Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T01:22:24.510Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are Stressful Life Events Causally Related to the Severity of Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms? A Monozygotic Twin Difference Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 April 2020

P. Vidal-Ribas*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, PO Box 85, 16, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
A. Stringaris
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, PO Box 85, 16, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
C. Rück
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Division of Psychiatry, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
E. Serlachius
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Division of Psychiatry, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
P. Lichtenstein
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
D. Mataix-Cols
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Division of Psychiatry, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, United Kingdom
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail address:[email protected] (P. Vidal-Ribas).

Abstract

Traumatic or stressful life events have long been hypothesized to play a role in causing or precipitating obsessive-compulsive symptoms but the impact of these environmental factors has rarely been investigated using genetically informative designs. We tested whether a wide range of retrospectively-reported stressful life events (SLEs) influence the lifetime presence and severity of obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCS) in a large Swedish population-based cohort of 22,084 twins. Multiple regression models examined whether differences in SLEs within twin pairs were significantly associated with differences in OCS. In the entire sample (i.e., both monozygotic [MZ] and dizygotic twin pairs), two SLEs factors, “abuse and family disruption” and “sexual abuse”, were significantly associated with the severity of OCS even after controlling for depressive symptoms. Other SLEs factors were either not associated with OCS (“loss”, “non-sexual assault”) or were no longer associated with OCS after controlling for depression (“illness/injury”). Within MZ pair analyses, which effectively control for genetic and shared environmental effects, showed that only the “abuse and family disruption” factor remained independently related to within-pair differences in OCS severity, even after controlling for depressive symptoms. Despite being statistically significant, the magnitude of the associations was small; “abuse and family disruption” explained approximately 3% of the variance in OCS severity. We conclude that OCS are selectively associated with certain types of stressful life events. In particular, a history of interpersonal abuse, neglect and family disruption may make a modest but significant contribution to the severity of OCS. Further replication in longitudinal cohorts is essential before causality can be firmly established.

Type
Original article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Copyright
Copyright © Elsevier Masson SAS 2014

1. Introduction

Controlled family studies have consistently found that obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a familial disorder. For example, first-degree relatives of OCD probands are approximately 4 to 5 times more likely to have OCD themselves, compared to relatives of unaffected controls [Reference Pauls53,Reference Taylor69,Reference Mataix-Cols, Boman, Monzani, Ruck, Serlachius and Langstrom44,Reference Nestadt, Samuels, Riddle, Bienvenu, Liang and LaBuda52]. This familial risk decreases as the genetic distance with the proband increases. For example, second- and third-degree relatives are approximately 2 and 1.5 times more likely to have OCD, respectively, compared to relatives of unaffected controls, strongly suggesting a genetic component to OCD [Reference Mataix-Cols, Boman, Monzani, Ruck, Serlachius and Langstrom44]. Twin studies primarily conducted in population-based (non-clinical) samples estimate the genetic contribution to be in the region of 40–50% [69,44,72]. Consistently, gene-searching efforts are well underway [Reference Stewart, Yu, Scharf, Neale, Fagerness and Mathews67] and, though they have met with limited success to date, they are likely to continue [Reference Pauls53].

Both family and twin studies also converge to suggest that shared environmental factors (e.g. growing up in the same neighbourhood) are unlikely to play a major role in causing OCD. Instead, the types of environmental influences that seem important in OCD are thought to be of the “non-shared” type, that is, environmental factors that affect specific individuals rather than families [Reference Van Grootheest, Cath, Beekman and Boomsma72,Reference Jonnal, Gardner, Prescott and Kendler28]. Such non-shared environmental factors could be biological (e.g. infections) or psychosocial (e.g. traumatic and stressful life events [SLEs], like being involved in an accident, being physically or sexually abused, or the death of one's partner). While the link between such SLEs and the pathogenesis of depression has been well-established [Reference Kendler and Gardner31,Reference Kessler34], the evidence supporting a link between SLEs and OCD is much less conclusive because most studies have been conducted in clinical samples, based on retrospective reports and using designs that did not permit causal inferences. For example, research in clinical samples has shown that the association between OCD and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is larger than expected by chance [Reference Gershuny, Baer, Parker, Gentes, Infield and Jenike20,Reference Lafleur, Petty, Mancuso, McCarthy, Biederman and Faro38]. One study [Reference McKeon, Roa and Mann47] found that, compared to a control group, adults with OCD retrospectively-reported a greater number of SLEs one year before symptom onset. Similarly, in a large clinical sample of 265 adults with OCD, Cromer et al. [Reference Cromer, Schmidt and Murphy13] found that retrospectively-reported SLEs were associated with higher severity of OC symptoms (OCS) after controlling for a range of variables including depression severity. Similar findings have been reported in clinical samples of pediatric OCD [Reference Gothelf, Aharonovsky, Horesh, Carty and Apter23,Reference Sarkhel, Praharaj and Sinha63]. However, clinical and convenience samples are liable to referral and Berkson's type biases, which may result in an overestimation of the associations.

A range of epidemiological, largely retrospective studies have also found associations between specific SLEs and OCD. For example, Mathews et al. [Reference Mathews, Kaur and Stein45] found an association between history of physical abuse, physical neglect and emotional abuse in childhood and OCS in a sample of 938 college students. Saunders et al. [Reference Saunders, Villeponteaux, Lipovsky, Kilpatrick and Veronen64] and Boudreaux et al. [Reference Boudreaux, Kilpatrick, Resnick, Best and Saunders6] found that reported rape was associated with both lifetime and current OCD in a community sample of 391 women. Grisham et al. [Reference Grisham, Fullana, Mataix-Cols, Moffitt, Caspi and Poulton24] found an association between retrospectively-reported sexual and physical abuse before age 11 and a diagnosis of OCD at ages 26 or 32, even after controlling for comorbid post-traumatic stress disorder. While, taken together, these findings are suggestive of an association between SLEs and OCD, they do not help clarifying the nature of this association. In particular, these studies could not control for genetic variability between individuals. This is particularly important as genetic background may not only put people at risk of developing psychopathology but also at risk of exposure to specific SLEs [Reference Kendler and Baker30]. There is ample evidence that exposure to SLEs is at least partly heritable [Reference Kendler and Baker30,Reference Plomin, Lichtenstein, Pedersen, McClearn and Nesselroade56]. Therefore, isolating the non-shared variance of both OCS and SLEs is a crucial step to understand their association.

Genetically informative studies, in particular those employing the discordant monozygotic (MZ) twin design, are better suited to test whether the association between an environmental measure and an observed phenotype is likely to be consistent with a causal effect because they provide excellent control of both genetic and shared environmental effects. Because MZ-twins share 100% of their genetic background and grow up largely in the same environment, any observed phenotypic differences between members of a MZ-twin pair (e.g. differences in OCS) may be attributable to non-shared environment (e.g. non-shared SLEs) [Reference Vitaro, Brendgen and Arseneault73]. To our knowledge, only one previous study employed such design to test the association between SLEs and OCS in a population-based twin sample [Reference Cath, van Grootheest, Willemsen, van Oppen and Boomsma12]. The authors selected 25 pairs of MZ-twins discordant for OCS (based on pre-determined cut-offs on a self-report questionnaire) and compared high vs low scoring twins on a number of potential environmental stressors, including retrospectively ascertained SLEs. They found suggestive evidence that SLEs, and particularly sexual assaults, were associated with OCS at the trend level (P = 0.08). It should be noted that this study did not adjust for other variables, such as depression, when comparing SLEs within discordant pairs. This is important because, not only there is a strong association between SLEs and depression, but also between depression and OCS [31,5,25]. Given that the evidence for the association between SLEs and OCS is only suggestive, it is plausible that part of this association might be explained by the presence of depressive symptoms. Thus, it is tenable that controlling for symptoms of depression may weaken the association between SLEs and OCS. Nevertheless, the suggestive results of Cath et al. study [Reference Cath, van Grootheest, Willemsen, van Oppen and Boomsma12] merit replication in larger samples and expanding the number of SLEs examined.

In the present study, we used a genetically informative design to explore the relationship between a wide range of different SLEs and the severity of OCS in a large population-based sample of twins. Previous studies have often measured a limited range of different SLEs. In order to control for both genetic and shared environment effects and maximize the variance provided in our dataset, we employed the MZ-difference method [Reference Vitaro, Brendgen and Arseneault73,Reference Pike, Reiss, Hetherington and Plomin54]. This approach allows exploring whether the difference in SLEs between twins of a pair is associated with corresponding differences in the severity of OCS. This affords more statistical power than selecting high vs low symptom groups based on categorical cut-offs. Because there is a strong association between SLEs and depression and between depression and OCS [31,5,25], all our analyses controlled for depression severity in order to establish whether the presumed causal effect of SLEs on OCS is independent from depressive symptoms. Based on the previous literature, we made the following predictions:

  • there will be a significant association between SLEs and OCS severity in the entire sample of twins (not controlling for genetic or family factors);

  • SLEs will remain associated with OCS severity when genetic and shared environmental factors are strictly controlled in the analysis using the MZ-twin difference method;

  • the association between SLEs and OCS severity will be generally weakened by the introduction of depression in the models as some of the variance may be explained by the known association between SLEs and depression.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants were drawn from the study of twin adults: genes and environment (STAGE), a study of 25,381 Swedish twins born between 1959 and 1985 [Reference Lichtenstein, Sullivan, Cnattingius, Gatz, Johansson and Carlstrom40]. Further details on the recruitment and data collection methods can be found elsewhere [Reference Lichtenstein, Sullivan, Cnattingius, Gatz, Johansson and Carlstrom40,Reference Donahue, D’Onofrio, Lichtenstein and Langstrom16]. Briefly, twins were sent a letter inviting them to participate in the study and were given a personal login to the study web page. Non-responders were approached with up to three reminders. Twins could also choose to complete the questionnaire by telephone with a trained interviewer using a computer-based data collection method, supplemented with a mailed self-administered paper questionnaire for sensitive topics. Seventy-two percent of participants completed the web-based survey and 28% undertook the telephone interview. One hundred twins were re-contacted after 2 to 5 months in order to compare methods of data collection. For the OCD section, the agreement between the web-based survey and the telephone interview was perfect (Kappa = 1) [Reference Lichtenstein, Sullivan, Cnattingius, Gatz, Johansson and Carlstrom40]. The regional ethics committee of the Karolinska Institutet approved the project. All subjects provided informed consent electronically during the web-based survey or orally during the telephone interview.

Out of the original cohort, 22,517 twins completed a measure of OCS (see Measures section below). From these participants, we excluded all those who had at least one missing item on the OCS questionnaire. The final sample consisted of 22,084 twins (87% of the original cohort and 98% of the participants who completed the OCS questionnaire).

2.2. Measures

All participants completed the following self-report questionnaires as part of a larger wave of data collection.

2.2.1. Stressful life events (SLEs)

Stressful life events were assessed with a modified version of the women, co-occurring disorders, and violence study (WCDVS) version of the life stressor checklist-revised (LSC-R) [Reference McHugo, Caspi, Kammerer, Mazelis, Jackson and Russell46,Reference Wolfe, Kimerling, Wilson and Keane75]. This adaptation showed to have acceptable test-retest reliability in all lifetime items as well as in the summary variables [Reference McHugo, Caspi, Kammerer, Mazelis, Jackson and Russell46].

The instrument enquires about 34 different kinds of SLEs (e-Table 1, Supplementary data). The first 20 items, not included in the original LSC-R, enquire about general negative life events and are measured dichotomously (“yes” or “no”) depending on whether the event has happened to the respondent or not. The remaining 14 items, derived from the LSC-R, ask about interpersonal abuse and neglect. For each item, it asks about the occurrence (“yes” or “no”), the age when the event happened for the first time and its lifetime frequency (0 = never, 1 = once, 2 = a few times, 3 = a lot). For the purpose of this study, and in order to employ all 34 items, we only used information about the occurrence or non-occurrence of each event.

2.2.2. Obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCS)

A seven-item scale was employed to assess the lifetime presence and severity of the most common OCS (e-Table 2, Supplementary data). Each item is scored on a 3-point scale (0 = no, 1 = a little, 2 = a lot). This measure has been previously validated [Reference Mataix-Cols, Boman, Monzani, Ruck, Serlachius and Langstrom44] in a clinical sample of 91 OCD patients who participated in a controlled clinical trial of internet-based cognitive behaviour therapy for OCD [Reference Andersson, Enander, Andrén, Hedman, Ljótsson and Hursti3]. Briefly, the total score (sum of items 1–7) showed acceptable internal consistency (0.66) and correlated strongly with the criterion standards: Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (YBOCS) [Reference Goodman, Price, Rasmussen, Mazure, Fleischmann and Hill22]) total (Spearman's rho = 0.51), Obsessive-compulsive inventory-revised (OCI-R) [Reference Foa, Huppert, Leiberg, Langner, Kichic and Hajcak17] (rho = 0.76) and Dimensional Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (DOCS) [Reference Abramowitz, Deacon, Olatunji, Wheaton, Berman and Losardo1] (rho = 0.66). These correlations are comparable than those reported in previous studies that employed alternative, more established self-report measures of OCS [Reference Cath, van Grootheest, Willemsen, van Oppen and Boomsma12,Reference Denys, de Geus, van Megen and Westenberg14]. Conversely, the scale correlated more weakly and non-significantly with the Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS: [Reference Montgomery and Asberg49]) (rho = 0.20, n.s.), indicating good discriminant validity.

2.2.3. Depression

Because depression could confound the association between SLEs and OCS, we also included a measure of depression. We used the 11-item, Iowa version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale [Reference Carpenter, Andrykowski, Wilson, Hall, Rayens and Sachs11]. This scale asks about depressive symptoms during the last 2 weeks, with the higher scores indicating higher depression severity. This shortened form of the original version of the CESD has proved to have robust psychometric properties, showing a high internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha ranged 0.71–0.88) and large correlations with the original version (range r = 0.81–0.93) in different populations [Reference Carpenter, Andrykowski, Wilson, Hall, Rayens and Sachs11,Reference Kohout, Berkman, Evans and Cornoni-Huntley36].

2.3. Statistical analyses

2.3.1. Data reduction

In order to reduce the number of SLEs items, we used a two-step factor analytic approach. First, in a random half of the sample, we used the 34 SLEs items in an Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) with categorical factor indicators using a promax rotation. This yielded 8 different factors (1 through to 8 factors) with eigenvalues higher than 1. Second, in the other random half of the sample, we used a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to compare between these 8 models after removing all non-loading items. We compared models using the Akaike Information Criteria (AIC) and the Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC), where lower values indicate better fit [Reference Levy and Hancock39,Reference Burnham and Anderson9]. Goodness-of-fit was assessed in all models using Tucker Lewis Index (TLI), Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and Root Mean Square Error Of Approximation (RMSEA). For CFI and TLI, values greater than 0.95 are preferred and values near 0.90 are considered acceptable. For RMSEA, values of 0.05 or below are preferred and values up to 0.08 are considered acceptable [Reference Hooper, Coughlan and Mullen26]. Factor analyses were conducted in Mplus, version 5.1 [Reference Muthén and Muthén50].

As shown in e-Table 3, Supplementary data, the best fit to the data was for a model with the following five factors: “illness/injury”, “abuse and family disruption”, “loss”, “sexual abuse” and “non-sexual assault”. The 5-factor model fitted the data better than a more parsimonious 4-factor model (Δ AIC 918.937, Δ BIC 890.416 and chi-square difference test: 6264.450, df = 9, P < 0.0001).

The CFA indicated that 28 of the 34 initial variables loaded clearly (> 0.32) and independently (difference > 0.20) onto one or two of the five factors (e-Table 1, Supplementary data) [Reference Tabachnick and Fidell68]. Items that loaded in two factors were Adoption (loading in factors “abuse and family disruption” and “loss”), emotional neglect (loading in factors “abuse and family disruption” and “sexual abuse”) and victim of a hate crime (loading in factors “sexual abuse” and “non-sexual assault”). Item of break-up with a friend strictly met criteria for being added in the “illness/injury” factor. However, this item was removed because it was thought to have no relation with the topic of the factor and the difference required to meet the independence criteria was in the lower bound (difference = 0.201). Finally, another six items were removed from the analysis because of low loadings on all factors. Thus, 27 items grouped in 5 factors were used in the analyses. Total scores for each factor were computed summing raw scores of individual items and standardizing them to z-scores to retain the scale metric. This has the same effect as using average scores, which are useful to do comparisons across factors when there are differing numbers of items per factor [Reference DiStefano, Zhu and Mindrila15]. This approach is generally acceptable for most exploratory research situations [Reference Tabachnick and Fidell68]. It should be noted that 20% of the participants had missing SLEs data and they were thus excluded from the CFA.

2.3.2. Phenotypic relation between SLEs and OCS

Multivariate linear regression was used in the whole sample to determine whether SLEs were associated with OCS severity. In a second step, we used current depressive symptoms as a covariate to explore whether the results persisted. Note that these analyses do not control for either shared environment or genetic effects. As twins of a pair cannot be considered fully independent observations, not controlling for this relation would lead to the overestimation of P-values. Therefore, we employed a robust cluster option to account for the non-independence of twin pairs, allowing the standard errors to correlate within-pairs.

2.3.3. Within-pair analyses using the MZ-difference method

We analysed the relation between the lifetime occurrence of SLEs and the severity of OCS in all MZ complete pairs using within-pair difference scores in these variables [Reference Vitaro, Brendgen and Arseneault73,Reference Pike, Reiss, Hetherington and Plomin54]. The MZ-difference method uses relative difference scores, thus allowing for the examination of the association between SLEs and OCS severity with strict control of genetic and shared environmental factors. Relative difference scores were obtained by randomly assigning one of the twins as Twin 1 and the other as Twin 2, and subtracting the score of Twin 2 from the score of Twin 1. Multiple regression analyses were conducted to assess the contributions of differences in lifetime occurrence of SLEs and in current depressive symptoms between Twin 1 and Twin 2 to the prediction of differences in OCS severity between Twin 1 and Twin 2. Due to the high number of MZ-twin pairs with zero values in the OCS measure in both twins (57%), the distribution of differences in the OCS measure showed an excess positive kurtosis. Consequently, we decided to leave these pairs out of the analysis. As a result, 877 MZ-twin pairs were included in this analysis (819 pairs in the adjusted model).

All analyses described above were conducted in STATA 11 [65].

3. Results

3.1. Missing data analysis

In the entire sample, non-respondents to the OCS measure (n = 433) were more likely to be male (50% non-respondent vs 43% respondent; χ2 (1, n = 22,517) = 7.52, P < 0.01) and younger (32.4 vs 33.7 years old; t = 3.53, df = 22,515, P < 0.001) than respondents (n = 22,084). Non-respondents also reported higher depression severity than respondents (t = −2.96, df = 21,386, P < 0.01).

As mentioned earlier, of the 22,084 respondents to the OCS measure, around 20% had missing data on SLEs questions. People with missing SLEs data were more likely to be male (56% non-respondent vs 40% respondent; χ2 (1, n = 22,084) = [270.30, 386.26], P < 0.001), reported less severe OCS (t = [3.27,5.88], df = 22,082, P < 0.01) and more severe depressive symptoms (t = [−54.09, −51.18], df = 21,156, P < 0.0001).

Approximately 4% (n = 926) of the whole sample had missing data on depression severity. Non-respondents to the CESD were more likely to be female (57% respondent vs 61% of non-respondent females; χ2 (1, n = 22,084) = 7.67, P < 0.01) and reported higher OCS scores (t = −2.71, df = 22,082, P < 0.01).

Subsequent analyses were done with all the available data. Therefore, number of observations may vary across analyses.

3.2. Sample characteristics

Table 1 shows the sample composition by zygosity, and gender differences in age, OCS severity, depression severity (CESD score) and SLEs factors. In the whole sample, males were older than females. In addition, both in the entire sample and the MZ-twin subsample, females had significantly higher OCS scores and depression symptom severity. Whereas males reported more life events related to “illness/injury” and “non-sexual assault”, females were more likely to report life events related to “abuse and family disruption”, “loss”, and “sexual abuse”.

Table 1 Sample characteristics and gender differences in OCS severity, depression severity and stressful life events (SLEs).

SLEs: stressful life events; SD: stress disorder; MZ: monozygotic.

Ranges: age 19–47 years; OCS: obsessive-compulsive symptoms 0–14; CESD: Center For Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale 0–33; illness/injury 0–4; abuse and family disruption 0–7; loss 0–6; sexual abuse 0–7; non-sexual assault 0–6.

a Percentage within the whole sample.

Regarding the association with age, younger participants scored higher in OCS and depression symptom severity, both in the whole sample (OCS: r = −0.10, 95% CI −0.11, −0.09, P < 0.0001; CESD score: r = −0.08, 95% CI −0.09, −0.06, P < 0.0001) and the MZ-twin subsample (OCS: r = −0.10, 95% CI −0.12, −0.07, P < 0.0001; CESD score: r = −0.09, 95% CI −0.11, −0.07, P < 0.0001).

Older participants in the whole sample reported more SLEs related to “illness/injury” (r = 0.04, 95% CI 0.03, 0.06, P < 0.0001), “loss” (r = −0.17, 95% CI 0.16, 0.19, P < 0.0001), and “non-sexual assault” (r = 0.02, 95% CI 0.001, 0.03, P < 0.05). In the MZ subsample, only “illness/injury” and “loss” were positively correlated with age (r = 0.04, 95% CI 0.02, 0.07, P < 0.001 and r = 0.15, 95% CI 0.12, 0.17, P < 0.0001, respectively).

Given these results, we decided to use gender and age as covariates in the subsequent analyses.

3.3. Phenotypic association between SLEs and OCS

As seen in Table 2, SLEs factors of “illness/injury”, “abuse and family disruption” and “sexual abuse” were all independently related to OCS severity in the whole sample. However, when adjusting for current depressive symptoms, only “abuse and family disruption” and “sexual abuse” remained significantly associated with OCS severity.

Table 2 Summary of multiple linear regression predicting the severity of obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCS) from stressful life events in the whole sample of DZ and MZ-twins.

No adjustment for depression model, n = 16530; adjustment for depression model, n = 15919; β: standardised coefficient; analysis was done using robust clustering for twin pairs; all analyses are adjusted for age and gender.

a P < 0.01.

b P < 0.001.

3.4. Within-pair analyses using a MZ-difference method controlling for genetic and shared environment effects

Table 3 shows that, when controlling for genetic and shared environment effects, only differences in the factor “abuse and family disruption” were positively and independently related to differences in OCS severity. Moreover, this association remained significant when adjusting for differences in current depressive symptoms. The factor “abuse and family disruption” explained 3% of the variance in OCS.

Table 3 Summary of multiple regression analyses predicting within monozygotic pair differences in the severity of obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCS) from within monozygotic pair differences in stressful life events.

No adjustment for depression model, n = 877 pairs; adjustment for depression model, n = 819 pairs; β: standardised coefficient; MZ-twin pairs where both twins scored zero on the OCS measure are excluded from the analyses; all measures are based on difference scores; all analyses are adjusted for age and gender.

a P < 0.01.

b P < 0.001.

In order to get a deeper understanding of individual items contributing to these findings, we next performed paired t-tests within MZ-twins who were discordant for each individual item contained in this factor (Table 4). Monozygotic twins who reported exposure to “witnessing family violence before 18 years old”, “emotional neglect”, “physical neglect”, “physical abuse” and “serious family problem” scored higher in OCS severity than their respective unexposed twins. Conversely, there were no significant differences in OCS severity for the following two items: “separation or divorce of parents” and “having been adopted”.

Table 4 Difference in OCS severity between MZ-twin pairs discordant for individual items within the “Abuse and family disruption” factor.

OCS: obsessive-compulsive symptoms; MZ: monozygotic; SD: stress disorder.

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to explore the nature of the known association between SLEs and OCS in a large population-based sample of twins. To this end, we employed a genetically informative design, the MZ-twin difference method, which allows a rigorous control of both genetic and shared environment factors. In addition, our models controlled for current depressive symptoms because OCS and depression often co-occur and there is a robust link between SLEs and depression. The results showed that there was a significant association between certain types of SLEs (history of physical abuse, neglect and family disruption) and OCS severity. Because this association remained significant when genetic factors, shared environmental factors and co-occurring depression were controlled for, we conclude that these SLEs could potentially play a modest (about 3% of the variance) but significant role in the severity of OCS. These findings, their implications and limitations of the study are discussed further below.

Our finding of a phenotypic association between certain types of SLEs and the severity of OCS in the entire sample (that is, when genetic and shared environmental factors are not controlled for) confirms our prediction and replicates the findings of numerous clinical and epidemiological studies where OCS were found to be associated to a wide range of SLEs, including physical and sexual abuse [Reference Cromer, Schmidt and Murphy13,Reference Mathews, Kaur and Stein45,Reference Saunders, Villeponteaux, Lipovsky, Kilpatrick and Veronen64,Reference Boudreaux, Kilpatrick, Resnick, Best and Saunders6,Reference Grisham, Fullana, Mataix-Cols, Moffitt, Caspi and Poulton24,Reference Stein, Walker, Anderson, Hazen, Ross and Eldridge66,Reference Burnam, Stein, Golding, Siegel, Sorenson and Forsythe8].

When we used the MZ-difference method to isolate the non-shared environment effect (i.e. controlling for genetic and shared environment effects), only SLEs related to “abuse and family disruption” were associated to differences in OCS severity. This association remained significant after controlling for depressive symptoms. This SLEs factor includes items such as “family violence in childhood”, “emotional abuse or neglect”, “physical neglect”, “physical abuse”, “serious family problem”, “separation or divorce of parents” and “having been adopted”. This finding is reminiscent of the literature on parenting styles in OCD. Indeed, this research suggests that some dysfunctional parenting styles could be associated to the severity of OCS. Specifically, high expressed emotion, parental over-control, parental over-protection, emotional rejection, and low emotional warmth have been previously linked to higher levels of OCS [Reference Waters and Barrett74] and OCD [Reference Alonso, M Menchón, Mataix-Cols, Pifarré, Urretavizcaya and Crespo2]. However, we are unaware of studies directly investigating the link between SLEs and parenting styles in OCD.

It could be argued that the “abuse and family disruption” factor contains items that are, by definition, shared environment. Therefore, differences in this factor may simply reflect differences in recalling SLEs. When we examined individual items included in this factor, we found differences in OCS severity within MZ pairs discordant for “family violence in childhood”, “emotional abuse or neglect”, “physical neglect”, “physical abuse”, and “serious family problem”. Because STAGE twins are 19–47 years old, not all these items necessarily refer to one's early life. For example, the item “serious family problem” may involve difficulties within the individual's current family structure (e.g. with spouses or children). Likewise, the item “emotional abuse or neglect” not only involves the current family but also classmates and co-workers. On the other hand, items such as “family violence before 18” and “physical neglect” are more likely to have occurred in childhood and may therefore reflect “shared” experiences. However, it is important to note that even family factors traditionally considered as shared environment could technically be classified as non-shared (e.g., parenting being experienced as different among siblings) [Reference Plomin and Commentary:55]. It is clear that prospective longitudinal studies are needed before these factors can be considered truly causal.

The significant association between sexual abuse and the severity of OCS lost its significance when genetic and shared environmental effects were controlled for. Similarly, Cath et al. [Reference Cath, van Grootheest, Willemsen, van Oppen and Boomsma12] found a non-significant trend for higher rates of sexual assault in the high-scoring twins of MZ-discordant pairs for OCS. Together, these findings may suggest that perhaps the association between sexual abuse and OCS may be attributable to other mechanisms such as gene-environment correlations (i.e., a specific genetic liability also predispose to specific environments) or gene by environment interactions (i.e., specific genotypes can make individuals more or less reactive to different environments) [Reference Neale and Cardon51]. These mechanisms have been shown to play a role in the association between SLEs and depression [Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott33,Reference Kendler and Karkowski-Shuman32] and are also thought to be at play in OCD [Reference Mataix-Cols, Boman, Monzani, Ruck, Serlachius and Langstrom44,Reference Taylor and Jang70]. The precise genes that correlate or interact with certain types of SLEs to potentially cause OCS are not known but our findings delineate a clear avenue for future research in the sense that not all types of SLEs seem associated with the severity of OCS.

The SLEs factor we termed “illness/injury”, which includes items such as injury, physical illness or being involved in an accident, was also found to be associated with OCS severity but this association was no longer significant after controlling for depressive symptoms. Physical health and depression symptoms are strongly associated [Reference Frerichs, Aneshensel, Yokopenic and Clark19]. Likewise, strong associations exist between depression and OCD, with comorbidities ranging between 50–80% in adult samples [Reference Rasmussen and Eisen58,Reference Ruscio, Stein, Chiu and Kessler60]. Therefore, our results may suggest that SLEs related to physical health could be associated with greater depressive symptomatology (and vice versa), rather than being specifically associated with the severity of OCS. Alternatively, it could be that depression mediates the association between these SLEs and OCS severity. Longitudinal studies would be needed to examine the potential mediation effects of depression in the association between “illness/injury” SLEs and OCS.

It is interesting that other types of SLEs, such as the factors we termed “loss” (e.g. death of loved ones, loss of one's home) or “non-sexual assault” (e.g. being a victim of robbery or being physically attacked), were not significantly associated with the severity of OCS. The literature on depression and PTSD also reveals that interpersonal SLEs (i.e. physical abuse, emotional abuse, sexual abuse, etc.) but not other types of non-interpersonal SLEs (i.e. witnessing a crime, exposure to an accident or disaster, death of parent) are significantly associated with these disorders and their severity [42,18,27]. It has been suggested that exposure to interpersonal trauma as opposed to non-interpersonal trauma have an impact in affect self-regulation, biological self-regulation, self-perception and relationships in the future [Reference Ford, Stockton, Kaltman and Green18]. Regarding specific associations, emotional abuse has shown to be more strongly related to depressive disorders than anxiety disorders, whereas other types of interpersonal trauma, such as physical or sexual abuse, have shown a similar association with both anxiety and depressive disorders [Reference Hovens, Wiersma, Giltay, Van Oppen, Spinhoven and Penninx27,Reference Gibb, Chelminski and Zimmerman21]. On a more detailed inspection of the OCD literature, it also appears that the most frequently mentioned SLEs are those relating to interpersonal events [Reference Mathews, Kaur and Stein45,Reference Boudreaux, Kilpatrick, Resnick, Best and Saunders6,Reference Grisham, Fullana, Mataix-Cols, Moffitt, Caspi and Poulton24,Reference Lochner, du Toit, Zungu-Dirwayi, Marais, van Kradenburg and Seedat41]. Non-interpersonal SLEs, such as birth of a child, arguments with family or change/difficulties at work/school have also been mentioned, albeit less frequently [Reference Bogetto, Venturello, Albert, Maina and Ravizza4,Reference Real, Labad, Alonso, Segalas, Jimenez-Murcia and Bueno59]. Few studies have tested interpersonal and non-interpersonal SLEs simultaneously [6,24,12]. It has been suggested that SLEs related to OCD/OCS are perceived as having more negative impact, being more undesirable and being more uncontrollable [Reference McKeon, Roa and Mann47,Reference Gothelf, Aharonovsky, Horesh, Carty and Apter23,Reference Kulhara and Rao37,Reference Khanna, Rajendra and Channabasavanna35,Reference Valleni-Basile, Garrison, Waller, Addy, McKeown and Jackson71]. Also, it has been suggested that higher levels of “thought suppression” after a SLEs may be associated with higher levels of OCS [Reference McLaren and Crowe48]. Similar results have been reported in PTSD [Reference Cameron, Palm and Follette10].

Current cognitive behavioural models of the disorder [Reference Rachman57,Reference Salkovskis61] have long postulated that certain “experiences” can result in the development of dysfunctional beliefs and maladaptive schemas about the environment and the self [Reference Salkovskis, Forrester, Frost and Steketee62]. In turn, these beliefs are thought to have a small yet significant etiological role in OCD [Reference Taylor and Jang70]. Our results shed some light on the possible types of “experiences” that may be etiologically implicated in the development of such dysfunctional beliefs and eventually result in distressing OCS. Precisely how these particular life experiences, and not other SLEs, result in OCD-related dysfunctional beliefs, OCS or both is unclear.

Although this study has a number of strengths, like the large twin sample and the broad coverage of interpersonal and non-interpersonal SLEs, some limitations should be noted. First, like in most previous studies, the assessment of SLEs was retrospective and recall biases cannot be fully ruled out. Moreover, as mentioned earlier, it is possible that individuals with greater OCS or depression severity may have a greater propensity to recalling certain types of SLEs. To explore this possibility further, we looked at the item “Did your parents ever separate or divorce while you were living with them?”. Although it is possible that parents got divorced when one of the twins in a pair was no longer living at home, eventually this item should be the same for both twins in a pair. Discordant MZ pairs on this item were almost 6% of pairs (134 pairs). There were no differences in depression scores or OCS severity between those twins who answered “yes” and those who answered “no” to this question. This means that divorce of parents has the same effect for both twins (whether they were living with them or not when they got divorced) or that, in case of recall bias, this is not affected by depression or OCS severity. However, we found differences in OCS severity within MZ-twin pairs discordant for other items included in the factor “abuse and family disruption”. Prospective and longitudinal studies are needed in order to establish which part of the variance in OCS is due to the causal effect of SLEs vs simply due to recall bias. One important limitation related to the cross-sectional design is that we lack information about the precise timing of SLEs and OCS and whether the former preceded the latter. Participants were asked for the time of occurrence of SLEs and the onset of OCS but, unfortunately, only 1.6% (range 0.1–5% depending on the SLE) had provided this information. A further limitation was that we only used information about the occurrence or non-occurrence of each SLE in order to employ all the possible types of life events. Information about the frequency or impact of SLEs might be of value for future studies, as the experience of a specific event may differ among individuals. The self-reported nature of the instruments and lack of corroborative evidence from multiple informants also potentially diminished the reliability of the reported information. The 7-item OCS measure is not a widely used instrument though its psychometric properties are remarkably sound [Reference Mataix-Cols, Boman, Monzani, Ruck, Serlachius and Langstrom44]. In the future, these findings should be replicated employing longer and more extensively validated measures of OCS, such as the obsessive-compulsive inventory-revised [Reference Foa, Huppert, Leiberg, Langner, Kichic and Hajcak17] which may provide a broader range of scores. The use of such measures would also allow exploring whether specific associations exist between different SLEs and different OC symptom dimensions [Reference Mataix-Cols, do Rosario-Campos and Leckman43], in the same way as different types of SLEs seem to be related to different types of depressive symptom profiles [Reference Keller, Neale and Kendler29]. Another possible limitation is that we could only adjust for current depressive symptoms, age and gender. Other variables, including personality and anxiety levels could be mediating the association between SLEs and OCS [23,45,7]. Finally, the generalizability of findings of this study may be somewhat reduced by the small but significant demographic and psychopathological differences between respondents and non-respondents to the questionnaires.

In conclusion, and with the above limitations in mind, this study suggests that certain types of stressful life events are selectively associated with the severity of OCS. In particular, a history of abuse, neglect and family disruption may make a modest but significant contribution to the severity of OCS. While the results are consistent with a causal interpretation, further replication in longitudinal cohorts is needed before causality can be confidently established.

Disclosure of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest concerning this article.

Acknowledgements

P.V.-R. gratefully acknowledges the support of the Alicia Koplowitz Foundation. P.L. gratefully acknowledges the support of the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare and the Swedish Research Council.

Appendix A Supplementary data

Supplementary material (e-Tables 1–3) associated with this article can be found at http://www.sciencedirect.com, in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.11.008.

References

Abramowitz, J.S., Deacon, B.J., Olatunji, B.O., Wheaton, M.G., Berman, N.C., Losardo, D., et al.Assessment of obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions: development and evaluation of the Dimensional Obsessive-Compulsive Scale. Psychol Assess 2010;22 (1):180198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alonso, P., M Menchón, J., Mataix-Cols, D., Pifarré, J., Urretavizcaya, M., Crespo, J.M., et al.Perceived parental rearing style in obsessive–compulsive disorder: relation to symptom dimensions. Psychiatry Res 2004;127 (3):267278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersson, E., Enander, J., Andrén, P., Hedman, E., Ljótsson, B., Hursti, T., et al.Internet-based cognitive behaviour therapy for obsessive–compulsive disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Psychol Med 2012;42 (10):21932203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogetto, F., Venturello, S., Albert, U., Maina, G., Ravizza, L.. Gender-related clinical differences in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Psychiatry 1999;14 (8):434441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolhuis, K., McAdams, T.A., Monzani, B., Gregory, A.M., Mataix-Cols, D., Stringaris, A., et al.Aetiological overlap between obsessive-compulsive and depressive symptoms: a longitudinal twin study in adolescents and adults. Psychol Med 2013;111. [Epub 2013/08/08].Google ScholarPubMed
Boudreaux, E., Kilpatrick, D.G., Resnick, H.S., Best, C.L., Saunders, B.E.. Criminal victimization, posttraumatic stress disorder, and comorbid psychopathology among a community sample of women. J Trauma Stress 1998;11 (4):665678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briggs, E.S., Price, I.R.. The relationship between adverse childhood experience and obsessive-compulsive symptoms and beliefs: the role of anxiety, depression, and experiential avoidance. J Anxiety Disord 2009;23 (8):10371046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burnam, M.A., Stein, J.A., Golding, J.M., Siegel, J.M., Sorenson, S.B., Forsythe, A.B., et al.Sexual assault and mental disorders in a community population. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988;56 (6):843850 [Epub 1988/12/01].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnham, K.P., Anderson, D.R.. Multimodel inference - understanding AIC and BIC in model selection. Sociol Method Res 2004;33 (2):261304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, A., Palm, K., Follette, V.. Reaction to stressful life events: what predicts symptom severity?. J Anxiety Disord 2010;24 (6):645649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, J.S., Andrykowski, M.A., Wilson, J., Hall, L.A., Rayens, M.K., Sachs, B., et al.Psychometrics for two short forms of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression Scale. Issues Ment Health Nurs 1998;19 (5):481494 [Epub 1998/10/23].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cath, D.C., van Grootheest, D.S., Willemsen, G., van Oppen, P., Boomsma, D.I.. Environmental factors in obsessive-compulsive behavior: evidence from discordant and concordant monozygotic twins. Behav Genet 2008;38 (2):108120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cromer, K.R., Schmidt, N.B., Murphy, D.L.. An investigation of traumatic life events and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav Res Ther 2007;45 (7):16831691 [Epub 2006/10/28].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denys, D., de Geus, F., van Megen, H.J., Westenberg, H.G.. Symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: factor analysis on a clinician-rated scale and a self-report measure. Psychopathology 2004;37 (4):181189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DiStefano, C., Zhu, M., Mindrila, D.. Understanding and using factor scores: considerations for the applied researcher. Pract Assess Res Eval 2009;14 (20):111.Google Scholar
Donahue, K.L., D’Onofrio, B.M., Lichtenstein, P., Langstrom, N.. Testing putative causal associations of risk factors for early intercourse in the study of twin adults: genes and environment (STAGE). Arch Sex Behav 2013;42 (1):3544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, E.B., Huppert, J.D., Leiberg, S., Langner, R., Kichic, R., Hajcak, G., et al.The obsessive-compulsive inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychol Assess 2002;14 (4):485496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, J.D., Stockton, P., Kaltman, S., Green, B.L.. Disorders of extreme stress (DESNOS) symptoms are associated with type and severity of interpersonal trauma exposure in a sample of healthy young women. J Interpers Violence 2006;21 (11):13991416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frerichs, R.R., Aneshensel, C.S., Yokopenic, P.A., Clark, V.A.. Physical health and depression: an epidemiologic survey. Prev Med 1982;11 (6):639646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gershuny, B.S., Baer, L., Parker, H., Gentes, E.L., Infield, A.L., Jenike, M.A.. Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in treatment-resistant obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety 2008;25 (1):6971 [Epub 2007/02/24].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibb, B.E., Chelminski, I., Zimmerman, M.. Childhood emotional, physical, and sexual abuse, and diagnoses of depressive and anxiety disorders in adult psychiatric outpatients. Depress Anxiety 2007;24 (4):256263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, W.K., Price, L.H., Rasmussen, S.A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R.L., Hill, C.L., et al.The Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale: I. Development, use, and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;46 (11):10061011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gothelf, D., Aharonovsky, O., Horesh, N., Carty, T., Apter, A.. Life events and personality factors in children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder and other anxiety disorders. Compr Psychiatry 2004;45 (3):192198 [Epub 2004/05/05].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grisham, J.R., Fullana, M.A., Mataix-Cols, D., Moffitt, T.E., Caspi, A., Poulton, R.. Risk factors prospectively associated with adult obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med 2011;41 (12):24952506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanna, G.L., Himle, J.A., Hanna, B.S., Gold, K.J., Gillespie, B.W.. Major depressive disorder in a family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder with pediatric probands. Depress Anxiety 2011;28 (6):501508 [Epub 2011/05/04].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., Mullen, M.Evaluating model fit: a synthesis of the structural equation modelling literature. 7th European Conference on research methodology for business and management studies 2008 195200.Google Scholar
Hovens, J., Wiersma, J., Giltay, E., Van Oppen, P., Spinhoven, P., Penninx, B., et al.Childhood life events and childhood trauma in adult patients with depressive, anxiety and comorbid disorders vs. controls. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2010;122 (1):6674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonnal, A.H., Gardner, C.O., Prescott, C.A., Kendler, K.S.. Obsessive and compulsive symptoms in a general population sample of female twins. Am J Med Genet 2000;96 (6):791796.3.0.CO;2-C>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, M., Neale, M., Kendler, K.. Association of different adverse life events with distinct patterns of depressive symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 2007;164 (10):15211529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K.S., Baker, J.H.. Genetic influences on measures of the environment: a systematic review. Psychol Med 2007;37 (5):615626 [Epub 2006/12/21].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K.S., Gardner, C.O.. Monozygotic twins discordant for major depression: a preliminary exploration of the role of environmental experiences in the aetiology and course of illness. Psychol Med 2001;31 (3):411423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K.S., Karkowski-Shuman, L.. Stressful life events and genetic liability to major depression: genetic control of exposure to the environment?. Psychol Med 1997;27 (3):539547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K.S., Karkowski, L.M., Prescott, C.A.. Causal relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major depression. A J Psychiatry 1999;156 (6):837841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R.C.. The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annu Rev Psychol 1997;48 (1):191214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khanna, S., Rajendra, P.N., Channabasavanna, S.M.. Life events and onset of obsessive compulsive disorder. Int J Soc Psychiatry 1988;34 (4):305309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohout, F.J., Berkman, L.F., Evans, D.A., Cornoni-Huntley, J.. Two shorter forms of the CES-D depression symptoms index. J Aging Health 1993;5 (2):179193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kulhara, P., Rao, P.G.. Life events in obsessive compulsive neurosis. Indian J Psychiatry 1986;28 (3):221224 [Epub 1986/07/01].Google ScholarPubMed
Lafleur, D.L., Petty, C., Mancuso, E., McCarthy, K., Biederman, J., Faro, A., et al.Traumatic events and obsessive compulsive disorder in children and adolescents: is there a link?. J Anxiety Disord 2011;25 (4):513519 [Epub 2011/02/08].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, R., Hancock, G.R.. A framework of statistical tests for comparing mean and covariance structure models. Multivariate Behav Res 2007;42 (1):3366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenstein, P., Sullivan, P.F., Cnattingius, S., Gatz, M., Johansson, S., Carlstrom, E., et al.The Swedish twin registry in the third millennium: an update. Twin Res Hum Genet 2006;9 (6):875882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lochner, C., du Toit, P.L., Zungu-Dirwayi, N., Marais, A., van Kradenburg, J., Seedat, S., et al.Childhood trauma in obsessive-compulsive disorder, trichotillomania, and controls. Depress Anxiety 2002;15 (2):6668 [Epub 2002/03/14].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luthra, R., Abramovitz, R., Greenberg, R., Schoor, A., Newcorn, J., Schmeidler, J., et al.Relationship between type of trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder among urban children and adolescents. J Interpers Violence 2009;24 (11):19191927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mataix-Cols, D., do Rosario-Campos, M.C., Leckman, J.F.. A multidimensional model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2005;162 (2):228238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mataix-Cols, D., Boman, M., Monzani, B., Ruck, C., Serlachius, E., Langstrom, N., et al.Population-based multigenerational family clustering study of obsessive-compulsive disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 2013;70 (7):709717 [Epub 2013/05/24].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathews, C.A., Kaur, N., Stein, M.B.. Childhood trauma and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Depress Anxiety 2008;25 (9):742751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McHugo, G.J., Caspi, Y., Kammerer, N., Mazelis, R., Jackson, E.W., Russell, L., et al.The assessment of trauma history in women with co-occurring substance abuse and mental disorders and a history of interpersonal violence. J Behav Health Serv Res 2005;32 (2):113127 [Epub 2005/04/19].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKeon, J., Roa, B., Mann, A.. Life events and personality traits in obsessive-compulsive neurosis. Br J Psychiatry 1984;144 :185189. [Epub 1984/02/01].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaren, S., Crowe, S.F.. The contribution of perceived control of stressful life events and thought suppression to the symptoms of obsessive–compulsive disorder in both non-clinical and clinical samples. J Anxiety Disord 2003;17 (4):389403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, S.A., Asberg, M.. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry 1979;134 (4):382389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L.K., Muthén, B.O.Mplus user's guide, 6th ed.Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2007.Google Scholar
Neale, M., Cardon, L.R.Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht, NL: Kluver Academic Publishers; 1992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nestadt, G., Samuels, J., Riddle, M., Bienvenu, O.J., Liang, K.Y., LaBuda, M., et al.3rd.A family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57 (4):358363 [Epub 2000/04/18].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pauls, D.L.. The genetics of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2010;12 (2):149163 [Epub 2010/07/14].Google ScholarPubMed
Pike, A., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E.M., Plomin, R.. Using MZ differences in the search for nonshared environmental effects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996;37 (6):695704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., Commentary:, . Why are children in the same family so different? Non-shared environment three decades later. Int J Epidemiol 2011;40 (3):582592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., Lichtenstein, P., Pedersen, N.L., McClearn, G.E., Nesselroade, J.R.. Genetic influence on life events during the last half of the life span. Psychol Aging 1990;5 (1):2530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S.. A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behav Res Ther 1997;35 (9):793802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, S.A., Eisen, J.L.. The epidemiology and differential diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 1992;53 (Suppl.):410.Google ScholarPubMed
Real, E., Labad, J., Alonso, P., Segalas, C., Jimenez-Murcia, S., Bueno, B., et al.Stressful life events at onset of obsessive-compulsive disorder are associated with a distinct clinical pattern. Depress Anxiety 2011;28 (5):367376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruscio, A., Stein, D., Chiu, W., Kessler, R.. The epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Mol Psychiatry 2008;15 (1):5363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salkovskis, P.M.The cognitive approach to anxiety: threat beliefs, safety-seeking behaviour, and the special case of health anxiety and obsessions. Frontiers of cognitive therapy New York: Guilford; 1996. p. 4874.Google Scholar
Salkovskis, P.M., Forrester, E.Responsibility. Frost, R.O., Steketee, G., editors. Cognitive approaches to obsessions and compulsions: theory, assessment and treatment. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Pergamon/Elsevier Science; 2002. p. 4561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarkhel, S., Praharaj, S.K., Sinha, V.K.. Role of life events in obsessive compulsive disorder. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci 2011;48 (3):182185 [Epub 2011/12/06].Google ScholarPubMed
Saunders, B.E., Villeponteaux, L.A., Lipovsky, J.A., Kilpatrick, D.G., Veronen, L.J.. Child sexual assault as a risk factor for mental-disorders among women - a community survey. J Interpers Violence 1992;7 (2):189204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
StataCorp. Stata statistical software: release 11. College Station. TX: StataCorp LP; 2009.Google Scholar
Stein, M.B., Walker, J.R., Anderson, G., Hazen, A.L., Ross, C.A., Eldridge, G., et al.Childhood physical and sexual abuse in patients with anxiety disorders and in a community sample. Am J Psychiatry 1996;153 (2):275277.Google Scholar
Stewart, S.E., Yu, D., Scharf, J.M., Neale, B.M., Fagerness, J.A., Mathews, C.A., et al.Genome-wide association study of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2013;18 (7):788798 [Epub 2012/08/15].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabachnick, B.G., Fidell, L.S.Using multivariate statistics, 5th ed.New York: Allyn and Bacon; 2007.Google Scholar
Taylor, S.Molecular genetics of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comprehensive meta-analysis of genetic association studies. Mol Psychiatry 2013;18 (7):799805 [Epub 2012/06/06].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S., Jang, K.L.Biopsychosocial etiology of obsessions and compulsions: An integrated behavioral–genetic and cognitive–behavioral analysis. J Abnorm Psychol 2011;120 (1):174186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valleni-Basile, L.A., Garrison, C.Z., Waller, J.L., Addy, C.L., McKeown, R.E., Jackson, K.L., et al.Incidence of obsessive-compulsive disorder in a community sample of young adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996;35 (7):898906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Grootheest, D.S., Cath, D.C., Beekman, A.T., Boomsma, D.I.Genetic and environmental influences on obsessive-compulsive symptoms in adults: a population-based twin-family study. Psychol Med 2007;37 (11):16351644 [Epub 2007/06/27].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Arseneault, L.The discordant MZ-twin method: one step closer to the holy grail of causality. Int J Behav Dev 2009;33 (4):376382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waters, T.L., Barrett, P.M.The role of the family in childhood obsessive–compulsive disorder. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2000;3 (3):173184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfe, J., Kimerling, R.Gender issues in the assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder. In: Wilson, J.P., Keane, T.M., editors. Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD New York: Guilford; 1997. p. 192238.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Sample characteristics and gender differences in OCS severity, depression severity and stressful life events (SLEs).

Figure 1

Table 2 Summary of multiple linear regression predicting the severity of obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCS) from stressful life events in the whole sample of DZ and MZ-twins.

Figure 2

Table 3 Summary of multiple regression analyses predicting within monozygotic pair differences in the severity of obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCS) from within monozygotic pair differences in stressful life events.

Figure 3

Table 4 Difference in OCS severity between MZ-twin pairs discordant for individual items within the “Abuse and family disruption” factor.

Supplementary material: File

Vidal-Ribas et al. supplementary material

Table S1-S3

Download Vidal-Ribas et al. supplementary material(File)
File 39.9 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.