Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T00:13:24.581Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Se Habla Español: Spanish-Language Appeals and Candidate Evaluations in the United States

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 March 2023

MARQUES G. ZÁRATE*
Affiliation:
Rice University, United States
ENRIQUE QUEZADA-LLANES*
Affiliation:
Rice University, United States
ANGEL D. ARMENTA*
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky, United States
*
Marques G. Zárate, Ph.D. Candidate, Department of Political Science, Rice University, United States, [email protected]
Enrique Quezada-Llanes, Ph.D. Candidate, Department of Political Science, Rice University, United States, [email protected]
Angel Armenta, Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, United States, [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Political candidates use Spanish-language appeals in efforts to increase their support among Hispanic voters. We argue that candidates, Hispanic or not, can use Spanish to signal closeness to Hispanics and posit that the effectiveness of these appeals is conditional on proficiency. To test this, we run two experiments where participants listen to an audio clip of a hypothetical candidate’s stump speech. We vary the ethnicity of the candidate (Anglo or Hispanic) and the language of the speech (English, non-native Spanish, and native-like Spanish). We find that Hispanic support for the Anglo and Hispanic candidates is higher in the native-like Spanish condition compared with the English-only condition. Relative to the English condition, non-native Spanish does not increase support for the Anglo candidate, but it decreases support for the Hispanic candidate. We find mixed effects for Anglo participants. Our results suggest that candidates can effectively appeal to Hispanic voters using Spanish-language messages.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the American Political Science Association

INTRODUCTION

Many political candidates and elected officials have used Spanish-language appeals to increase their support among Hispanic voters. They have done this by speaking to constituents in Spanish, creating campaign ads in Spanish, or, more recently, showcasing their Spanish-language skills on the debate stage. For example, in the first 2020 Democratic Party presidential debate, candidates Beto O’Rourke, Julián Castro, and Cory Booker each delivered part of their remarks in Spanish (Contreras and Anderson Reference Contreras and Anderson2019). On the Republican side, Senator Marco Rubio and former Florida Governor Jeb Bush have interviewed on Spanish-language networks and did so while campaigning in the 2016 Republican Presidential primary (González and Nowicki Reference González and Nowicki2015). Existing studies suggest that speaking Spanish may be a useful strategy for candidates hoping to appeal to a large group of Hispanic voters across national origins (Alamillo and Collingwood Reference Alamillo and Collingwood2017; Flores and Coppock Reference Flores and Coppock2018; Lavariega Monforti, Michelson, and Franco Reference Lavariega Monforti, Michelson and Franco2013). However, as the 2020 Democratic presidential candidates made clear, politicians are not equal in their ability to speak the language. While Booker and Castro struggled with their Spanish, O’Rourke spoke the language with relative ease. The significant variation in Spanish-language ability with which candidates speak could change the effectiveness of these appeals. In this study, we ask whether Hispanic voters care if a political candidate speaks to them in Spanish and whether it matters how proficient the candidate sounds.

Spanish, the second most spoken language in U.S. homes (Gonzalez-Barrera and Lopez Reference Gonzalez-Barrera and Lopez2013), plays an important role in the lives of Hispanics. According to a report by the Pew Research Center, 73% of Hispanics in the United States spoke Spanish at home in 2015 (Krogstad and Lopez Reference Krogstad and Lopez2017). While scholars have long noted that Hispanic Americans are not a monolithic group, Spanish is spoken in nearly every Latin American country from which Hispanics in the United States may trace their ethnic heritage.Footnote 1 However, Hispanic Americans have historically faced discrimination, with accusations of refusal to assimilate into traditional American values (Alba and Nee Reference Alba and Nee1997; de la Garza, Falcon, and Garcia Reference de la Garza, Falcon and Chris Garcia1996) and the questioning of one’s immigration status and place in American society, in part due to the use of Spanish (e.g., Stack Reference Stack2019). Experiences of language discrimination in the United States led many Hispanics to dissociate themselves from the group and the accompanying stereotypes of inferiority and foreignness by not speaking Spanish (García Bedolla Reference García Bedolla2005; Zou and Cheryan Reference Zou and Cheryan2017). In this context, the use of Spanish on a public stage by political elites may help Hispanic voters feel more included in the political process.

While language discrimination toward Spanish and other minority languages still exists, political candidates are displaying an increasing willingness to make Spanish-language appeals. This is true for not only national debate stages, but also for campaign advertisements. According to work by Abrajano (Reference Abrajano2010), in the 2000 and 2004 House races, eight congressional districts in each race had at least one candidate using a Spanish-language ad. During these elections, a total of 22 candidates created a Spanish-language ad. To compare, we conducted a content analysis of all Spanish campaign advertisements in the House from 2010 to 2018.Footnote 2 In 2010, 13 House districts had at least one candidate who created a Spanish-language ad, and in the 2012 to 2018 elections, that number jumped to at least 18 districts with a high of 22 in 2018. In contrast to the 22 candidates in 2000 and 2004, the 2018 House election alone had nearly double the candidates, 40, who used a Spanish-language ad. Moreover, candidates using Spanish-language ads are not exclusively Hispanic nor do they speak Spanish with the same level of proficiency. During the 2010–2018 period, 62 of the 151 candidates who produced a Spanish-language ad were Anglo with most of the other candidates being Hispanic. While the expectation is that the Hispanic candidates will speak Spanish with native proficiency, 17% of the Hispanic candidates in our sample spoke the language with a non-native, American accent. On the other hand, Anglo candidates are not expected to be able to speak Spanish let alone with native proficiency, yet 79% spoke Spanish at one point in the ad with 3% of all Anglo candidates being able to do so with a native-sounding accent. Despite this diversity, particularly among Hispanic candidates, no study, to the best of our knowledge, has systematically studied how this variation in language proficiency shapes Hispanics’ evaluations of the candidates who make this appeal.

We argue that a candidate’s ability to speak Spanish acts as a community commitment signal (Collingwood Reference Collingwood2020; Stout Reference Stout2020; Wamble Reference Wamble2019), conveying cultural familiarity and time spent in a Spanish-speaking community. Language proficiency in Spanish indicates a significant investment of time and energy on the part of an Anglo candidate and highlights a Hispanic candidate’s connection to their ethnic heritage. As a result, we expect Hispanic voters to have more positive evaluations of candidates who use Spanish-language appeals. We posit that the impact of these appeals, however, is conditional on the quality of the appeal as defined by the level of language proficiency. Whereas speaking with native-like proficiency acts as a strong signal, limited proficiency sends a weaker signal.Footnote 3 For a Hispanic candidate, limited proficiency Spanish may indicate distance from their ethnic heritage, whereas in the case of an Anglo candidate, it may lead them to be perceived as disingenuous.

Existing studies do not address how a candidate’s Spanish proficiency affects Hispanics’ evaluations of that candidate nor do they fully isolate the effect of language from that of shared ethnicity. While some studies show that Anglo candidates can increase their support among Hispanic voters by emphasizing their ability to speak Spanish, respondents in these studies only read or hear about the candidate’s language ability (Alamillo and Collingwood Reference Alamillo and Collingwood2017; Collingwood Reference Collingwood2020; Lavariega Monforti, Michelson, and Franco Reference Lavariega Monforti, Michelson and Franco2013). Research where respondents are exposed to several candidates speaking Spanish shows that not all candidates benefit from Spanish-language appeals (Flores and Coppock Reference Flores and Coppock2018). Furthermore, these studies have focused primarily on Anglo candidates, and in the cases when Hispanic candidates are included, the research design does not allow researchers to examine whether and how the impact of Spanish-language appeals varies by candidate ethnicity.

Across two studies using an experimental design with audio treatments, our paper examines the effect of Spanish-language appeals on Hispanic voters’ evaluations of political candidates. Our first study is a 2-by-3 between-subject experiment where we present respondents with a hypothetical candidate running for state legislature. We vary the ethnicity of the candidate (Anglo or Hispanic) as well as the language used in part of the speech (all English, partially in non-native-accented Spanish, or partially in native-accented Spanish).Footnote 4, Footnote 5 This experimental design permits us to isolate the effect of language from the effect of shared ethnicity and test the role of language proficiency. We find that Hispanics prefer candidates who use Spanish-language appeals but only when the candidate speaks with native-like proficiency. This is true for both the Anglo and Hispanic candidates in our experiment. Since appeals toward racial and ethnic minorities may disrupt electoral coalitions that depend on moderate Anglo voters (see, e.g., Fraga and Leal Reference Fraga and Leal2004; Frymer Reference Frymer1999), we test the impact of Spanish-language appeals on Anglo respondents. Against our expectations, Spanish-language appeals do not have a consistent impact, positive or negative, on Anglos’ candidate evaluations, although further exploratory analyses suggest that Democrat and liberal Anglos have better evaluations of Hispanic candidates overall and candidates who speak Spanish. In our second study, we successfully replicate these findings and find support for our proposed mechanisms. These studies contributes to the study of racial and ethnic politics by testing the role of language proficiency in Spanish-language appeals, demonstrating that a candidate’s ability to speak Spanish, and speak it well, matters for Hispanics. More generally, these findings underscore the importance that language cues can have for candidates and their campaigns. Language proficiency, independent of the content of an appeal, has an impact of candidate evaluations and communicates relevant information to voters about a candidate’s connection and commitment to their group. Because language serves a key function in the maintenance of group identity and as marker of group membership (García Bedolla Reference García Bedolla2003; Lippi-Green Reference Lippi-Green2012; Milroy Reference Milroy1982), campaigns can look toward ways to use language-based appeals for different groups.

ETHNIC APPEALS AND THE HISPANIC VOTE

Scholars of Latino politics have examined how political parties and candidates seek to gain support among Latinos. One way this happens is by having Latino candidates on the ballot. Research shows that shared ethnicity matters: Latino voters are more likely to turnout to vote when there is a co-ethnic on the ballot and prefer co-ethnic candidates over non-co-ethnics (Barreto Reference Barreto2007; Reference Barreto2010; Barreto, Villarreal, and Woods Reference Barreto, Villarreal and Woods2005; Manzano and Sanchez Reference Manzano and Sanchez2010; McConnaughy et al. Reference McConnaughy, White, Leal and Casellas2010; Sanchez Reference Sanchez2006b). Scholars posit that attachment to an ethnic identity, the shared experiences it communicates, and the belief that group members share common goals explains the significance of ethnic cues at the ballot box (Casellas, Gillion, and Wallace Reference Casellas, Gillion and Wallace2019; Casellas and Wallace Reference Casellas and Wallace2015). This is supported by a number of studies showing that higher levels of Latino linked fate led to greater support for co-ethnic candidates (McConnaughy et al. Reference McConnaughy, White, Leal and Casellas2010; Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2013; Wallace Reference Wallace2014).Footnote 6 Furthermore, as Manzano and Sanchez (Reference Manzano and Sanchez2010) demonstrate, shared ethnicity can help Latino candidates overcome perceived deficits in qualifications: Latinos with higher levels of perceived discrimination, greater support for collective action, and whose primary language is Spanish are more likely to support a co-ethnic even when they are less qualified than their Anglo counterpart.

Despite its importance in shaping Hispanic political behavior, ethnicity is not the only cue Hispanic voters use when evaluating a candidate. Electoral campaigns often include other sources of information such as party labels, and research shows that party is typically a stronger predictor of vote choice than candidate ethnicity (Michelson Reference Michelson2005). In addition, shared ethnicity does not presume closeness to the group (Mansbridge Reference Mansbridge1999; McClain et al. Reference McClain, Johnson Carew, Walton and Watts2009) nor does the lack of shared ethnicity preclude successful appeals to Hispanic voters. Alamillo and Collingwood (Reference Alamillo and Collingwood2017) argue that Anglo candidates can “tap into Latino identity” by showing their connection and respect to the group which they term “cross-racial mobilization” (534). They show that Anglo candidates can increase their support among Hispanics through this type of campaigning which involves, for example, emphasizing any Hispanic family the candidate may have, pointing toward Hispanics on their staff, and creating Spanish-language ads. In line with Alamillo and Collingwood (Reference Alamillo and Collingwood2017), we argue that a candidate’s ability to speak Spanish serves as an important cue and seek to separate the impact of Spanish-language ability from that of other cues (e.g., ethnicity). While Hispanic Americans have different national origins, they share a common language. This makes Spanish a useful way to target Hispanics across national heritages.Footnote 7

Previous work has found that Spanish-language appeals are effective at increasing turnout and support for a candidate among Hispanics. Field experiments on get-out-the-vote efforts demonstrate that Spanish-language appeals can increase turnout, although they remain less effective than English ads (Abrajano and Panagopoulos Reference Abrajano and Panagopoulos2011; Mann, Michelson, and Davis Reference Mann, Michelson and Davis2020; Panagopoulos and Green Reference Panagopoulos and Green2011). Spanish-language campaign ads also lead to higher turnout but only among individuals who are Spanish dominant (DeFrancesco Soto and Merolla Reference DeFrancesco Soto and Merolla2006; Ramírez Reference Ramírez, Voss and Bloemraad2011). While scholars have found evidence suggesting that using Spanish-language appeals is an effective campaign strategy to increase Hispanic support for a candidate (e.g., Alamillo and Collingwood Reference Alamillo and Collingwood2017; Collingwood Reference Collingwood2020; Flores and Coppock Reference Flores and Coppock2018; Lavariega Monforti, Michelson, and Franco Reference Lavariega Monforti, Michelson and Franco2013), existing studies cannot separate the effect of language from that of shared ethnicity. In other words, we do not know if Hispanic and Anglo candidates obtain the same benefit when they speak Spanish. Furthermore, most studies have relied on telling respondents the candidate’s Spanish-speaking ability rather than exposing them to these appeals (but see Flores and Coppock Reference Flores and Coppock2018). The distinction is important as voters may react differently to hearing the candidate speak Spanish, where they can evaluate the candidate’s ability. While being proficient may send a signal that the candidate is close to the community, anything less than native-like Spanish may be considered a disingenuous attempt at attracting Hispanic voters and may result in less favorable evaluations of the candidate. We seek to address these concerns through an experimental design which considers how proficiency may condition the effectiveness of Spanish-language appeals and that allows us to directly test how individuals react to the same Spanish-language appeal made by a Hispanic and an Anglo candidate.

LANGUAGE PROFICIENCY AS A SIGNAL OF COMMITMENT

We argue that candidates, Hispanic or otherwise, can establish rapport with Hispanic voters via the use of Spanish-language appeals. As Wamble (Reference Wamble2019) explains, candidates may signal their commitment to minority communities by emphasizing any social connections they may have with the community or by pointing to actions of personal sacrifice they have taken to advance minority interests. Spanish-language proficiency may act as such a signal of social connection and commitment for Hispanic voters. Similar to how shared ethnicity fosters a connection between Hispanic candidates and voters (Barreto Reference Barreto2010; Casellas, Gillion, and Wallace Reference Casellas, Gillion and Wallace2019; Casellas and Wallace Reference Casellas and Wallace2015), the ability to speak Spanish communicates the candidate’s closeness to the group and understanding of what concerns them. However, whereas shared ethnicity—being dependent on one’s ascribed membership to an ethnic group (see McClain et al. Reference McClain, Johnson Carew, Walton and Watts2009)—is an ethnic cue which only pertains to Hispanic candidates, language is a learned characteristic both co-ethnics and non-co-ethnics can use.

The use of Spanish by candidates during debates, campaign events, and political advertisements can serve to signal a candidate’s ability and willingness to represent Hispanics and their interests. First, speaking Spanish may signal an ability to represent Hispanic interests because, whether learned as an adult or acquired as a child, speaking a group’s language conveys a connection to that community. In the context of Spanish and the United States, being a native Spanish speaker implies growing up in a Hispanic household. For non-native speakers, learning Spanish signifies a substantial investment of time and energy and, in some cases, immersion in a Spanish-speaking community. Second, Spanish-language appeals may communicate to Hispanic voters a candidate is willing to represent them given the history of language discrimination in the United States. Because Spanish was seen as a hindrance to economic and social mobility, many second- and third-generation Hispanics were brought up with little to no Spanish (García Bedolla Reference García Bedolla2003; Lippi-Green Reference Lippi-Green2012). Still today, one can find examples of elected officials and members of the public being criticized for speaking Spanish (e.g., Bever Reference Bever2019; Stack Reference Stack2019). The fact that some politicians are now doing so while campaigning, even at a nationally televised presidential debate, provides a stark contrast to long-standing practices of language exclusion in the United States. Furthermore, speaking Spanish in these contexts is a deliberate move away from the default of English-language campaigning and an unequivocal appeal to Hispanic voters. These explicit appeals may increase Hispanic support as they help inform the community’s expectations for what the candidate will do in office (Stout Reference Stout2020; Wamble Reference Wamble2019).

While both Hispanic and Anglo candidates may benefit from speaking Spanish, its use may convey different meanings to voters. For Hispanic candidates, the use of Spanish in public speech can help them reinforce their connection to Hispanic voters by emphasizing their shared ethnicity. Anglo candidates can use Spanish to signal commitment and bridge the social identity gap between them and Hispanic voters (see Alamillo and Collingwood Reference Alamillo and Collingwood2017; Collingwood Reference Collingwood2020). In other words, Anglo candidates could improve their image among Hispanic voters as the latter group may see these efforts as a sign of respect and “a desire to break down cultural barriers through reduction of linguistic dissimilarities” (Koslow, Shamdasani, and Touchstone Reference Koslow, Shamdasani and Touchstone1994, 576). This leads to our first hypothesis.

Spanish-language appeals hypothesis (H1): Hispanic voters will give more positive evaluations to a candidate who speaks Spanish over one who does not.

Consistent with previous research on the importance of ethnicity as a cue, we hypothesize that, on average, Hispanics will prefer Hispanic candidates over Anglo candidates. Hispanic candidates should have closer ties to Hispanic voters and will have more cultural competency, making them more likely to have a better understanding of how to represent them (Barreto Reference Barreto2007; Wallace Reference Wallace2014). Furthermore, individuals typically value the in-group more so than the out-group as they seek value in belonging to something (Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel and Turner1979).

Shared ethnicity hypothesis (H2): Hispanic voters will give higher evaluations to a Hispanic candidate than an Anglo candidate.

While previous research has shown that language can have a positive impact in how Hispanics view candidates, we argue that it is crucial to examine the candidate’s Spanish-language proficiency. Individuals can show substantial variation in their command of the language and this may condition the effect of these appeals. On the one hand, native speakers, and individuals who achieve a similar level of proficiency, convey the strongest signal of commitment, ability, and willingness to Hispanic voters. On the other hand, whether because of poor syntax, pronunciation, vocabulary, or a combination of these, individuals with limited proficiency send a much weaker signal of commitment and their appeals may be seen as insincere. Thus, we expect the strength of Spanish as a signal of commitment to vary by speaker where limited proficiency is a weak signal and native-like proficiency is a strong signal.

