Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T06:44:57.933Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Productivity and the acquisition of gender

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2021

Sigríður Mjöll BJÖRNSDÓTTIR*
Affiliation:
The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø
*
Address for correspondence: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Children's differing learning trajectories cross-linguistically have been at the forefront of gender acquisition research, often with conflicting results and conclusions. As a result, the source of children's different learning behaviors in gender acquisition has been unclear. I argue that children's gender acquisition is driven by the search for productive patterns. First, I provide corpus studies where the predictions of a learning model (Yang, 2016) are formulated. Second, I report the results of an elicited production task on Icelandic-speaking children (N = 26, ages 2;6-6;3 years) and adults (N = 18) that puts these predictions to test. The results suggest that Icelandic-speaking children and adults draw a categorical distinction between productive and unproductive suffixes in Icelandic gender assignment. I discuss the implications of these findings for morphological learning beyond gender acquisition.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re- use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Grammatical gender has conventionally been defined as the sorting of nouns into classes as reflected in agreement morphology (Corbett, Reference Corbett1991; Hockett, Reference Hockett1958). Gender systems differ cross-linguistically with respect to what kind of information is predictive of gender assignment. A distinction has been made between strict semantic systems, as exemplified by the gender systems of the Dravidian languages, and formal systems, as exemplified by typologically diverse languages, such as Qafar and Russian (Corbett, Reference Corbett, Dryer and Haspelmath2013). Given the typological diversity of gender systems, children must be able to detect a wide range of formal and semantic regularities on the basis of language-specific data.

In her seminal study, Karmiloff-Smith (Reference Karmiloff-Smith1979) showed that French children were able to assign gender on the basis of noun endings. Moreover, the children seemed to rely on noun endings even if the resulting gender were at odds with the biological sex of the referent. Similar results have been obtained many times cross-linguistically (Clark, Reference Clark and Slobin1985; Hernández-Pina, Reference Hernández-Pina1984; Levy, Reference Levy1983; Mills, Reference Mills1986; Rodina & Westergaard, Reference Rodina and Westergaard2012; Reference Rodina and Westergaard2013; Reference Rodina and Westergaard2015). Collectively, the results of this body of research suggest that children can learn gender systems that are detached from any semantic motivation. However, research on more typologically diverse gender systems is needed in order to determine whether this early formal bias is an artifact of the language sample or a finding about early grammatical representation.

Children's learning trajectories of grammatical gender vary cross-linguistically (Mills, Reference Mills1986). Gender systems have been divided into two groups from an acquisitional perspective: Transparent and opaque (Slobin, Reference Slobin and Macnamara1977). Transparent gender systems have a set of productive patterns for gender assignment, whereas opaque gender systems have few or none. Productive rules in transparent systems, such as Spanish and Russian, are typically in place by the age of three (Lew-Williams & Fernald, Reference Lew-Williams and Fernald2007; Rodina & Westergaard, Reference Rodina and Westergaard2012), whereas the paucity of such rules in opaque systems, like Norwegian and Dutch, results in late mastery (Rodina & Westergaard, Reference Rodina and Westergaard2013; Unsworth & Hulk, Reference Unsworth and Hulk2010). Transparent or opaque, gender acquisition involves detecting language-specific patterns and evaluating whether they are useful for learning or not. In other words, the child learner must somehow outweigh the evidence for and against a pattern in order to determine whether or not it can be used to form a generalization about gender assignment.

Even within a transparent gender system, gender assignment rulesFootnote 1 may be learned at different rates. Mills (Reference Mills1986) proposed, using evidence from German, that gender assignment rules were acquired in order of clarity. By her definition, clarity is determined by the scope of the rule and the number of exceptions; the greater the scope of the rule and the fewer exceptions, the earlier the rule is acquired. For example, she argued that the rule with the greatest scope in German is “nouns that end in –e are feminine” because of the high frequency of the pattern and the low number of exceptions (p. 85). However, even the role of frequency has been debated. For instance, Henzl (Reference Henzl1975) argued, using evidence from Czech, that children first formulated gender assignment rules on the basis of noun endings which are “least ambiguous”, irrespective of frequency.

Hitherto it has been unclear what makes a gender system either transparent or opaque to the child learner. In parallel, it has been unclear how the child learner can determine the scope of a gender assignment pattern. Therefore, a theory of gender acquisition is needed that can both identify the conditions under which a gender assignment pattern is useful to the learner – and when these conditions are not met.

In this paper, I propose an approach whereby gender acquisition is characterized by a search for productive gender assignment rules guided by a learning model (Yang, Reference Yang2005; Reference Yang2016). First, I discuss prior research on productivity in first language acquisition. Second, I introduce the Tolerance Principle, a quantitative model of productivity (Yang, Reference Yang2005; Reference Yang2016). I discuss the relevance of quantitative methods for research on gender acquisition and demonstrate how the approach works using grammatical gender in Spanish as a test case. Next, I show how predictions for Icelandic gender acquisition can be made on the basis of child-directed speech and child naturalistic data. Moreover, I show how these predictions robustly hold when samples are created from other corpora to approximate children's vocabulary size during the stages of gender acquisition. Subsequently, I present the results of an elicited production task on Icelandic children and adults. Finally, I discuss an alternative view of productivity (Baayen, Reference Baayen1989; Reference Baayen, Booij and van Marle1992; Reference Baayen, Booij and van Marle1993) and evaluate its predictions against the empirical results. The paper concludes with a discussion of the implications of these findings for morphological learning beyond gender acquisition.

Productivity and absence thereof in language acquisition

Language acquisition involves learning words and how to inflect them. The source of children's ability to learn inflectional patterns has been a point of contention for theories of morphological learning. In her famous Wug experiments, Berko (Reference Berko1958) showed convincingly that English-speaking children extend productive inflectional patterns like, for example, the plural suffix –s, when inflecting novel words. Children have also been found to over-generalize productive patterns in naturalistic settings even though this may result in forms that are not attested in the input, such as *foots and *breaked (Pinker & Prince, Reference Pinker, Prince, Lima, Corrigan and Iverson1994). Children's ability to extend productive patterns in both experimental and naturalistic settings has been taken as evidence for rule-based learning in acquisition.

However, sometimes productivity fails. Gaps within an inflectional paradigm are the result of having no acceptable morphological option or default (Baronian & Kulinich, Reference Baronian and Kulinich2012; Halle, Reference Halle1973; Fanselow & Féry, Reference Fanselow, Féry, Fanselow and Féry2002; Orgun & Sprouse, Reference Orgun and Sprouse1999; Pertsova, Reference Pertsova, Heinz, Martin and Pertsova2005). Morphological gaps are common cross-linguistically. For instance, many English speakers find the past participles of certain irregular verbs, like stride, problematic (Pinker, 1999). Similarly, there are no acceptable 1SG forms for a handful of verbs in Spanish (Albright, Reference Albright, Garding and Tsujimura2003). There are no semantic reasons for this ineffability. Rather, it seems to reflect speakers’ failure to generate a systematic pattern or a rule. Morphological gaps have posed a challenge to rule-based accounts, as the unavailability of a rule or a default form is unexpected.

