Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T15:24:21.095Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How well do schoolchildren and adolescents know the form and meaning of different derivational suffixes? Evidence from a cross-sectional study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 April 2024

Dalia Martinez*
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
Danielle Colenbrander
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for the Advancement of Literacy (ACAL), Australian Catholic University, North Sydney, NSW, Australia
Tomohiro Inoue
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, New Territories, Hong Kong
George K. Georgiou
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Dalia Martinez; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

As children advance through school, derived words become increasingly common in their reading materials. Previous studies have shown that children’s knowledge of derivational morphology develops relatively slowly, but there is more to learn about this development. This study examined differences in knowledge of the form and meaning of suffixes across grade levels (Grades 3, 5, and 8) and different types of derivational suffixes (adjectives and nominals). We assessed 309 English-speaking children on word reading and receptive vocabulary tests and two tasks designed to assess the form (orthographic knowledge) and meaning (semantic knowledge) of 28 derivational suffixes (14 adjectives and 14 nominals). Overall, our findings showed a significant improvement in identifying and understanding derivational suffixes from Grade 3 to Grade 5 and a smaller, but still significant, improvement from Grade 5 to Grade 8. Our findings regarding suffix types were mixed. While written forms of adjectives were identified more accurately than nominals across all grades, this advantage did not extend to the students’ understanding of the meaning of the suffixes. These results highlight the distinction between the identification of suffixes and the understanding of their meaning. We discuss our results in relation to suffix frequency in children’s reading materials.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

How well do schoolchildren and adolescents know the form and meaning of different derivational suffixes? Evidence from a cross-sectional study

Reading materials for children in upper elementary school grades display a notable increase in the incidence of polymorphemic words (i.e., those containing more than one morpheme; see Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Hsiao, Tan, Banerji and Nation2023; Grainger & Ziegler, Reference Grainger and Ziegler2011; Kearns & Hiebert, Reference Kearns and Hiebert2022; Nippold, Reference Nippold2018; Rastle, Reference Rastle2019). Of particular interest are derived words, which are formed by adding a derivational morpheme, or affix, before (e.g., re-) or after (e.g., -ful) a base word (e.g., “replay” from “play by adding re-, or “playful by adding -ful). The English orthographic system is described as morphophonemic as it represents morphological information in addition to phonological information in words’ reading and spelling (Venezky, Reference Venezky1967). For example, morphemic boundaries can influence how words are parsed into graphemes—the letters <p> and <h> usually form a digraph <ph>, which corresponds to the sound /f/ as in “phone” or “sphere,” yet this is not the case for words like “uphill” or “shepherd,” where the letters cross a morphemic boundary.

Knowledge of the morphemic structure of words has been shown to be associated with children’s word spelling and reading accuracy and fluency (Apel & Henbest, Reference Apel and Henbest2016; Burani et al., Reference Burani, Marcolini, Traficante and Zoccolotti2018; Deacon et al., Reference Deacon, Benere and Pasquarella2013; Levesque et al., Reference Levesque, Kieffer and Deacon2017, Reference Levesque, Breadmore and Deacon2021) as well as with vocabulary development (Carlisle, Reference Carlisle, Wagner, Muse and Tannenbaum2007; McBride-Chang et al., Reference McBride-Chang, Tardif, Cho, Shu, Fletcher, Stokes, Wong and Leung2008; Pacheco & Goodwin, Reference Pacheco and Goodwin2013; Ramirez et al., Reference Ramirez, Walton and Roberts2014), and morphology has been said to provide “islands of regularity” (Rastle et al., Reference Rastle, Davis, Marslen-Wilson and Tyler2000, p. 527) within the English spelling system. Thus, children’s ability to process the written form and meaning of derivational morphemes, either implicitly or explicitly, may be important for word reading and reading comprehension, even in the older grades of schooling where many content-specific vocabulary words are derived words (e.g., “measurement,” “astrology” or “germination”; Nippold, Reference Nippold2018). Despite this, only a handful of studies have examined students’ knowledge and understanding of derivational morphemes (Gaustad et al., Reference Gaustad, Ronald, Payne and Lylak2002; Mitchell & Brady, Reference Mitchell and Brady2014; Nippold & Sun, Reference Nippold and Sun2008) and they have some important limitations (see below). Thus, in this study we aimed to examine students’ knowledge of derivational morphology and whether this knowledge varies across grade levels and types of derivational suffixes (adjectives and nominals).

Development of morphological knowledge

A morpheme is the smallest unit of language that carries meaning. Base morphemes carry the main meaning in a word and can be free (stand-alone, e.g., “sun” and “flower”) or bound (e.g., -dict- in the word “prediction”). Prefixes are bound morphemes that we attach to the front of words or bases (e.g., un- in “unfair”), and suffixes are bound morphemes that we attach to the end of words or bases (e.g., -ate in “dictate”). There are three types of polymorphemic words: (1) Compounds, created by combining two free base morphemes, (2) inflected words, or words with inflectional suffixes that change the grammatical characteristics of the word, such as tense or number, and (3) derived words, which contain derivational prefixes and/or suffixes, that can change the word’s grammatical category or alter its meaning. For example, the base “pack” has compounds (e.g., “backpack”), inflected forms (e.g., “packing”), and prefixed and suffixed derivations (e.g., “unpack” and “packer”).

Research suggests that the acquisition of oral morphology follows a developmental progression. Compound words are understood and used at early ages (Clark, Reference Clark1993), the majority of inflections are mastered by Grade 1 (Berko, Reference Berko1958; see also Maynard et al., Reference Maynard, Brissaud, Armand, Berthiaume, Daigle and Desrochers2018, for a recent compilation of studies on inflectional morphology), and derivations continue to present difficulty even in upper grades (e.g., Ford et al., Reference Ford, Davis and Marslen-Wilson2010; Gaustad et al., Reference Gaustad, Ronald, Payne and Lylak2002; Nippold & Sun, Reference Nippold and Sun2008; Nunes & Bryant, Reference Nunes and Bryant2006)Footnote 1 . The greater difficulty in learning derivations is arguably because, compared to inflections, they lack systematicity, morphological family sizes are smaller, and they are more likely to cause phonological and orthographic shifts to the base (Carlisle & Katz, Reference Carlisle and Katz2006; Ford et al., Reference Ford, Davis and Marslen-Wilson2010; Quémart & Casalis, Reference Quémart and Casalis2014).

A recent analysis of children’s Language Arts textbooks in the U.S. showed that by third grade, the number of derived words found in texts is double that of Grade 1 (Kearns & Hiebert, Reference Kearns and Hiebert2022). These results resonate with those of Dawson et al. (Reference Dawson, Hsiao, Tan, Banerji and Nation2023) which also showed a significant increase in the number of derived words by grade level (see also Nippold, Reference Nippold2018, for a recent corpora analysis of derived words in children’s textbooks by school subject). Thus, children who struggle to comprehend derived words may struggle to understand the content and key concepts presented in age-appropriate texts. This is especially true in non-fiction content areas where specialized vocabulary is often used (Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Hsiao, Tan, Banerji and Nation2023; Nippold, Reference Nippold2018).