It is important to consider the possibility that voters will evaluate candidates with lower Spanish proficiency differently based on whether the candidate is Hispanic or Anglo. If an Anglo candidate speaks non-native sounding Spanish, Hispanics may still reward them as there are no set expectations for them to be able communicate in Spanish, let alone with native-like proficiency. Since an appeal made in Spanish is clearly intended for Hispanics, even if the individual’s language proficiency is poor and they are unable to hold a conversation, Hispanics may still reward the effort. By contrast, for a Hispanic candidate, lack of proficiency in Spanish may convey a level of removal from their ethnic heritage. Non-native sounding Spanish from a co-ethnic candidate can be interpreted as meaning that they do not have as many shared experiences and are therefore culturally different from a Spanish-speaking Hispanic. In short, these co-ethnic candidates may not be considered “real” Hispanics (see García Bedolla Reference García Bedolla2003), and Hispanic voters may feel less affinity toward them. While limited Spanish-language proficiency may not benefit an Anglo candidate, we hypothesize that it will actively hurt a Hispanic candidate.

Non-Native Spanish hypothesis (H3): Relative to the Anglo candidate speaking English, Hispanics will punish Hispanic candidates that speak non-native sounding Spanish.

As discussed above, however, not all Hispanics show a preference for co-ethnics. Those that highly identify as Latino look more favorably upon their in-group, making them even more likely to support a co-ethnic (McConnaughy et al. Reference McConnaughy, White, Leal and Casellas2010; Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2013; Wallace Reference Wallace2014). We hypothesize that the moderating role of ethnic group strength—that is, how central being part of the ethnic group is to one’s identity—will extend to Hispanics’ reaction to Spanish-language appeals. We expect that the positive impact of ethnicity and Spanish-language appeals, as well as the negative effect of non-native Spanish proficiency, will be larger among high identifiers compared with low identifiers. Having a candidate willing to represent Hispanic interests is a higher priority for high-identifying Latinos compared with low-identifying Latinos. Thus, Hispanics who see their ethnic identity as very important will prefer a candidate with native-like proficiency because it communicates an ability and willingness to represent them.

Group strength hypothesis (H4a): The effect of shared ethnicity will be stronger for Hispanics with higher levels of group strength.

Group strength hypothesis (H4b): The effect of Spanish-language appeals will be stronger for Hispanics with higher levels of group strength.

AN EMPIRICAL TEST OF LANGUAGE-BASED APPEALS

Experimental Design

To test our hypotheses, we conducted a pair of studies from two different samples where we varied the ethnicity and Spanish proficiency—via the accent—of a hypothetical candidate running for state legislature.Footnote 8 Participants listened to a brief audio clip of the candidate giving a part of their stump speech and responded to a battery of questions that measure evaluations of the candidate. For Study 1, we obtained a national sample of 503 Hispanic and 506 Anglo respondents through the survey firm Prolific.Footnote 9, Footnote 10 The survey was fielded from October 26–31, 2020, just before the 2020 November election. Overall, our Hispanic and Anglo samples were younger, had higher levels of education, were more liberal, and were more likely to identify as a Democrat compared with population estimates of their respective subgroups. Given the liberal skew of our Study 1 sample, from December 28, 2021 to January 11, 2022, we ran an additional study recruiting one thousand Hispanic and one thousand Anglo respondents through Lucid where the partisan composition of our Hispanic and Anglo samples corresponds to the two-party 2020 presidential vote share in their respective group. In addition to seeing if our results replicate, Study 2 allows us to tease out our proposed mechanisms and test the robustness of our findings to the inclusion of an in-party cue.Footnote 11

Prior to beginning both experiments, all participants were told that they were being asked to participate in a research study examining how people evaluate state legislature candidate campaign speeches. Participants were required to give consent to participate in the study before being allowed to continue. At the beginning of the survey, we asked several demographic questions including age, gender, ideology, and partisanship. In Study 1, participants were randomly assigned to one of six conditions where they were instructed to listen to a short audio clip from a campaign speech of a candidate running for a seat in a state legislature. We varied whether the candidate is Hispanic or Anglo, and whether part of the campaign speech is spoken in non-native Spanish, native-like Spanish, or fully in English. To vary the ethnicity of the candidate, we gave a Hispanic- or Anglo-sounding name to the hypothetical candidate (Josué Martínez or Josh Martin) and explicitly stated the candidate’s ethnicity or race. For the language treatment, respondents were assigned to one of three different audio recordings, all of which shared the same substantive content. The only differences between these audio files was in the language, English or Spanish, used in two sentences (shown in brackets below) and the candidate’s accent when speaking Spanish. For Study 2, we also vary whether the candidate is running in a nonpartisan or partisan primary election for a total of 12 treatment conditions. Participants in Study 2 either did not know the candidate’s partisanship or were told that the candidate was running in their self-identified political party primary (for more information on Study 2, see Section B of the Supplementary Material). The survey vignette for Study 1 (with the transcript of the audio clip) was as follows:

In a recent campaign event, state representative [Josh Martin/Josué Martínez] talked about his plans for his district if he were to be reelected and why he believes he is the best choice to represent his constituents in the state legislature. Representative [Martin/Martínez], who is [White/Latino], currently represents a mostly rural district that is 70.8% Latino with a median income of $46,232.

Below you will find an excerpt from [Josh Martin’s/Josué Martínez’s] remarks, where he makes his case to constituents. Please listen to the brief audio clip before continuing.

[AUDIO TRANSCRIPT] “Thank you all for being here. As your state representative I have worked tirelessly to represent your interests. [I am here to listen to your concerns and make sure the government addresses your needs. You can trust me for this job. / Estoy aquí para escuchar sus preocupaciones y asegurarme de que el gobierno atienda sus necesidades. Pueden confiar en mí para este trabajo.] Together, we can keep up the work of improving our schools, rebuilding our infrastructure, and bringing this great community closer together. If given the opportunity, I would be honored to continue representing you. I’m counting on each one of you so that we can forge ahead and make our vision for our district and our great state a reality.”

In the English conditions, the candidate spoke entirely in English. In both Spanish conditions, the candidate spoke mostly in English with two sentences in Spanish. In the non-native Spanish conditions, the candidate spoke Spanish with an American English accent. On the other hand, the candidate in the native-like Spanish conditions spoke with native-accented Spanish.Footnote 12 It is important to note that both Spanish conditions use the same text, which is grammatically correct; the difference is in how it is spoken not what is spoken.Footnote 13 To control for any effects of timbre or pitch (see Dietrich, Hayes, and O’Brien Reference Dietrich, Hayes and O’Brien2019), a single voice actor was used for all three recordings and the same recording was used regardless of candidate ethnicity.Footnote 14 Furthermore, we decided against adding any substantive policy content or background information of the candidate (e.g., partisanship) other than ethnic identity to isolate the impact of language ability.

After receiving the treatment, we included four questions to measure our outcome of interest: evaluations of the candidate. We measure candidate evaluations by asking respondents how likely they are to vote for the candidate, whether they trust the candidate, how much they like or dislike the candidate, and if they would feel represented with the candidate in office. All items are measured on a 5-point scale.Footnote 15 For our dependent variable, we construct an index of candidate evaluations by summing the scores of these four variables. Our index ranges from 0 to 1 where higher numbers indicate more favorable evaluations.Footnote 16

Following the candidate evaluation measures, we collected other relevant social and demographic information such as generational status and ability to write and speak in Spanish. In Study 1, we included questions about racial and ethnic identity, as well as importance of ethnic identification (our measure of group strength), after the treatment.Footnote 17 We chose not to ask participants’ questions regarding racial or ethnic identity prior to our experimental manipulation to avoid any potential for corrupting our treatments due to priming effects. We acknowledge that asking these questions after the treatment may introduce posttreatment bias in our estimates when we condition on these variables (Montgomery, Nyhan, and Torres Reference Montgomery, Nyhan and Torres2018). However, as Klar, Leeper, and Robison (Reference Klar, Leeper and Robison2020) discuss, when it comes to characteristics such as racial and ethnic identity, these questions have higher potential of inducing priming effects than to show change as a result of an experimental manipulation (but see Egan Reference Egan2020). While there is no one-size-fits-all solution to avoid both priming effects and posttreatment bias, we believe that in our particular case, the risk of corrupting our treatment was greater than that of conditioning on posttreatment variables. In Study 2, we ask for racial identification information at the beginning of the survey and randomly assign participants to receive the group strength questions either before or after the treatment. We find no differences in levels of ethnic group strength between those who answered these questions pre- and posttreatment.Footnote 18 Finally, in both studies, all participants are debriefed at the end of the experiment.

Results

To examine whether Hispanics give higher evaluations to candidates who make Spanish-language appeals (H1), we use a linear regression with the candidate evaluation index as the dependent variable and our language conditions as a factor variable where English serves as the baseline category. Table 1 displays the results among our Hispanic samples for Studies 1 and 2. Models 1 and 3 of Table 1 show mixed support our Spanish-language appeals hypothesis (H1): Hispanics give higher evaluations to candidates when they speak Spanish, regardless of whether the candidate is Hispanic or Anglo. However, candidates only received better evaluations if they spoke Spanish with a native-like accent. Respondents’ evaluation of the candidate in the non-native Spanish condition was statistically indistinguishable from their candidate evaluation in the English condition. On the other hand, native-like Spanish has a large, positive, and statistically significant impact ( $ b=0.122\hskip-0.25em $ , $ p<0.001 $ Model 1). The effect size is equivalent to 12% of the total range of the candidate evaluation index. We obtain a significant and positive, albeit smaller, result for native-like Spanish in Study 2 ( $ b=0.044 $ , $ p<0.001 $ ; Model 3). These results indicate that candidates, Hispanic or Anglo, can use Spanish-language appeals to foster more favorable evaluations among Hispanics but only if they have native-like proficiency.Footnote 19

TABLE 1. Effects of Language and Ethnicity on Candidate Evaluations among Hispanics

Note: *p<0.1; **p<0.05; ***p<0.01. Standard errors are in parentheses.

