergeorge2016). As dimensional models of (adult) psychopathology continue to gain momentum (e.g., Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby, Brown, Carpenter, Caspi, Clark, Eaton, Forbes, Forbush, Goldberg, Hasin, Hyman, Ivanova, Lynam, Markon and Zimmerman2017), there is much room for collaboration with developmental psychopathologists to incorporate developmental and contextual perspectives and to facilitate a better understanding of the development of psychopathology across the lifespan.

Conclusion

In summary, developmental psychopathology has made tremendous strides and scientific contributions over the last four decades. The discipline has facilitated our understanding of typical and atypical development, developmental mechanisms and cascades, complexity and heterogeneity of developmental processes, and the translation of research into prevention and intervention approaches. As founder and Editor of the field’s leading journal for the last 35 years, Dante Cicchetti has provided exceptional leadership for the discipline with his clear vision for the future, his nurturance of ideas, and his mentorship of students and junior scholars. Moreover, he created a top-tier platform for developmental psychopathologists to share their research with each other and the scientific community through the creation of the journal Development & Psychopathology. Notably, in an editorial on reactions, reflections and projections for developmental psychopathology, written in 1993, Cicchetti articulated “If I could choose one prediction that I hope will actually reach fruition, it would be the continuation and elaboration of the mutually enriching interchanges occurring between investigators of normal developmental theory and psychopathology” (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1993, p. 496). As Cicchetti concludes his extraordinary tenure as Editor, it is clear that his prediction has indeed reached fruition; he has inspired new generations of scholars to continue developmental psychopathology research and the journal ensures these mutually enriching interchanges across disciplines will continue to have a home.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the R01HD091235.

Competing interests

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

Achenbach, T. M., Ivanova, M. Y., Rescorla, L. A., Turner, L. V., & Althoff, R. R. (2016). Internalizing/externalizing problems: Review and recommendations for clinical and research applications. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(8), 647656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alcendor, D. J. (2020). Racial disparities-associated COVID-19 mortality among minority populations in the US. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 9(8), 2442. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9082442 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alegría, M., Vallas, M., & Pumariega, A. J. (2010). Racial and ethnic disparities in pediatric mental health. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics, 19(4), 759774. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2010.07.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alvarez, K., Cervantes, P. E., Nelson, K. L., Seag, D. E. M., Horwitz, S. M., & Hoagwood, K. E. (2022). Review: Structural racism, children’s mental health service systems, and recommendations for policy and practice change. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 61(9), 10871105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2021.12.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anda, R. F., Porter, L. E., & Brown, D. W. (2020). Inside the Adverse Childhood Experience score: Strengths, limitations, and misapplications. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 59(2), 293295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2020.01.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, M., & Jiang, J. (2018). Teens, social media & technology 2018. Pew Research Center, 31(2018), 16731689.Google Scholar
Anderson, R. E., Jones, S. C., Navarro, C. C., McKenny, M. C., Mehta, T. J., & Stevenson, H. C. (2018). Addressing the mental health needs of Black American youth and families: A case study from the EMBRace intervention. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(5), 898. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15050898 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnett, J. J., Žukauskienė, R., & Sugimura, K. (2014). The new life stage of emerging adulthood at ages 18-29 years: Implications for mental health. The Lancet Psychiatry, 1(7), 569576. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00080-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
August, G. J., & Gewirtz, A. (2019). Moving toward a precision-based, personalized framework for prevention science: Introduction to the special issue. Prevention Science, 20(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-018-0955-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, Z. D., Krieger, N., Agénor, M., Graves, J., Linos, N., & Bassett, M. T. (2017). Structural racism and health inequities in the USA: Evidence and interventions. The Lancet, 389(10077), 14531463. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30569-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, J. R., Caspi, A., Meehan, A. J., Ambler, A., Arseneault, L., Fisher, H. L., Harrington, H. L., Matthews, T., Odgers, C. L., Poulton, R., Ramrakha, S., Moffitt, T. E., Danese, A. (2021). Population vs individual prediction of poor health from results of adverse childhood experiences screening. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(4), 385393. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.5602 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, R., Kirkorian, H., Radesky, J., Coyne, S., Nichols, D., Blanchfield, O., Rusnak, S., Stockdale, L., Ribner, A., Durnez, J., Epstein, M., Heimann, M., Koch, F.-S., Sundqvist, A., Birberg-Thornberg, U., Konrad, C., Slussareff, M., Bus, A., Bellagamba, F., Fitzpatrick, caroline (2020). Beyond screen time: A synergistic approach to a more comprehensive assessment of family media exposure during early childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1283. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01283 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, J., & Fleming, A. S. (2011). Annual research review: All mothers are not created equal: Neural and psychobiological perspectives on mothering and the importance of individual differences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(4), 368397. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02306.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P. (2003). Taxometrics and developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 15(3), 501527. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579403000270 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergman, L. R., & Magnusson, D. (1997). A person-oriented approach in research on developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 9(2), 291319. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457949700206X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., Chen, Y. F., Kogan, S. M., Murry, V. M., Logan, P., & Luo, Z. (2008). Linking perceived discrimination to longitudinal changes in African American mothers’ parenting practices. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70(2), 319331. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2008.00484.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bryan, C. J., Tipton, E., & Yeager, D. S. (2021). Behavioural science is unlikely to change the world without a heterogeneity revolution. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(8), 980989. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01143-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, K. B., & Paysnick, A. A. (2012). Resilience in the transition to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 24(2), 493505. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000119 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrow, Y., Pajak, R., Specker, P., & Nickerson, A. (2020). Perceptions of mental health and perceived barriers to mental health help-seeking amongst refugees: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 75, 101812. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101812 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Ambler, A., Danese, A., Elliott, M. L., Hariri, A., Harrington, H. L., Hogan, S., Poulton, R., Ramrakha, S., Rasmussen, L. J. H., Reuben, A., Richmond-Rakerd, L., Sugden, K., Wertz, J., Williams, B. S., Moffitt, T. E. (2020). Longitudinal assessment of mental health disorders and comorbidities across 4 decades among participants in the Dunedin birth cohort study. JAMA Network Open, 3(4), e203221. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3221 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Causadias, J. M. (2013). A roadmap for the integration of culture into developmental psychopathology. Development & Psychopathology, 25(4pt2), 13751398. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579413000679 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Causadias, J. M. (2020). What is culture? Systems of people, places, and practices. Applied Developmental Science, 24(4), 310322. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2020.1789360 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Causadias, J. M., & Cicchetti, D. (2018). Cultural development and psychopathology. Development & Psychopathology, 30(5), 15491555. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418001220 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129830 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1990). A historical perspective on the discipline of developmental psychopathology. In Rolf, J. E., Masten, A. S., Cicchetti, D., Neuchterlein, K. H., & Weintraub, S. (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 228). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511752872.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1993). Developmental psychopathology: Reactions, reflections, projections. Developmental Review, 13(4), 471502. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1993.1021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2003). Experiments of nature: Contributions to developmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 15(4), 833835. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579403000397 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (2008). A multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective on research in development and psychopathology. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (pp. 2757). John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2023). A multiple levels of analysis developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence and young adulthood. In Crockett, L. J., Carlo, G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (Eds.), APA handbook of adolescent and young adult development (pp. 487503). https://doi.org/10.1037/0000298-030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Lynch, M. (1993). Toward an ecological/transactional model of community violence and child maltreatment: Consequences for children’s development. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 56(1), 96118. https://doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1993.11024624 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(4), 597600. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400007318 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(1-2), 1625. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01979.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Valentino, K. (2006). An ecological transactional perspective on child maltreatment: Failure of the average expectable environment and its influence upon child development. In Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk, disorder, and adaptation. vol. 3, (2nd ed. pp. 129201). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Collins, F. S., & Varmus, H. (2015). A new initiative on precision medicine. New England Journal of Medicine, 372(9), 793795. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp1500523 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, L. M., Murphy, S. A., & Stretcher, V. (2008). The multiphase optimization strategy (MOST) and the sequential multiple assignment randomized trial (SMART): New methods for more potent eHealth interventions. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 32, S112S118. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.amepre.2007.0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummings, E. M., & Valentino, K. (2015). Developmental psychopathology. In Overton, W. F., Molenaar, P. C. M., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Theory and method (pp. 566606). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy115 Google Scholar
Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of within-person and between-person effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 583619. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100356 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daar, A. S., Chang, T., Salomon, A., & Singer, P. A. (2018). Grand challenges in humanitarian aid. Nature, 559(7713), 169173. https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-018-05642-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deisenhofer, A.-K., Barkham, M., Beierl, E. T., Schwartz, B., Aafjes-van Doorn, K., Beevers, C. G., Berwian, I. M., Blackwell, S. E., Bockting, C. L., Brakemeier, E.-L., Brown, G., Buckman, J. E. J., Castonguay, L. G., Cusack, C. E., Dalgleish, T., de Jong, K., Delgadillo, J., DeRubeis, R. J., Driessen, E.,…, Cohen, Z. D. (2024). Implementing precision methods in personalizing psychological therapies: Barriers and possible ways forward. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 172, 104443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2023.104443 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demeusy, E. M., Handley, E. D., Manly, J. T., Sturm, R., & Toth, S. L. (2021). Building Healthy Children: A preventive intervention for high-risk young families. Development and Psychopathology, 33(2), 598613. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001625 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeRubeis, R. J. (2019). The history, current status, and possible future of precision mental health. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 123, 103506. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.103506 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, K. L., Roberts, J. D., Banacos, N., Neuberger, L., Koebele, E., Blanch-Hartigan, D., & Shanahan, E. A. (2021). Structural racism and the COVID-19 experience in the United States. Health Security, 19(S1), S14. https://doi.org/10.1089/hs.2021.0031 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunifon, R., Hynes, K., & Peters, H. E. (2006). Welfare reform and child well-being. Children and Youth Services Review, 28(11), 12731292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2006.01.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durak, M., Karakose, S., & Yow, W. Q. (2023). Late-life psychopathology. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1204202. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1204202 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edler, K., & Valentino, K. (in press). Parental self-regulation and engagement in emotion socialization: A systematic review. Psychological Bulletin. Advanced online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000423 Google Scholar
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., Koss, M. P., & Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 245258. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, A. J., Bosley, H. G., Fernandez, K. C., Reeves, J. W., Soyster, P. D., Diamond, A. E., & Barkin, J. (2019). Open trial of a personalized modular treatment for mood and anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 116, 6979. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.01.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, A. J., Medaglia, J. D., & Jeronimus, B. F. (2018). Lack of group-to-individual generalizability is a threat to human subjects research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(27), E6106E6115. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1711978115 Google ScholarPubMed
Franklin, J. C., Jamieson, J. P., Glenn, C. R., & Nock, M. K. (2015). How developmental psychopathology theory and research can inform the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) project. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44(2), 280290. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2013.873981 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García Coll, C., Akerman, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). Cultural influences on developmental processes and outcomes: Implications for the study of development and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 333356. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400003059 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García Coll, C., Crnic, K., Lamberty, G., & Wasik, B. H. (1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Development, 67(5), 18911914. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131600 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaylord-Harden, N. K., Burrow, A. L., & Cunningham, J. A. (2012). A cultural-asset framework for investigating successful adaptation to stress in African American youth. Child Development Perspectives, 6(3), 264271. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00236.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gee, D. G., & Cohodes, E. M. (2021). Influences of caregiving on development: A sensitive period for biological embedding of predictability and safety cues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30(5), 376383. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211015673 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
George, M. J., Russell, M. A., & Odgers, C. L. (2017). Using mobile technologies to advance the study of psychopathology among children and adolescents. In Centifanti, L. C., & Williams, D. M. (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of developmental psychopathology (pp. 4560). Wiley Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118554470.ch3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gettler, L. T., McDade, T. W., Feranil, A. B., & Kuzawa, C. W. (2011). Longitudinal evidence that fatherhood decreases testosterone in human males. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 108(39), 1619416199. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1105403108 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greve, W., & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Resilience in later adulthood and old age: Resources and potentials for successful aging. In Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, A. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (2nd ed. pp. 796840).Google Scholar
Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. P. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20(1), 102116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrell, S. P. (2000). A multidimensional conceptualization of racism-related stress: Implications for the well-being of people of color. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 70(1), 4257. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0087722 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinshaw, S. P., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). Stigma and mental disorder: Conceptions of illness, public attitudes, personal disclosure, and social policy. Development and Psychopathology, 12(4), 555598. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400004028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoekzema, E., Barba-Müller, E., Pozzobon, C., Picado, M., Lucco, F., García-García, D., & Vilarroya, O. (2017). Pregnancy leads to long-lasting changes in human brain structure. Nature Neuroscience, 20(2), 287296. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4458 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howland, M. A. (2023). Recalibration of the stress response system over adult development: Is there a perinatal recalibration period? Development and Psychopathology, 123. Advanced online publication. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000998 Google Scholar
Hughes, D., Rodriguez, J., Smith, E. P., Johnson, D. J., Stevenson, H. C., & Spicer, P. (2006). Parents' ethnic-racial socialization practices: A review of research and directions for future study. Developmental Psychology, 42(5), 747770. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.747 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., Sanislow, C., & Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748751. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, J. M. (1972). Prejudice and racism. Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Jones, S. C. T., Simon, C. B., Yadeta, K., Patterson, A., & Anderson, R. E. (2023). When resilience is not enough: Imagining novel approaches to supporting Black youth navigating racism. Development and Psychopathology, 19. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000986 Google ScholarPubMed
Kabali, H. K., Irigoyen, M. M., Nunez-Davis, R., Budacki, J. G., Mohanty, S. H., Leister, K. P., & Bonner, R. L. (2015). Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics, 136(6), 10441050. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-2151 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, T., Volkmann, C., Volkmann, A., Karyotaki, E., Cuijpers, P., & Brakemeier, E.-L. (2022). Heterogeneity of treatment effects in trials on psychotherapy of depression. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 29(3), 294303. https://doi.org/10.1037/cps0000079 Google Scholar
Kazdin, A. E. (2016). Single-case experimental research designs. In Kazdin, A. E. (Eds.), Methodological issues and strategies in clinical research (pp. 459483). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14805-029 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, P., Strathearn, L., & Swain, J. E. (2016). The maternal brain and its plasticity in humans. Hormones and Behavior, 77, 113123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, L. S., Querdasi, F. R., Humphreys, K. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2021). Dimensions of the language environment in infancy and symptoms of psychopathology in toddlerhood. Developmental Science, 24(5), e13082. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13082 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitayama, S., Berg, M. K., & Chopik, W. J. (2020). Culture and well-being in late adulthood: Theory and evidence. American Psychologist, 75(4), 567576. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000614 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Achenbach, T. M., Althoff, R. R., Bagby, R. M., Brown, T. A., Carpenter, W. T., Caspi, A., Clark, L. A., Eaton, N. R., Forbes, M. K., Forbush, K. T., Goldberg, D., Hasin, D., Hyman, S. E., Ivanova, M. Y., Lynam, D. R., Markon, K.,… , Zimmerman, M. (2017). The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(4), 454477. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000258 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laursen, B., & Hoff, E. (2006). Person-centered and variable-centered approaches to longitudinal data. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52(3), 377389. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2006.0029 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavori, P. W., & Dawson, R. (2004). Dynamic treatment regimes: Practical design considerations. Clinical Trials, 1(1), 920. https://doi.org/10.1191/1740774504cn002o CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenzo-Luaces, L., Peipert, A., De Jesus Romero, R., Rutter, L. A., & Rodriguez-Quintana, N. (2021). Personalized medicine and cognitive behavioral therapies for depression: Small effects, big problems, and bigger data. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 14(1), 5985. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41811-020-00094-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491495. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., & Narayan, A. J. (2012). Child development in the context of disaster, war, and terrorism: Pathways of risk and resilience. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 227257. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100356 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCool, J., Dobson, R., Whittaker, R., & Paton, C. (2022). Mobile health (mHealth) in low-and middle-income countries. Annual Review of Public Health, 43(1), 525539. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-052620-093850 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metzger, I. W., Anderson, R. E., Are, F., & Ritchwood, T. (2021). Healing interpersonal and racial trauma: Integrating racial socialization into trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy for African American youth. Child Maltreatment, 26(1), 1727. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559520921457 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molenaar, P. C. M., & Campbell, C. G. (2009). The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 112117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01619.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulder, R., Murray, G., & Rucklidge, J. (2017). Common versus specific factors in psychotherapy: Opening the black box. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(12), 953962. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30100-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, S. A. (2005). An experimental design for the development of adaptive treatment strategies. Statistics in Medicine, 24(10), 14551481. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.2022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murry, V. M. B., Gonzalez, C. M., Hanebutt, R. A., Bulgin, D., Coates, E. E., Inniss-Thompson, M. N., Debreaux, M. L., Wilson, W. E., Abel, D., & Cortez, M. K. B. (2022). Longitudinal study of the cascading effects of racial discrimination on parenting and adjustment among African American youth. Attachment & Human Development, 24(3), 322338. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2021.1976926 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navarro, J. L., & Tudge, J. R. (2023). Technologizing Bronfenbrenner: Neo-ecological theory. Current Psychology, 42(22), 1933819354. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-02738-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ng, M. Y., & Weisz, J. R. (2016). Annual Research Review: Building a science of personalized intervention for youth mental health. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(3), 216236. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12470 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nickerson, A., Bryant, R. A., Silove, D., & Steel, Z. (2011). A critical review of psychological treatments of posttraumatic stress disorder in refugees. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(3), 399417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.10.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nye, A., Delgadillo, J., & Barkham, M. (2023). Efficacy of personalized psychological interventions: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 91(7), 389397. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000820 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odea, S. S. (2020). Global unique mobile subscribers from 2010-2025, by region (millions). Statista. https://www.statista.com/statistics/740154/worldwide-unique-mobile-subscribers-by-region/ (accessed 12 January 2024).Google Scholar
Office of the Surgeon General (OSG) (2021). Protecting youth mental health: The U.S. Surgeon General’s advisory. US Department of Health and Human Services. https://www.hhs.gov/surgeongeneral/priorities/youth-mental-health/index.html Google Scholar
Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., & Boker, S. M. (2009). Resilience comes of age: Defining features in later adulthood. Journal of Personality, 77(6), 17771804. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00600.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradies, Y., Ben, J., Denson, N., Elias, A., Priest, N., Pieterse, A., Gupta, A., Kelaher, M., & Gee, G. (2015). Racism as a determinant of health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PloS One, 10(9), e0138511. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138511 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, H. A., Sandler, M., Manly, J. T., & Valentine, L. (2013). Building healthy children: Evidence-based home visitation integrated with pediatric medical homes. Pediatrics, 132(Supplement_2), S174S179. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-1021R CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, I. J. K., Wang, L., Williams, D. R., & Alegría, M. (2018). Coping with racism: Moderators of the discrimination-adjustment link among Mexican-origin adolescents. Child Development, 89(3), e293e310. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12856 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paschall, K. W., & Mastergeorge, A. M. (2016). A review of 25 years of research in bidirectionality in parent-child relationships: An examination of methodological approaches. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(5), 442451. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415607379 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascoe, E. A., & Smart Richman, L. (2009). Perceived discrimination and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 531554. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016059 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perou, R., Bitsko, R. H., Blumberg, S. J., Pastor, P., Ghandour, R. M., Gfroerer, J. C., Hedden, S. L., Crosby, A. E., Visser, S. N., Schieve, L. A., Parks, S. E., Hall, J. E., Brody, D., Simile, C. M., Thompson, W. W., Baio, J., Avenevoli, S., Kogan, M. D., & Huang, L. N., (2013). Mental health surveillance among children -- United States, 2005-2011. MMWR. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report Supplements, 62(2), 135.Google ScholarPubMed
Peverill, M., Dirks, M. A., Narvaja, T., Herts, K. L., Comer, J. S., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2021). Socioeconomic status and child psychopathology in the United States: A meta-analysis of population-based studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 83, 101933. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101933 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Racine, N., McArthur, B. A., Cooke, J. E., Eirich, R., Zhu, J., & Madigan, S. (2021). Global prevalence of depressive and anxiety symptoms in children and adolescents during COVID-19: A meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(11), 11421150. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.2482 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roisman, G. I., Masten, A. S., Coatsworth, J. D., & Tellegen, A. (2004). Salient and emerging developmental tasks in the transition to adulthood. Child Development, 75(1), 123133. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00658.