The increase in exposure to written morphology that occurs from the later elementary grades onwards has the potential to highlight form-meaning links that are not always noticeable in spoken language (Rastle, Reference Rastle2019). Therefore, as children are exposed to more examples of complex words that contain derivational morphemes, we might expect an improvement in both their ability to identify the written forms and their understanding of the meaning of these morphemes. In the literature on word recognition, there is evidence for increasingly automatic identification of suffix forms across development, with students as young as 7 years of age showing some ability to implicitly process written suffixes (e.g., Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Rastle and Ricketts2018). However, to our knowledge, only a handful of studies have examined children’s knowledge of derived word meanings using written tasksFootnote 2 . In a study by Gaustad et al. (Reference Gaustad, Ronald, Payne and Lylak2002), college (ages 19 to 34 years old) and middle school (ages 11 to 12 years old) students were asked to complete a multiple-choice task that tested their semantic knowledge of bound morphemes, including inflections and derivations (e.g., what is the meaning of re- as in “rewrite”: (a) important, (b) again, (c) moving, (d) after). College students scored an average of 94%, and middle school students scored an average of 79%, indicating that knowledge of derived words is still developing in middle school. Performance dropped to 89% for college students and 70% for middle schoolers when the items contained embedded bound morphemes that were less familiar (e.g., what is the meaning of therm- as in “thermal”), suggesting that performance on this task was also influenced by lexical vocabulary knowledge.

In another study, Nippold and Sun (Reference Nippold and Sun2008) tested knowledge of morphologically complex words in 10-year-old children and 13-year-old adolescents and divided items into adjectives (e.g., “acceptable” and “blissful”) and nominals (e.g., “citizenship” and “hostility”). Their results showed higher knowledge of adjectives (76.9% for children and 89.7% for adolescents) compared to nominals (63.2% for children and 79.4% for adolescents), which suggests that learning words that contain adjectival suffixes might be less challenging compared to those with nominal suffixes. According to Nippold and Sun, these differences could be driven by contextual cues provided by adjacent words, where adjectives are typically followed by a noun, whereas nouns can be followed by a wider variety of words such as prepositions, verbs, or adverbs. However, these results should be interpreted with some caution. Nippold and Sun used a cloze task with four choices (e.g., When Ali Baba’s wife saw the gold coins, she was (a) speechified, (b) specialized, (c) speechmaker, and (d) speechless), but it was not clear how the difficulty of foils was balanced across adjective and nominal conditions. Therefore, answers for certain questions across conditions might have been more salient given the differences in the frequency of the foils (e.g., Question 26 tested the knowledge of the adjective “molecular.” The four possible answers were (a) molecularity, (b) mollescent, (c) molecular, and (d) mollified). Likewise, the criteria used to control sentence context informativeness across conditions were unclear. Nippold and Sun further acknowledged that they did not control for the number of derivational suffixes attached to words. For instance, the five most difficult words for students were “concealment,” “consolable,” “dictatorship,” “tactfulness,” and “strenuousness.” Notably, four of these words are nominal, and most contain more than one derivational affix. Thus, the difficulty of some words might have reflected not solely the derivational affix’s difficulty but also the morphological complexity of the whole word and the number of orthographic and semantic shifts it underwent.

Nippold and Sun’s study highlights the complexities associated with assessing derivational suffix knowledge using real-world stimuli. In addition to knowledge of derived words, it is interesting to know whether students can identify and understand their constituent parts, because this may help us tease apart the development of lexical knowledge from the development of morphological knowledge. The use of nonwords with either real affixes or real bases can be useful in this respect. In 2014, Mitchell and Brady compared the knowledge of real words (e.g., interoffice) and nonwords (e.g., interlanosts) with the same affix in Grade 3 and Grade 5 students. While their results did not show a significant difference in overall performance between words and nonwords, patterns of knowledge were different across the two measures at the item level (e.g., some students knew the word “closure” but not the suffix -ure). These results suggest that knowledge of a derived word does not always equate to knowledge of the suffix within the word. Moreover, the results also showed that not all suffixes are mastered equally, a question also raised by Nippold and Sun (Reference Nippold and Sun2008).

Only a limited number of studies have explored whether the knowledge of derived words differs depending on their part of speech, and these studies have found mixed results. As above, Nippold and Sun (Reference Nippold and Sun2008) found evidence that the meanings of derived adjectives were better known than the meanings of nominals, whereas a study by Marinellie & Kneile, Reference Marinellie and Kneile2012) demonstrated no significant differences between the two. It is of interest to know whether different types of suffixes have different developmental trajectories because such insights have important practical implications, such as when and how different types of suffixes are better taught. They may also shed light on the factors that contribute to the relative ease or difficulty of suffix acquisition.

Furthermore, knowledge of the meaning of a suffix does not imply knowledge of the orthographic form or vice versa (e.g., Apel et al., Reference Apel, Diehm and Apel2013; Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Petscher, Carlisle and Mitchell2017; Kristensen et al., Reference Kristensen, Andersson, Bratlie and Torkildsen2023), and these two aspects of knowledge, while interconnected, may have different developmental trajectories. Masked priming studies have shown that for adolescents and adult skilled readers, the parsing of morphologically complex words can be driven by orthographic characteristics without an influence from meaning (e.g., parsing the written word corner into corn+er, e.g., Beyersmann et al., Reference Beyersmann, Castles and Coltheart2012; Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Rastle and Ricketts2021). With this in mind, we aimed to explore the potential differences in knowledge of the derivational suffixes at two levels: form and meaning. Such knowledge may have implications for the instruction or remediation of children with reading, spelling, and/or language difficulties. However, to our knowledge, no previous study has examined the derivational suffix knowledge at these two levels and distinguished this knowledge by suffix type.

The present study

The purpose of this study was twofold: (a) First, to explore whether there were any differences in form (orthographic) and meaning (semantic) knowledge of written suffixes in Grades 3, 5, and 8, and (b) to examine whether the pattern of knowledge differed by suffix type and compared performance for adjectival and nominal suffixes. We measured and controlled for word reading and vocabulary because both skills are closely associated with morphological knowledge (e.g., Adams, Reference Adams1990; Deacon et al., Reference Deacon, Kieffer and Laroche2014; Haase & Steinbrink, Reference Haase and Steinbrink2022; Inoue et al., Reference Inoue, Georgiou and Parrila2023; Kuo & Anderson, Reference Kuo and Anderson2006; Mitchell & Brady, Reference Mitchell and Brady2014; Nagy et al., Reference Nagy, Berninger, Abbott, Vaughan and Vermeulen2003). Controlling for word reading was particularly important as our tasks were written tasks completed individually and in silence.

In sum, we aimed to answer the following research questions:

  • Q1. How well do students know the written form and meaning of nominal and adjectival derivational suffixes in different grade levels?

  • Q2. Does the development of derivational suffixes vary as a function of suffix type (nominals vs. adjectives)?

Because there is very little data comparing both form and meaning knowledge of the same suffixes across development, we did not have a directional hypothesis. Regarding suffix type, we expected that children would perform better on tasks of adjectival suffix knowledge than on tasks of nominal suffix knowledge. Importantly, in this study, we expanded on Nippold and Sun’s (Reference Nippold and Sun2008) work by comparing the knowledge of different suffix types using nonword stimuli. We also carefully controlled foil characteristics, reduced the potential influence of sentence context, and used a larger range of suffixes. In addition, we extended Mitchell and Brady’s (Reference Mitchell and Brady2014) work by using two different measures to assess suffix knowledge, one measuring form knowledge (i.e., orthographic knowledge) and the other measuring meaning or semantic knowledge (see also Apel et al., Reference Apel, Henbest and Petscher2022 and Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Petscher, Carlisle and Mitchell2017, for discussions of why multiple measures of morphological knowledge are useful).

Method

Participants

To select our participants, we first sent letters describing our study to the parents of 118 Grade 3, 148 Grade 5, and 114 Grade 8 students attending 11 public schools in Edmonton, Canada. The schools were located in different parts of the city to increase the representation of different demographics in our study as much as possible. We received parental consent from 108 Grade 3, 125 Grade 5, and 90 Grade 8 students that were subsequently invited to participate in the testing. All students had English as their first language and did not experience any intellectual, behavioral, or sensory difficulties (based on their teachers’ reports). Ethics approval from the University of Alberta (Pro00119949) was also obtained prior to testing. From our original sample, 4 participants (2 in Grade 5, and 2 in Grade 8) were removed due to very low reading scores (standard scores in word reading accuracy below 70) and 10 participants (5 in Grade 3, 3 in Grade 5, and 2 in Grade 8) were removed for not following instructions (selecting more than one option in the multiple-choice task or failing to respond the last page of the task) or answering randomly (circling the last two letters for all items in the Suffix Identification Task-Nonwords). This left a total sample of 103 Grade 3 (51 females, M age = 8.9 years; SD = .53), 120 Grade 5 (58 females, M age = 10.9 years; SD = .49), and 86 Grade 8 (38 females, M age = 13.9 years; SD = .48) students.

Materials

Word reading accuracy

To assess word reading accuracy, we administered the Word Reading task from the Wide Range Achievement Test-5 (WRAT-5 blue form; Wilkinson & Robertson, Reference Wilkinson and Robertson2017). Children were asked to read aloud 15 letters and 55 words of increasing difficulty. The task was discontinued after five consecutive errors, and a participant’s score was the total number correct (max = 70). The raw score was subsequently converted to a standard score following the instructions in the manual. Cronbach’s alpha reliability has been reported to be .91 in Grade 3, .95 in Grade 5, and .93 in Grade 8 (Wilkinson & Robertson, Reference Wilkinson and Robertson2017).

Vocabulary knowledge

Vocabulary knowledge was assessed with the Listening Comprehension subtest from the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test-2 (WIAT-2; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2005). Children were first asked to listen to a word provided orally by the examiner and then select one of four pictures that best depicted the word’s meaning. The task was discontinued after four consecutive errors, and a participant’s score was the total number of correct responses (max = 19). The raw score was subsequently converted to a standard score following the instructions in the manual. Cronbach’s alpha reliability has been reported to be .85 in Grade 3, .83 in Grade 5, and .85 in Grade 8 (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2005).

Derivational suffix knowledge

Two measures of derivational suffix knowledge were administered: The Suffix Identification Task-Nonwords (SIT-N) and the Suffix Meaning Task-Nonwords (SMT-N). Both tasks were designed for the present study to measure students’ knowledge of derivational suffixes separately from the influences of base word or whole word knowledge by using nonwords as the base of the novel-created derived items (e.g., “plemette” meaning a small “plem”). Because evidence has shown that suffix frequency, family size, and length can influence how words are processed and understood (Carlisle & Katz, Reference Carlisle and Katz2006; Ford et al., Reference Ford, Davis and Marslen-Wilson2010; Sánchez-Gutiérrez et al., Reference Sánchez-Gutiérrez, Mailhot, Deacon and Wilson2018), all the suffixes attached to the nonword bases were matched on frequency, length, and family size. The complete tasks are available at https://osf.io/wx2q9/.

Suffix identification task—Nonwords (SIT-N). The SIT-N was adapted from Apel et al. (Reference Apel, Diehm and Apel2013). The SIT-N assessed children’s ability to identify real derivational suffixes in the context of nonwords. This task contained nonword bases (e.g., drex) with real suffixes attached (e.g., -ness to create the derived word “drexness”; more examples are given in Appendix A in Supplementary material). All nonwords for the bases were selected from the English Lexicon Project database (Balota et al., Reference Balota, Yap, Cortese, Hutchison, Kessler, Loftis, Neely, Nelson, Simpson and Treiman2007) with the characteristics of being monosyllabic, three-to-five letters long (M = 4.4) and having an orthographic neighborhood density no higher than 25 (M = 5.81). The suffixes used in the SIT-N were 14 derivational adjectives (e.g., –ic, –ish, –able) and 14 derivational nominals (e.g., -ity, -er, -itis) taken from the MorphoLex database (Sánchez-Gutiérrez et al., Reference Sánchez-Gutiérrez, Mailhot, Deacon and Wilson2018). Derivational noun suffixes were matched to derivational adjective suffixes on summed token frequencyFootnote 3 , length, and family size. All target suffixes, grouped by type, and their characteristics are listed in Appendix B in Supplementary material. Each suffix was joined to two nonword bases for a total of 56 target items. Additionally, four items in the task contained a pseudosuffix (e.g., -mut to create the word “feemut”). These items were distractors and were distributed amongst the other items to discourage students from simply circling the last 2–3 letters of each word. The examiner provided the following directions: “This activity has lots of silly words you have never seen before. These words have real suffixes or add-ons at the end of the word. You use and have seen many of these suffixes (add-ons) before. Your job is to find and circle them.” Then, the examiner would show the word “cars,” circle the -s at the end of the word, and say, “The word cars has the suffix -s that means more than one. Now we are going to try to find the suffixes in these silly words.” Next, the examiner would show the participant two nonwords (e.g., “pleemed”) in written form and ask the participant to circle the suffix in each example. The examiner answered all questions and confirmed the correct response for all practice trials. In cases where the participant provided an incorrect response, the examiner would present the correct response and provide an explanation using real words to emphasize why it was the correct answer. For instance, in the word “pleemed,” the examiner would highlight that we needed to circle -ed because it is the add-on that we find at the end of the word to indicate that something happened in the past, similar to words like “jumped.” The participant was then asked to circle the suffixes in all the test items printed on paper. The task was done in silence without a discontinuation rule. Only the real suffixes were scored (i.e., responses on the four distractor items were not scored). Thus, the maximum possible score was 56. Cronbach’s alpha reliability was .95 in Grade 3, .92 in Grade 5, and .90 in Grade 8, indicating high levels of internal consistency.

Suffix meaning taskNonwords (SMT-N). The SMT-N was designed after the study by Berko (Reference Berko1958) and adapted from an original task designed by Colenbrander (Reference Colenbrander2015). In the SMT-N task, participants were asked about the meaning of 24 derivational suffixes (12 adjectives and 12 nominals; target suffixes and their characteristics are listed in Appendix B in Supplementary material) in a written, multiple-choice format. This task included the same adjectives and nominals used for the SIT-N, except for four suffixes that were removed (-ness, -ance, -ic, -ile) due to their abstract nature, which made it challenging to construct unambiguous definitions. These items were eliminated based on comments provided by ten university students who participated in a pilot testing of the SMT-N before data collection.