We also find support for the shared ethnicity hypothesis (H2), successfully replicating a consistent finding in the Hispanic politics literature (e.g., Barreto Reference Barreto2010). Models 2 and 4 in Table 1 show the results of a linear model with candidate evaluation as the dependent variable and an interaction between the ethnicity treatment and the language treatment as our explanatory variables.Footnote 20 The coefficient for Hispanic candidate, representing the difference in the candidate evaluation index between the Anglo and Hispanic candidates when both speak English, is positive and statistically significant (Study 1: $ b=0.118 $ , $ p<0.001 $ ; Study 2: $ b=0.047 $ , $ p<0.05 $ ). This effect can also be observed in the gap between the two leftmost estimates in each panel of Figure 1, where we plot the average candidate evaluation for each treatment condition for Studies 1 and 2.Footnote 21 Group means are shown with 84% confidence intervals around them to allow for visual inspection of statistically significant differences (Payton, Greenstone, and Schenker Reference Payton, Greenstone and Schenker2003; Schenker and Gentleman Reference Schenker and Gentleman2001).Footnote 22 In Study 1, the average evaluation for the Anglo candidate in the English condition is 0.44 and goes up to 0.56 when the candidate is Hispanic. For Study 2, the average evaluation for the English-speaking Anglo candidate is 0.59 and the average evaluation for the English-speaking Hispanic candidate is 0.64.

FIGURE 1. Mean Candidate Evaluation among Hispanics by Treatment Group

Note: Results from the Prolific sample (October 2020) are shown on the left; Lucid sample (December 2021 to January 2022) is shown on the right. Group means are plotted with 84% confidence intervals. Dependent variable ranges from 0 to 1. Results in tabular form are shown in Table I.1 in Section I of the Supplementary Material.

Our experimental design allows us to account for how a candidate’s Spanish-language accent and ethnicity affect their evaluation among Hispanic voters. We theorized that Spanish-language appeals may have different effects depending on whether they come from an Anglo or a Hispanic candidate. Overall, we find that Anglo candidates can benefit from Spanish-language appeals when speaking with native-like proficiency and Hispanic candidates with non-native Spanish are punished by their co-ethnics. For Study 1, our Hispanic sample evaluated the Anglo candidate speaking native-like Spanish much more favorably compared with the Anglo candidate speaking exclusively in English. The average evaluation for the Anglo candidate increased from 0.44 in the English-only condition to 0.63 in the native-like Spanish condition ( $ b=0.189 $ ). This effect size is larger than the effect size of shared ethnicity ( $ b=0.118 $ ). Furthermore, this effect is replicated in our Study 2 where average evaluations of the Anglo candidate increased from 0.59 when only speaking English to 0.65 when they used a native-like Spanish appeal. Looking at the effect of non-native Spanish, the Anglo candidate does not receive better or worse evaluations when speaking non-native Spanish compared to the English condition. Although the impact is positive among the Study 1 sample, it is negative among respondents in Study 2, and neither has statistical significance. When it comes to the evaluations of Josué Martínez, the Hispanic candidate, respondents give lower evaluations when the candidate speaks non-native Spanish than when the candidate speaks only in English ( $ p=0.013 $ ). It is only when speaking Spanish with a native accent that the evaluation for Josué Martínez moves up from 0.56 to 0.61 (Study 1), but the difference is not statistically significant in either of our two Hispanic samples. In other words, Hispanic candidates are punished for speaking non-native sounding Spanish and may not necessarily be rewarded when making appeals with a native-like accent. That said, we do not find support for our Non-Native Spanish hypothesis (H3). Latinos do not evaluate the Hispanic candidate speaking non-native Spanish lower than the English-speaking Anglo candidate.

We can also analyze the data by looking within each language condition. The effect of shared ethnicity is only present in the English condition, where there is a gap between the candidates of about 12 and 6 percentage points in Studies 1 and 2, respectively. In the non-native Spanish and native-like Spanish conditions, Hispanic evaluations of the Anglo and Hispanic candidates are statistically indistinguishable. Furthermore, our results support the thesis that Anglo candidates can close the social identity gap between them and Hispanic voters by making Spanish-language appeals (Alamillo and Collingwood Reference Alamillo and Collingwood2017). The Anglo candidate speaking native-like Spanish received similar or higher evaluations than the Hispanic candidate.Footnote 23 Thus, it appears to be that Anglo candidates stand to benefit from Spanish-language appeals if they have native-level proficiency, whereas Hispanic candidates who speak Spanish with a non-native accent risk losing the advantage of shared ethnicity.

Previous work on ethnic appeals has shown that an appeal’s impact is stronger among individuals with higher levels of ethnic group identification. To test this hypothesis (H4), we run a model where we interact our measure of ethnic group strength with the treatment conditions. Figure 2 shows predicted values of candidate evaluation for each treatment condition across levels of group strength in our Prolific sample.Footnote 24 The left panel shows the results for the conditions with the Anglo candidate; the results for the Hispanic candidate appear on the right. In general, as group strength increases, so too does the evaluation of the Hispanic candidate. Furthermore, moving from low to high on the ethnic group strength scale, candidate evaluations increase in all Spanish conditions. The only exception is when the Anglo candidate speaks in non-native-accented Spanish, but this difference does not reach statistical significance. Of note are the evaluations low- and high-identifying Hispanic respondents give to candidates based on Spanish-language proficiency. Low-identifying Hispanics (i.e., group strength set at its minimum) do not give significantly different evaluations to candidates with non-native and native-like Spanish. By contrast, high-identifying Hispanics differentially evaluate candidates according to their accent, responding more favorably to Anglo and Hispanic candidates with native-like Spanish.

FIGURE 2. Predicted Candidate Evaluation among Hispanics by Treatment Group across Levels of Ethnic Identity (Study 1)

Note: Larger values on the x-axis indicate greater importance of ethnicity to an individual’s identity. Predicted values are plotted with 84% confidence intervals. Dependent variable ranges from 0 to 1. Full model results are shown in Model 1 of Table K.2 in Section K of the Supplementary Material.

In addition to our stated hypotheses, we theorized that Spanish-language appeals increase candidate evaluations among Hispanics because these appeals signal the candidate’s ability and willingness to represent them and their community’s interests. We expect that this will be conditioned by the proficiency of the candidate. In Study 2, we included several items to tap into respondent’s perceptions of candidate ability and willingness, and created an index to measure each of these variables that ranges from 0 to 1.Footnote 25 Figure 3 shows the mean ability score (left panel) and mean willingness score (right panel) for each treatment group. The candidate’s ethnicity as well as their Spanish-language proficiency significantly impact perceived ability and willingness. Specifically, the Hispanic candidate is seen as better able and more willing to represent Hispanics compared with the Anglo candidate when both candidates speak only in English. The difference in perceived ability and willingness between these two conditions is of more than 0.2 points, which accounts for a fifth of the scale. When it comes to Spanish-language appeals, they appear to only be a credible signal when the candidate speaks native-like Spanish. Hispanic respondents perceive candidates with native-level Spanish as better able and more willing than candidates in the English-only conditions. Meanwhile, there is no difference in perceived ability or willingness between the Anglo candidate in the English-only condition, the Anglo candidate with non-native Spanish, and the Hispanic candidate with non-native Spanish. This evidence suggests that language, in addition to ethnicity, conveys information that shapes how voters perceive the candidate will act if elected.

FIGURE 3. Mean Perceived Candidate Ability and Willingness among Hispanics by Treatment Group

Note: Results are from the Lucid sample. Panel A reports the group means for the ability index. Panel B reports the group means for the willingness index. Group means are plotted with 84% confidence intervals. The dependent variables range from 0 to 1. Results in tabular form are shown in Table I.2 in Section I of the Supplementary Material.

In sum, the results from our experiment show that Hispanics have more positive evaluations of Hispanic and Anglo candidates who make appeals in Spanish. However, Hispanic voters react differently to these language appeals depending on the race and ethnicity of the candidate. While Anglo candidates benefit from speaking native-like Spanish, Hispanic candidates may be evaluated more poorly when speaking Spanish with a non-native accent. Lastly, our results confirm that ethnic group strength matters by showing that high-identifying Hispanics prefer Hispanic candidates over Anglo candidates in most cases.Footnote 26

HOW ANGLO VOTERS RESPOND

While Spanish-language appeals by candidates result in more positive evaluations among Hispanic voters, it is possible that they result in less positive evaluations among other groups (Hersh and Schaffner Reference Hersh and Schaffner2013). If this is true, then considerations about net negative electoral outcomes may lead candidates to refrain from making such targeted appeals. This may be especially true when the targeted group is in a state of electoral capture and politicians are not concerned with these voters jumping ship (Fraga and Leal Reference Fraga and Leal2004; Frymer Reference Frymer1999). Even though Hispanic voters may not be considered a captured group, over 60% of Hispanic registered voters identify as Democrats (Lopez et al. Reference Lopez, Gonzalez-Barrera, Krogstad and López2016). This reduces the incentives to make unequivocal ethnic appeals to Hispanics if there is an expectation that “appeals to the group will disrupt the party’s electoral coalition” (Frymer Reference Frymer1999, 8, emphasis in original). While trying to appeal toward Hispanic Americans by speaking Spanish, politicians could be hurting their chances with Anglo voters.

We expect that Anglo voters will have more positive evaluations of an Anglo candidate compared with a Hispanic candidate for two reasons. First, similar to how Hispanics prefer members of their own group to represent them, Anglos for whom their racial identity is important and who perceive their group to face discrimination exhibit a preference for Anglo candidates (Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2017).Footnote 27 Second, Hispanic candidates may be perceived as less competent and more foreign than their Anglo counterparts (Zou and Cheryan Reference Zou and Cheryan2017). Previous work has shown that anti-immigrant sentiment among Anglo voters is negatively associated with evaluations of competency and likelihood of vote for Hispanic candidates (McConnaughy et al. Reference McConnaughy, White, Leal and Casellas2010). Without any information about the candidate’s qualifications, Anglo respondents may believe that Hispanic candidates are less competent and unable to represent their interests as well as an Anglo candidate.

We also expect that Anglo voters will react negatively to candidates who use Spanish-language appeals. Accustomed to being the main target of campaign appeals, these individuals may feel as though their status is slipping. Their position in the racial hierarchy—and the resulting implicit attitudes regarding the status of their group (see social position theory; e.g., Bobo and Hutchings Reference Bobo and Hutchings1996; Masuoka and Junn Reference Masuoka and Junn2013; Pérez Reference Pérez2016)—may impact their reaction to Spanish-language appeals in a political setting. That is, Anglo voters might be less likely to support a candidate who makes these ethnic appeals because they may be perceived as a cultural threat and as diminishing the existing racial hierarchy (Bellovary, Armenta, and Reyna Reference Bellovary, Armenta, Reyna, Nadler and Voyles2020; de la Garza, Falcon, and Garcia Reference de la Garza, Falcon and Chris Garcia1996; Pérez Reference Pérez2016).Footnote 28 In addition, research has also shown that linguistic ostracism, or the communication setting where an individual cannot understand the language being spoken, leads listeners to have negative evaluations of the speaker (Flores and Coppock Reference Flores and Coppock2018; Hitlan et al. Reference Hitlan, Kelly, Schepman, Schneider and Zárate2006; Reference Hitlan, Zárate, Kelly and Catherine DeSoto2016). Assuming that the average Anglo individual is not fluent in Spanish, they may feel ostracized when politicians speak in Spanish because they cannot understand what is being said and feel left out of the conversation.

Table 2 shows the results of four models where we regress our candidate evaluation index on the language treatments among Anglo respondents in Studies 1 and 2. Parallel to our analysis of the Hispanic sample, Models 1 and 3 pool the Hispanic and Anglo candidate treatment conditions, whereas Models 2 and 4 include an interaction between the language and ethnicity conditions. We hypothesized that Spanish-language appeals would result in backlash from Anglo voters. Our results, however, do not support this. In Study 1, Anglo respondents in the native-like Spanish condition, regardless of candidate ethnicity, give more positive evaluations than respondents in the English condition ( $ b=0.066 $ , $ p<0.01 $ ; Model 1). Against our expectations, Anglo respondents also give more positive evaluations to the Hispanic candidate when both candidates speak English ( $ b=0.093 $ , $ p<0.01 $ ; Model 2). That said, respondents in this sample were more liberal and more likely to identify as a Democrat compared with Anglos in the general population.Footnote 29 In Study 2, where our sample matches the population on these characteristics, we find no significant effects for ethnicity or language in either direction (see Models 3 and 4).

TABLE 2. Effects of Language and Ethnicity on Candidate Evaluations among Anglos

Note: *p<0.1; **p<0.05; ***p<0.01. Standard errors are in parentheses.

For easier interpretation of the effect of language and candidate ethnicity on candidate evaluations, Figure 4 shows the mean candidate evaluation score in each condition from our two samples of Anglo respondents. In Study 1 (left panel), the only condition in which respondents evaluate the Hispanic and Anglo candidates differently is in the English condition, similar to what we find among Hispanic respondents. The evaluation for the Hispanic candidate is lower when moving from the English to the non-native Spanish condition, although this is only significant at the 0.1 level ( $ p=0.073 $ ). Furthermore, speaking native-level Spanish does not increase evaluations for the Hispanic candidate ( $ p=0.24 $ ). When evaluating the Anglo candidate, the candidate evaluation score increases 19%, from 0.51 in the English condition to 0.61 in the native-like Spanish condition ( $ p=0.003 $ ). Again, in Study 2, we do not see any significant effects positive or negative for ethnicity or language. That we do not observe the same results in a sample with more representative numbers of Democrats and Republicans, together with results from additional analyses included in Appendix N of the Supplementary Material, suggests that partisanship moderates responses to these appeals.Footnote 30 That said, neither the Hispanic candidate nor Spanish-speaking candidates receive lower evaluations from Anglo respondents. Our results suggest that candidates may not face any backlash for speaking Spanish among the Anglo population, and in fact may be rewarded by a subset of the population.

FIGURE 4. Mean Candidate Evaluation among Anglos by Treatment Group

Note: Results from the Prolific sample are shown on the left; Lucid sample is shown on the right. Group means are plotted with 84% confidence intervals. Dependent variable ranges from 0 to 1. Results in tabular form are shown in Table I.3 in Appendix I of the Supplementary Material.

DISCUSSION

Our results suggest that if Hispanic or Anglo candidates want to court Hispanic voters, putting to use their Spanish-language skills while campaigning can be an effective strategy. The ability to speak native-like Spanish acts as a signal to the Hispanic community that the candidate has the ability and willingness to represent Hispanic interests. However, only native-level proficiency appears to act as a credible signal of this understanding and commitment. When the candidate moves from speaking only in English to speaking Spanish with a non-native accent, Hispanics’ evaluations of the Anglo candidate remain the same but are significantly lower for the Hispanic candidate. In other words, speaking non-native Spanish as a Hispanic candidate seems to remove the connection that Hispanic voters feel based on shared ethnicity. This has important implications for how candidates choose to reach out to Hispanic candidates. Hispanic candidates without a native-like accent should avoid memorized and practiced lines in Spanish. Anglo candidates, not carrying the burden of being expected to speak Spanish, stand to gain from language-based appeals. At the highest level of proficiency, Anglo candidates can close the gap among Hispanics in overall evaluation between them and Hispanic candidates.

The results from our experiment may provide context for our content analysis of congressional Spanish ads. Although our content analysis suggests that it is rare for Hispanic candidates to speak Spanish with a non-native accent, if Hispanic candidates know they will be punished for poor Spanish-language skills, they may be avoiding it altogether. While it is even rarer for Anglo candidates to have a native-like accent when speaking Spanish, the experimental results suggest that honing their language skills is a fruitful path for appealing to Hispanics.Footnote 31

Our work does not mean to imply that Hispanic voting behavior can be explained solely by ethnic appeals, whether through shared ethnicity or language. While Hispanic voters use ethnicity as a cue (Barreto Reference Barreto2010), it does not supersede other potential sources of information such as party label (Casellas, Gillion, and Wallace Reference Casellas, Gillion and Wallace2019; Michelson Reference Michelson2005).Footnote 32 Furthermore, Hispanic voters rate policy issues as more important than language ability. In a 2019 poll of eligible Latino voters by the advocacy group UnidosUS, 61% of respondents said that they thought it was “very important” that a presidential candidate had plans for issues of interest to the Latino community, whereas only 33% said the same about a candidate’s ability to speak Spanish (UnidosUS and Decisions 2019). Our results show, however, that while Hispanics may not explicitly care about a candidate’s Spanish-language ability, and often rank it low on their list of important qualities in a candidate, Hispanics’ evaluations of candidates are shaped by such efforts nonetheless.

In addition to examining how Hispanic Americans react to Spanish-language appeals, we also looked at the reactions of Anglo voters. Contrary to our expectations that Anglo voters would evaluate candidates making these appeals less favorably, Anglo respondents in Study 1 gave more positive evaluations to the Hispanic candidate and to candidates that spoke native-like Spanish. Results from Study 2 also go against our expectations, as we find no differences in candidate evaluations among Anglo respondents based on candidate ethnicity or Spanish-language appeals. Based on exploratory analyses of the moderating role of ideology and partisanship, we are inclined to attribute our original results to our Anglo sample being, on average, more liberal and more Democratic compared to Anglos in the general population (see Appendix N of the Supplementary Material). Previous work shows that self-identified liberals tend to score higher on the openness to experience personality trait, which could result in those individuals being more likely to want diverse representation. Individuals high in openness to experience as well as other psychological propellers of change (i.e., individual difference variables that facilitate one’s craving for change) may seek further change in their environment being accustomed to demographic change (see, e.g., cultural inertia theory; Bellovary, Armenta, and Reyna Reference Bellovary, Armenta, Reyna, Nadler and Voyles2020; Zárate, Reyna, and Alvarez Reference Zárate, Reyna and Alvarez2019). In sum, in contrast with earlier studies that found negative effects for Spanish-language appeals among Anglos (Flores and Coppock Reference Flores and Coppock2018; Lavariega Monforti, Michelson, and Franco Reference Lavariega Monforti, Michelson and Franco2013), our study provides evidence that candidates can appeal to Hispanic voters by speaking Spanish without fear of alienating Anglo voters from their electoral coalition.

Judging by the increasing proportion of minority elected officials in the last two decades, the U.S. electorate appears to be more accepting of minority candidates than before. In Congress, for instance, while only 11% of individuals in the House and Senate identified as a racial or ethnic minority in 2001, this number had increased to 23% in 2021 (Schaeffer Reference Schaeffer2021). In addition, recent work shows that when minority candidates run, they are just as likely to win compared to white candidates (Juenke and Shah Reference Juenke and Shah2016). In the context of these developments and the census projections that the U.S. population will be majority–minority by 2045 (Frey Reference Frey2018), politicians may be willing to try and appeal to these groups by speaking their ethnic language more often in the coming years. Our findings on the importance of language proficiency point to some crucial aspects of campaigning that warrant further attention from scholars and practitioners. First, the quality of a campaign appeal impacts their effectiveness. Politicians may attempt symbolic appeals that do not have the intended effect. For example, Ben Carson running a rap campaign advertisement or Gerald Ford eating a tamal with the corn husk on did not connect with their target audience. Not all appeals are executed equally and scholars should pay attention to how these appeals, such as speaking Spanish in a non-native accent, affect voter evaluations. Second, the language a candidate speaks, and the accent with which they speak it, conveys information to the voter. Individuals “make assumptions about the speaker on the basis of language markers that signal alliance to certain social groups, primarily those having to do with race, ethnicity and economic factors” (Lippi-Green Reference Lippi-Green2012, 15). Considering the myriad of accents within the United States, scholars should look at how a candidate’s accent affects their evaluation among different groups of voters.

CONCLUSION

The proportion of Hispanic voters is growing in the United States making their vote increasingly more sought-after. One method that politicians have used to try to sway Hispanic voters is speaking Spanish at public events. Previous work suggests that Hispanic support for Anglo and Hispanic candidates may be higher when candidates speak Spanish, but it relies on telling respondents about the candidate’s language ability (Alamillo and Collingwood Reference Alamillo and Collingwood2017; Lavariega Monforti, Michelson, and Franco Reference Lavariega Monforti, Michelson and Franco2013). Other work exposes respondents to real-life campaign ads but finds an effect for only one of two Anglo Republican candidates (Flores and Coppock Reference Flores and Coppock2018). We argue that variation in language proficiency affects how effective language-based appeals are as a signal of ability and willingness to represent Hispanic interests. Our experimental design allows us to vary the ethnicity of the candidate and their language proficiency while holding other characteristics such as prosody (i.e., the rhythm and intonation of speech) and substantive content constant. Results show that on average, Hispanics give more favorable evaluations for co-ethnic candidates and candidates who speak Spanish at a high level of proficiency regardless of ethnicity. However, when the Hispanic candidate has non-native-accented Spanish, their evaluation is similar to that of an Anglo candidate speaking English. This study suggests that Hispanic candidates who have limited Spanish proficiency may lose the positive evaluation that shared ethnicity would otherwise give them.

While we are confident in our ability to isolate the effect of Spanish-language appeals and ethnicity, we chose to focus on how Spanish words are pronounced as the source of variation in language proficiency rather than on what is said. Our hypothetical candidate pronounced the same grammatically correct sentences but with a different accent. In reality, individuals also show varying levels of proficiency with a language’s grammar. Future work should explore how poor grammar impacts the effectiveness of language-based appeals whether they are delivered on the debate stage or through advertisements, social media messages, or campaign websites (Rodriguez Reference Rodriguez2019). In addition, Hispanic Americans have different national origins and corresponding Spanish-language variants. Scholars could study whether differences in word choice and pronunciation, or signaling a specific national identity, matter when making campaign appeals to the Hispanic community. Furthermore, it is worth noting that our findings are obtained in the context of a low-information environment. The effect of these Spanish-language appeals may be conditioned by the candidate’s stances on policy issues of interest to the Hispanic community. Candidates who speak Spanish with a strong non-native accent but who emphasize issues that Hispanics care about may see an overall positive impact, whereas a candidate with native-like speech but who advocates for harmful policies might not see any benefit.

Considering that the United States is a multiracial society and in a couple of decades there will be no racial or ethnic majority, scholars should continue to explore how politicians campaign in a diverse society (Collingwood Reference Collingwood2020). In addition to studying other languages and accents, future research should address how targeted appeals toward Hispanics affect the candidate evaluations among other racial minorities such as African Americans and Asian Americans, as well as how the effectiveness of targeted appeals toward different groups is conditioned by the quality of the appeal. Through this, we can develop a more complete understanding of how citizens form their impressions of candidates.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055423000084.

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

Research documentation and data that support the findings of this study are openly available in the American Political Science Review Dataverse at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/GT15SC. Limitations on data availability are discussed in the Supplementary Material.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We would like to thank Matthew Hayes, Leslie Schwindt-Bayer, Rick Wilson, Randy Stevenson, Robert Stein, Jason Casellas, our conference discussants including Pei-Te Lien and Sarah Allen Gerson, and three anonymous reviewers for the feedback and comments they provided for this project.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

Marques G. Zárate and Enrique Quezada-Llanes contributed equally.

FUNDING STATEMENT

This research was funded by the Douglas S. Harlan Program at Rice University.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare no ethical issues or conflicts of interest in this research.

ETHICAL STANDARDS

The authors declare the human subjects research in this article was reviewed and approved by Rice University’s Institutional Review Board and certificate numbers are provided in the Supplementary Material. The authors affirm that this article adheres to the APSA’s Principles and Guidance on Human Subject Research.

Footnotes

1 We use the term Hispanic rather than Latino because we are primarily concerned with how individuals with a Spanish-speaking national heritage evaluate candidates who make appeals in Spanish. When discussing the existing literature, we may use Latino as this is the more commonly used term by scholars.

2 For a full description of our content analysis, see Section G.2 of the Supplementary Material.

3 We adopt the definition of proficiency used by Bachman and Palmer (Reference Bachman and Adrian2010), which focuses on grammatical knowledge (cited in Treffers-Daller Reference Treffers-Daller2019). This includes features such as syntax and morphology as well as phonology (how language sounds). For a discussion of this and other related terms, see Section E of the Supplementary Material.

4 The term Anglo is used throughout the article to refer to white, English-speaking individuals.

5 The word accent does not have a technical definition in linguistics, but people usually use it to mean how people sound when they speak. This includes intonation and stress patterns at the word and sentence level (prosodical features) as well as the sounds of vowels and consonants (segmental features; Lippi-Green Reference Lippi-Green2012). See Section E of the Supplementary Material for a more complete discussion.

6 Linked fate is the degree to which an ethnic group member believes that their interests are dependent on the fate of the larger group. While this concept was first proposed to explain the political behavior of African Americans (Dawson Reference Dawson1994), scholars have examined whether other racial and ethnic minority groups exhibit similar levels of linked fate and group consciousness (McClain et al. Reference McClain, Johnson Carew, Walton and Watts2009). Among Latinos, determinants of linked fate change with the political environment, but research finds that economic status, generational status, and perceptions of discrimination are the strongest predictors of Latino linked fate (Sanchez and Masuoka Reference Sanchez and Masuoka2010; Sanchez, Masuoka, and Abrams Reference Sanchez, Masuoka and Abrams2019). In turn, these group orientations shape Latino political participation and policy attitudes (Sanchez Reference Sanchez2006a).

7 Because of the varieties of Spanish that are spoken throughout Latin America, it is possible that Hispanic Americans of a specific heritage only respond to candidates when they speak their own variant.

8 The hypotheses and the analysis plan were preregistered for both studies. The preregistration for Study 1 can be found at https://osf.io/6ebav/ and at https://osf.io/g36q9 for Study 2. In addition to the hypotheses included in the main text, we also preregistered hypotheses to test the role of ideology as a potential moderator among Hispanics and Anglos as well as how racial group strength moderates responses among Anglos. Due to space constraints, we include these analyses in Sections M and F of the Supplementary Material, respectively.

9 Based on the results of a pilot study conducted in the 2020 spring and summer terms at the University of Texas El Paso, results from a power analysis suggested we collect about four hundred responses. We decided to collect five hundred responses for each group in order to account for missing data as well as boost our statistical power that would allow us to test for the moderating effects of group strength.

10 Prolific is a crowdsourcing platform promising high-quality data for academic research. In a comparison with similar platforms, Peer et al. (Reference Peer, Brandimarte, Samat and Acquisti2017) conclude that Prolific is a viable alternative to Amazon Mechanical Turk: Data quality—assessed through attention checks and ability to replicate findings of psychological research—is high and the subject pool is “more naive to common experimental research tasks” (161).

11 Table A.4 in Section A.2 of the Supplementary Material contains the demographic breakdown of our Prolific sample. See Table B.5 in Section B.3 of the Supplementary Material for full demographic characteristics and comparisons with population estimates among our Lucid sample. For information on the compensation of our sample, go to Section H of the Supplementary Material.

12 Audio clips may be accessed at Zárate, Quezada-Llanes, and Armenta (Reference Zárate, Quezada-Llanes and Armenta2023). See Section A of the Supplementary Material for more detail.

13 To examine whether our language treatments worked as expected, our manipulation checks asked respondents to indicate the languages they heard the candidate speak. In Study 1, 93% of respondents in a Spanish condition correctly identified that the candidate spoke Spanish. When asked to rate the quality on a scale from 0 to 10, those in the non-native condition gave an average rating of 4.55 for the Anglo candidate and 4.06 for the Hispanic candidate. Those in the native-like Spanish conditions ranked the Anglo candidate’s Spanish 8.09 and the Hispanic candidates’ 8.55. See Section A.2 of the Supplementary Material for more detail on Study 1 or Section B.3 for this information on Study 2. The audio clips used in Studies 1 and 2 are the same.

14 The voice actor featured is a native Spanish speaker and bilingual.

15 See Section A of the Supplementary Material for question wordings.

16 The Chronbach’s alpha for these variables is high at 0.89 and 0.88 among our Hispanic samples from Studies 1 and 2, respectively. All of the items strongly load onto a single factor. For more information on our scale development, go to Section D of the Supplementary Material.

17 We measure group strength by asking participants “How important is being (white/Latino) to your identity?” Participants are only asked about white identity if they marked being white and only asked about Latino identity if they marked being Latino.

18 In Appendix R of the Supplementary Material, we provide evidence that our treatments did not impact strength of ethnic identification in either sample.

19 We ran a separate model where we regressed the candidate evaluation index on a binary indicator where 1 indicates being in either our non-native or native-like Spanish conditions. The coefficient for the grouped Spanish conditions was small but positive and statistically significant ( $ b=0.056 $ , $ p<0.001 $ ). However, as we show, once we separate the Spanish conditions by accent, the results show that the positive impact is due entirely to the native-like Spanish treatment. We report these models for both Hispanic and Anglo participants in Section C of the Supplementary Material.

20 We ran both models with and without controls for age, gender, education, ideology, partisanship, and income. Overall, the results in all models are virtually unchanged when we include these controls. See Table J.1 in Section J of the Supplementary Material for full results.

21 Results for Study 2 are pooled by election type (nonpartisan and in-party primary). We report the results by election type in Section B.4 of the Supplementary Material. For results for the four individual variables, see Section I of the Supplementary Material.

22 This is equivalent to using a t-test to compare two group means where $ a=0.05 $ (Payton, Greenstone, and Schenker Reference Payton, Greenstone and Schenker2003). The use of 95% confidence intervals around predicted values to assess whether the difference between two estimates is statistically significant represents a more conservative test than the $ a=0.05 $ standard (Schenker and Gentleman Reference Schenker and Gentleman2001).

23 In Study 1, the Anglo candidate with native-like Spanish received statistically significantly higher evaluations than the English-only Hispanic candidate ( $ p=0.013 $ ). There were no significant differences between these two conditions in Study 2.

24 See Section F of the Supplementary Material for more detail, including Study 2 results.

25 For more detail on Study 2 as well as question wordings and how these items scale together, go to Sections B and D of the Supplementary Material.

26 We conducted additional exploratory analyses to evaluate the potential moderating effect of respondents’ own Spanish-language ability, ideology, and partisanship. These analyses can be found in Sections P, M, and N of the Supplementary Material, respectively.

27 This reflects our shared-ethnicity hypothesis for Anglo voters in our preregistration plan.

28 For information on our Anglo mechanism testing, go to Section S of the Supplementary Material.

29 For information on the demographic breakdown of our Study 1 sample, go to Section A.2 of the Supplementary Material. You can find this information for Study 2 in Section B.3.

30 In Study 1, ideology has a limited role in how Anglos evaluate the Anglo candidate but has no impact on evaluations of the Hispanic candidate. Liberals show greater support for the Anglo candidate who speaks non-native Spanish compared with conservative respondents. Partisanship also plays only a limited role with the only clear difference being that Republican respondents show higher support for the Anglo candidate who speaks only in English compared with Democratic respondents. In Study 2, we split the sample by party and find that some of the results from Study 1 replicate among Democrats but not Republicans.

31 We would like to thank one of our anonymous reviewers for this comment.

32 Results from Study 2 lend further support to this point as in-party English-speaking candidates receive similar evaluations to candidates who speak native-like Spanish. See Appendix T of the Supplementary Material.

References

Abrajano, Marisa. 2010. Campaigning to the New American Electorate: Advertising to Latino Voters. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Abrajano, Marisa, and Panagopoulos, Costas. 2011. “Does Language Matter? The Impact of Spanish Versus English-Language GOTV Efforts on Latino Turnout.” American Politics Research 39 (4): 643–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alamillo, Rudy, and Collingwood, Loren. 2017. “Chameleon Politics: Social Identity and Racial Cross-Over Appeals.” Politics, Groups, and Identities 5 (4): 533–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alba, Richard, and Nee, Victor. 1997. “Rethinking Assimilation Theory for a New Era of Immigration.” International Migration Review 31 (4): 826–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachman, Lyle, and Adrian, Palmer. 2010. Language Assessment in Practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barreto, Matt A. 2007. “¡Sí Se Puede! Latino Candidates and the Mobilization of Latino Voters.” American Political Science Review 101 (3): 425–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barreto, Matt A. 2010. Ethnic Cues: The Role of Shared Ethnicity in Latino Political Participation. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barreto, Matt A., Villarreal, Mario, and Woods, Nathan D.. 2005. “Metropolitan Latino Political Behavior: Voter Turnout and Candidate Preference in Los Angeles.” Journal of Urban Affairs 27 (1): 7191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellovary, Andrea, Armenta, Angel D., and Reyna, Christine. 2020. “Stereotypes of Immigrants and Immigration in the United States.” In Stereotypes: The Incidence and Impacts of Bias, eds. Nadler, Joel T. and Voyles, Elora C., 146–64. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bever, Lindsey. 2019. “‘This Is Not Mexico’: Texas Official Criticizes County Judge for Speaking Spanish.” The Texas Tribune, March 27. https://www.texastribune.org/2019/03/27/texas-official-criticizes-judge-spanish/.Google Scholar
Bobo, Lawrence, and Hutchings, Vincent L.. 1996. “Perceptions of Racial Group Competition: Extending Blumer’s Theory of Group Position to a Multiracial Social Context.” American Sociological Review 61 (6): 951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casellas, Jason P., Gillion, Daniel Q., and Wallace, Sophia Jordán. 2019. “Race, Partisanship, and Attitudes Toward Public Policy Commonality and Legislative Districts.” Journal of Race, Ethnicity, and Politics 4 (1): 3259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casellas, Jason P., and Wallace, Sophia J.. 2015. “The Role of Race, Ethnicity, and Party on Attitudes Toward Descriptive Representation.” American Politics Research 43 (1): 144–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collingwood, Loren. 2020. Campaigning in a Racially Diversifying America. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Contreras, Russell, and Anderson, Curt. 2019. “Democrats’ Use of Spanish in Debate Evokes Praise, Eye Rolls.” https://apnews.com/article/c6bff5aeda8944518dcbbee587721372.Google Scholar
Dawson, Michael C. 1994. Behind the Mule: Race and Class in African-American Politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
de la Garza, Rodolfo O., Falcon, Angelo, and Chris Garcia, F.. 1996. “Will the Real Americans Please Stand Up: Anglo and Mexican-American Support of Core American Political Values.” American Journal of Political Science 40 (2): 335–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeFrancesco Soto, Victoria M., and Merolla, Jennifer L.. 2006. “Vota Por Tu Futuro: Partisan Mobilization of Latino Voters in the 2000 Presidential Election.” Political Behavior 28 (4): 285304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dietrich, Bryce J., Hayes, Matthew, and O’Brien, Diana Z.. 2019. “Pitch Perfect: Vocal Pitch and the Emotional Intensity of Congressional Speech.” American Political Science Review 113 (4): 941–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egan, Patrick J. 2020. “Identity as Dependent Variable: How Americans Shift Their Identities to Align with Their Politics.” American Journal of Political Science 64 (3): 699716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flores, Alejandro, and Coppock, Alexander. 2018. “Do Bilinguals Respond More Favorably to Candidate Advertisements in English or in Spanish?Political Communication 35 (4): 612–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraga, Luis Ricardo, and Leal, David L.. 2004. “PLAYING THE “LATINO CARD”: Race, Ethnicity, and National Party Politics.” Du Bois Review: Social Science Research on Race 1 (2): 297317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frey, William H. 2018. “The US Will Become ‘Minority White’ in 2045, CENSUS PROJECTS.” https://www.brookings.edu/blog/the-avenue/2018/03/14/the-us-will-become-minority-white-in-2045-census-projects/.Google Scholar
Frymer, Paul. 1999. Uneasy Alliances: Race and Party Competition in America. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
García Bedolla, Lisa. 2003. “The Identity Paradox: Latino Language, Politics and Selective Dissociation.” Latino Studies 1 (2): 264–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García Bedolla, Lisa. 2005. Fluid Borders: Latino Power, Identity, and Politics in Los Angeles. Berkeley: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez-Barrera, Ana, and Lopez, Mark Hugo. 2013. “Spanish Is the Most Spoken Non-English Language in U.S. Homes, Even Among Non-Hispanics.” Technical Report, Pew Research Center.Google Scholar
González, Daniel, and Nowicki, Dan. 2015. “Trump, Rivals Spar over Speaking Spanish, Immigration.” https://www.azcentral.com/story/news/2015/09/17/trump-rivals-spar-over-speaking-spanish-immigration/32534755/.Google Scholar
Hersh, Eitan D., and Schaffner, Brian F.. 2013. “Targeted Campaign Appeals and the Value of Ambiguity.” Journal of Politics 75 (2): 520–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hitlan, Robert Thomas, Kelly, Kristine M., Schepman, Stephen, Schneider, Kimberly T., and Zárate, Michael A.. 2006. “Language Exclusion and the Consequences of Perceived Ostracism in the Workplace.” Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice 10 (1): 5670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hitlan, Robert Thomas, Zárate, Michael A., Kelly, Kristine M., and Catherine DeSoto, M.. 2016. “Linguistic Ostracism Causes Prejudice: Support for a Serial Mediation Effect.” Journal of Social Psychology 156 (4): 422–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juenke, Eric Gonzalez, and Shah, Paru. 2016. “Demand and Supply: Racial and Ethnic Minority Candidates in White Districts.” Journal of Race, Ethnicity, and Politics 1 (1): 6090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klar, Samara, Leeper, Thomas, and Robison, Joshua. 2020. “Studying Identities with Experiments: Weighing the Risk of Posttreatment Bias Against Priming Effects.” Journal of Experimental Political Science 7 (1): 5660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koslow, Scott, Shamdasani, Prem N., and Touchstone, Ellen E.. 1994. “Exploring Language Effects in Ethnic Advertising: A Sociolinguistic Perspective.” Journal of Consumer Research 20 (4): 575–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krogstad, Jens Manuel, and Lopez, Mark Hugo. 2017. “Spanish Speaking Declines for Hispanics in U.S. Metro Areas.” Technical Report, Pew Research Center.Google Scholar
Lavariega Monforti, Jessica, Michelson, Melissa, and Franco, Annie. 2013. “¿Por Quién Votará? Experimental Evidence about Language, Ethnicity and Vote Choice (Among Republicans).” Politics, Groups and Identities 1 (4): 475–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lippi-Green, Rosina. 2012. English with an Accent: Language, Ideology and Discrimination in the United States, 2nd edition. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez, Mark Hugo, Gonzalez-Barrera, Ana, Krogstad, Jens Manuel, and López, Gustavo. 2016. “Democrats Maintain Edge as Party ‘More Concerned’ for Latinos, but Views Similar to 2012.” Technical Report, Pew Research Center.Google Scholar
Mann, Christopher B., Michelson, Melissa R., and Davis, Matt. 2020. “What Is the Impact of Bilingual Communication to Mobilize Latinos? Exploratory Evidence from Experiments in New Jersey, North Carolina, and Virginia.” Electoral Studies 65: 102132. doi:10.1016/j.electstud.2020.102132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mansbridge, Jane. 1999. “Should Blacks Represent Blacks and Women Represent Women? A Contingent ‘Yes’.” Journal of Politics 61 (3): 628–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manzano, Sylvia, and Sanchez, Gabriel R.. 2010. “Take One for the Team? Limits of Shared Ethnicity and Candidate Preferences.” Political Research Quarterly 63 (3): 568–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masuoka, Natalie, and Junn, Jane. 2013. The Politics of Belonging: Race, Public Opinion, and Immigration. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClain, Paula D., Johnson Carew, Jessica D., Walton, Eugene, and Watts, Candis S.. 2009. “Group Membership, Group Identity, and Group Consciousness: Measures of Racial Identity in American Politics?Annual Review of Political Science 12: 471–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConnaughy, Corrine M., White, Ismail K., Leal, David L., and Casellas, Jason P.. 2010. “A Latino on the Ballot: Explaining Coethnic Voting Among Latinos and the Response of White Americans.” Journal of Politics 72 (4): 1199–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michelson, Melissa R. 2005. “Does Ethnicity Trump Party? Competing Vote Cues and Latino Voting Behavior.” Journal of Political Marketing 4 (4): 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milroy, Lesley. 1982. “Language and Group Identity.” Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 3 (3): 207–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, Jacob M., Nyhan, Brendan, and Torres, Michelle. 2018. “How Conditioning on Posttreatment Variables Can Ruin Your Experiment and What to Do About It.” American Journal of Political Science 62 (3): 760–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panagopoulos, Costas, and Green, Donald P.. 2011. “Spanish-Language Radio Advertisements and Latino Voter Turnout in the 2006 Congressional Elections: Field Experimental Evidence.” Political Research Quarterly 64 (3): 588–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payton, Mark E., Greenstone, Matthew H., and Schenker, Nathaniel. 2003. “Overlapping Confidence Intervals or Standard Error Intervals: What Do They Mean in Terms of Statistical Significance?Journal of Insect Science 3 (1): 34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peer, Eyal, Brandimarte, Laura, Samat, Sonam, and Acquisti, Alessandro. 2017. “Beyond the Turk: Alternative Platforms for Crowdsourcing Behavioral Research.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 70: 153–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez, Efrén O. 2016. Unspoken Politics: Implicit Attitudes and Political Thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ramírez, Ricardo. 2011. “Mobilization En Español: Spanish-Language Radio and the Activation of Political Identities.” In Rallying for Immigrant Rights: The Fight for Inclusion in 21st Century America, eds. Voss, Kim and Bloemraad, Irene, 63–81. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Rodriguez, Jesús. 2019. “Latino Outreach or Google Translate? 2020 Dems Bungle Spanish Websites.” https://politico.com/interactives/2019/2020-democrats-spanish-translation/.Google Scholar
Sanchez, Gabriel R. 2006a. “The Role of Group Consciousness in Latino Public Opinion.” Political Research Quarterly 59 (3): 435–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanchez, Gabriel R. 2006b. “The Role of Group Consciousness in Political Participation Among Latinos in the United States.” American Politics Research 34 (4): 427–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanchez, Gabriel R., and Masuoka, Natalie. 2010. “Brown-Utility Heuristic? The Presence and Contributing Factors of Latino Linked Fate.” Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 32 (4): 519–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanchez, Gabriel, Masuoka, Natalie, and Abrams, Brooke. 2019. “Revisiting the Brown-Utility Heuristic: A Comparison of Latino Linked Fate in 2006 and 2016.” Politics, Groups, and Identities 7 (3): 673–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaeffer, Katherine. 2021. “Racial, Ethnic Diversity Increases yet Again with the 117th Congress.” Technical Report, Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2021/01/28/racial-ethnic-diversity-increases-yet-again-with-the-117th-congress.Google Scholar
Schenker, Nathaniel, and Gentleman, Jane F.. 2001. “On Judging the Significance of Differences by Examining the Overlap Between Confidence Intervals.” American Statistician 55 (3): 182–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schildkraut, Deborah J. 2013. “Which Birds of a Feather Flock Together? Assessing Attitudes About Descriptive Representation Among Latinos and Asian Americans.” American Politics Research 41 (4): 699729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schildkraut, Deborah J. 2017. “White Attitudes About Descriptive Representation in the US: The Roles of Identity, Discrimination, and Linked Fate.” Politics, Groups, and Identities 5 (1): 84106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stack, Liam. 2019. “A Border Agent Detained Two Americans Speaking Spanish. Now They Have Sued.” New York Times, February 14.Google Scholar
Stout, Christopher T. 2020. The Case for Identity Politics Polarization, Demographic Change, and Racial Appeals. Charlottesville: University of Virginia Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, Henri, and Turner, John C.. 1979. “An Integrative Theory of Intergroup Conflict.” In The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations, eds. William G. Austin and Stephen Worchel, 33–47. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Treffers-Daller, Jeanine. 2019. “What Defines Language Dominance in Bilinguals?Annual Review of Linguistics 5: 375–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
UnidosUS and Latino Decisions. 2019. “The 2020 National Latino Electorate Survey.” Technical Report, UnidosUS.Google Scholar
Wallace, Sophia J. 2014. “Examining Latino Support for Descriptive Representation: The Role of Identity and Discrimination.” Social Science Quarterly 95 (2): 311–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wamble, Julian J. 2019. “The Chosen One: How Community Commitment Makes Certain Representatives More Preferable.” Paper presented at the Midwest Political Science Association Annual Meeting, Chicago.Google Scholar
Zárate, Michael A., Reyna, Christine, and Alvarez, Miriam J.. 2019. “Cultural Inertia, Identity, and Intergroup Dynamics in a Changing Context.” Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 59: 175233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zárate, Marques G., Quezada-Llanes, Enrique, and Armenta, Angel D.. 2023 “Replication Data for: Se Habla Español: Spanish-Language Appeals and Candidate Evaluations in the United States.” Harvard Dataverse. Dataset. https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/GT15SC.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zou, Linda X., and Cheryan, Sapna. 2017. “Two Axes of Subordination: A New Model of Racial Position.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 112 (5): 696717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

TABLE 1. Effects of Language and Ethnicity on Candidate Evaluations among Hispanics

Figure 1

FIGURE 1. Mean Candidate Evaluation among Hispanics by Treatment GroupNote: Results from the Prolific sample (October 2020) are shown on the left; Lucid sample (December 2021 to January 2022) is shown on the right. Group means are plotted with 84% confidence intervals. Dependent variable ranges from 0 to 1. Results in tabular form are shown in Table I.1 in Section I of the Supplementary Material.

Figure 2

FIGURE 2. Predicted Candidate Evaluation among Hispanics by Treatment Group across Levels of Ethnic Identity (Study 1)Note: Larger values on the x-axis indicate greater importance of ethnicity to an individual’s identity. Predicted values are plotted with 84% confidence intervals. Dependent variable ranges from 0 to 1. Full model results are shown in Model 1 of Table K.2 in Section K of the Supplementary Material.

Figure 3

FIGURE 3. Mean Perceived Candidate Ability and Willingness among Hispanics by Treatment GroupNote: Results are from the Lucid sample. Panel A reports the group means for the ability index. Panel B reports the group means for the willingness index. Group means are plotted with 84% confidence intervals. The dependent variables range from 0 to 1. Results in tabular form are shown in Table I.2 in Section I of the Supplementary Material.

Figure 4

TABLE 2. Effects of Language and Ethnicity on Candidate Evaluations among Anglos

Figure 5

FIGURE 4. Mean Candidate Evaluation among Anglos by Treatment GroupNote: Results from the Prolific sample are shown on the left; Lucid sample is shown on the right. Group means are plotted with 84% confidence intervals. Dependent variable ranges from 0 to 1. Results in tabular form are shown in Table I.3 in Appendix I of the Supplementary Material.

Supplementary material: PDF

Zárate et al. supplementary material

Zárate et al. supplementary material
Download Zárate et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 256.8 KB
Supplementary material: Link

Zárate et al. Dataset

Link
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.