The learning trajectory of Polish noun inflection suggests that children do not need to resort to defaults in order to learn inflectional morphology (Dabrowska, Reference Dabrowska2001; Reference Dabrowska2005). Polish nouns are inflected for gender, case and number. The most important factor in determining the choice of inflectional ending is gender (Dabrowska, Reference Dabrowska2001, p. 558). The most interesting case is the choice of ending for masculine genitive singular nouns: masculine singular nouns in Polish can take either –a or –u as a genitive ending in a seemingly unpredictable fashion. While –a is the most frequent masculine genitive singular ending, it does not seem to have the status of a default, since loanwords and low frequency masculine singulars can take either ending.

In a series of longitudinal corpus case studies, Dabrowska (Reference Dabrowska2001) showed that Polish noun inflection was largely in place by the age of 2;0. Furthermore, Polish-speaking children made few errors with masculine genitive singular nouns in spite of the arbitrary distribution of the two endings. In case of errors, children made unsystematic choices of either ending.

These findings have been taken as evidence against rule-based learning (Clahsen, Reference Clahsen1999; Pinker, 1999). Instead, Dabrowska (Reference Dabrowska2001, Reference Dabrowska2005) argued that they lent support to USAGE-BASED approaches to language acquisition (Tomasello, Reference Tomasello1992; Reference Tomasello2003). Hence, the absence of productivity has raised key questions about the nature of the mechanism underlying linguistic creativity.

Predicting productivity and absence thereof

The Tolerance Principle

There is general agreement that language has both productive and unproductive patterns. However, the division line between the two has been a point of contention. Yang (Reference Yang2005; 2016) has proposed a model of linguistic productivity, the Tolerance Principle, to account for how children distinguish between productive and unproductive patterns on the basis of positive evidence in the input. The Tolerance Principle quantifies the precise conditions for productive rule formation. The model hypothesizes that a general rule will be formed when doing so is computationally more efficient than storing lexical forms. The principle is stated in (1).

(1) The Tolerance Principle

If R is a productive rule applicable to N candidates, then the following relation holds between N and e, the number of exceptions that could but do not follow R:

$${\rm e} \le {\rm \theta N\ where \ \theta N} = {\rm N/lnN}$$

The Tolerance Principle states that it is computationally more efficient to form a productive rule only when the number of exceptions is less than the number of items divided by the natural log of the number of items. Computational efficiency is computed by calculating the time complexity required for forming a rule with the time complexity required for accessing individual lexical forms. Crucially, the division between productive and unproductive processes is a categorical one on this approach.

The Tolerance Principle makes use of the Elsewhere Condition (Kiparsky, Reference Kiparsky, Anderson R and Kiparsky1973), which states that when a more specific form (or rule) is available, it is preferred over a more general one. For example, went is the past tense form for the verb go, so it overrides the regular but ungrammatical *goed. The Elsewhere Condition is implemented by the Tolerance Principle as a serial search procedure, which is empirically motivated by research on language processing (see Yang, Reference Yang2016, pp. 49–60).

To illustrate this serial procedure, one can think of past tense acquisition in English. The child is faced with verbs that adhere to the regular pattern, “add -d”, and verbs that do not. The Tolerance Principle assumes that, in order to be maximally efficient in forming the past tense of verbs in English, the child is faced with two options: 1) Store all past tense verb forms individually 2) Form a productive rule. In the first case scenario, every item is stored in a list ranked by frequency. This means that the learner must search the list every time there is an occasion to express the past tense of a verb. In the second case scenario, only the exceptions are stored in a frequency-ranked list. The list of exceptions must be searched first before the productive rule can be applied.

The Tolerance Principle operates on type counts. Therefore, productivity in grammar learning on this approach is connected to the number of types over which linguistic patterns are expressed, rather than the number of tokens. The same view has been adopted by a wide variety of research programs (Aronoff, Reference Aronoff1976; Baayen, Reference Baayen, Booij and van Marle1993; Bybee, Reference Bybee1985; Plunkett & Marchman, Reference Plunkett and Marchman1991).

Given a well-defined hypothesis space, the Tolerance Principle can be used as a quantitative measure to predict whether any given linguistic pattern can be perceived by the child learner as productive or not. The Tolerance Principle is just one thresholding function and has a wide range of empirical support (consult Yang, Reference Yang2016 for case studies). In addition, the predictions of the Tolerance Principle have been borne out for children in experimental settings (Schuler et al., Reference Schuler, Yang and Newport2016).

Language acquisition involves not only detecting productive patterns, but also unproductive patterns. The Tolerance Principle not only models the conditions for productive rule formation; it can also identify conditions under which no productive rule is available (Gorman & Yang, Reference Gorman, Yang, Rainer, Gardani, Luschützky and Dressler2018). For example, the Tolerance Principle can predict the absence of a default genitive ending for Polish masculine singulars on a numerical basis. Table 1 shows the numerical distribution of Polish masculine genitive singular nouns by ending (adapted from Yang, Reference Yang2016, based on CHILDES).

Table 1. Numerical Distribution of Genitive Endings for Masculine Singular Nouns in Polish

An analysis using the Tolerance Principle revealed that in spite of the statistical majority of –a as the genitive ending of masculine singulars, the number of nouns that take the alternative ending is too great for –a to be productive. On this approach, therefore, absence of productivity does not constitute as evidence against rule-based learning. Rather, it is the direct consequence of a learning process guided by a search for productivity that fails to succeed and results in rote memorization.

Relevance to gender acquisition

Approaches using quantificational methods have the advantage of being able to make clear, testable predictions on the basis of input data. In this section, I will briefly showcase how the present approach works using the Spanish gender system as an example.

The Spanish gender system distinguishes between masculine and feminine nouns. There are correlations between nominal morphology and gender assignment: Nouns that take the suffix –o tend to be masculine, whereas nouns that take the suffix –a tend to be feminine. In an eye-tracking study, Lew-Williams and Fernald (Reference Lew-Williams and Fernald2007) showed that Spanish-learning children, aged 2;10–3;6 years, were able to use gender-marked articles to establish reference of such nouns. Thus, young Spanish-learning children had internalized productive gender assignment rules in spite of an estimated vocabulary of only 500 words.

The distribution of noun types across gender and suffix in a longitudinal corpus of Spanish child-directed speech (Linaza et al., Reference Linaza, Sebastián and del Barrio1981) is provided below in Table 2. The corpus reflects the interaction between a caregiver and their child between the ages of two and four. Therefore, it should give a reasonable estimate of a child's vocabulary size in Spanish gender acquisition.

Table 2. Numerical Distribution of Noun Types by Gender and Suffix in Spanish Child-Directed Speech

An analysis using the Tolerance Principle confirmed the productivity of –o to masculine and –a to feminine. In the absence of a suffix, the Tolerance Principle predicted masculine to be the default gender in Spanish.

These predictions are consistent with studies on Spanish gender acquisition in both naturalistic and experimental settings: Children generalize masculine to nouns with the suffix –o and feminine to nouns with the suffix –a. In the absence of a productive suffix, they resort to the default gender: namely, masculine (see, among many, Clark, Reference Clark and Slobin1985; Hernández-Pina, Reference Hernández-Pina1984; Mariscal, Reference Mariscal2008; Pérez-Pereira, Reference Pérez-Pereira1991).

The Icelandic gender system

Icelandic has a gender system that distinguishes between masculine, feminine and neuter. Typologically, the Icelandic gender system has been classified as formal (Corbett, Reference Corbett, Dryer and Haspelmath2013). Icelandic has rich agreement morphology that manifests itself on the definite article, which is a suffix (2a), adjectives (2b), the past participle (2c) and pronouns (2d). Anaphoric pronouns must refer to the formal gender of the referent noun irrespective of animacy or biological sex.

  • (2)

    1. a. Stóll-inn, skál-in, borð-.

      Chair-m.def, bowl-f.def table-n.def

      ‘The chair, the bowl, the table.’

    2. b. Flott-ur stóll, flott-ø skál, flott-ø borð.

      Nice-m chair-m, Nice-f bowl-f, nice-n table-n

      ‘A nice chair, a nice bowl, a nice table.’

    3. c. Stóllinn er brot-inn, skálin er brot-in,

      The chair-m is broken-m, the bowl-f is broken-f,

      borðið er brot-.

      the table-n is broken-n

      ‘The chair is broken, the bowl is broken, the table is broken.’

    4. d. Hann er brotinn, hún er brotin, það er brotið.

      He is broken, she is broken, it is broken.

      ‘He (the chair) is broken, she (the bowl) is broken, it (the table) is broken.’

The three genders are roughly equally frequent: 32% are masculine, 38% feminine and 30% are neuter (Helgadóttir et al., Reference Helgadóttir2010). These numbers are consistent with the input corpora that will be examined later in the paper.

In addition to gender, Icelandic distinguishes between four cases: Nominative, accusative, dative and genitive. Gender and inflection in Icelandic interact to form inflection classes, which are standardly defined as a set of roots that each share the same set of inflectional realizations (Aronoff, Reference Aronoff1994).

Icelandic reference grammars (see e.g., Kvaran, Reference Kvaran2005) have standardly followed the lead of Old Norse reference grammars (Iversen, Reference Iversen1922; Noreen, Reference Noreen1903) by stating the correspondence between gender and inflection without discussing specific gender assignment rules. The idea is that the gender of a noun can be determined by its inflection class membership to some extent.

Nominative singular is the most frequent inflectional form in Icelandic, constituting 40% of all nominal forms (Helgadóttir et al., Reference Helgadóttir2010). Furthermore, due to syncretism in the nominal paradigm, many forms are identical to the nominative singular in oblique cases. There are strong correlations between nominative singular morphology and gender assignment in Icelandic as in other fusional languages like, for example, German and Russian (Corbett, Reference Corbett1991). In particular, three nominative singular suffixes are predictive of either masculine or feminine, respectively.Footnote 2

  • (3)

    1. a. Nouns that take the nominative singular suffix –r are typically masculine.Footnote 3

    2. b. Nouns that take the nominative singular suffix –i are typically masculine.

    3. c. Nouns that take the nominative singular suffix –a are typically feminine.

Table 3 demonstrates how these suffixes map on to real nouns in Icelandic.

Table 3. Mappings between Gender and Nominative Singular Suffixes in Icelandic

While these patterns are robust in Icelandic, they do have exceptions. For instance, some feminine nouns take the nominative singular suffix –r. Diachronically, most of these nouns have shifted to masculine (Iversen, Reference Iversen1922; Noreen, Reference Noreen1903).

The absence of an overt nominative singular suffix is indicated by -ø. Some nouns do not take the phonemes in Table 3 by suffixation. Instead, they form part of the noun‘s stem, as shown in (4). These nouns tend to have low type but high token frequency. Most of these nouns are neuter, although nouns with stem-final /i/ can be either feminine or neuter (4b).

  • (4)

    1. a. Auga-ø, eyra-ø.

      Eye-n.nom.sg, ear-n.nom.sg

      ‘An eye, an ear.’

    2. b. Tæki-ø, gleði-ø.

      Device-n.nom.sg, joy-f.nom.sg

      ‘A device, joy.’

    3. c. Ber-ø, ker-ø.

      Berry-n.nom.sg, tub-n.nom.sg

      ‘A berry, a tub.’

While these nouns have oblique forms different from nouns that take these sounds by suffixation, they could be ambiguous to the child learner in gender acquisition given the statistical dominance of nominative singular forms in the input. Therefore, these nouns are counted as exceptions to the general patterns stated in (3) in subsequent quantitative analyses.

The choice of nominative singular suffix is a result of morphological, rather than phonological selection. The same root may select for more than one suffix to yield a minimal pair as in (5a). Some borrowed nouns show variation in the choice of suffix, which in turn affects gender assignment (cf. 5b-c).

  • (5)

    1. a. Sæt-i, sæt-a.

      Cutie-m, cutie-f

      ‘Male cutie, female cutie.’

    2. b. Djóku-r, Djók-ø.

      Joke-m, joke-n

      ‘A joke.’

    3. c. lúpp-a, lúpp-ø.

      loop-f, loop-n

      ‘A loop.’

There is no productive nominative singular suffix for neuter nouns. The stem-final segment of neuter nouns can consist of any phonotactically legal consonant or a vowel (see above). There are no clear phonological patterns specific to neuter. For instance, many neuter monosyllabic nouns rhyme with feminine monosyllabic nouns.

  • (6)

    1. a. Borg-ø, torg-ø.

      city-f, square-n

      ‘A city, a square.’

    2. b. Ull-ø, gull-ø,

      wool-f, gold-n

      ‘Wool, gold.’

Neuter has standardly been assumed to be the default gender in Icelandic (Steinmetz, Reference Steinmetz and Faarlund1985). This assumption will be challenged later in this paper.Footnote 4

Most nouns in Icelandic are assigned only one gender. In case of variation in gender assignment, however, nouns that lack an overt nominative singular suffix are the primary targets. These nouns have also undergone gender shifts diachronically (Noreen, Reference Noreen1903; Iversen, Reference Iversen1922). The attested variation seems arbitrary. Similarly, there is both inter-speaker and intra-speaker variation in the gender assignment of some borrowed nouns in Icelandic. Thus, while the choice of nominative singular suffix clearly determines the gender of both jeppi and paranója, the absence of such a suffix seems to correlate with variation in gender assignment, as shown in Table 4.

Table 4. Gender Assignment of Borrowed Nouns in Icelandic

To conclude this section; given the statistical dominance of nominative singular morphology, it seems plausible to assume that Icelandic children learn these inflectional patterns early and use them as base forms in gender acquisition.

Gender acquisition in icelandic: a longitudinal corpus case study

Data

The data consist of longitudinal recordings of a caregiver's speech to an Icelandic-speaking child and the child's spontaneous speech in response (Sigurjónsdóttir, Reference Slobin and Macnamara2007). A total of 82 recordings were made approximately once a month when the child was between the ages of 1;6–4;3 years. The child-directed speech contained around half a million tokens; whereas the child's spontaneous speech contained around 7000 tokens.

Procedure

Nominative singular noun types were extracted from the corpus and tagged for gender and suffix. Child and adult data were analyzed separately. The purpose of the child analysis was to test whether the same predictions could be made on the basis of the child's vocabulary. Both child and adult data were subjected to a quantitative analysis using the Tolerance Principle. In addition, the child naturalistic data was subjected to an error analysis.

Analysis of child-directed speech

The caregiver's speech contained 478 nominative singular noun types, which constituted approximately 41% of all noun types that were produced. Their numerical distribution by gender and suffix is provided in Table 5. Token numbers are given in brackets.

Table 5. Numerical Distribution of Nominative Singular Noun Types in Icelandic Child- Directed Speech

Both nominative singular suffixes –r and –i were predicted to be productive of masculine by the Tolerance Principle, as the number of non-masculine nouns with these suffixes was below the exception threshold (θN). Likewise, –a was predicted to be productive of feminine.

In the absence of a nominative singular suffix, however, no gender was predicted to be productive. Thus, in spite of the statistical dominance of neuter within this category, the number of non-neuter nouns exceeded the exception threshold. As a result, Icelandic was predicted to lack a default gender in the absence of a productive nominative singular suffix.

Analysis of child naturalistic production

The child produced a total of 345 nominative singular noun types, which constituted approximately half of all noun types that were produced. Their numerical distribution by gender and suffix is provided in Table 6. Token numbers are given in brackets.

Table 6. Numerical Distribution of Nominative Singular Noun Types in Child Naturalistic Production

The same predictions were made on the basis of the child‘s spontaneous speech as on the child-directed speech, even if the child‘s production contained fewer noun types. The child was predicted to have internalized three productive rules of gender assignment in the absence of a default gender.

Error analysis of child naturalistic speech

The child was 100% target-consistent with nouns that take suffixes –r, –i and –a in the corpus. This means that the child had internalized the gender of these nouns before their second birthday. The child‘s non-target-consistent gender agreement exclusively targeted nouns that had no overt nominative singular suffix (–ø), with an error rate of 4.6%. The nouns affected alternated between all three genders. Examples of this are provided below in Table 7.

Table 7 Non-Target-Consistent Gender Agreement in Icelandic Child Naturalistic Production Child Production

The child's non-target consistent gender agreement did not suggest the application of a default gender. Rather, the pattern attested appeared unsystematic.

Corpora as an estimate of linguistic experience

Corpus data is a sample of linguistic experience. Any two sets of corpora are unlikely to contain the exact same linguistic items. This is analogous to child language acquisition; children's linguistic experience is inevitably variable.

So far, the corpus analyses in this paper have been based on small corpora. However, a small vocabulary is developmentally appropriate in the study of gender acquisition. Gender, in languages with productive gender assignment rules, is largely in place by the age of three when children typically know only a few hundred words (Hart & Risley, Reference Hart and Risley1995; Reference Hart and Risley2003; Szagun et al., Reference Szagun, Steinbrink, Franik and Stumper2006). The question is how children can converge on the target gender system on the basis of a vocabulary that is both small and variable from child to child.

One way to address this question is to study differences between corpora of different sizes and genres. Kodner (Reference Kodner2019) studied the differences between corpora derived from adult literary genres and child-directed speech in a series of case studies. He found that once adult literary corpora had been trimmed by frequency, they had statistically similar type counts to child-directed speech corpora in spite of lexical differences. In other words, the main difference between adult literary corpora and child-directed speech involved low frequency lexical items. One implication of these findings is that children's grammar learning may be based on high frequency lexical items, rather than adult-size lexicons.

In this section, predictions will be made using the Tolerance Principle on the basis of an adult online corpus. The objective is to establish whether the same predictions can be made when lexical items are drawn at random using a computer simulation model from a much larger language sample.

Furthermore, predictions will be formulated on the basis of the top few hundred most frequent noun types.

Data

The data consist of a corpus of 8.6 million tokens (https://github.com/hermitdave/FrequencyWords/blob/master/content/2018/is/is_full.txt) that were extracted from the SUBTLEX corpus (http://www.opensubtitles.org/). Corpora based on subtitles have been shown to be a good approximation of spoken languages (https://www.ugent.be/pp/experimentele-psychologie/en/research/documents/subtlexus).

Procedure

A computer simulation model was run on the corpus. The model was instructed to draw 500,000 noun tokens, to match the token size of the Icelandic child-directed speech corpus, at random and proportionally to word frequencies. Noun types that occurred less frequently than once per million words were excluded from the analysis. Nominative singular noun types were extracted from the sample and categorized by gender and suffix. They were then subjected to a quantitative analysis using the Tolerance Principle.

Results

563 nominative singular noun types were attested in a random sample of 500,000 words in the SUBTLEX corpus. Their numerical distribution by gender and suffix is provided in Table 8. Token numbers are given in brackets.

Table 8. Distribution of Noun Types by Gender and Suffix in the SUBTLEX Corpus

The Tolerance Principle made the same predictions based on the SUBTLEX corpus as on Icelandic child-directed speech (cf. Table 5) in spite of differences both in terms of lexical items and type counts.

Formulating predictions for small vocabularies

Table 9 shows the predictions of the Tolerance Principle on the basis of the top 100 and top 300 most frequent nominative singular noun types in the SUBTLEX corpus.

Table 9. Distribution of the most Frequent Noun Types in the SUBTLEX Corpus by Gender and Suffix

The Tolerance Principle made the same predictions as before, irrespective whether the analysis was based on the top 100 or top 300 most frequent noun types.

Discussion

Children‘s linguistic experience is inevitably variable: Children are unlikely to know the exact same words and their vocabulary sizes differ, even for children at the exact same age. In spite of lexical differences, however, children acquiring the same language are able to discover what the target grammar is.

The Tolerance Principle operates on types. As a consequence, what matters for learning is the number of lexical items that exhibit a specific property, rather than which exact lexical items those are. In this section, I have shown that, while the type counts of grammatical properties may differ from corpus to corpus, the predictions are the same. This is because the proportion of exceptions that go against a linguistic pattern relative to the types that conform to a linguistic pattern yields the same results, regardless of the exact number of types involved in the calculations.

Child-directed speech and adult corpora have been shown to converge on high frequency lexical items (Kodner, Reference Kodner2019). Therefore, it is plausible that children base their grammar learning mainly on high frequency lexical items. An analysis of the most frequent noun types in the SUBTLEX corpus using the Tolerance Principle predicted an early division between productive and unproductive suffixes in Icelandic gender assignment.

Experimental study

Participants

26 children (M = 4;5 years, SD = 1.33 years, age range = 2;9–6;3 years; 14 females, 12 males) and eighteen adult controls participated in this study. An additional four children participated, but were excluded from analysis due to failure to understand the task or unwillingness to engage with the game. Children were recruited from a day-care centre in suburban Reykjavík, where the study was conducted. Adult participants were recruited at the University of Iceland, Reykjavík. All participants were native speakers of Icelandic with normal hearing and normal to corrected-to-normal vision. No participant identified as bilingual/multilingual or reported to have a history of language delay.

Design

An elicited production task was designed with two conditions: Productive and Unproductive. In the Productive condition, participants were exposed to a novel noun with either suffix –r, –i or –a. In the Unproductive condition, participants were exposed to a novel noun, monosyllabic or disyllabic, that did not bear such a suffix.

Predictions

The Tolerance principle predicted that participants would make categorical suffix-based choice in gender assignment in the Productive condition, but arbitrary gender choices in the Unproductive condition.

Materials

28 nonce nouns were designed. The novel nouns all conformed to phonetic and phonological restrictions in Icelandic. To control for phonological neighbourhood density, the Phonological Corpus Tools software (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Blake, Fry, Mackie and McAuliffe2016) was used to check for minimal pairs with nouns included in Pind's (Reference Pind1991) frequency list of Icelandic. The stem-final segment of novel nouns in the Unproductive condition could be any consonant except /r/. The novel nouns are given in Table 10.

Table 10. Test Items by Nominative Singular Suffix

The novel nouns were paired with inanimate novel objects from the Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) database (Horst & Hout, Reference Horst and Hout2016). Figure 1 shows an example of a novel object used in the study:

Figure 1. A Novel Object at Exposure to Test

There were fourteen test items per condition. The test items were organized into seven trials. In each trial, the participant was presented with four test items, two for each condition, in a randomized order.

The test sentence served the purpose of a magical charm to be uttered by the participant in lieu of more traditional charms like ‘hocus pocus’. The construction induced gender agreement on the definite suffix and possessive pronominal, as shown for real nouns in (7):

  • (7)

    1. a. Hvar er hattu-r-inn minn?

      where is hat-m.def.sg my-m

      ‘Where is my hat?’

    2. b. Hvar er penn-i-nn minn?

      where is pen-m.def.sg my-m

      ‘Where is my pen?’

    3. c. Hvar er kann-a-n mín?

      where is mug-f.def.sg my-f

      ‘Where is my mug?’

    4. d. Hvar er egg-ið mitt?

      where is egg-n.def.sg my-n

      ‘Where is my egg?’

The construction was chosen in light of the fact that children acquiring Icelandic have been shown to comprehend and produce main clause wh-questions early. Moreover, wh-questions with where are among the earliest interrogative questions attested in Icelandic child language, with no reported erroneous use (Sigurjónsdóttir, Reference Sigurjónsdóttir1991).

Procedure

The task was embedded in an animated interactive movie that was played off a computer screen. The movie was designed using Animaker, an online animation video maker and was thirteen minutes long. Children and adults were tested individually in a quiet location at a day care center and at the University of Iceland.

The objective of the task was to help the movie's story protagonist obtain novel toys by magic. However, in order for the novel toys to come to be obtained, the participant had to be able to use the name of the novel toy in a sentence at test. The participant was shown a picture of the novel object and heard its name twice in syntactic contexts where the nominative singular is obligatory, as (8) demonstrates.

  • (8)

    1. a. Þetta er lerfur.

      this is lerfur-m.nom.sg.

      ‘This is a lerfur.’

    2. b. Vá! Lerfur!

      wow lerfur-m.nom.sg.

      ‘Wow! A lerfur!’

After the participant had produced the test sentence, the novel object appeared by magic as shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Magic at work in the Test Scene

Prior to test, there was a training session in which the participant observed the story protagonist either succeed or fail with the magic. The purpose of these scenes was to provide the participant with both positive and negative reinforcement. Subsequently, the participant was trained on three real nouns of each gender.

Results

Children

Children's behavior across the two conditions is summarized in Figure 3. Dots represent individual performance in each condition. Bars are standard error. Productive gender assignment in the Productive condition corresponds to mean systematic suffix-based choice of gender: Masculine for nouns with –r or –i, feminine for nouns with –a. In order to confirm the unproductivity of neuter in Icelandic, productive gender assignment in the Unproductive condition corresponds to mean neuter assignment.

Figure 3. Children: Gender Assignment across Conditions

Children made a categorical, suffix-based choice of either masculine or feminine in the Productive condition. They assigned masculine consistently to novel nouns with either suffix –r or –i (M = 0.99, SD = .037, SE = .007). Likewise, they assigned feminine consistently to novel nouns with the suffix –a (M = 0.98, SD = .04, SE = .009). The percentage of neuter assignment in the Productive condition was 2.35%, which is not statistically significant from zero. In the Unproductive condition, children did not make a systematic choice of neuter (M = 0.29, SD = 0.28, SE = .05). A paired t-test confirmed a significant difference between the means of the two conditions: t(25) =11.93, p < .001.

Figure 4 shows the distribution of children's responses in the Unproductive condition. Omission was defined as silence at test. Variable assignment was defined as the repetition of a test item twice, or more often, with variable gender agreement.

Figure 4. Children: Gender Assignment in the Unproductive Condition

Gender assignment in the Unproductive condition was characterized by a great deal of inter-and intra-speaker variation. Collectively, the children did not behave categorically in this condition, although six children did make categorical choices of gender. Nevertheless, these children were categorical in different ways: Three assigned feminine categorically or near-categorically, two assigned masculine categorically and one assigned neuter categorically.

A paired t-test revealed no significant difference between mean neuter assignment of monosyllabic and disyllabic nouns: t(24) =−0.52, p = 0.61. Figure 5 shows gender assignment of monosyllabic and disyllabic nouns in the Unproductive condition.

Figure 5. Children: Gender Assignment and Syllable Number in the Unproductive condition

In order to assess the relationship between age and neuter assignment, a simple regression analysis was conducted. The relationship is visualized in Figure 6. The result of the analysis showed no correlation between age and mean neuter assignment (r = .09).

Figure 6. Effect of Age on Neuter Assignment

Adults

Adults’ behavior across the two conditions is summarized in Figure 7. Dots represent individual performance in each condition. Bars are standard error. As before, productive gender assignment in the Productive condition corresponds to mean systematic suffix-based choice of gender: Masculine for nouns with –r or –i, feminine for nouns with –a. In order to confirm the unproductivity of neuter in Icelandic, productive gender assignment in the Unproductive condition corresponds to mean neuter assignment.

Figure 7. Adults: Gender Assignment across Conditions

Adults made a categorical, suffix-based choice of either masculine or feminine in the Productive condition. They assigned masculine at ceiling (100%) to novel nouns with either suffix –r or –i. Similarly, they assigned feminine consistently to novel nouns with the suffix –a (M = 0.99, SD = .03, SE = .009). Mean neuter assignment in the Unproductive condition was 48% (SD = 0.24, SE = .013). A paired t-test confirmed a significant difference between the two conditions: t(17) = 9.32, p < .001.

Figure 8 displays the distribution of adults’ responses in the Unproductive condition. Gender assignment in the Unproductive condition was characterized by inter-and intra-speaker variation. Collectively, adults did not behave categorically in this condition, although three chose consistently neuter.

Figure 8. Adults: Gender Assignment in the Unproductive Condition

A paired t-test showed no significant difference between mean neuter assignment of monosyllabic and disyllabic nouns: t(17) =−0.24, p = 0.81. Figure 9 shows the distribution of gender assignment by syllable number.

Figure 9. Adults: Gender Assignment and Syllable Number in the Unproductive Condition

Discussion

Overall, there were minimal differences between children's and adults’ behavior in the task. However, adults assigned neuter significantly more frequently than children, as measured by a Welch's t-test: t(31.54) = 2.39, p = .023. There was no effect of age on children's performance. This suggests that a categorical distinction between productive and unproductive suffixes in Icelandic gender assignment can be made before the age of three on the basis of lexical experience, as predicted by the Tolerance Principle.

An alternative view of productivity

Productivity: categorical or gradient?

The Tolerance Principle predicted a categorical division between productive and unproductive processes in Icelandic gender assignment. However, a body of research has argued for an alternative view of productivity. On this view, productivity should be viewed and measured as a gradient phenomenon (Hay & Baayen, Reference Hay and Baayen2005; McClelland & Bybee, Reference McClelland and Bybee2007). As a consequence, the difference between productive and unproductive patterns is not a categorical one and a pattern may be semi-productive.

A series of metrics to quantify morphological productivity at a scalar level have been proposed by Baayen and colleagues (Baayen, Reference Baayen1989; Reference Baayen, Booij and van Marle1992; Reference Baayen, Booij and van Marle1993). All of the metrics are centered around hapax legomena: namely, singleton words that appear precisely once in any given corpus. The general idea is that low token frequency should be a strong indication of productivity, given that lexicalized types in general have a higher token frequency than unlexicalized types.

The most studied metric proposed by Baayen and colleagues is P, which measures whether a given process is productive or not on the basis of token frequency. P is stated in (9), where n1 represents the number of singleton words that a process applies to and N is the sum of the token frequencies of these items.

  1. (9) N = n1/N

The primary goal of P is to give a statistical measure of the probability of encountering new types (Baayen, Reference Baayen, Booij and van Marle1993, p. 183). The larger the number of possible types, the more likely it is that they will not all occur in a given corpus or that some of them will occur only once.

A second metric, P*, compares one process against all other processes (Baayen, Reference Baayen, Booij and van Marle1993). P* is stated in (10), where N1 represents the total number of all singleton words that a process applies to.

  1. (10) P* = n1/N1

The primary goal of P* is to give a numerical estimate of the relative rate at which a category is expanding.

Baayen (Reference Baayen, Booij and van Marle1993, p. 194) proposed that P and P* should be viewed as two complementary measures; the primary use of P being to distinguish between productive and unproductive processes as such, while P* ranks proceses by degrees of productivity.Footnote 5

Baayen‘s P and P* metrics were not explicitly designed to account for learning. Nevertheless, they have clear implications for learning. A comparison of the predictions of the Tolerance Principle and Baayen‘s metrics contributes to the dispute whether morphological learning involves detecting categorical or gradient patterns. Therefore, the three data sets presented in this paper were subjected to quantitative analyses using Baayen‘s P and P* metrics and their predictions evaluated against the empirical results.

Analysis using Baayen‘s P and P* metrics

Both P and P* are gradient measures of productivity, whereas the results of the elicited production task suggest that both children and adults make a categorical distinction between productive and unproductive suffixes in Icelandic gender assignment. This does not necessarily invalidate P and P* as quantitative measures. For instance, it is conceivable that there exists some quantitative threshold value that can be used to define productivity or absence thereof. How to construct such a threshold is beyond the scope of this paper. However, in the analysis below, I demonstrate important inconsistencies of the two metrics and discuss what gives rise to them.

Table 11 provides the results of a quantitative analysis using Baayen's P and P* metrics on Icelandic child-directed speech (adult), child naturalistic speech (child) and the SUBTLEX corpus. The denominator of P was the total number of tokens that take a particular suffix. The denominator of P* was the sum of all singletons attested for each gender.

Table 11. Quantitative Analysis of Adult, Child and SUBTLEX Corpora

There were two major types of inconsistencies in the values of the measures. First, P yielded radically different values depending on the corpus size due to its reliance on token counts (see Bauer, Reference Bauer2001, p. 153 for similar concerns). As a result, productive suffixes could be assessed as less productive than unproductive suffixes. Bold font in Table 11 indicates values that predict the productivity of unproductive patterns.

P* ranked suffixes more accurately; i.e. –r and –i were predicted to be most productive of masculine and –a was predicted to correlate with high or semi-productivity of feminine. Still, the ranking of the productive suffixes was variable between the two corpora (e.g., the productivity of –r and –i to masculine). This is because the value of P* is dependent on type counts which may vary between suffixes from corpus to corpus. As a result, the prediction for gender acquisition is that children should treat these suffixes differently depending on their type counts. However, neither children nor adults made such a distinction between the three productive suffixes in the elicited production task. Instead, they made a categorical distinction between productive and unproductive suffixes which is unaccounted for on a gradient approach to productivity.

General discussion and conclusion

In this paper, I have presented an approach whereby gender acquisition is driven by a search for productive patterns. Prior accounts have proposed that transparency is predictive of children's behavior in gender acquisition. I argue that transparency is a direct reflection of productivity. As a consequence, I propose that the term transparency be replaced with productivity.

Typological research on gender systems has revealed a wide range of possible gender assignment patterns (Corbett, Reference Corbett1991; Reference Corbett, Dryer and Haspelmath2013). Therefore, a theory of gender acquisition is needed that can account for how children can detect any kind of gender assignment pattern; be it semantic, morphological or phonological.

The present theory offers a general approach to how children detect gender assignment patterns. I have shown how predictions can be made using corpora as an estimate of the child's lexical experience in gender acquisition. As a result, any generalization about gender assignment can be subjected to the kind of quantitative analysis, proposed here, to make testable predictions.

Prior accounts of learning have argued that children categorically follow patterns that are frequent in the input in either experimental or naturalistic settings (Hudson Kam & Newport, Reference Kam and & Newport2005; Reference Kam and & Newport2009; Newport, Reference Newport and Landau2019). However, a learning account must also be able to explain why children fail to generalize categorically on the basis of high frequency forms. Roughly a third of all noun tokens in Icelandic are neuter. Neuter nouns are also statistically dominant in the class of nouns that lack an overt nominative singular suffix. Still, neuter was not consistently chosen in the Unproductive condition. The unproductivity of neuter was predicted by the Tolerance Principle due to the number of masculine and feminine nouns of the same pattern.

Results from artificial language learning studies have shown that children tend to regularize linguistic patterns in the input data, even when these patterns show variability or inconsistencies (Hudson Kam & Newport, Reference Kam and & Newport2005; Reference Kam and & Newport2009). Thus, children do not merely reproduce the input statistics. However, the same studies found a different behavioral pattern for adults. Unlike children, adults matched the token frequencies of linguistic patterns instead of producing them in a categorical fashion.

Children and adults‘s response patterns in the present study were strikingly similar. The main difference involved the choice of neuter in the Unproductive condition, where adults used neuter significantly more often than children. This may suggest that some adult participants were trying to match the input statistics. Prior studies have shown that adults use irregular forms more often than children in experimental settings (see e.g., Berko, Reference Berko1958). The source of child and adult differences in experimental settings remains unclear. In the present study, however, differences were only apparent in the Unproductive condition.

The results of the present study suggest that learning involves forming type-driven generalizations. Many contrasting theoretical approaches have recognized the role of type frequency in productivity. However, the main point of contention has been the division line between productive and unproductive processes. For instance, Bybee's (Reference Bybee1985) Network model argues against a categorical division between productive and unproductive processes. Instead, the degrees of productivity of both productive and unproductive processes are determined by their token frequencies. As we have seen, such an approach makes inaccurate predictions with respect to Icelandic gender assignment. Baayen's approach is in the same gradient spirit and both types and tokens are made use of in his productivity calculations.

The empirical results presented in this paper do not support a gradient view of productivity: There were no differences in the degrees of productivity of the three suffixes in the Productive condition. In spite of statistical dominance, neuter was not consistently chosen in the Unproductive condition. Rather, the absence of a default gender manifested itself in inter-and intra-speaker variation. Hence, productivity resulted in categorical, uniform responses, whereas absence thereof resulted in inconsistency and differences in response patterns.

Footnotes

1 The term rule is used in an atheoretical way in this paper and is compatible with other related terms such as pattern, regularity or schema. On the present approach, rule formation is a consequence of a search for productive patterns in language acquisition. The author makes no commitment as to how rules discussed in this paper should be formulated or represented in theoretical terms.

2 There exist two other correlations between nominative singular forms and gender assignment in Icelandic. Namely, nouns that end in either –ing or –un are invariantly feminine. However, only five noun types with –ing and two with –un were encountered in a corpus of child-directed speech (Sigurjónsdóttir, Reference Sigurjónsdóttir2007). It is, therefore, a possibility that these patterns are not frequent enough to be detected by young children in gender acquisition.

3 The majority of nouns in this class have an /u/ inserted between the suffix –r and –i. This is standardly assumed to be the result of an epenthesis rule (Thráinsson, Reference Thráinsson, Bowern and Zanuttini2017). In other words, the epenthesis is a purely phonological process, independent of gender assignment: that is, triggered automatically under suffixation.

4 In linguistic research, default forms are expected when agreement is inert like, for instance, in the case of clausal subjects. However, it is at present unclear what role such forms play in the acquisition of gender assignment rules. For instance, Tsimpli and Hulk (Reference Tsimpli and Hulk2013) pointed out that children acquiring Dutch and Russian, over-generalize masculine despite that theoretically neuter has been claimed to be the default in both languages.

5 Baayen has proposed additional metrics to address some concerns raised by his critics, but discussing them specifically is beyond the scope of this paper. The later metrics introduced by Baayen all rest on the same theoretical assumptions.