The 12 remaining derivational adjectives and 12 derivational nominals were taken from the MorphoLex database (Sánchez-Gutiérrez et al., Reference Sánchez-Gutiérrez, Mailhot, Deacon and Wilson2018). The two groups of suffixes were matched on summed token frequency (adjectives: M = 888,870.83, SD = 1192138.95; nominals: M = 909,751.92, SD = 1311387.91), length (adjectives: M = 3.00, nominals: M = 3.00), and family size (adjectives: M = 518.58, SD = 694.38; nominals: M = 410.50, SD = 620.70). For each target suffix, a question was constructed, for a total of 24 questions. Each question asked about the definition of a suffix in the context of a nonword (e.g., Trab. Which one means something like “without trab”? (a) trabbish, (b) trabbive, (c) trabful, (d) trabless; more examples are given in Appendix A in Supplementary material). One point was given for each correct response, and all questions had only one correct response, for a total of 24 points. For each question, the three foils were matched to the target on suffix frequency, family size, and suffix type. The definitions were taken from Gaustad et al. (Reference Gaustad, Ronald, Payne and Lylak2002) and Colenbrander (Reference Colenbrander2015). The definitions were designed to contain simple language and to be no more than three words in length (e.g., a person who…, full of…, the study of…, a bit like…, having lots of…).

One individual SMT-N booklet that contained all 24 questions was given to each student. The first page of the booklet had two practice items read aloud by the examiner. After reading practice item 1 (Wug. Which one means “more than one wug”? (A) wuggy, (B) wugging, (C) wugs, (D) wugged), the examiner asked the group to call out the best answer along with an explanation for their response. The examiner then confirmed the correct response and asked all participants to circle that choice in their booklets. The same procedure was repeated for practice item 2, and after both practice questions, the participants continued working individually, answering each question on their booklets in silence. The maximum possible score was 24. Internal consistency as measured by Cronbach’s alpha in our sample was .60 in Grade 3, .72 in Grade 5, and .77 in Grade 8.

Procedure

Testing took place during the months of May and June (towards the end of the school year in Canada). All tasks were administered during school hours by trained assistants with experience in psychoeducational assessments. The SIT-N, WIAT-2, and WRAT-5 were assessed first in a quiet room in a one-on-one session that lasted approximately 15 minutes. Participants then returned to their regular activities for about an hour until the examiner was ready to deliver the second part of the assessment. The second part included only the SMT-N, which was administered as a large group activity in the children’s classrooms with their teachers present at all times. While the participants completed their work, the examiner walked around the classroom to ensure all participants were on task. Once a participant had answered all questions, the examiner collected their booklet and had a quick look to ensure all questions were addressed. Participants were then asked to remain silent until the whole group had finished. All participants completed the task within 20 minutes. For schools with more than one group participating in the project, two examiners delivered the assessment to ensure that the data for Part 1 and Part 2 were collected on the same day.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using R Version 4.2.2 (R Core Team, 2022) through RStudio Version 2023.03.0+386 (RStudio Team, 2020). Separate logistic mixed effects models were fitted using the binomial dependent variable (coded as 0 for incorrect responses and 1 for correct responses) for the two suffix knowledge tasks (SIT-N and SMT-N) to account for the nested structure of our data: Items (Level 1) were nested within Participants (Level 2). For model construction procedures for each task (for details, see Appendices G and H in Supplementary material), we started with a baseline model that included only random intercepts at the Item and Participant levels (Model 0). We then entered fixed and random effects into the models in a stepwise manner as follows: the fixed effects of word reading accuracy and vocabulary knowledge (both continuous variables) in Model 1; the fixed effects of grade (a three-level factor) and suffix type (a two-level factor) in Model 2; the fixed effect of the interaction between grade and suffix type in Model 3; the random effect of suffix type at the Participant level in Model 4; the random effect of grade at the Item level in Model 5; both of these random effects in Model 6. Word reading accuracy and vocabulary knowledge were centered before the analyses. Grade and suffix type were coded using the contr.sdif function in the MASS package Version 7.3-59 (Venables & Ripley, Reference Venables and Ripley2002). Grade was coded with repeated contrasts to compare two consecutive grades, namely Grade 3 vs. Grade 5 and Grade 5 vs. Grade 8; the three grades were coded as −2/3, 1/3, and 1/3 in the first contrast, while they were coded as −1/3, −1/3, and 2/3 in the second contrast. Suffix type was coded with a simple contrast; adjectives and nominals were coded as −0.5 and 0.5, respectively. The best-fitting models were selected based on the models’ fit indices (AIC, BIC, and Log Likelihood values) and the results of likelihood ratio tests for model comparisons between nested models. In addition, the marginal and conditional R 2 values for the models were calculated using the MuMIn package Version 1.47.5 (Barton, Reference Barton2019); marginal R 2 indicates the variance explained by fixed effects, and conditional R 2 indicates the variance explained by both fixed and random effects (Nakagawa & Schielzeth, Reference Nakagawa and Schielzeth2013; Orelien & Edwards, Reference Orelien and Edwards2008).

All models were fit using the glmer function in the lmerTest package (Version 3.1-3; Kuznetsova et al., Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen2017). The data and analysis code for all models are available at https://osf.io/wx2q9/.

Results

Descriptive statistics for all the variables are presented in Table 1. A closer examination of the Participant level variables (word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and the proportions of correct responses in Suffix Identification Task-Nonwords [SIT-N] and Suffix Meaning Task-Nonwords [SMT-N]) showed one univariate outlier on word reading in Grades 5 and 8, one outlier on the adjective items of the SIT-N in Grades 5 and 8, and one outlier on the nominal items on the SIT-N in Grade 8 (scores were 3 SD above/below the group mean). To avoid overemphasizing their effects on the results, we winsorized their scores by replacing them with a value equal to the next highest/lowest non-outlier-score plus 1 unit of measurement before further analyses (Tabachnick & Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2012). Pearson’s r and Spearman’s ρ correlations between the variables are presented in Table 2. Both SIT-N and SMT-N were weakly to moderately correlated with word reading across grades (rs ranged from .32 to .39 for Grade 3, .17 to .51 for Grade 5, and .38 to .52 for Grade 8). Their correlations with vocabulary knowledge were relatively weaker than those with word reading, except for SMT-N in Grade 8 (rs ranged from .02 to .22 for Grade 3, .09 to .34 for Grade 5, and .17 to .48 for Grade 8).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the measures used in the study

Note. SIT-N = Suffix Identification Task-Nonwords; SMT-N = Suffix Meaning Task-Nonwords; Adj = adjectives; Nom = nominals.

Table 2. Correlations between the variables for each grade

Note. Pearson’s rs are shown below the diagonal, and Spearman’s ρs are shown above the diagonal. SIT-N = Suffix Identification Task-Nonwords; SMT-N = Suffix Meaning Task-Nonwords; Adj = adjectives; Nom = nominals.

*p < .05. **p < .01.

