Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T04:40:33.980Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The set-shifting profiles of anorexia nervosa and autism spectrum disorder using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 April 2016

H. Westwood
Affiliation:
Psychological Medicine, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London, UK
D. Stahl
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London, UK
W. Mandy
Affiliation:
University College London, Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, London, UK
K. Tchanturia*
Affiliation:
Psychological Medicine, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London, UK Ilia State University, Tbilisi, Georgia
*
*Address for correspondence: K. Tchanturia, Psychological Medicine, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London, UK and Ilia State University, Tbilisi, Georgia. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Difficulties in set-shifting are commonly reported in both autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and anorexia nervosa (AN) populations. Despite this, it is not known whether this cognitive profile persists across different ages, or whether the profiles seen in ASD and AN are comparable. This systematic review and meta-analyses aimed to compare the set-shifting profiles, as measured by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) in adults and younger people with either ASD or AN, relative to healthy controls (HCs) and to statistically compare performance on the WCST between ASD and AN. In all, 24 studies on ASD and 22 studies on AN were identified. In ASD, there were significant differences between the clinical group and HCs, with the ASD group making significantly more perseverative errors, indicating greater difficulty in set-shifting [pooled effect size of d = 0.67, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.53–0.81, p ⩽ 0.001]. This effect was consistent across the age span. For AN studies, there was a significant difference between adults with AN and HCs (d = 0.52, 95% CI 0.36–0.68, p ⩽ 0.001) but a non-significant effect in child studies (d = 0.25, 95% CI −0.05 to 0.55, z = 1.66, p = 0.096). Meta-regression indicated no effect of diagnosis (AN or ASD) on performance in adult studies but there was a non-significant trend (p = 0.053) towards children with ASD performing worse than children with AN. While difficulties with set-shifting appear to be stable in ASD, there may be differences between children and adults with AN, which warrant further investigation.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016

Introduction

A growing body of literature indicates a link between anorexia nervosa (AN) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD). A systematic review of the prevalence of ASD in AN reported a mean rate of 23% (Huke et al. Reference Huke, Turk, Saeidi, Kent and Morgan2013); however, six of the included studies came from the same Swedish community sample and this line of research is ongoing (Nielsen et al. Reference Nielsen, Anckarsätar, Gillberg, Rastam and Wentz2015). There are also similarities in cognitive style between AN and ASD (Gillberg et al. Reference Gillberg, Billstedt, Wentz, Anckarsater, Rastam and Gillberg2010; Oldershaw et al. Reference Oldershaw, Treasure, Hambrook, Tchanturia and Schmidt2011; Courty et al. Reference Courty, Maria, Lalanne, Ringuenet, Vindreau, Chevallier, Pouga, Pinabel, Philippe, Adrien, Barry and Berthoz2013; Tchanturia et al. Reference Tchanturia, Lloyd and Lang2013a ) reported in the literature. Although elevated levels of autistic traits have consistently been found within AN populations (Baron-Cohen et al. Reference Baron-Cohen, Jaffa, Davies, Auyeung, Allison and Wheelwright2013; Tchanturia et al. Reference Tchanturia, Smith, Weineck, Fidanboylu, Kern, Treasure and Baron Cohen2013b ; Huke et al. Reference Huke, Turk, Saeidi, Kent and Morgan2014; Rhind et al. Reference Rhind, Bonfioli, Hibbs, Goddard, Macdonald, Gowers, Schmidt, Tchanturia, Micali and Treasure2014; Westwood et al. Reference Westwood, Eisler, Mandy, Leppanen, Treasure and Tchanturia2016) it is not known whether the presence of these traits represents a shared, underlying mechanism (Zucker et al. Reference Zucker, Losh, Bulik, LaBar, Piven and Pelphrey2007; Odent, Reference Odent2010; Allely, Reference Allely2013) or whether individuals with AN come to resemble those with ASD due to the chronic, starved state associated with the disorder (Pellicano & Hiller, Reference Pellicano and Hiller2013; Treasure, Reference Treasure2013). Direct comparison of the seemingly shared traits, as well as systematic exploration of these traits across stages of illness, e.g. childhood and adulthood, will be essential to understanding the extent to which these traits may be shared mechanisms in both AN and ASD.