References

Albright, A. (2003). A quantitative study of Spanish paradigm gaps. In Garding, G. & Tsujimura, M. (Eds.), WCCFL Proceedings, 22, 114.Google Scholar
Aronoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Aronoff, M. (1994). Morphology by itself: stems and inflectional classes. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baayen, H. (1989). A corpus-based approach to morphological productivity: statistical analysis and psycholinguistic interpretation (Doctoral dissertation).Google Scholar
Baayen, H. (1992). Quantitative aspects of morphological productivity. In Booij, G. & van Marle, J. (Eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 1991 (pp. 109149). Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, H. (1993). On frequency, transparency and productivity. In Booij, G. & van Marle, J. (Eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 1992 (pp. 181208). Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baronian, L., & Kulinich, E. (2012). Paradigm gaps in whole word morphology. Irregularity in morphology (and beyond). Studia typologica, 11, 81100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, L. (2001). Morphological productivity. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berko, J. (1958). The child's learning of English morphology. Word, 14, (2–3), 150177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (1985). Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form. John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H. (1999). Lexical entries and rules of language: a multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 9911069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. (1985). The acquisition of Romance, with special reference to French. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The cross- linguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 1. The data. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Corbett, G. (1991). Gender. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, G. G. (2013). Systems of gender assignment. In Dryer, M. S. & Haspelmath, M. (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.Google Scholar
Dabrowska, E. (2001). Learning a morphological system without a default: The Polish genitive. Journal of Child Language, 28(3), 545574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dabrowska, E. (2005). Productivity and beyond: mastering the Polish genitive inflection. Journal of Child Language, 32(1), 191205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanselow, G., & Féry, C. (2002). Ineffability in grammar. In Fanselow, G. & Féry, C. (Eds.), Resolving conflicts in grammars: optimality theory in syntax, morphology and phonology (pp. 265307). Hamburg: Helmut Buske Verlag.Google Scholar
Gorman, K., & Yang, C. (2018). When nobody wins. In Rainer, F., Gardani, F., Luschützky, H. C. & Dressler, W. U. (Eds.), Competition in inflection and word formation (pp. 169193). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Hall, K., Blake, A., Fry, M., Mackie, S., & McAuliffe, M. (2016). Phonological corpus tools, version 1.2. [computer program].Google Scholar
Halle, M. (1973). Prolegomena to a theory of word formation. Linguistic Inquiry, 4(1), 316.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (2003). The early catastrophe: The 30-million-word gap by age 3. American Educator, 27(1), 49.Google Scholar
Hay, J., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Shifting paradigms: gradient structure in morphology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(7), 342348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helgadóttir, S., et al. (2010). The Tagged Icelandic Corpus (MÍM). Proceedings of the workshop on language technology of under-resourced languages (pp. 6772). Available at malheildir.arnastofnun.is/mim.Google Scholar
Henzl, V. M. (1975). Acquisition of grammatical gender in Czech. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 188200.Google Scholar
Hernández-Pina, F. (1984). Teorias psicosociolinguisticas y su aplicacion a la adquisicion del espanol como lengua materna. Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1958). A course in modern linguistics. Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horst, J. S., & Hout, M. C. (2016). The novel object and unusual (NOUN) database: A collection of novel images for use in experimental research. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 13931409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kam, Hudson, & Newport, C. L., E. (2005). Regularizing unpredictable variation: the roles of adult and child learners in language formation and change. Language Learning and Development, 1(2), 151195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kam, Hudson, & Newport, C. L., E. (2009). Getting it right by getting it wrong: when learners change languages. Cognitive psychology, 59(1), 3066.Google ScholarPubMed
Iversen, R. (1922). Norrøn grammatikk. 7th edition. Tano.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). A functional approach to child language. A study of determiners and reference. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (1973). Elsewhere in phonology. In Anderson R, S.., & Kiparsky, P. (Eds.), A festschrift for Morris Halle (pp. 93106). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Kodner, J. (2019). Estimating child linguistic experience from historical corpora. Glossa, 4(1), 122, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kvaran, G. (2005). Orð. Handbók um beygingar-og orðmyndunarfræði [Word. A Handbook of Icelandic inflection and derivation.] Almenna bókafélagið.Google Scholar
Levy, Y. (1983). The acquisition of Hebrew plurals: the case of missing gender category. Journal of Child Language, 10, 188200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 18(3), 193198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linaza, J., Sebastián, M. E., & del Barrio, C. (1981). Lenguaje, comunicación y comprensión. La adquisición del lenguaje. Monografía de Infancia y Aprendizaje, 195198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mariscal, S. (2008). Early acquisition of gender agreement in the Spanish noun phrase: starting small. Journal of Child Language 35, 129.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Bybee, J. (2007). Gradience of gradience: a reply to Jackendoff. The Linguistic Review, 24(4), 437455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, A. (1986). The acquisition of gender: a study of English and German grammatical development. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newport, E. (2019). Children and adults as language learners: rules, variation and maturational change. In Landau, B. (Ed.), Topics in cognitive science, 117.Google Scholar
Noreen, A. (1903). Altisländische und Altnorwegische Grammatik. Max Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Orgun, C. O., & Sprouse, R. (1999). From Mparse to control: deriving ungrammaticality. Phonology, 20, 191224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pertsova, K. (2005). How lexical conservatism can lead to paradigm gaps. In Heinz, J.., Martin, A. and Pertsova, K. (Eds.), UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics 11: Papers in Phonology 6. UCLA Linguistics Department, Los Angeles, 1330.Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (1991). The acquisition of gender: what Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language, 18, (3), 571590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pind, J. (1991). Íslensk orðtíðnibók [Frequency in Icelandic]. Orðabók Háskólans.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1994). Regular and irregular morphology and the psychological status of rules of grammar. In Lima, S. D., Corrigan, R. L. & Iverson, G. K. (Eds.), The reality of linguistic rules, pages (pp. 230251). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1991). U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perception: implications for child language acquisition. Cognition, 38(1), 10771106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodina, Y., & Westergaard, M. (2012). A cue-based approach to the acquisition of grammatical gender in Russian. Journal of Child Language, 39, 10771106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodina, Y., & Westergaard, M. (2013). The acquisition of gender and declension class in a non-transparent system: monolinguals and bilinguals. Studia Linguistica, 67, 4767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodina, Y., & Westergaard, M. (2015). Grammatical gender in Norwegian: language acquisition and language change. Journal of Germanic Linguistics, 27(2), 145187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuler, K., Yang, C., & Newport, E. (2016). Testing the Tolerance Principle: children form productive rules when it is more computationally efficient to do so. In The 38th Cognitive Society Annual Meeting, Philadelphia, PA.Google Scholar
Sigurjónsdóttir, S. (1991). Interrogative Sentences in the Language of Two Icelandic Children (MA thesis).Google Scholar
Sigurjónsdóttir, S. (2007). The Fia corpus. University of Iceland, Reykjavík.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. (1977). Language change in childhood and history. In Macnamara, J. (Ed.), Language Learning and Thought. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Steinmetz, D. (1985). Gender in German and Icelandic: inanimate nouns. In Faarlund, J. (Ed.), Germanic Linguistics. Papers from a symposium at the University of Chicago. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Szagun, G., Steinbrink, C., Franik, M., & Stumper, B. (2006). Development of vocabulary and grammar in young German-speaking children assessed with a German language development inventory. First Language, 26(3), 259280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thráinsson, H. (2017). U-umlaut in Icelandic and Faroese: Survival and death. In Bowern, C. & Zanuttini, R. (Eds.), On looking into words (and beyond ) (pp.99113). Berlin: Language Science Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: a case study of early grammatical development. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Hulk, A. (2013). Grammatical gender and the notion of default: insights from language acquisition. Lingua 137, 128144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S., & Hulk, A. (2010). L1 acquisition of neuter gender in Dutch: production and judgment. Proceedings of Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition 2009 (pp. 5051).Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2005). On productivity. Linguistic Variation Yearbook, 5(1), 333370.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2016). The price of linguistic productivity: How children learn to break rules of language. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Numerical Distribution of Genitive Endings for Masculine Singular Nouns in Polish

Figure 1

Table 2. Numerical Distribution of Noun Types by Gender and Suffix in Spanish Child-Directed Speech

Figure 2

Table 3. Mappings between Gender and Nominative Singular Suffixes in Icelandic

Figure 3

Table 4. Gender Assignment of Borrowed Nouns in Icelandic

Figure 4

Table 5. Numerical Distribution of Nominative Singular Noun Types in Icelandic Child- Directed Speech

Figure 5

Table 6. Numerical Distribution of Nominative Singular Noun Types in Child Naturalistic Production

Figure 6

Table 7 Non-Target-Consistent Gender Agreement in Icelandic Child Naturalistic Production Child Production

Figure 7

Table 8. Distribution of Noun Types by Gender and Suffix in the SUBTLEX Corpus

Figure 8

Table 9. Distribution of the most Frequent Noun Types in the SUBTLEX Corpus by Gender and Suffix

Figure 9

Table 10. Test Items by Nominative Singular Suffix

Figure 10

Figure 1. A Novel Object at Exposure to Test

Figure 11

Figure 2. Magic at work in the Test Scene

Figure 12

Figure 3. Children: Gender Assignment across Conditions

Figure 13

Figure 4. Children: Gender Assignment in the Unproductive Condition

Figure 14

Figure 5. Children: Gender Assignment and Syllable Number in the Unproductive condition

Figure 15

Figure 6. Effect of Age on Neuter Assignment

Figure 16

Figure 7. Adults: Gender Assignment across Conditions

Figure 17

Figure 8. Adults: Gender Assignment in the Unproductive Condition

Figure 18

Figure 9. Adults: Gender Assignment and Syllable Number in the Unproductive Condition

Figure 19

Table 11. Quantitative Analysis of Adult, Child and SUBTLEX Corpora