The results of the best-fitting models for each of the two suffix knowledge tasks are presented in Tables 3 and 4 (see Appendices E and F in Supplementary material, for the results of model comparisons). For both SIT-N and SMT-N, the models that included the fixed effects of word reading accuracy, vocabulary knowledge, grade, suffix type, and the interaction between grade and suffix type, as well as the random effects of suffix type at the Participant level and grade at the Item level showed the best fit (see the footnotes of the tables for the model equations). For SIT-N (see Table 3), word reading had a significant fixed effect (estimate = 0.029, p < .001), while vocabulary knowledge did not (estimate = 0.000, p = .986). In addition, the fixed effects of the two grade contrasts (estimates = 1.554, p < .001 for the Grade 3 vs. Grade 5 contrast and 0.454, p = .014 for the Grade 5 vs. Grade 8 contrast) and suffix type (estimate = −0.672, p = .020) were significant. The former result indicates that the probability of correct responses increased with grade level, while the latter indicates that the probability of correct responses was relatively higher for adjectives than for nominals across grades (see Figure 1). The interaction between grade and suffix type was not significant (estimates = 0.206, p = .489 for the Grade 3 vs. Grade 5 contrast and 0.119, p = .558 for the Grade 5 vs. Grade 8 contrast).

Table 3. Results of the best-fitting model for SIT-N

Note. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit. Number of observations = 17304; number of participants = 309; number of items = 56. Model equation: accuracy ∼ word_reading + vocabulary_knowledge + grade + suffix_type + grade:suffix_type + (1 + suffix_type | participant) + (1 + grade | item).

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Table 4. Results of the best-fitting model for SMT-N

Note. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit. Number of observations = 7416; number of participants = 309; number of items = 24. Model equation: accuracy ∼ word_reading + vocabulary_knowledge + grade + suffix_type + grade:suffix_type + (1 + suffix_type | participant) + (1 + grade | item).

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Figure 1. Performance levels of each grade on the SIT-N task.

Note. The plots are a combination of violin plots and box plots. Violin plots show the density distribution of the proportion correct, and box plots show the median, the interquartile range, and 1.5 times the interquartile range.

For SMT-N (see Table 4), both word reading and vocabulary knowledge had a significant fixed effect (estimates = 0.023, p < .001 for word reading and 0.010, p < .001 for vocabulary knowledge). The fixed effects of the two grade contrasts were also significant (estimates = 0.779, p < .001 for the Grade 3 vs. Grade 5 contrast and 0.325, p = .008 for the Grade 5 vs. Grade 8 contrast), indicating that the probability of correct responses increased with grade level. In contrast, the fixed effect of suffix type was not significant (estimate = 0.070, p = .850). The interaction between the Grade 5 vs. Grade 8 contrast and suffix type was significant (estimate = 0.759, p < .001), while that between the Grade 3 vs. Grade 5 contrast and suffix type was not (estimate = −0.185, p = .303). These results indicate that the probabilities of correct responses for the two suffix types were similar in Grades 3 and 5, while they differed between Grades 5 and 8, showing that Grade 8 children had a higher probability of correct responses to nominals than adjectives (see Figure 2).

Figure 2. Performance levels of each grade on the SMT-N task.

Note. The plots are a combination of violin plots and box plots. Violin plots show the density distribution of the proportion correct, and box plots show the median, the interquartile range, and 1.5 times the interquartile range.

Discussion

The present study examined students’ knowledge of the written form and meaning of derivational suffixes, assessed through two experimenter-designed tasks (SIT-N to assess form and SMT-N for meaning) in which real suffixes were paired with nonword bases (e.g., “spoochful”). Nonwords were used to ensure that we were measuring students’ knowledge of suffixes independently of their lexical vocabulary knowledge. The study examined differences in knowledge across grade levels (third, fifth, and eighth grades) and different types of derivational suffixes (adjectives and nominals). The findings indicated substantially greater knowledge of the form and meaning of derivational suffixes in Grade 5 compared to Grade 3 and a smaller, albeit significant, growth in Grade 8 compared to Grade 5. These results are consistent with those of previous studies showing that the development of derivational morphology is a protracted process (e.g., Berninger et al., Reference Berninger, Abbott, Nagy and Carlisle2010; Dawson et al., 2018; Ford et al., Reference Ford, Davis and Marslen-Wilson2010; Gaustad et al., Reference Gaustad, Ronald, Payne and Lylak2002; Ku & Anderson, Reference Ku and Anderson2003; Nippold & Sun, Reference Nippold and Sun2008).

When it comes to growth patterns across suffix types, our findings diverged from the limited existing research comparing adjectives versus nominals. For the identification task (morphological orthographic knowledge), participants in all grades showed better performance for adjectival suffixes. In contrast, on the meaning task (morphological semantic knowledge), there was no difference between suffix types in Grades 3 and 5, but participants in Grade 8 scored higher on nominal suffixes. This result differs from the results of Nippold and Sun (Reference Nippold and Sun2008), which showed higher performance on adjectives across grade levels in a task that simultaneously tapped both form and meaning knowledge of real morphologically complex words. In other words, once we controlled for lexical vocabulary knowledge (by using nonword bases) and foil characteristics (by balancing foils on frequency, family size, grammatical category, and length), the advantage for adjectival suffixes was only evident in the identification task.

This suggests that although students may be more familiar with the written form of certain adjectives and, therefore, more likely to recognize them, this does not necessarily imply a better understanding of their meaning. Previous studies on the dimensionality of morphological knowledge (Apel et al., Reference Apel, Diehm and Apel2013, Reference Apel, Henbest and Petscher2022; Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Petscher, Carlisle and Mitchell2017, Reference Goodwin, Petscher and Tock2021) have also shown that individuals can have varying degrees of proficiency across different dimensions of morphological knowledge. Studies that support the view of morphological knowledge as a multidimensional construct make a broad distinction between implicit morphological knowledge (or morphological processing), which refers to the knowledge at the orthographic level driven by the orthographic co-occurrences that morphemes represent, and more in-depth knowledge that emerges when students start to reflect on the structure of the word, the meaning, and the roles of the affixes (this type of knowledge is also known as morphological analysis, see Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Gilbert, Cho and Kearns2014). However, work from the masked priming literature suggests that implicit morpho-orthographic processing initially relies on a degree of semantic knowledge, but later becomes semantically “blind” (Diependaele et al., Reference Diependaele, Sandra and Grainger2005; Rastle et al., Reference Rastle, Davis and New2004).

Our findings suggest the possibility that for more abstract, later-acquired suffixes, it may be the case that morpho-orthographic learning is semantically “blind” from the beginning. In other words, students may perceive the suffixes as orthographic “chunks” given their co-occurrence but have yet to assign meaning. This raises the question of whether these orthographic chunks are treated as real productive morphemes.

