Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T17:54:55.371Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why indeed? Invited commentary on… Why aren't depot antipsychotics prescribed more often and what can be done about it?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

A recent review concluded that replicated, evidence-based studies have demonstrated several areas of advantage for long-acting antipsychotics over oral antipsychotics. These include improved global outcome and reduced risk of rehospitalisation, psychopharmacological benefits such as more consistent bioavailabilty and more predictable dose–blood level correlations, an improved pharmacokinetic profile allowing lower dosages to be used with a consequent reduced likelihood of side-effects, and a reduced burden of care when injections are required only every 2–6 weeks (Robert & Geppert, 2004). Further, if a patient relapses despite receiving uninterrupted depot treatment, this indicates the need to consider reasons for deterioration other than poor adherence. However, perhaps the critical advantage over oral preparations is the avoidance of covert non-adherence (Barnes & Curson, 1994). With depot treatment, any decision by the patient not to continue medication will be signalled by failure to attend for, or refusal of, injection. The clinical team can therefore act to intervene appropriately, bearing in mind that non-adherence may be both a cause and consequence of worsening of illness. Lastly, with depot preparations the risk of self-poisoning is reduced.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2005 

A recent review concluded that replicated, evidence-based studies have demonstrated several areas of advantage for long-acting antipsychotics over oral antipsychotics. These include improved global outcome and reduced risk of rehospitalisation, psychopharmacological benefits such as more consistent bioavailabilty and more predictable dose–blood level correlations, an improved pharmacokinetic profile allowing lower dosages to be used with a consequent reduced likelihood of side-effects, and a reduced burden of care when injections are required only every 2–6 weeks (Reference Robert and GeppertRobert & Geppert, 2004). Further, if a patient relapses despite receiving uninterrupted depot treatment, this indicates the need to consider reasons for deterioration other than poor adherence. However, perhaps the critical advantage over oral preparations is the avoidance of covert non-adherence (Reference Barnes and CursonBarnes & Curson, 1994). With depot treatment, any decision by the patient not to continue medication will be signalled by failure to attend for, or refusal of, injection. The clinical team can therefore act to intervene appropriately, bearing in mind that non-adherence may be both a cause and consequence of worsening of illness. Lastly, with depot preparations the risk of self-poisoning is reduced.

The disadvantages relate to the relatively stable plasma drug concentrations, leading to a lack of flexibility should side-effects develop or when titrating the dose clinically. However, clinical experience suggests that long-acting formulations in standard dosage have a relatively low side-effect burden, and specific notions that such preparations are associated with an increased risk of neuroleptic malignant syndrome and extrapyramidal side-effects, particularly tardive dyskinesia, have not been supported by reviews of the published evidence (Reference Glazer and KaneGlazer & Kane, 1992).

An image problem

Given the apparent benefits of depot preparations, why, as Reference Patel and DavidPatel & David (2005, this issue) ask, are they not used as often as they used to be? These authors essentially give two main reasons. First, they refer to an ‘image problem’: clinicians believe that patients and the wider community have negative attitudes to depot. Patel & David argue that this is a misapprehension, and adduce results from a host of studies that suggest that at least a proportion of people with severe, enduring mental illness prefer depot administration of antipsychotics, because of its perceived efficacy, its function as a ‘safety net’ protecting them from risk of relapse and hospitalisation (Reference Svedberg, Backenroth-Ohsako and LützénSvedberg et al, 2003) and its usefulness in obviating the need to remember to take tablets on a daily basis. Nevertheless, despite such positive reports, people switched from a depot first-generation antipsychotic to an oral second-generation drug may favour the latter (Reference Desai, Huq and MartinDesai et al, 1999; Reference Godleski, Goldsmith and ViewegGodleski et al, 2003).

Demonstrating that some patients are satisfied with their current treatment does not address what is perhaps a broader image problem for depot medication. Clinicians may have a lingering concern that, in the public perception, the parenteral route of administration is associated with the patient as a passive recipient at best, and with coercion at worst. The use of the depot route may be seen as the cautious choice for clinicians faced with cultural, ethnic or communication barriers (Reference Ziguras, Lambert and McKenzieZiguras et al, 1999). There is also the awareness that, with their long half-lives, depot preparations inevitably delay the opportunity for a patient to reverse any decision about continuing with medication. Thus, clinicians may fear that the process of the regular administration of injections, apart from having the potential to be rather ignominious and painful, might symbolise a relationship between prescriber and patient that is incompatible with a clinical culture that seeks to approach patients in a respectful and empathic manner and encourage communication of any treatment concerns. But it is precisely in the context of such a culture of concordance, involving informed choice and unbiased discussion of the treatment options available, that the ethical and clinical issues surrounding depot treatment can be adequately addressed, and the restrictive notion of depot as a ‘treatment of last resort’ dispelled (Reference Robert and GeppertRobert & Geppert, 2004).