Interest in the potential relationship between AN and ASD has led to examination of cognitive traits, believed to underpin ASD in AN. While some traits associated with ASD, such as emotional theory of mind, have been found to reduce in recovered AN groups (Oldershaw et al. Reference Oldershaw, Hambrook, Tchanturia, Treasure and Schmidt2010), other traits including difficulties with set-shifting, or flexible thinking, appear to persist after weight gain (Tchanturia et al. Reference Tchanturia, Morris, Anderluh, Collier, Nikolaou and Treasure2004; Gillberg et al. Reference Gillberg, Billstedt, Wentz, Anckarsater, Rastam and Gillberg2010; Danner et al. Reference Danner, Sanders, Smeets, Van Meer, Adan, Hoek and Van Elburg2012; Lindner et al. Reference Lindner, Fichter and Quadflieg2014). Recent reports suggest that set-shifting inefficiencies are present in children with AN who have had a relatively short duration of illness (Lang et al. Reference Lang, Lloyd, Khondoker, Simic, Treasure and Tchanturia2015a ) and relatives with AN (Holliday et al. Reference Holliday, Tchanturia, Landau, Collier and Treasure2005; Roberts et al. Reference Roberts, Tchanturia and Treasure2010; Lang et al. Reference Lang, Treasure and Tchanturia2015b ). Conversely, fasting in healthy controls (HCs) has been found to reduce set-shifting test performance, suggesting that starvation may exacerbate underlying difficulties in this domain (Bolton et al. Reference Bolton, Burgess, Gilbert and Serpell2014; Pender et al. Reference Pender, Gilbert and Serpell2014). Thus, despite evidence of cognitive similarity between AN and ASD, the extent and nature of this likeness remain unclear. Exploring differences in the set-shifting profiles of children and adults with AN would speak to debates of whether neurocognitive traits such as set-shifting underlie the eating disorder or whether these cognitive traits are more related to eating disorder pathology such as starvation.

Differences in set-shifting have been observed between children and adults with AN. A systematic review of set-shifting ability, comparing adults with AN with HCs identified five papers employing the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Heaton et al. Reference Heaton, Chelune, Talley, Kay and Curtiss1993), a widely used measure of set-shifting, with a meta-analysis reporting a significant small effect size (d = 0.36; Roberts et al. Reference Roberts, Tchanturia, Stahl, Southgate and Treasure2007). A further meta-analysis of studies in children and adolescents with AN (Lang et al. Reference Lang, Stahl, Espie, Treasure and Tchanturia2014), however, found a non-significant pooled effect size (d = 0.20). A more recent study comparing children and adolescents with AN with HCs using a larger sample size (AN = 41, HC = 43) found that young people with AN made significantly more perseverative errors than HCs (d = 0.49; Lang et al. Reference Lang, Lloyd, Khondoker, Simic, Treasure and Tchanturia2015a ), suggesting that this cognitive profile is present early on in the disorder before the effects of starvation become prominent. A meta-analysis by Wu et al. (Reference Wu, Brockmeyer, Hartmann, Skunde, Herzog and Friederich2014) included a range of neuropsychological tasks assessing set-shifting, rather than just the WCST. This revealed a medium effect size for inefficient set-shifting in restrictive AN (g = −0.51) but no significant effect size for binge/purge AN. Although the meta-analysis by Wu et al. (Reference Wu, Brockmeyer, Hartmann, Skunde, Herzog and Friederich2014) did include studies with children and adolescents, the results did not differentiate between age groups.

Within the ASD literature, five reviews have attempted to synthesize the literature on cognitive flexibility (Hill, Reference Hill2004; Kenworthy et al. Reference Kenworthy, Yerys, Anthony and Wallace2008; Sanders et al. Reference Sanders, Johnson, Garavan, Gill and Gallagher2008; Geurts et al. Reference Geurts, Corbett and Solomon2009; Leung & Zakzanis, Reference Leung and Zakzanis2014). Studies using the WCST have consistently shown difficulties with set-shifting which are stable over time (Hill, Reference Hill2004); however, this review did not differentiate between studies with adult or child participants and no quantitative synthesis was attempted. A further review (Kenworthy et al. Reference Kenworthy, Yerys, Anthony and Wallace2008) reports that whilst several studies of cognitive flexibility in children and adults with ASD have demonstrated difficulties relative to HCs, the evidence is inconclusive with two of nine studies using the WCST, one with adults and one with children, producing unclear results. A recent meta-analysis (Leung & Zakzanis, Reference Leung and Zakzanis2014) comparing neuropsychological performance between patients with ASD and HCs on number of perseverative errors on the computerized WCST, which again pooled both child and adult studies, reported a medium mean effect size (d = 0.68). A review of the WCST in ASD (Landry & Al-Taie, Reference Landry and Al-Taie2016) provided effect sizes for four different WCST outputs including perseveration. In this review, age was found to be negatively correlated with perseveration, the opposite of what has been observed in the AN literature. To our knowledge, however, no reviews have directly compared the set-shifting profiles of children and adults with ASD using the WCST, although, generally, results do suggest that difficulties in set-shifting may be more stable across different age groups, than in AN studies.

Despite the set-shifting difficulties found in people with AN seeming to resemble those of ASD, the cognitive profiles of these two disorders have rarely been directly compared. Oldershaw et al. (Reference Oldershaw, Treasure, Hambrook, Tchanturia and Schmidt2011) statistically compared the neuropsychological profile of currently ill AN patients with published data of individuals with ASD on the WCST and found the two groups to be statistically similar. To our knowledge, this is the only study directly comparing the profiles of the two groups and thus further work is needed to confirm whether the set-shifting profile seen in AN truly resembles that of ASD. Furthermore, the Oldershaw et al. (Reference Oldershaw, Treasure, Hambrook, Tchanturia and Schmidt2011) study only included adult patients. Exploring differences in the set-shifting profiles of children and adults with AN would speak to debates of whether neurocognitive traits such as set-shifting underlie the disorder. Since the publication of the study of Oldershaw et al. (Reference Oldershaw, Treasure, Hambrook, Tchanturia and Schmidt2011), additional studies have been published in both AN and ASD, as demonstrated by the review presented here. As these studies are cross-sectional and not longitudinal, inferences on the aetiology of ASD and AN cannot be made but statistically comparing the severity of set-shifting difficulties will aid in the comparison of the neuropsychological profiles of the two disorders.

Difficulties with set-shifting have been implicated in several other psychiatric disorders including schizophrenia (Pantelis et al. Reference Pantelis, Barber, Barnes, Nelson, Owen and Robbins1999); major depression (Austin et al. Reference Austin, Mitchell, Wilhelm, Parker, Hickie, Brodaty, Chan, Eyers, Milic and Hadzi-Pavlovic1999) and obsessive–compulsive disorder (Leopald & Backenstrass, Reference Leopold and Backenstrass M2015), suggesting that this could be a general risk factor for the development of mental disorder, rather than a trait, specific to either AN or ASD. However, evidence of deviant set-shifting in these disorders is less consistent than in AN and ASD, for example, due to the heterogeneous nature of the disorders. As specific attention has been paid to the shared behavioural traits and intermediate phenotypes in ASD and AN (Treasure, Reference Treasure2013), determining both the severity and stability of set-shifting difficulties in the two disorders through meta-analysis is a necessary step to exploring shared underlying mechanisms.