In other words, our findings support a distinction between morphological processing (at the orthographic level) and morphological analysis (at the semantic level). However, the different growth patterns for adjectives and nominals raise questions about the factors that help consolidate their learning. Empirical evidence shows that suffix frequency and family size influence how words are processed and understood (Ford et al., Reference Ford, Davis and Marslen-Wilson2010; Sánchez-Gutiérrez et al., Reference Sánchez-Gutiérrez, Mailhot, Deacon and Wilson2018), but our study controlled for these factors. Concreteness has also been proposed as a factor that may influence the acquisition of different suffix types (Nippold & Sun, Reference Nippold and Sun2008; Strik-Lievers et al., Reference Strik-Lievers, Bolognesi and Winter2021). Recently, Strik-Lievers et al. (Reference Strik-Lievers, Bolognesi and Winter2021) calculated the level of concreteness of a variety of derivational suffixes. From their data, we were able to obtain concreteness scores for seven adjectives and five nominals. Contrary to what Nippold and Sun’s (and to some extent, our own) data suggest, most of the adjectives (5 out of 7) showed low concreteness scores, while the nominals displayed high concreteness scores. Our findings show that low concreteness scores do not necessarily translate into low scores for suffix identification. Alternatively, it is conceivable that after the orthographic representation of the morpheme has been learned, concreteness assumes an important role in consolidating meaning, which might explain why semantic knowledge for nominals was higher only in Grade 8. Nevertheless, it is important to interpret this with care as we only possess concreteness scores for approximately half of the items featured in our task, and concreteness could potentially interact with other variables in the learning process.

Our findings are better explained by data on suffix frequency and the role of exposure to suffixes in children’s reading materials. As mentioned before, the number of derived words in children’s texts increases as they progress to higher grades, but the rate of increase is not consistent. Dawson et al. (Reference Dawson, Hsiao, Tan, Banerji and Nation2023) analyzed derivational suffix frequency in children’s reading material at three stages (corresponding to education levels in England and Wales); Key Stage 1, which included reading material for students ages 5 to 7 (early elementary school), Key Stage 2, corresponding to reading material for students ages 7 to 11 (later elementary school), and Key Stage 3, reading material for students ages 11 to 14 (late elementary school and early secondary school). Their results showed that the increase in the number of derived words between Key Stages 1 and 2 (early to late elementary school) is more than double the increase reported between Key Stages 2 and 3 (late elementary school to early secondary, see Appendix G in Supplementary material). In a broad sense, this could explain why Grade 5 students have significantly more knowledge of the form and meaning of derivational morphemes than Grade 3 students, but the difference is less pronounced between Grade 5 and Grade 8 students.

Using data from Dawson et al. (Reference Dawson, Hsiao, Tan, Banerji and Nation2023), we were able to explore results for adjectives and nominals more closely. Interestingly, in Stage 1, the frequency of adjectives is considerably higher than that of nominals, but the reading material of students at later key stages showed no increase, and even a decline, in the frequency of adjectives, while the occurrence of nominals continued to increase. For example, the suffix -ful had a frequency of 24,772 per million suffixed words in the reading material for students ages 7 to 11, but a frequency of 17,252 per million in texts for students ages 11 to 14. This trend is also visible for other adjectives such as -ar and -ous (for more examples, see Appendix G in Supplementary material).

This drop in frequency might not have a significant impact on suffixes that have already reached a high level of mastery by Grade 5, such as -ful and -less. However, it appears to hinder further development of other suffixes that still require consolidation, especially in terms of understanding their meaning. For example, suffixes such as -ar and -ous that show a decrease in text frequency also show no increase in performance in our tasks between Grades 5 and 8, where they seem to plateau at scores around 80% accuracy for identification and 60% accuracy for meaning. Together, these results suggest that the knowledge of certain derivational adjectives begins to approach a plateau by Grade 5. The growth pattern in adjectival suffix knowledge appears closely related to the language children are exposed to via their reading experience.

An increase in performance for nominal derivational suffixes at each grade level could also be attributed to the type of written language children are exposed to. Research has shown that nominalizations are around four times more common in academic writing compared to fiction and speech (Biber et al., Reference Biber, Conrad and Reppen1998). Many nominal derivational suffixes are frequently used in academic writing to nominalize verbs and adjectives, which can reflect a more formal and depersonalized style (see Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Hsiao, Tan, Banerji and Nation2023, for further discussion on nominalizations). Dawson et al.’s (Reference Dawson, Hsiao, Tan, Banerji and Nation2023) analysis shows that nominal derivational suffixes consistently increase in frequency as students move to upper grades. For example, the suffix -itis had a frequency of 0 with no appearances in the reading material for children aged 5 to 7, a frequency of 48 in the texts for children aged 7 to 11; and finally, a frequency of 320 in reading material for ages 11 to 14. Dawson et al. (Reference Dawson, Hsiao, Tan, Banerji and Nation2023) reported a substantial increase in frequency for all nominal suffixes in texts aimed at older children, with the only exception being the suffix -ism (see Appendix G in Supplementary material, for more information on each suffix). Therefore, the consistent increase in knowledge of nominal derivational suffixes across grade levels could be linked to their increasing prevalence in more advanced and formal texts.

Limitations and future directions

Some limitations of the present study should be reported. First, the Suffix Meaning Task-Nonwords (SMT-N), created for this study to measure semantic knowledge of suffixes, showed relatively low internal consistency, particularly in Grade 3. This could be due to the constrained-choice aspect of the task, the small number of items, and the grade level. Previous studies have reported low-reliability scores for early grades in constrained-choice tasks (see Ursachi et al., Reference Ursachi, Horodnic and Zait2015, for a review on further external factors that influence reliability scores). Future research should consider additional assessment formats, such as expressive questions and multiple testing sessions to increase the number of items per suffix and improve internal reliability. Second, the study was cross-sectional, which limits our ability to identify developmental changes within the same sample. To determine if there is genuinely limited growth in the knowledge of adjectival derivational suffixes between Grades 5 and 8, future longitudinal studies are necessary. Finally, our study focused on English, and it is important to note that our findings may not generalize to other languages. The importance we placed on children’s reading materials as a potential factor influencing the growth in suffix knowledge suggests the need for further investigations in other languages where the types and frequency of polymorphemic words differ (see Borleffs et al., Reference Borleffs, Maassen, Lyytinen and Zwarts2017, for a discussion on morphological complexity across languages in alphabetic orthographies). Exploring languages with different levels of morphological richness and orthographic consistency can help clarify language-specific differences and enrich our understanding of morphological development.

The different growth paths for adjectives and nominals found in our study highlight the importance of using multiple forms of assessment to evaluate morphological knowledge. Future research should consider an examination of derivational suffix knowledge using a wider range of tasks that carefully tackle the orthographic, semantic, and syntactic dimensions of morphological knowledge (see Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Petscher, Carlisle and Mitchell2017, Reference Goodwin, Petscher and Tock2021). Studies have demonstrated that different aspects of morphological knowledge may contribute uniquely to different aspects of reading, with orthographic knowledge impacting speed and accuracy, and semantic knowledge influencing comprehension (see e.g., Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Petscher, Carlisle and Mitchell2017). Consequently, future research on derivational affix knowledge should consider incorporating a range of reading measures to investigate how different aspects of suffix knowledge contribute to each measure of reading.

Furthermore, studies that look into the effect of the positional constraint of the morphemes (i.e., prefixes versus suffixes) and a wider variety of grammatical and syntactic functions (including verbs and adverbs in addition to adjectives and nominals) could advance our understanding of written morphological development and inform instruction tailored to the specific requirements of each affix type.

Conclusion

Our study showed that children’s knowledge of written nominal and adjectival derivational suffixes progresses at each grade level (Grades 3, 5, and 8). When comparing the form and meaning knowledge of these two types of suffixes, we observed distinct growth patterns. While students of all grade levels demonstrated stronger identification skills for adjectival suffixes, this proficiency did not carry over to their understanding of suffix meaning. Notably, Grade 8 students showed superior semantic knowledge of nominals compared to adjectives. The notable differences between identification and meaning across suffixes highlight the significance of assessing suffix knowledge across multiple dimensions, such as orthographic and semantic knowledge, as proficiency in one does not guarantee mastery in the other. Moreover, our findings draw attention to the importance of reading experience, as the growth patterns in suffix knowledge for each suffix type align with data on children’s exposure to new words in written materials. Engaging in reading can contribute to the development of morphological knowledge through exposure to polymorphemic words that contain morphemes consistent in form and meaning. Additionally, practice in reading can help strengthen metalinguistic competencies, previously proven to hold a positive correlation with morphological skills (see Larsen & Nippold, Reference Larsen, Nippold and Nippold2007).

There is consensus that knowledge of morphology is closely related to literacy development (see Levesque et al., Reference Levesque, Breadmore and Deacon2021) with derivational morphology being particularly important at older grades (Nippold, Reference Nippold2018). Our work on the knowledge of typically developing children can help inform assessment tasks aimed at identifying children who may have difficulty reading, spelling, or comprehending polymorphemic words and thus need extra instruction. It can also help guide the content of intervention programs aimed at teaching morphology to children with reading or language difficulties. Our data can help inform the content of intervention programs by presenting accuracy rates for both form and meaning of a large number of suffixes across three grade levels; adding to the literature offering empirical evidence that recognizing a suffix does not always imply understanding its meaning, emphasizing the need for diverse tasks to monitor progress; and highlighting the importance of exposing students to numerous examples of polymorphemic words to support further morphological development.

Given the different growth patterns for adjectives and nominals, further empirical research on when and how different types of derivational affixes are learned is crucial for enhancing our understanding of derivational morphology and how to best support its acquisition and development.

Replication package

The data and analysis code for all models is available at https://osf.io/wx2q9/.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716424000043

Competing interests

We have no known conflict of interest to disclose.

Footnotes

1 While this study focuses on development in English, it is important to note that languages differ in their morphological structure, and the developmental progression may differ across languages. See Duncan (Reference Duncan, Berthiaume, Daigle and Desrochers2018) for a cross-linguistic review on morphology. See also Diamanti et al. (Reference Diamanti, Benaki, Mouzaki, Ralli, Antoniou, Papaioannou and Protopapas2018) for evidence of the later development of derivational morphology in Greek, an orthographically transparent language, Ben-Zvi and Levie (Reference Ben-Zvi, Levie and Berman2016) for evidence in Hebrew, a morphologically rich language, and Ku and Anderson (Reference Ku and Anderson2003) for evidence in Chinese, a non-alphabetic language.

2 Although previous studies have examined the understanding of derived words using an oral task (e.g., Carlisle, Reference Carlisle2000), we are primarily interested in studies using written tasks. Further, we acknowledge that the study by James et al. (Reference James, Currie, Tong and Cain2021) used a written derivational morphology task. However, their task assessed morphological awareness rather than morphological semantic and/or orthographic knowledge.

3 Summed frequency of all members in the morphological family of a morpheme. For example, the frequency of the suffix -ance would be the sum of the frequency of the words that contain this suffix (e.g., attendance, pleasance, appearance). The frequency count used for this calculation was the HAL frequency provided in the English Lexicon Project.