The restricted evidence

The second main reason propounded by Patel & David for the reduction in depot use is that clinicians may not adequately consider the risk–benefit balance with depot antipsychotics. This may partly reflect the nature and amount of relevant evidence available. For example, the studies comparing depot and oral medication in terms of risk of relapse that show only modest differences may for two reasons underestimate the value of depot in clinical practice. First, as Patel & David point out, in most cases the trial period was too short to reveal the longer-term benefits of depot treatment. In one of the longer studies comparing depot and oral antipsychotic treatment, Reference Hogarty, Schooler and UlrichHogarty et al(1979) found no difference in the proportion relapsing within the first year, but a significant advantage for depot emerged in the second. Second, the patients for whom depot is commonly indicated, that is, those with a history of poor adherence, may be underrepresented in the studies. Recruitment seems to have been biased towards the inclusion of patients with a lower risk of non-adherence to treatment (Reference SchoolerSchooler, 2003), which would serve to diminish any advantage for depot compared with unreliable tablet-taking in the oral treatment group. That patients consenting to participate in clinical studies will tend to be more treatment adherent than those failing to consent is a problem relating to generalisability of results that may be ubiquitous in trials in schizophrenia (Reference Bowen and BarnesBowen & Barnes, 1994).

Clinicians will face a similar lack of robust evidence if they try to weigh the possible risks and benefits of traditional depot drugs, all of which are first-generation antipsychotics, against those of a second-generation drug, only one of which, risperidone, is as yet available as a long-acting injection. There is accumulating evidence that the second-generation drugs may have advantages over those of the first-generation in terms of efficacy, including relapse prevention (Reference Leucht, Barnes and KisslingLeucht et al, 2003), and safety, with a lower liability for extrapyramidal side-effects, including tardive dyskinesia (Reference Correll, Leucht and KaneCorrell et al, 2004), but these findings predominantly relate to comparisons of oral first- and second-generation antipsychotics. At present, there is a paucity of long-term trials systematically comparing depot formulations and oral second-generation drugs on key clinical outcome measures.

References

Barnes, T. R. E. & Curson, D. A. (1994) Long-acting depot antipsychotics: a risk–benefit assessment. Drug Safety, 10, 464479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowen, J. & Barnes, T. R. E. (1994) The clinical characteristics of schizophrenic patients consenting and not consenting to a placebo-controlled trial. Human Psychopharmacology, 9, 423433.Google Scholar
Correll, C. U., Leucht, S. & Kane, J. M. (2004) Lower risk for tardive dyskinesia associated with second-generation antipsychotics: a systematic review of 1-year studies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 414425.Google Scholar
Desai, N. M., Huq, Z., Martin, S. D. et al (1999) Switching from depot antipsychotics to risperidone: results of a study of chronic schizophrenia. The Schizophrenia Treatment and Assessment Group. Advances in Therapy, 16, 7888.Google ScholarPubMed
Glazer, W. M. & Kane, J. M. (1992) Depot neuroleptic therapy: an underutilized treatment option. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 53, 426433.Google ScholarPubMed
Godleski, L. S., Goldsmith, L. J., Vieweg, W. V. et al (2003) Switching from depot antipsychotic drugs to olanzapine in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64, 119122.Google Scholar
Hogarty, G. E., Schooler, N. R., Ulrich, R. et al (1979) Fluphenazine and social therapy in the aftercare of schizophrenic patients. Relapse analyses of a two-year controlled study of fluphenazine decanoate and fluphenazine hydrochloride. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 12831294.Google Scholar
Leucht, S., Barnes, T. R. E., Kissling, W. et al (2003) Relapse prevention in schizophrenia with new generation antipsychotics. A systematic review and explorative meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 12091222.Google Scholar
Patel, M. X. & David, A. S. (2005) Why aren't depot antipsychotics prescribed more often and what can be done about it? Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 11, 203211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robert, L. W. & Geppert, C. M. A. (2004) Ethical use of long-acting medications in the treatment of severe and persistent mental illness. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 45, 161167.Google Scholar
Schooler, N. R. (2003) Relapse and rehospitalisation: comparing oral and depot antipsychotics. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64 (suppl. 16), 1417.Google Scholar
Svedberg, B., Backenroth-Ohsako, G. & Lützén, K. (2003) On the path to recovery. Patients' experiences of treatment with long-acting injections of antipsychotic medication. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 12, 110118.Google Scholar
Ziguras, S., Lambert, T. J. R., McKenzie, D. P. et al (1999) The influence of client's ethnicity on psychotropic medication management in community mental health services. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 33, 882888.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.