If it is the case that both children and adults with ASD display inefficiencies in set-shifting, it would add evidence to suggest that this trait is stable across the trajectory of the disorder. AN studies have tended to find less difficulty with set-shifting in children than in adults but this finding is difficult to interpret as it may reflect type II errors due to small sample sizes in child studies, with the exception of one study (Lang et al. Reference Lang, Lloyd, Khondoker, Simic, Treasure and Tchanturia2015a ). It is therefore unknown whether older and younger people with AN show similar cognitive profiles. The present review aims to use sufficiently powered analysis using pooled previous samples to summarize the literature on the use of the WCST as a measure of set-shifting in both ASD and AN populations and to determine: (a) whether individuals with either ASD or AN have difficulties with set-shifting; and (b) whether the severity of these difficulties is comparable in the two disorders in both child and adult populations. This will move us closer to understanding whether the difficulties in set-shifting are stable over the course of AN and will be a necessary but not sufficient step in understanding the relationship between AN and ASD.

Method

The meta-analyses were conducted according to the ‘PRISMA’ (preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analysis) statement (Moher et al. Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman and Group2009). The quality of each study was assessed using the Clinical Appraisal Skills Programme checklist for case–control studies (Critical Appraisal Skills Programme, 2013). The tool consists of 11 questions, which yield a mixture of ‘yes’, ‘no’ and more qualitative answers. In order to calculate an overall quality rating, several questions were split into subquestions and a score of 1 was awarded for every ‘yes’ answered, with a maximum possible score of 16. The quality rating for each study is shown in Tables 1 and 2.

Table 1. Demographic information for AN studies

AN, Anorexia nervosa; s.d., standard deviation; BMI, body mass index; IQ, intelligence quotient; HC, healthy control; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; AN-R, anorexia nervosa, restrictive subtype; ED, eating disorder; SCID, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Disorders; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; EDE, Eating Disorder Examination; AN-BP, anorexia nervosa binge-purge subtype; SES, socio-economic status.

a Verbal IQ.

b Measured in months.

Table 2. Demographic information for ASD studies

ASD, Autism spectrum disorder; s.d., standard deviation; IQ, intelligence quotient; HC, healthy control; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; AS, Asperger's syndrome; VIQ, verbal IQ; PIQ, performance IQ; ICD-10, International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems; ADI, Autism Diagnostic Interview; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; HFA, high functioning autism; FIQ, full-scale IQ; APA, American Psychiatric Association; GARS, Gilliam Autism Rating Scale; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; AQ, Autism Spectrum Quotient; CARS, Childhood Autism Rating Scale; PARS, Pervasive Developmental Disorders Autism Society Japan Rating Scale; AQ-J, Autism Spectrum Quotient, Japanese version; PDD, pervasive developmental disorder; ABC, Autism Behaviour Checklist; 3Di, Developmental, Dimensional and Diagnostic Interview; SRS, Social Responsiveness Scale.

a Measured in months.

b Performance IQ.

Eligibility criteria

Studies using the WCST and reporting the number or percentage of perseverative errors with a clinical population of either ASD or AN and HCs were included in the review. Due to the heterogeneous nature of ASD and the broadening diagnostic criteria of both ASD and AN in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-V; American Psychiatric Association, 2013), all variants of the two disorders were included.

Information sources and search

The electronic databases Scopus, PsycINFO, PubMed and Web of Science were searched up to and including January 2016. Search terms included anorexia nervosa OR autism AND set-shifting, Wisconsin, executive function OR cognitive flexibility. Limits included English-language, peer-reviewed articles. Reference lists of eligible papers were also screened, as were existing systematic reviews related of set-shifting in either ASD or AN (Roberts et al. Reference Roberts, Tchanturia, Stahl, Southgate and Treasure2007; Lang et al. Reference Lang, Stahl, Espie, Treasure and Tchanturia2014; Tchanturia, Reference Tchanturia2015; Landry & Al-Taie, Reference Landry and Al-Taie2016).