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothermund, K., Englert, C., & Gerstorf, D. (2023). Explaining variation in individual aging, its sources, and consequences: A comprehensive conceptual model of human aging. Gerontologia, 69(12), 14371447. https://doi.org/10.1159/000534324 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salo, V. C., Pannuto, P., Hedgecock, W., Biri, A., Russo, D. A., Piersiak, H. A., & Humphreys, K. L. (2021). Measuring naturalistic proximity as a window into caregiver-child interaction patterns. Behavior Research Methods, 54(4), 15801594. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01681-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sameroff, A. J., & Emde, R. N. Eds (1989). Relationship disturbances in early childhood: A developmental approach. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Saxbe, D., Martínez-Garcia, M., Cardenas, S. I., Waizman, Y., & Carmona, S. (2023). Changes in left hippocampal volume in first-time fathers: Associations with oxytocin, testosterone, and adaptation to parenthood. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 35(7), e13270. https://doi.org/10.1111/jne.13270 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxbe, D., Rossin-Slater, M., & Goldenberg, D. (2018). The transition to parenthood as a critical window for adult health. American Psychologist, 73(9), 11901200. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000376 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schork, N. J. (2015). Personalized medicine: Time for one-person trials. Nature, 520(7549), 609611. https://doi.org/10.1038/520609a CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sisk, L. M., & Gee, D. G. (2022). Stress and adolescence: Vulnerability and opportunity during a sensitive window of development. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 286292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.10.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spencer, M. B. (2006). Phenomenology and ecological systems theory: Development of diverse groups. In Damon, W., & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development. vol. 1, (6th ed. pp. 829893). Wiley.Google Scholar
Spencer, R. A., Livingston, M. D., Komro, K. A., Sroczynski, N., Rentmeester, S. T., & Woods-Jaeger, B. (2021). Association between Temporary Assistance for Needy Families (TANF) and child maltreatment among a cohort of fragile families. Child Abuse & Neglect, 120, 105186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2021.105186 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 1729. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129832 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A. (1979). The coherence of individual development: Early care, attachment, and subsequent developmental issues. American Psychologist, 34(10), 834841. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.834 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1990). Considering normal and abnormal together: The essence of developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 2(4), 335347. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400005769 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stroebe, M., Schut, H., & Stroebe, W. (2007). Health outcomes of bereavement. The Lancet, 370(9603), 19601973. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61816-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplee, L. H., & Duggan, A. (2019). Innovative research methods to advance precision in home visiting for more efficient and effective programs. Child Development Perspectives, 13(3), 173179. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12334 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tackett, J. L., & Hallquist, M. (2022). The need to grow: Developmental considerations and challenges for modern psychiatric taxonomies. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 131(6), 660663. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000751 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teo, R. H., Cheng, W. H., Cheng, L. J., Lau, Y., & Lau, S. T. (2023). Global prevalence of social isolation among community-dwelling older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 107, 104904. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2022.104904 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valentino, K., & Edler, K. (in press). Atypical development and developmental psychopathology. In Bornstein, M. H., & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook..Google Scholar
Wang, L.(P), & Maxwell, S. E. (2015). On disaggregating between-person and within-person effects with longitudinal data using multilevel models. Psychological Methods, 20(1), 6383. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000030 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, D. G., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Cikara, M., Barch, D. M., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2023). State-level macro-economic factors moderate the association of low income with brain structure and mental health in US children. Nature Communications, 14(1), 2085. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-37778-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, D. R., Lawrence, J. A., & Davis, B. A. (2019). Racism and health: Evidence and needed research. Annual Review of Public Health, 40(1), 105125. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-040218-043750 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D. R., & Williams-Morris, R. (2000). Racism and mental health: The African American experience. Ethnicity & Health, 5(3-4), 243268. https://doi.org/10.1080/713667453 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, A. G. C., & Woods, W. C. (2020). Personalized models of psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 4974. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-102419-125032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yon, Y., Mikton, C. R., Gassoumis, Z. D., & Wilber, K. H. (2017). Elder abuse prevalence in community settings: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Global Health, 5(2), e147e156. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2214-109X(17)30006-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zilcha-Mano, S. (2021). Toward personalized psychotherapy: The importance of the trait-like/state-like distinction for understanding therapeutic change. American Psychologist, 76(3), 516528. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000629 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed