Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:18:16.111Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Different measurements of bilingualism and their effect on performance on a Simon task

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 December 2020

Marie-France Champoux-Larsson*
Affiliation:
Mid Sweden University
Alexandra S. Dylman
Affiliation:
Stockholm University
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We investigated how operationalizing bilingualism affects the results on a Simon task in a population of monolingual and bilingual native English speakers (N = 166). Bilingualism was measured in different ways within participants, and the measurements were used both as dichotomous and continuous variables. Our results show that the statistical significance and effect size varied across operationalizations. Specifically, the Composite Factor Score (the Language and Social Background Questionnaire’s general score), showed a bilingual disadvantage on reaction times regardless of how it was used (dichotomously or continuously). When dividing participants into monolinguals and bilinguals based on the Nonnative Language Social Use score (a Language and Social Background Questionnaire subscore), differences in accuracy and reaction times were found between the groups, but the Nonnative Language Social Use score did not predict accuracy when used as a continuous variable (only reaction times). Finally, earlier age of acquisition predicted faster reaction times, but only when used on a continuum. Effect sizes were between the small and medium range. No differences on the Simon effect were found. Our results call for cautiousness when comparing studies using different types of measurements, highlight the need for clarity and transparency when describing samples, and stresses the need for more research on the operationalization of bilingualism.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

In recent years, there has been a lively debate regarding the so-called bilingual advantage in executive functions. It has been suggested that bilingualism causes structural and functional changes in the brain due to the constant control that bilinguals must have on their languages, which, because they are activated simultaneously (e.g., Marian & Spivey, Reference Marian and Spivey2003; Wu & Thierry, Reference Wu and Thierry2010), constantly compete for selection (e.g., Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2015, Reference Bialystok2017; Bialystok, Craik, & Luk, Reference Bialystok, Craik and Luk2012; Li, Legault, & Litcofsky, Reference Li, Legault and Litcofsky2014). The bilingual’s task to continuously monitor the environment, choose the appropriate language, and inhibit the other, is suggested to lead to domain-general changes in the brain that extend beyond linguistic control (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2009, Reference Bialystok2015, Reference Bialystok2017, Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok, Craik and Luk2012; Li et al., Reference Li, Legault and Litcofsky2014). Therefore, the rationale is that bilinguals should show an advantage in nonlinguistic tasks recruiting the same executive functions that are used when they control their languages (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2009, Reference Bialystok2015, Reference Bialystok2017; Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok, Craik and Luk2012). While there is a large body of research that has found such a bilingual advantage in children (e.g., Blom et al., Reference Blom, Boerma, Bosma, Cornips and Everaert2017; Martin-Rhee & Bialystok, Reference Martin-Rhee and Bialystok2008, see Barac, Bialstok, Castro, & Sachez, 2014, for a review) and adults (e.g., Bialystok, Craik, Klein, & Viswanathan, Reference Bialystok, Craik, Klein and Viswanathan2004; Costa, Hernández, & Sebastián-Gallés, Reference Costa, Hernández and Sebastián-Gallés2008; Damian, Ye, Oh, & Yang, Reference Damian, Ye, Oh and Yang2018, see Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok, Craik and Luk2012, for a review), several studies have failed to replicate said results (e.g., Costa, Hernández, Costa-Faidella, & Sebastián-Gallés Reference Costa, Hernández, Costa-Faidella and Sebastián-Gallés2009; Namazi & Thordardottir, Reference Namazi and Thordardottir2010; Paap, Johnson, & Sawi, Reference Paap, Johnson and Sawi2014; Paap, Anders-Jefferson, Mason, Alvarado, & Zimiga, Reference Paap, Anders-Jefferson, Mason, Alvarado and Zimiga2018) or have even found a bilingual disadvantage in certain executive functions (e.g., Folke et al., Reference Folke, Ouzia, Bright, De Martino and Filippi2016; Paap & Greenberg, Reference Paap and Greenberg2013; Paap & Sawi, Reference Paap and Sawi2014; Paap et al., Reference Paap, Myuz, Anders, Bockelman, Mikulinsky and Sawi2017; Papageorgiou, Bright, Tomas, & Filippi Reference Papageorgiou, Bright, Tomas and Filippi2018). The quest to understanding which mechanisms could lay behind a potential bilingual advantage, and why it cannot always be found, has led to numerous investigations tackling the issue from different angles (see, for instance, Lehtonen et al., Reference Lehtonen, Soveri, Laine, Järvenpää, de Bruin and Antfolk2018, for a meta-analysis). Despite the extensive number of studies investigating various aspects of executive functioning and potential differences between monolinguals and bilinguals, for instance at the neurological level (see Grundy, Anderson, & Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2017, for a review), the debate concerning the existence (and true nature) of the bilingual advantage in executive functions remains, leaving us to wonder why no clear conclusion can be found.

However, when looking closely at studies where the executive functions of bilinguals are investigated, it becomes clear that the way bilingualism itself is defined and measured varies greatly between different studies (de Bruin, Reference de Bruin2019). In a study by Surrain and Luk, (Reference Surrain and Luk2019), where the different labels used to describe bilinguals in the scientific literature in recent years are reviewed, it is clear not only that different studies use different labels or characteristics to describe their bilingual sample but also that those labels are operationalized and measured differently as well. Not only does the specific facet of bilingualism that is used vary greatly across studies, but the clarity with which the operationalization of bilingualism is described and the extent to which it is measured vary as well (Surrain & Luk, Reference Surrain and Luk2019). Clearly, then, there is an enormous lack of consistency in the definition, operationalization, and measurement of bilingualism across studies, which makes the comparison of different results from independent studies problematic, and even impossible in some cases.

This variation may not be surprising considering the complexity of the concept of bilingualism. Important aspects such as proficiency, home versus society language use, language history, and the specific sociolinguistic context (Surrain & Luk, Reference Surrain and Luk2019) all are relevant facets of bilingualism that create variability even within bilinguals. For instance, there is a large body of research showing that the type and frequency of code switching in particular (see, for instance, the dynamic restructuring model, Pliatsikas, Reference Pliatsikas2020; and the adaptive control hypothesis, Abutalebi & Green, Reference Abutalebi and Green2016) lead to functional and structural neurocognitive variations within a given group of bilinguals. Because there are neurological differences between bilingual individuals depending on how they use their languages, it cannot be assumed that all types of bilingualism will potentially affect executive functions in the same way. De Bruin (Reference de Bruin2019) showed in a review that performance on tasks tapping into executive functions can vary greatly between different types of bilinguals depending on how they are defined, and how their bilingualism is measured. Bilingualism is not a homogenous concept and differences within bilinguals is an important issue that impedes the direct comparison of results across studies. In addition, if the central characteristic of the participants is operationalized in disparate ways without knowing whether these are equivalent and can be used interchangeably, it could be methodologically problematic to compare different “types” of bilinguals with each other. This has led to an appeal to both define and operationalize bilingualism more rigorously in bilingualism research (e.g., de Bruin, Reference de Bruin2019; Poarch & Krott, Reference Poarch and Krott2019; Surrain & Luk, Reference Surrain and Luk2019).

This methodological issue (of whether different operationalizations of bilingualism are comparable with each other), may be a contributing factor to the contradictory results found in bilingualism research. For instance, in a study by Gathercole et al. (Reference Gathercole, Thomas, Jones, Guasch, Young and Hughes2010), a bilingual advantage was found in elementary school children on a Stroop task. There, participants were classified as bilingual or monolingual based on an extensive background questionnaire where home and school language use was measured. However, in a different study, the participants (also elementary school children) were categorized as bilinguals based on exposure to and proficiency in Spanish and Basque (Duñabeitia et al., Reference Duñabeitia, Hernández, Antón, Macizo, Estévez, Fuentes and Carreiras2014). Duñabeitia et al. did not find that bilingual participants performed better on the Stroop task than the monolingual children. Nevertheless, whether these disparate findings are the result of the different ways bilingualism was defined and measured is unclear.

To make matters even more complex, bilingualism is often measured as a categorical variable. However, few people are either monolingual or bilingual, but rather, most fall somewhere on a range in between the two extremes. Thus, it is increasingly argued that bilingualism should be operationalized as a continuous variable in order to better reflect its true nature and thus increase the precision and sensitivity of the operationalization (e.g., Champoux-Larsson & Dylman, Reference Champoux-Larsson and Dylman2019; DeLuca, Rothman, Bialystok, & Pliatsikas, Reference DeLuca, Rothman, Bialystok and Pliatsikas2019; Edwards, Reference Edwards, Bhatia and Ritchie2012; Gullifer et al., Reference Gullifer, Chai, Whitford, Pivneva, Baum, Klein and Titone2018; Gullifer & Titone, Reference Gullifer and Titone2020; Incera & McLennan, Reference Incera and McLennan2018; Jylkkä et al., Reference Stoet2017; Kaushanskaya & Prior, Reference Kaushanskaya and Prior2015; Luk & Bialystok, Reference Luk and Bialystok2013; Sulpizio, Del Maschio, Del Mauro, Fedeli, & Abutalebi, Reference Sulpizio, Del Maschio, Del Mauro, Fedeli and Abutalebi2020; Surrain & Luk, Reference Surrain and Luk2019, but see Kremin & Byers-Heinlein, Reference Kremin and Byers-Heinlein2020, for a suggestion on how to use both categorical and continuous approaches simultaneously based on a factor mixture model and on a grade-of-membership model). Yet, there are no systematic investigations to determine whether this can directly affect the outcomes of studies.

Therefore, in order to take the first step in systematically investigating the effect of defining and measuring bilingualism based on different factors, we conducted a study where we operationalized bilingualism in two different ways (i.e., based on different characteristics of bilingualism, both dichotomously and continuously) and investigated whether these different operationalizations would affect the results on a Simon task within the same participants. Because our aim was to illustrate that different types of operationalization can lead to different results, but not to systematically investigate all possible operationalizations of bilingualism, we chose to use the Language and Social Background Questionnaire (LSBQ; Anderson, Mak, Chahi, & Bialystok, Reference Anderson, Mak, Chahi and Bialystok2018) to measure bilingualism. The LSBQ is a comprehensive and validated tool that suited the purpose of this study well as it measures several aspects of bilingualism (including proficiency, language use in different contexts, and code switching) and through which bilingualism can be operationalized categorically or on a continuum. The LSBQ has also the advantage of providing different composite scores based on various aspects of bilingualism that cluster together, and where each item is weighted according to its relative contribution to the concept. Of particular interest here is the Composite Factor Score, an extensive and comprehensive estimate of bilingualism that comprises all the aspects of language use, including proficiency, that are covered in the LSBQ. Two other composite scores of interest can be found in the LSBQ, namely, the Nonnative Home Use and Proficiency score and the Nonnative Language Social Use score. As both language use at home and in society are two important aspects of bilingualism (Surrain & Luk, Reference Surrain and Luk2019), these two composite scores are highly relevant when operationalizing bilingualism. Although both proficiency and code switching are included in at least one of the composite scores from the LSBQ, code switching is particularly interesting on its own due to the evidence that it is associated with structural and functional neurological differences not only between monolinguals and bilinguals but also across different bilinguals (e.g., Abutalebi & Green, Reference Abutalebi and Green2016; Pliatsikas, Reference Pliatsikas2020), and we therefore chose to investigate it separately. However, an aspect of bilingualism that is relatively often used in research but that is not included in the composite scores of the LSBQ is age of acquisition. Because of its frequency in the bilingual literature, this is also an aspect that we chose to investigate on its own.

As for the Simon task, it is a paradigm that has been used in several studies where the executive functions of bilinguals were investigated (e.g., Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok, Craik, Klein and Viswanathan2004, Reference Bialystok, Craik, Grady, Chau, Ishii, Gunji and Pantev2005; Paap et al., Reference Paap, Johnson and Sawi2014). The Simon task consists of stimuli of two different types (e.g., a square or a circle, or two different letters) shown on the left or the right side of a screen, where the participant’s task is to determine which type of stimulus is shown by pressing a key on the right or the left of the keyboard. Of importance, some trials are spatially congruent (e.g., the answer key for a circle is placed on the left side of the keyboard and the circle is shown on the left side of the screen), while other trials are spatially incongruent (e.g., the circle is shown on the right side of the screen). Incongruent trials usually lead to slower response times and more mistakes than congruent trials. The Simon effect is thus the difference in accuracy or reaction time between congruent and incongruent trials. While a bilingual advantage has been found in some studies, meaning that bilinguals showed a reduced Simon effect (e.g., Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok, Craik, Klein and Viswanathan2004, Reference Bialystok, Craik, Grady, Chau, Ishii, Gunji and Pantev2005), this advantage is not always replicated (e.g., Paap et al., Reference Paap, Johnson and Sawi2014). However, note that the terms advantage and disadvantage, even though they are used extensively in the bilingual-advantage debate may be misleading terms. For instance, while slower reaction times are often seen as a disadvantage, they may simply reflect adaptive cognitive mechanisms that can lead to equally good performance overall compared to a group that is faster (e.g., Gullifer & Titone, in press).

Thus, with this study, we aimed to investigate two main questions. First, we investigated whether the definition of bilingualism based on different factors led to different results on the Simon task in terms of accuracy, reaction times, and the Simon effect (measured as the difference between congruent and incongruent trials). Second, we investigated whether the operationalization of bilingualism as a dichotomous versus as a continuous variable affected the interpretation of the results on a Simon task also in terms of accuracy, reaction times, and the Simon effect.

Method

Participants

A total of 166 participants (M age = 37.5, SD age = 10.4; 50% males) were recruited online via Prolific and MechanicalTurk. However, 8 participants failed to fill out the survey properly, making it impossible to evaluate their language profile accurately, and were therefore excluded. The final sample consisted of 158 participants (M age = 37.4, SD age = 10.5; 49.4% males). The participants’ highest completed educational level (1 = elementary school or lower, 2 = high school, 3 = professional education, 4 = bachelor’s degree, 5 = master’s degree, 6 = PhD) was used as an indicator of their socioeconomic status (Mode = 4, n = 79). Forty-two participants reported speaking only English (M age = 38.3, SD age = 9.1; 57.1% males; Modeeducation = 4, n = 17). The remaining 116 participants (M age = 37.1, SD age = 11; 46.6% males; Modeeducation = 4, n = 62) reported having English as their first language, and a variety of second languages. The mean reported frequency of use of the most proficient second language on average for speaking, writing, listening, and reading (each on a scale from 0 to 4)was 1.29 (SD = 0.89). For a list of the reported second, third, fourth, and fifth languages, please see Table 1. See the sections below for a detailed description of the sample’s language profile based on the different measurements.

Table 1. Reported second, third, fourth, and fifth languages

Procedure

Members of Prolific or MechanicalTurk self-enrolled to the study in exchange for monetary compensation (1.70 GBP and 1.25 USD respectively). The Simon task was programmed and presented online via PsyToolkit (Stoet, Reference Stoet2010, Reference Stoet2017). In our version of the task, the letters Q and P were used as stimuli and presented on the left or the right side of the screen for 2000 ms or until an answer was provided. After 12 practice trials, a total of 48 trials were presented, of which half were spatially congruent, and half were spatially incongruent (equally distributed across the two stimuli). Participants were instructed to answer as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing the letter P or Q on the keyboard. A fixation cross appeared for 500 ms between each trial as soon as an answer was provided, or after 2000 ms if no answer had been provided. The task was programmed to detect the type of device used by the participants and only participants using a physical keyboard could complete the experiment.

After the Simon task was completed, participants were sent to an online survey where information about their gender, age, and highest completed level of education was collected. Furthermore, in order to measure the participants language profile, the LSBQ (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Mak, Chahi and Bialystok2018) was used. The questionnaire was digitalized and presented in Qualtrics. The LSBQ is a comprehensive and validated instrument that allows measuring different facets of bilingualism and provides different types of operationalizations. It consists of three parts, the first one covering demographic and other background information (Part A), the second one covering language background (Part B), and the third part concerning community language use behavior (Part C). In this study, we replaced Part A with our own demographic questions (i.e., gender, age, and education) and used Part B and Part C in their entirety. More specifically, Part B consisted of questions on which language(s) the participant speaks and understands (including English), in which context(s) each language had been acquired, and at what age. Proficiency questions for English followed for speaking, understanding, reading, and writing on a scale from 0 to 100, as well as frequency of use of English for speaking, understanding, reading, and writing on a scale from 0 to 4 (0 = none of the time, 1 = a little of the time, 2 = some of the time, 3 = most of the time, and 4 = all of the time). The proficiency and frequency of use questions were presented again, but for the participant’s second language (or, in cases where several second languages were reported, the one that was the most fluent). Participants who only spoke English could skip those questions. As for Part C, questions about language use were first answered on a scale from 0 to 4 (0 = only English, 1 = mostly English, 2 = both languages equally, 3 = mostly the second language, and 4 = only the second language). Those questions covered which language(s) were heard during different life periods (infancy, preschool age, primary school age, and high school age), which language(s) were used to communicate with different people (parents, siblings, grandparents, other relatives, partner, roommates/other people the person lives with, neighbors, and friends), which language(s) were used in different settings (at home, school, work, social activities, religious activities, hobbies, shopping and other commercial activities, healthcare, or contact with various authorities), and which language(s) were used for various activities (reading, e-mailing, texting, on social media, for writing lists and notes, for watching TV and listening to the radio, watching movies, surfing on the internet, or praying). Finally, a last block of question asked the participant how often code switching occurred on a scale from 0 to 4 (0 = never, 1 = rarely, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often, and 4 = always) with family members, friends, and on social media respectively.

Out of Part B and Part C, different scores can be calculated by using the provided LSBQ Factor Score Calculator (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Mak, Chahi and Bialystok2018). We chose the LSBQ in particular as one of its factors is designed specifically to be used either as a continuous variable or as a dichotomous variable, thus suiting the purpose of this study perfectly. Namely, the Composite Factor Score (CFS) is a measurement that includes all questions that are weighted according to the validation in Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Mak, Chahi and Bialystok2018). Another factor that can be computed is the Nonnative Home Use and Proficiency score (HUP), which includes a subset of questions related to second language use and proficiency only (for instance language used with grandparents, during infancy, proficiency in second language, etc.). Furthermore, another factor, the Nonnative Language Social Use score (LSU) includes a subset of questions related to second language use in the participant’s social life (e.g., at work, when writing e-mails, frequency of code switching with friends, etc.). A last factor, namely, English proficiency, can be computed, but was not used in this study.

Data preparation

For this study, the three scores from the LSBQ described above were used. More specifically, the CFS (possible range: –6.582 to 32.32, the higher the score, the more bilingual), the HUP (possible range: –13.9 to 24.163, the higher the score, the more proficient the second language, and the more it is used in home settings), and the LSU (possible range: –7.5 to 80.304, the higher the score the more frequently the second language is used in social settings). In addition, age of acquisition of the second language and code-switching frequency (CS, possible range: 0 to 4, based on the mean of three questions where answers ranged from never to always) were also used to operationalize the participants’ language profile since they are frequently used to operationalize bilingualism in bilingual research.

Of importance, all the variables were used to divide the participants into two separate groups, but they were also all used as continuous variables. For the CFS, the LSBQ’s guidelines were used to create a monolingual and bilingual group (monolinguals <–3.13: n = 60, bilinguals >1.23: n = 40, those with a score falling in between these thresholds were excluded: n = 58). As for the HUP and LSU score, a median split (Mdn HUP : –4.95; Mdn LSU : –3.91) was used to create a group of monolinguals (HUP: n = 79, LSU: n = 79) and of bilinguals (HUP: n = 79, LSU: n = 79) as it is a practice that is frequently used in research despite its limitations (MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher, & Rucker, Reference MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher and Rucker2002). Participants who reported knowing more than one language (n = 116) were divided into two groups based on the age of acquisition of their L2. Participants who began to acquire their L2 before the age of 5 years were categorized as early bilinguals (n = 40) and those who began acquiring their L2 from the age of 5 years (or older) were categorized as late bilinguals (n = 76). Age 5 was chosen as the cutoff age based on findings suggesting neurological differences between bilinguals acquiring their L2 before age 5 and those acquiring it afterwards (e.g., Berken, Chai, Chen, Gracco, & Klein Reference Berken, Chai, Chen, Gracco and Klein2016; Bloch et al., Reference Bloch, Kaiser, Kuenzli, Zappatore, Haller, Franceschini and Nitsch2009). Finally, for the participants who reported knowing more than one language, participants were divided into a group of nonswitchers for those who on average based on the three code-switching questions (with family, friends, on social media) reported code switching less frequently than “sometimes” (i.e., mean values below 2: n = 86) and between switchers (i.e., mean values of 2 and above: n = 30). For descriptive statistics for each independent variable when treated categorically and continuously, please see Table 2 and Table 3, respectively.

Table 2. Demographics based on language groups

Note: CFS, Composite Factor Score. HUP, Nonnative Home Use score. LSU. Nonnative Language Social Use score. AoA, age of acquisition of the second language. CS, frequency of code switching.

Table 3. Demographics based on continuous variables

Note: CFS, Composite Factor Score. HUP, Nonnative Home Use score. LSU, Nonnative Language Social Use score. AoA, age of acquisition of the second language. CS, frequency of code switching.

Both accuracy (number of correct answers) and reaction times (in milliseconds) were analyzed for congruent and incongruent trials. Furthermore, a Simon effect score was calculated for accuracy (difference between congruent and incongruent trials) and reaction times (difference between incongruent and congruent trials), where larger scores represent a larger Simon effect. Given that event-related potentials show that focusing spatial attention on a stimulus and preparing motor action occurs around the time period of the N2-wave (Luck, Reference Luck, Luck and Kappenman2012), answers that occurred within 200 ms were too quick for the participant to have had time to process the stimulus and were considered as mistakes. Trials where the participants did not answer within the time limit of 2000 ms were also considered as mistakes. Only correct answers were included in reaction time analysis.

The different dependent variables described above were tested in individual analyses with each predictor (CFS, HUP, LSU, age of acquisition, and frequency of code switching). Thus, for each independent variable, accuracy for congruent trials, accuracy for incongruent trials, reaction times for congruent trials, reaction times for incongruent trials, the Simon effect based on accuracy, and the Simon effect based on reaction times were analyzed. For the dichotomous independent variables (where two groups were created), mixed-model analyses of variance were performed for the accuracy measurements (congruent and incongruent trials), as well as for the reaction times (congruent and incongruent trials). For increased readability, the main effects of condition (congruent or incongruent), which were not the principal interest of this study, are reported in supplementary materials only. Furthermore, t tests for independent samples were performed for each of the Simon effect measurements (based on accuracy and based on reaction times). In addition, for the categorical variables, t tests were performed on age, Mann–Whitney U tests were performed on education level, and chi-square test for independence were performed on gender to control whether the groups differed on these variables. As for the continuous independent variables, simple linear regression analyses were conducted. The analyses were performed using JASP version 0.10.2. A summary of the means, standard deviations, p values and effect sizes for the different variables, groups, and conditions for all analyses is presented in Table 4 at the end of the Results section.

Table 4. Summary of results

Note: Acc., Accuracy. RT, reaction times (in ms). CFS, Composite Factor Score. HUP, Nonnative Home Use score. LSU, Nonnative Language Social Use score. AoA, age of acquisition of the second language. CS, frequency of code switching. Values in bold indicate a significant difference between the groups (dichotomous) or a significant (or approaching significance) predictor (continuous). * p < .05. ** p < .01. approaching significance.

Results

Dichotomous independent variables

CFS

The main effect of group for accuracy was not significant (monolinguals: M = 44.5, SD = 3.1; bilinguals: M = 44.9, SD = 3.3; F < 1), and neither was the interaction between group and condition (congruent, incongruent: F < 1). As for the reaction times, there was a significant difference between the groups where monolinguals (M = 449 ms, SD = 77) were faster than the bilinguals (M = 484 ms, SD = 90), F (1, 98) = 4.33, p = .04, η2 = .04. The interaction between group and condition was not significant for reaction times either (F < 1). None of the analyses for the Simon effect (accuracy or reaction times) were significant (both ts < 1).

As for background variables, the t test for age showed that the monolingual group (M = 40.1, SD = 11.4) was significantly older than the bilingual group (M = 33.8, SD = 8.8), t (98) = 2.98, p = .004, d = 0.61. In addition, although both groups had a median of 4 for education, the bilingual group (M = 4.2, SD = 0.99) had a higher level of education than the monolingual group (M = 3.5, SD = 0.89), U = 728, p < .001. The chi-square for gender was not significant, χ2 (1, n = 100) = 0.43, p = .51.

HUP

The main effect of group for accuracy was not significant (monolinguals: M = 44.8, SD = 2.8; bilinguals: M = 44.8, SD = 3.5; F < 1), and neither was the interaction between group and condition (congruent or incongruent: F < 1). Neither main effect of group for reaction times (monolinguals: M = 457 ms, SD = 75; bilinguals: M = 475, SD = 81; F < 1), nor the interaction between group and condition was significant (congruent or incongruent: F < 1). Further, none of the t tests for the Simon effect (accuracy or reaction times) were significant (both ts < 1).

As for background variables, the t test for age showed that the monolingual group (M = 39.8, SD = 10.7) was significantly older than the bilingual group (M = 35.1, SD = 9.8), t (156) = 2.9, p = .004, d = 0.46. In addition, although both groups had a median of 4 for education, the bilingual group (M = 4, SD = 1.1) had a significantly higher level of education than the monolingual group (M = 3.6, SD = 0.91), U = 2406, p = .008. The gender distribution was equal, χ2 (1, n = 158) = 0.41, p = .52.

LSU

The main effect of group for accuracy was significant, where bilinguals (M = 45.4, SD = 2.9) were more accurate than the monolinguals (M = 44.3, SD = 3.3), F (1, 156) = 4.96, p = .03, η2 = .03. The interaction between group and condition was not significant for accuracy (F < 1). As for reaction times, the model was significant, showing that the monolinguals (M = 446 ms, SD = 71) were faster than the bilinguals (M = 486 ms, SD = 80), F (1, 156) = 10.64, p = .001, η2 = .06. The interaction between group and condition was not significant for reaction times (F < 1). None of the t tests for the Simon effect (accuracy or reaction times) were significant: accuracy, t (156) = 1.51, p = .13, d = .24, reaction times: t < 1.

As for the background variables, the t test for age showed no significant difference between the monolingual group (M = 38.6, SD = 11.1) and the bilingual group (M = 36.3, SD = 9.9), t (156) = 1.4, p = .16, d = 0.22. In addition, while both groups had a median of 4 for education, the bilingual group (M = 4.1, SD = 0.98) had a higher level of education than the monolingual group (M = 3.5, SD = 0.97), U = 2,034, p < .001. The gender distribution between the groups was unequal, with 58.2% of the monolinguals being male (41.8% females), while 40.5% of the bilinguals were male (59.5% females), χ2 (1, n = 158) = 4.96, p = .03.

Age of acquisition

There was no main effect of group for accuracy (early: M = 44.55, SD = 3.7; late: M = 45.51, SD = 2.45), F (1, 114) = 2.79, p = .098, η2 = .02, and no significant interaction effect between group and condition, F (1, 114) = 2.26, p = .135, η2 = .01. As for reaction times, there were no differences between the groups (early: M = 470 ms, SD = 87; late: M = 475 ms, SD = 72; F < 1). The interaction between group and condition was not significant for reaction times either (F < 1). None of the t tests for the Simon effect (accuracy or reaction times) were significant: accuracy, t (114) = 1.5, p = .14, d = .29, reaction times: t < 1.

Here again, we took a closer look at the background variables. Age was significantly lower in the early bilinguals group (M = 32.3, SD = 8.1) than in the late bilinguals group (M = 39.6, SD = 11.6), t (114) = 3.56, p < .001, d = 0.7. Education level (both groups with a median of 4) and gender did not differ significantly.

Code switching

The main effect of group was not significant for accuracy (nonswitchers: M = 45.3, SD = 2.9; switchers: M = 44.9, SD = 3.1; F < 1) or for reaction times (nonswitchers: M = 465 ms, SD = 75; switchers: M = 484, SD = 81; F < 1). None of the interactions between group and condition for accuracy or reaction times were significant (both Fs < 1). None of the t tests for the Simon effect (accuracy or reaction times) were significant: accuracy, t < 1; reaction times, t (114) = 1.03, p = .306, d = 0.22. In addition, no significant differences were found for age (t < 1), education (both medians = 4), or gender.

Continuous independent variables

CFS

The model predicting reaction times on the congruent trials was significant, F (1, 156) = 4.62, p = .03, R 2 = .03, where the higher the CFS was, the longer the reaction times were. As for the model predicting reaction times on the incongruent trials, it approached significance, F (1, 156) = 3.43, p = .066, R 2 = .02, where the higher the CFS was, the longer the reaction times tended to be. The models for accuracy on the congruent and incongruent trials, as well as the models for the Simon effects (accuracy or reaction times) were not significant (all Fs < 1).

HUP

None of the models with the HUP score as predictor were significant: congruent reaction times, F (1, 156) = 2.13, p = .146, R 2 = .01; incongruent reaction times, F (1, 156) = 1.59, p = .209, R 2 = .01; all other Fs < 1.

LSU

The model for the reaction times for congruent trials was significant, F (1, 156) = 4.69, p = .032, R 2 = .03, but only approached significance for reaction times for incongruent trials, F (1, 156) = 3.47, p = .064, R 2 = .02. In both cases, the higher the LSU score, the longer the reaction times were. The models for accuracy for congruent and incongruent trials, as well as the models for the Simon effects (accuracy or reaction times) were all nonsignificant (all Fs < 1).

Age of acquisition

The model for reaction times for congruent trials was significant, F (1, 114) = 4.41, p = .04, R 2 = .04, and the model for reaction times for incongruent trials, F (1, 114) = 3.27, p = .073, R 2 = .03, approached significance, where a higher age of acquisition of a second language lead (or tended to lead) to slower reaction times on the Simon task. The models for both types of accuracy and for both types of Simon effects (accuracy and reaction times) were not significant (all Fs < 1).

Code switching

None of the models with frequency of code switching as the predictor were significant (all Fs < 1).

Discussion

In this study, we operationalized bilingualism in different ways and analyzed performance on a Simon task (where a bilingual advantage has at times been found in previous studies) based on various possible independent variables in order to investigate whether operationalizing bilingualism in different ways can affect the results. All predictors were used both as dichotomous variables and on a continuous scale in order to investigate whether dividing participants into distinct groups or treating bilingualism as a continuous variable would affect the results.

Our results showed that one of the predictors (the CFS), was almost constantly associated with slower reaction times for bilinguals. Namely, the more bilingual participants were according to the CFS, the slower they were on congruent trials on the Simon task, and there was a tendency toward a significant effect for the incongruent trials as well. When participants were divided into groups of monolinguals and bilinguals based on the CFS, the bilingual group was significantly slower.

Of interest, while a bilingual advantage on accuracy was found when the groups were based on the LSU score, this effect was not found when accuracy was predicted by the LSU scores on a continuous scale. Furthermore, when the LSU was used as a dichotomous variable, monolinguals were significantly faster than bilinguals for both congruent and incongruent trials. However, when LSU was treated as a continuous variable, slower reaction times were predicted by bilingualism for congruent trials only (the model only approached significance for the incongruent trials). This suggests that, in our sample, the more bilingual participants were based on the LSU score, the slower they responded to congruent trials. However, this effect was not found for incongruent trials.

Finally, while age of acquisition of the second language did not show effects on accuracy or reaction times when used dichotomously (early vs. late bilinguals), age of acquisition did significantly predict reaction times for congruent trials when bilingualism was measured on a continuum. Furthermore, the prediction of age of acquisition on incongruent trials approached significance. As for the HUP score and code switching, they did not predict any differences regardless of how they were used.

Markedly, a variation on the Simon effect was not predicted by any of the variables, whether it was measured based on accuracy or on reaction times. None of the effect sizes found in the significant results were impressively large, however, most of them being small or halfway toward medium in size. This implies that the results in this study should be interpreted with caution. It is also worth mentioning that, although we label slower reaction times as being a disadvantage, as we pointed out earlier, these slower reaction times may actually reflect a speed–accuracy trade-off as accuracy was higher for the bilinguals. It could be that the bilinguals use different cognitive strategies when performing the task. As Gullifer and Titone (in press) suggest, it could be that the bilinguals use active goal maintenance to a higher degree than monolinguals do when managing conflict, which could manifest itself behaviorally in terms of slower reaction times. This is not necessarily a disadvantage per say, however, especially when accuracy is improved.

Nonetheless, the results of this study demonstrate clearly that operationalization can have an effect on the results. For instance, while we found a bilingual advantage in accuracy and a disadvantage in speed when bilingualism was operationalized dichotomously for the LSU, the disadvantage only appeared for congruent (and not incongruent) trials when the LSU was used as a continuous variable. Even more interesting, while no effects whatsoever were detected when participants were divided into early and late bilinguals, an effect on reaction times for congruent trials was found when age of acquisition was used on a continuum. A possible explanation is that the effect of LSU was found for accuracy when groups were used only due to a Type I error, since dividing a continuous variable into categories increases the risk of a Type I error (Cohen, Reference Cohen1983). As for the effect of LSU on reaction times, it could also be that the effect was driven by the congruent trials only, although no significant interaction effects were found. In contrast, the advantage for bilinguals having acquired their second language earlier (when using age of acquisition as a continuum on congruent trials) disappeared completely when participants were divided into early and late bilinguals. This difference may reflect the more subtle and fine-grained differences that can be found when using a variable on a continuum. Dividing participants into groups based on a continuous variable may lead to as much, if not more, variability within the group as between the groups.

The measurement that was arguably the more consistent was the CFS, where no effects were found on accuracy regardless of whether the variable was used dichotomously or continuously, but where significant effects were found for reaction times regardless of how the variable was used. This consistency may be due to several reasons. A possibility is that the CFS covers several facets of bilingualism, thus making it a robust measurement including many of the aspects of bilingualism that are of importance for performance on a Simon task. In contrast, measurements such as the LSU cover social aspects of language use only, regardless of the participant’s reported language skills. This could be a possible explanation as to why a more complete measurement such as the CFS yielded more stable results than, for instance, the LSU. Another possibility is that the groups that are created based on the recommendations of the LSBQ exclude participants in a “gray zone” between monolingualism and bilingualism, thus creating groups with clear boundaries and decreasing the variability within those groups. At the same time, when used continuously, the CFS includes the “gray zone” participants and the advantage of using the variable as a continuum to detect fine-grained effects is preserved.

Another interesting aspect of this study is that, when using a median split on the LSU and HUP (which is a practice that is not necessarily optimal but that is nonetheless used), arbitrary cutoff limits were created. Thus, a bilingual in our sample may have been a monolingual in a different sample since the median will inevitably vary across samples. Here, this became even clearer as some participants ended up being bilingual according to either the LSU or the HUP, but monolingual according to the other factor. Twenty-four participants were categorized as monolingual on the LSU and bilingual on the HUP, and an additional 24 participants were categorized as bilingual on the LSU and monolingual on the HUP. None of the LSU-monolinguals were categorized as bilinguals on the CFS (21 were excluded, 3 were monolinguals). However, 2 of the HUP-monolinguals were categorized as bilinguals according to the CFS (20 were excluded, 2 were monolinguals), which is clearly problematic. These discrepancies not only demonstrate the operationalization issue well and why creating categories based on arbitrary cutoffs such as a median split makes comparison across studies problematic but also illustrates the complexity of the bilingual experience when it is measured based on different facets, as well as how important each and every of these facets are.

An important note to make is that, according to the CFS values (computed based on the LSBQ), our sample had very few “true” bilinguals, namely, participants that had a score above the suggested cutoff value for bilingualism (i.e., several participants fell in between the monolingual and bilingual categories or in the “gray zone”). Even if the LSBQ is a validated tool and although the CFS was computed using the calculator provided by Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Mak, Chahi and Bialystok2018), the values in Anderson et al. are from a North American population and the questionnaire has been validated in this population only. It may be that the different items in the questionnaire would load differently on the factors identified by Anderson et al. if they were validated in another population. Although it is beyond the scope of this paper to validate the LSBQ in a different population, we cannot reject the possibility that the LSBQ did not measure the language profile of our population as accurately as for the population used in Anderson et al. However, a large portion of our sample was recruited via MechanicalTurk, where a majority of users are also in North America and shared several similarities with Anderson et al.’s sample. The variety of second languages spoken by our participants was similar, as was the level of education and the gender distribution, even though our sample was slightly older. Thus, it is probable that our sample is similar to the sample used in the LSBQ validation study. However, since the average CFS, LSU, and HUP are not provided in Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Mak, Chahi and Bialystok2018), it is difficult to compare the samples directly, and the possibility remains that our sample was significantly different in terms of bilingual experience.

In addition, our participants were recruited exclusively online via crowd sourcing platforms (MechanicalTurk and Prolific). Although samples recruited via such services appear to be as reliable as other more traditional samples and have the advantage of including participants outside of the college and university students population, it may be that a sample recruited in a different context would behave differently. Hauser and Norbert (Reference Hauser and Norbert2016) showed that MTurkers (participants recruited via MechanicalTurk) pay better attention than non-MTurkers when performing cognitive tests. This could explain at least in part why the accuracy rates in our study were so high. Furthermore, in particular because accuracy rates in our study were so high, the results should be interpreted with caution. The use of statistical analysis such as analysis of variance can be problematic when a ceiling effect is present and can lead to Type I errors (Šimkovic & Träuble, Reference Šimkovic and Träuble2019).

Furthermore, there were some demographic differences between the groups that were created, and we cannot rule out that they may have affected the significance, or nonsignificance, of the results. Namely, CFS- and HUP-based bilinguals were younger and reported higher levels of education, but responded slower (but more accurately) when grouped based on the CFS. Based on the LSU, monolinguals reported lower levels of education and were predominantly males. Thus, the better accuracy of the bilingual group could be an effect of level of education. As for early bilinguals, they were also significantly younger, but then again, no effect was found between the groups. Only the nonswitchers versus switchers did not differ when it came to age, education, and gender, and no differences were found in terms of performance between those groups.

However, we would like to stress that the primary purpose of the current study was not to add yet another data set to the already ongoing debate of the existence of a bilingual advantage in executive functions. Rather, the main purpose was to explore whether operationalizing bilingualism in different ways could lead to different patterns of results, which is what we found. However, although this study highlights the fact that the specific way bilingualism is operationalized can affect the results and thus the conclusions that are drawn, it is not comprehensive enough to solve this methodological concern. First, the participants of the current study consisted of adults, while the bilingual advantage in executive functions is more consistently found in children (e.g., Bialystok & Viswanathan, Reference Bialystok and Viswanathan2009; Janus & Bialystok, Reference Janus and Bialystok2018; Kovács, Reference Kovács2009; Yow et al., Reference Stoet2017) or elderly populations (e.g., Bialystok, Poarch, Luo, & Craik, Reference Bialystok, Poarch, Luo and Craik2014; Borsa et al., Reference Borsa, Perani, Della Rosa, Videsott, Guidi, Weekes and Abutalebi2018; Cox et al., Reference Cox, Bak, Allerhand, Redmond, Starr, Deary and MacPherson2016; Gold, Kim, Johnson, Kryscio, & Smith, Reference Gold, Kim, Johnson, Kryscio and Smith2013). Replicating the current results with additional age groups such as children or elderly populations might therefore lead to further insight in this matter, as will replicating the current study but incorporating additional tasks, particularly tasks that recruit other types of executive functions such as switch cost.

Second, there are other facets of bilingualism that we did not measure in this study. For instance, there are other questionnaires available for measuring bilingualism both for adults (e.g., the Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire; Marian, Blumfeld, & Kaushanskaya, Reference Marian, Blumenfeld and Kaushanskaya2007) and children (e.g., the Bilingual Language Experience Calculator; Unsworth, Reference Unsworth2013), and other facets such as L2 proficiency or frequency of use of the L2 that were not explored in the current study (although they are included as part of the different LSBQ scores). In addition, there are tests beyond self-reports that may more objectively measure knowledge of a language, or at least some aspects of it such as receptive vocabulary (e.g., LexTALE: Lemhöfer & Broersma, Reference Lemhöfer and Broersma2012; Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; Dunn, Reference Dunn2018). As we did not look at the complexity of the social aspect of bilingualism in this study, it should be more thoroughly investigated in future studies as there is growing evidence that the context of use of a bilingual’s languages may be a main factor affecting executive functions (de Bruin, Reference de Bruin2019; Tiv, Gulifer, Feng, & Titone, in press). It has even been suggested that the potential advantage may emerge from the specific social linguistic context that a bilingual interacts in (e.g., Fan, Liberman, Keysar, & Kinzler, Reference Fan, Liberman, Keysar and Kinzler2015; Hartanto & Yang, Reference Hartanto and Yang2016; Wu & Thierry, Reference Wu and Thierry2013) and investigating the social aspect of bilingualism in addition to linguistic aspects of bilingualism could shed light on the debate, and how to best operationalize bilingualism.