References

Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Apel, K., Diehm, E., & Apel, L. (2013). Using multiple measures of morphological awareness to assess its relation to reading. Topics in Language Disorders, 33(1), 4256. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0b013e318280f57b CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Apel, K., & Henbest, V. S. (2016). Affix meaning knowledge in first through third grade students. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 47(2), 148156. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_LSHSS-15-0050 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apel, K., Henbest, V. S., & Petscher, Y. (2022). Morphological awareness performance profiles of first- through sixth-grade students. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 65(3), 10701086. https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00282 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Nelson, D. L., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The English Lexicon Project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445459. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barton, K. (2019). MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference. R package version 1.47.1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MuMIn Google Scholar
Ben-Zvi, G., & Levie, R. (2016). Development of Hebrew derivational morphology from preschool to adolescence. In Berman, R. A. (Ed.), Acquisition and development of Hebrew: From infancy to adolescence (pp. 135173). John Benjamins Publishing Company.10.1075/tilar.19.05benCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berko, J. (1958) The child’s learning of English morphology. WORD, 14(2–3), 150177. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1958.11659661 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Nagy, W., & Carlisle, J. (2010). Growth in phonological, orthographic, and morphological awareness in grades 1 to 6. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39(2), 141163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-009-9130-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beyersmann, E., Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2012). Morphological processing during visual word recognition in developing readers: Evidence from masked priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(7), 13061326. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.656661 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biber, D., Conrad, S., & Reppen, R. (1998). Corpus linguistics: Investigating language structure and language use. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511804489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borleffs, E., Maassen, B. A. M., Lyytinen, H., & Zwarts, F. (2017). Measuring orthographic transparency and morphological-syllabic complexity in alphabetic orthographies: A narrative review. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 30(8), 16171638. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9741-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burani, C., Marcolini, S., Traficante, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2018). Reading derived words by Italian children with and without dyslexia: The effect of root length. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 647. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00647 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12, 169190. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008131926604 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2007). Fostering morphological processing, vocabulary development, and reading comprehension. In Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E., & Tannenbaum, K. R. (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 78103). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F., & Katz, L. A. (2006). Effects of word and morpheme familiarity on reading of derived words. Reading and Writing, 19, 669693. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-5766-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511554377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colenbrander, D. (2015). Understanding the role of oral vocabulary in reading comprehension difficulties. [Doctoral dissertation, Macquarie University]. https://figshare.mq.edu.au/articles/thesis/Understanding_the_role_of_oral_vocabulary_in_reading_comprehension_difficulties/19438805/1 Google Scholar
Dawson, N., Hsiao, Y., Tan, A., Banerji, N., & Nation, K. (2023). Effects of target age and genre on morphological complexity in children’s reading material. Scientific Studies of Reading. Advanced online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2023.2206574 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, N., Rastle, K., & Ricketts, J. (2018). Morphological effects in visual word recognition: Children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(4), 645654. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000485 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, N., Rastle, K., & Ricketts, J. (2021). Finding the man amongst many: A developmental perspective on mechanisms of morphological decomposition. Cognition, 211, 104605. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104605 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquarella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grades 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49(6), 11131126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, S. H., Kieffer, M. J., & Laroche, A. (2014). The relation between morphological awareness and reading comprehension: Evidence from mediation and longitudinal models. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 432451. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.926907 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamanti, V., Benaki, A., Mouzaki, A., Ralli, A., Antoniou, F., Papaioannou, S., & Protopapas, A. (2018). Development of early morphological awareness in Greek: Epilinguistic versus metalinguistic and inflectional versus derivational awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(3), 545567. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000522 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Sandra, D., & Grainger, J. (2005). Masked cross-modal morphological priming: Unravelling morpho-orthographic and morpho-semantic influences in early word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20(1–2), 75114. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960444000197 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, L. G. (2018). A cross-linguistic analysis of the role of morphology in reading development. In Berthiaume, R., Daigle, D., & Desrochers, A. (Eds.), Morphological processing and literacy development: current issues and research (pp. 133148). Routledge.10.4324/9781315229140-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ford, M. A., Davis, M. H., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2010). Derivational morphology and base morpheme frequency. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 117130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.01.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaustad, M., Ronald, K., Payne, J.-A., & Lylak, E. (2002). Deaf and hearing students’ morphological knowledge applied to printed English. American Annals of the Deaf, 147(5), 521.https://doi.org/10.1353/aad.2012.0264 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, A., Petscher, Y., & Tock, J. (2021). Multidimensional morphological assessment for middle school students. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 7089. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12335 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, A. P., Gilbert, J. K., Cho, S.-J., & Kearns, D. M. (2014). Probing lexical representations: Simultaneous modeling of word and reader contributions to multidimensional lexical representations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(2), 448468. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034754 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, A. P., Petscher, Y., Carlisle, J. F., & Mitchell, A. M. (2017). Exploring the dimensionality of morphological knowledge for adolescent readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 40(1), 91117. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12064 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, Article 54. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haase, A., & Steinbrink, C. (2022). Associations between morphological awareness and literacy skills in German primary school children: the roles of grade level, phonological processing and vocabulary. Reading and Writing, 35, 16751709. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-021-10247-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inoue, T., Georgiou, G. K., & Parrila, R. (2023). The growth trajectories of morphological awareness and its predictors. Applied Psycholinguistics. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716423000218 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, E., Currie, N. K., Tong, S. X., & Cain, K. (2021). The relations between morphological awareness and reading comprehension in beginner readers to young adolescents. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 110130. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12316 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kearns, D., & Hiebert, E. (2022). The word complexity of primary-level texts: Differences between first and third grade in widely used curricula. Reading Research Quarterly, 57(1), 255285. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.429 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kristensen, J. K., Andersson, B., Bratlie, S. S., & Torkildsen, J. V. K. (2023). Dimensionality of morphological knowledge—Evidence from Norwegian third graders. Reading Research Quarterly. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.497 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ku, Y-M., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading and Writing, 16, 399442. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024227231216 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, L.-J., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41(3), 161180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen, J. A., & Nippold, M. A. (2007). Derivational morphology. In Nippold, M. A. (Ed.), Later language development: School-age children, adolescents, and young adults (3rd ed., pp. 4972). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021). How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 1026. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levesque, K. C., Kieffer, M. J., & Deacon, S. H. (2017). Morphological awareness and reading comprehension: Examining mediating factors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 160, 120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marinellie, S. A., & Kneile, L. A. (2012). Acquiring knowledge of derived nominals and derived adjectives in context. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 43(1), 5365. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0053) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maynard, C., Brissaud, C., & Armand, F. (2018). The acquisition of inflectional morphology in children. In Berthiaume, R., Daigle, D., & Desrochers, A. (Eds.), Morphological processing and literacy development: Current issues and research (pp. 217243). Routledge.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Tardif, T., Cho, J.-R., Shu, H., Fletcher, P., Stokes, S. F., Wong, A., & Leung, K. (2008). What’s in a word? Morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge in three languages. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(3), 437462. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640808020X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, A. M., & Brady, S. A. (2014). Assessing affix knowledge using both pseudoword and real-word measures. Topics in Language Disorders, 34(3), 210227. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000020 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W. E., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and at-risk fourth-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 730742. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.730 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakagawa, S., & Schielzeth, H. (2013). A general and simple method for obtaining R 2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 4, 133142. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nippold, M. (2018). The literate lexicon in adolescents: Monitoring the use and understanding of morphologically complex words. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 3, 211221. https://doi.org/10.1044/persp3.SIG1.211 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nippold, M., & Sun, L. (2008). Knowledge of morphologically complex words: a developmental study of older children and young adolescents. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39(3), 365373. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/034) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (2006). Improving literacy by teaching morpheme. Routledge.10.4324/9780203969557CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orelien, J. G., & Edwards, L. J. (2008). Fixed-effect variable selection in linear mixed models using R2 statistics. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 52, 18961907. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.csda.2007.06.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pacheco, M. B., & Goodwin, A. P. (2013). Putting two and two together: Middle school students’ morphological problem-solving strategies for unknown words. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 56, 541553. https://doi.org/10.1002/JAAL.181 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quémart, P., & Casalis, S. (2014). Effects of phonological and orthographic shifts on children’s processing of written morphology: A time-course study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(5), 363382. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.912218 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramirez, G., Walton, P., & Roberts, W. (2014). Morphological awareness and vocabulary development among kindergarteners with different ability levels. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 5464. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509970 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rastle, K. (2019). The place of morphology in learning to read in English. Cortex, 116, 4554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.02.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (2000). Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time-course study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(4–5), 507537. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960050119689 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., & New, B. (2004). The broth in my brother’s brothel: morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11(6), 10901098. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196742 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
RStudio Team (2020). RStudio: Integrated development for R . RStudio, PBC. https://www.rstudio.com/ Google Scholar
Sánchez-Gutiérrez, C. H., Mailhot, H., Deacon, S. H., & Wilson, M. A. (2018). MorphoLex: A derivational morphological database for 70,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 15681580. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0981-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strik-Lievers, F., Bolognesi, M., & Winter, B. (2021). The linguistic dimensions of concrete and abstract concepts: lexical category, morphological structure, countability, and etymology. Cognitive Linguistics, 32(4), 641670. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2021-0007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2012). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Pearson.Google Scholar
Ursachi, G., Horodnic, I. A., & Zait, A. (2015). How reliable are measurement scales? External factors with indirect influence on reliability estimators. Procedia Economics and Finance, 20, 679686. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2212-5671(15)00123-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venables, W. N., & Ripley, B. D. (2002). Modern Applied Statistics with S. Springer.10.1007/978-0-387-21706-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venezky, R. (1967). English orthography: Its graphical structure in relation to sound. Reading Research Quarterly, 2(3), 75105. https://doi.org/10.2307/747031 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2005). Wechsler Individual Achievement Test 2 (WIAT-2). Pearson.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S., & Robertson, G. J. (2017). Wide Range Achievement Test 5 (WRAT-5). Pearson.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the measures used in the study

Figure 1

Table 2. Correlations between the variables for each grade

Figure 2

Table 3. Results of the best-fitting model for SIT-N

Figure 3

Table 4. Results of the best-fitting model for SMT-N

Figure 4

Figure 1. Performance levels of each grade on the SIT-N task.Note. The plots are a combination of violin plots and box plots. Violin plots show the density distribution of the proportion correct, and box plots show the median, the interquartile range, and 1.5 times the interquartile range.

Figure 5

Figure 2. Performance levels of each grade on the SMT-N task.Note. The plots are a combination of violin plots and box plots. Violin plots show the density distribution of the proportion correct, and box plots show the median, the interquartile range, and 1.5 times the interquartile range.

Supplementary material: File

Martinez et al. supplementary material

Martinez et al. supplementary material
Download Martinez et al. supplementary material(File)
File 38.7 KB