Third, although there is a movement toward treating bilingualism as a continuous variable that is endorsed by many (e.g., Champoux-Larsson & Dylman, Reference Champoux-Larsson and Dylman2019; DeLuca et al., Reference DeLuca, Rothman, Bialystok and Pliatsikas2019; Edwards, Reference Edwards, Bhatia and Ritchie2012; Gullifer et al., Reference Gullifer, Chai, Whitford, Pivneva, Baum, Klein and Titone2018; Gullifer & Titone, Reference Gullifer and Titone2020; Incera & McLennan, Reference Incera and McLennan2018; Jylkkä et al., Reference Stoet2017; Kaushanskaya & Prior, Reference Kaushanskaya and Prior2015; Luk & Bialystok, Reference Luk and Bialystok2013; Sulpizio et al., Reference Sulpizio, Del Maschio, Del Mauro, Fedeli and Abutalebi2020; Surrain & Luk, Reference Surrain and Luk2019), as Kremin and Byers-Heinlein (Reference Kremin and Byers-Heinlein2020) point out, there are situations where this may not be possible or preferable (e.g., with small samples). Therefore, giving up a dichotomous classification of bilingualism altogether may not be the answer when it comes to operationalizing bilingualism (Kremin & Byers-Heinlein, Reference Kremin and Byers-Heinlein2020). Using a factor mixture model or a grade-of-membership model, which allow for categorization but considers the variation within the categories, would allow analysing results based on categories, on a continuous scale, or even on both (Kremin & Byers-Heinlein, Reference Kremin and Byers-Heinlein2020). The flexibility that such models provide would allow choosing an approach that would be driven by the research question, by the latent concept that is analyzed, and by the sample. Given the complexity of bilingualism, we believe that such a flexible approach would be appropriate to capture the different facets and nuances of the concept. Yet, using such models may not always be possible. Based on the recommendations of MacCallum et al. (Reference MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher and Rucker2002) concerning the dichotomization of continuous variables, bilingualism should, when possible, be operationalized as a continuous variable given that its nature is continuous. Dichotomizing a continuous variable will almost inevitably lead to misleading results (MacCallum et al., Reference MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher and Rucker2002), and serious considerations should be taken before dichotomizing bilingualism. This choice should be supported for instance by inspecting the data in order to determine whether participants cluster into defined clusters, or by providing a clear theoretical and conceptual argument for doing so.

However, our goal here was to investigate whether operationalization can affect results, making them significant in some cases but not in others when operationalization is slightly modified. In other words, our aim was not to establish a universal or optimal way to operationalize and define bilingualism, but rather to highlight the fact that defining and measuring bilingualism in different ways can lead to different conclusions. This supports the argument that a thorough and clear description of the sample of bilinguals that is investigated is necessary if comparisons can be made across studies (Surrain & Luk, Reference Surrain and Luk2019). We suggest that our results illustrate the complexity and difficulty of measuring a latent construct such as bilingualism, and that future research should take the different nuances of the concept into account. Therefore, researchers should carefully support their operationalization choices based on clearly defined theoretical models and empirical results rather than on arbitrary cutoffs or measurements. We suggest that, as long as it is not established which aspects of bilingualism or of the bilingual experience affect specific cognitive mechanisms, caution should be used when choosing not to measure a specific characteristic. For the time being, it may still be more methodologically sound to use questionnaires that are detailed and extensive (such as the LSBQ), and refrain from methods that are known to be flawed (such as using a median split).

It is important to point out however that several of the experimental paradigms used to reveal an advantage in executive functions in bilinguals (such as the Simon task) are not necessarily the most reliable tools to draw conclusions on between-individual differences. While low variability between participants is necessary in order to reliably find group-level effects, which experimental paradigms are designed to do, high between-subjects variability is necessary in order to reliably detect individual differences (Hedge, Powell, & Summer Reference Hedge, Powell and Summer2018), which is often what we aim to do in bilingual research. This suggests that caution should be taken when interpreting results to highlight differences between monolinguals and bilinguals (or between different types of bilinguals) and that methodological changes are necessary when designing tasks and analyzing results if such paradigms are used (see Hedge et al., Reference Hedge, Powell and Summer2018, for recommendations).

In sum, although the current study cannot settle how bilingualism is best measured, nor which facets of it are the most relevant, it achieves its aim of showing that operationalizing bilingualism in different ways (specifically, using different facet of bilingualism, and whether bilingualism is measured as a continuous or dichotomous variable) does affect the results. Here, we showed that dividing participants into groups instead of using their scores continuously tipped the value from approaching significance or not being significant to showing significant differences between monolinguals and bilinguals. Furthermore, our results also show that differences can (or cannot) be found depending on which aspect of the behavioral data is analyzed (i.e., accuracy vs. reaction times on congruent vs. incongruent trials, or on a score such as the Simon effect). This illustrates quite well how these results, had they come from different samples and been published as independent studies, would have led to inconsistent results where a bilingual advantage in accuracy would have been established in one study, but not in another, and where a bilingual disadvantage in speed would even have been supported by a third one. Simultaneously, there are several consistencies across the measurements and the dependant variables as well, suggesting that consistency across studies, despite using different types of operationalization, is not impossible. While this specific methodological issue is unlikely to be the sole source of all disparate results on the bilingual advantage in executive functions, it is possibly a contributing factor. Our results therefore stress the need for more research investigating operationalization and measurement of bilingualism. Future research should investigate which facets of bilingualism correlate together, which lead to similar neurological changes and effects on tasks tapping into executive functions, and which facets can and cannot be compared directly with each other. This study also highlights the need to clearly and transparently report how bilingualism is operationalized in studies investigating bilinguals in order for informed comparisons and meta-analyses to be possible. We believe that the current study contributes to the efforts toward more rigorous and standardised methodologies, and encourages increased openness, awareness, and transparency in the field of bilingual research.

References

Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2016). Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: Neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 689698. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000225 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. A., Mak, L., Chahi, A. K., & Bialystok, E. (2018). The Language and Social Background Questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 250263. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0867-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 699714. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berken, J. A., Chai, X., Chen, J.-K., Gracco, V. L., & Klein, D. (2016). Effects of early and late bilingualism on resting-state functional connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 11651172. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1960-15.2016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 311. doi: 10.1017/S1366728908003477 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 114121. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 233262. doi: 10.1037/bul0000099 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., & Pantev, C. (2005). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage, 24, 4049. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290303. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 240250. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29, 696705. doi: 10.1037/a0037254 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112, 494500. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloch, C., Kaiser, A., Kuenzli, E., Zappatore, D., Haller, S., Franceschini, R., … Nitsch, C. (2009). The age of second language acquisition determines the variability in activation elicited by narration in three languages in Broca’s and Wernicke’s area. Neuropsychologia, 47, 625633. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blom, E., Boerma, T., Bosma, E., Cornips, L., & Everaert, E. (2017). Cognitive advantages of bilingual children in different sociolinguistic contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 112. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00552 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borsa, V. M., Perani, D., Della Rosa, P. A., Videsott, G., Guidi, L. Weekes, B. S., … Abutalebi, J. (2018). Bilingualism and healthy aging: Aging effects and neural maintenance. Neuropsychologia, 111, 5161. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champoux-Larsson, M.-F., & Dylman, A. S. (2019). A prosodic bias, not an advantage, in bilinguals’ interpretation of emotional prosody. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22, 416424. doi: 10.1017/S1366728918000640 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1983). The cost of dichotomization. Applied Psychological Measurement, 7, 249253. doi: 10.1177/014662168300700301 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135149. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 5986. doi.10.1016/j.ognition.2006.12.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, S. R., Bak, T. H., Allerhand, M., Redmond, P., Starr, J. M., Deary, I. J., & MacPherson, S. E. (2016). Bilingualism, social cognition and executive functions: A tale of chickens and eggs. Neuropsychologia, 91, 299306. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damian, M. F., Ye, W., Oh, M., & Yang, S. (2018). Bilinguals as “experts”? Comparing performance of mono- to bilingual individuals via a mousetracking paradigm. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22, 11761193. doi: 10.1017/S1366728918000901 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Bruin, A. (2019). Not all bilinguals are the same: A call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behavioral Sciences, 9, 113. doi: 10.3390/bs9030033 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLuca, V., Rothman, J., Bialystok, E., & Pliatsikas, C. (2019). Redefining bilingualism as a spectrum of experiences that differentially affects brain structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 116, 7565–7574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811513116 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000243 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, D. M. (2018). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (5th ed., PPVT-5) [Measurement instrument]. London: Pearson.Google Scholar
Edwards, J. (2012). Conceptual and methodological issues in bilingualism and multilingualism research. In Bhatia, T. K. & Ritchie, W. C. (Eds.), The handbook of bilingualism and multilingualism (pp. 525). London: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fan, S. P., Liberman, Z., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2015). The exposure advantage: Early exposure to a multilingual environment promotes effective communication. Psychological Science, 26, 10901097. doi: 10.1177/0956797615574699 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folke, T., Ouzia, J., Bright, P., De Martino, B., & Filippi, R. (2016). A bilingual disadvantage in metacognitive processing. Cognition, 150, 119132. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.02.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Jones, L., Guasch, N. V., Young, N., & Hughes, E. K. (2010). Cognitive effects of bilingualism: Digging deeper for the contributions of language dominance, linguistic knowledge, socio-economic status and cognitive abilities. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13, 617664. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2010.488289 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kryscio, R. J., & Smith, C. D. (2013). Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 387396. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3837-12.2013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grundy, J. G., Anderson, J. A. E., & Bialystok, E. (2017). Neural correlates of cognitive processing in monolinguals and bilinguals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1396, 183201. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13333 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gullifer, J. W., Chai, X. J., Whitford, V., Pivneva, I., Baum, S., Klein, D., & Titone, D. (2018). Bilingual experience and resting-state brain connectivity: Impacts of L2 age of acquisition and social diversity of language use on control networks. Neuropsychologia, 117, 123134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gullifer, J. W., & Titone, D. (2020). Characterizing the social diversity of bilingualism using language entropy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23, 283294. doi: 10.1017/S1366728919000026 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullifer, J. W., & Titone, D. (in press). Engaging proactive control: Influences of diverse language experiences using insights from machine learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/7jq3w CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartanto, A., & Yang, H. (2016). Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition, 150, 1019. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, D. J., & Norbert, S. (2016). Attentive Turkers: MTurk participants perform better on online attention checks than do subject pool participants. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 400407. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0578-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Summer, P. (2018). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 11661186. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Incera, S., & McLennan, C. T. (2018). Bilingualism and age are continuous variables that influence executive function. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 25, 443463. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2017.1319902 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janus, M., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Working memory with emotional distraction in monolingual and bilingual children. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1582. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01582 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jylkkä, J., Soveri, A., Wahlström, J., Lehtonen, M., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2017). Relationship between language switching experience and executive functions in bilinguals: An Internet-based study. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29, 404419. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2017.1282489 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Prior, A. (2015). Variability in the effects of bilingualism on cognition: It is not just about cognition, it is also about bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 2728. doi: 10.1017/S1366728914000510 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovács, Á. M. (2009). Early bilingualism enhances mechanism of false-belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12, 4854. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00742.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kremin, L. V., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2020). Why not both? Rethinking categorical and continuous approaches to bilingualism. PsyArXiv preprints. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/nkvap CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Järvenpää, J. de Bruin, A., & Antfolk, J. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 394425. doi: 10.1037/bul0000142 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemhöfer, K., & Broersma, M. (2012). Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid lexical test for advanced learners of English. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 325343. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0146-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, P., Legault, J., & Litcofsky, K. A. (2014). Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex, 58, 301324. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S. J. (2012). Electrophysiological correlates of the focusing of attention within complex visual scenes: N2pc and related ERP components. In Luck, S. J. & Kappenman, E. S. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components (pp. 329360). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 605621. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.795574 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 1940. doi: 10.1037//1082-989X.7.1.19 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 940967. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97115. doi: 10.1017/S1366728903001068 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 8193. doi: 10.1017/S1366728907003227 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Namazi, M., & Thordardottir, E. (2010). A working memory, not bilingual advantage, in controlled attention. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13, 597616. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2010.488288 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., Anders-Jefferson, R., Mason, L., Alvarado, K., & Zimiga, B. (2018). Bilingual advantages in inhibition or selective attention: More challenges. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1409. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01409 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232258. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H., & Sawi, O. (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 615639. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.944914 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., Myuz, H. A., Anders, R. T., Bockelman, M. F., Mikulinsky, R., & Sawi, O. M. (2017). No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29, 89112. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2016.1248436 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Sawi, O. (2014). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 115. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00962 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papageorgiou, A., Bright, P., Tomas, E. P., & Filippi, R. (2018). Evidence against a cognitive advantage in the older bilingual population. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 13541363. doi: 10.1177/1747021818796475 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pliatsikas, C. (2020). Understanding structural plasticity in the bilingual brain: The Dynamic Restructuring Model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23, 459471. doi: 10.1017/S1366728919000130 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poarch, G. J., & Krott, A. (2019). A bilingual advantage? An appeal for a change in perspective and recommendations for future research. Behavioral Sciences, 9, 113. doi: 10.3390/bs9090095 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Šimkovic, M., & Träuble, B. (2019). Robustness of statistical methods when measure is affected by ceiling and/or floor effect. PLOS ONE, 14, 147. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoet, G. (2010). PsyToolkit—A software package for programming psychological experiments using Linux. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 10961104. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.4.1096 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoet, G. (2017). PsyToolkit: A novel web-based method for running online questionnaires and reaction-time experiments. Teaching of Psychology, 44, 2431. doi: 10.1177/0098628316677643 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sulpizio, S., Del Maschio, N., Del Mauro, G., Fedeli, D., & Abutalebi, J. (2020). Bilingualism as a gradient measure modulates functional connectivity of language and control networks. NeuroImage, 205, 116306. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116306 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surrain, S., & Luk, G. (2019). Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22, 401415. doi: 10.1017/S1366728917000682 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiv, M., Gullifer, J., Feng, R., & Titone, D. (in press). Using Network Science to map what Montréal bilinguals talk about across languages and communicative contexts. Journal of Neurolinguistics. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/bmry2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013). Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 86110. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000284 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2010). Chinese–English bilinguals reading English hear Chinese. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 76467651. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1602-10.2010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2013). Fast modulation of executive function by language context in bilinguals. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1353313537. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4760-12.2013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yow, W. Q., Li, X., Lam, S., Gliga, T., Chong, Y. S., Kwek, K., & Broekman, B. F. P. (2017). A bilingual advantage in 54-montholds’ use of referential cues in fast mapping. Developmental Science, 20, 115. doi: 10.1111/desc.12482 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Reported second, third, fourth, and fifth languages

Figure 1

Table 2. Demographics based on language groups

Figure 2

Table 3. Demographics based on continuous variables

Figure 3

Table 4. Summary of results