Selection

The first and principal authors (H.W., K.T.) identified potential titles and screened the abstracts for relevance. Full texts were then retrieved and read to determine eligibility by H.W. Texts deemed eligible were then further screened by K.T. and any papers that did not meet inclusion criteria were excluded. Reasons for exclusion included: the WCST was not used; perseverative errors were not reported; the WCST had been adapted for use with a particular population or there was no HC group; recovered AN cases were also excluded.

Data collection and items

The data items collected from each eligible study were: diagnosis; number of participants; mean age of participants; WCST version; percentage of female participants; how HCs were matched to clinical samples; how co-morbidities were controlled for; diagnostic tools; participant intelligent quotient (IQ); country where the study was conducted; and source of participant recruitment. For AN studies, body mass index (BMI) and illness duration were also extracted. In addition to demographic and experimental paradigm data, the means and standard deviations of perseverative errors on the WCST were used within the meta-analysis.

Risk of bias in individual studies

The risk of bias in individual studies was assessed by considering how methodology would make an impact on effect size in each study, for example, by attending to how HCs were matched to clinical samples, how the WCST was administered and how participants were recruited.

Summary measure

The principal summary measure used for analysis from all studies was the difference in means and standard deviations of perseverative errors or percentage of perseverative errors between the clinical sample (ASD or AN) and HCs on the WCST. Standardized mean scores were used within the analysis to account for the variation in version of the WCST used in the studies. The WCST involves sorting cards into one of four categories according to one of three sorting rules: colour, shape or number. Once a card has been sorted, the participant is given feedback as to whether they sorted the card according to the correct rule. The paradigm yields a number of different outputs which can be analysed including perseverative errors, when the participant continues to sort cards according to a previous rule (Lezak, Reference Lezak1995). The WCST is a widely used measure of general executive function (Heaton et al. Reference Heaton, Chelune, Talley, Kay and Curtiss1993; Greve et al. Reference Greve, Stickle, Love, Bianchini and Stanford2005). The WCST is assumed to consist of three factors: perseveration, failure to maintain set and idiosyncratic sorting (Greve et al. Reference Greve, Ingram and Bianchini1998). An increased number of perseverative errors has been associated with frontal lobe dysfunction (Milner, Reference Milner1963; Nelson, Reference Nelson1976) and, as a robust measure of conceptual flexibility, was therefore the main focus for this review.

Only studies using the WCST, rather than other set-shifting paradigms, were included because this is a widely used measure across a wide range of disorders, believed to reliably assess cognitive flexibility (Holmén et al. Reference Holmén, Juuhl-Langseth, Thormodsen, Sundet, Melle and Rund2012). There is less variability in terms of test properties (64- or 128-card version, computerized or pen-paper) compared with other paradigms such as the trail-making test (Atkinson & Ryan, Reference Atkinson and Ryan2008). Unlike other paradigms, the WCST also provides numerous different outputs, including perseveration, the most relevant measure in relation to set-shifting. It is widely used in both AN and ASD research allowing for direct comparison between these groups. The WCST is also considered to be a purer measure of set-shifting, compared with other paradigms which may tap into other cognitive domains (Steinglass et al. Reference Steinglass, Walsh and Stern2006).

Synthesis of data

Synthesis of results for meta-analysis meant that only studies reporting mean scores of perseverative errors were included in the analysis. Therefore, studies which reported standardized rather than raw scores or presented results visually were not included. The reasons studies were excluded are presented in Fig. 1. The meta-analyses were performed by pooling the standardized effect sizes of each study using a random-effects model. Random-effects models assume that as well as within-group variability caused by sampling variability of scores, mean effect size is also caused by differences between studies (Borenstein et al. Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). If the between-study heterogeneity is present, the random-effects model results in estimates with wider confidence intervals (CIs) than fixed-effects models.

Fig. 1. Systematic review search process. WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; PE, perseverative error; PR, perseverative response; M, mean; SD, standard deviation; HC, healthy control; AN, anorexia nervosa.

Statistical analysis

Analysis was carried out in STATA 13 (StataCorp, USA) with the following user contributed commands metan (Bradburn et al. Reference Bradburn, Deeks and Altman1998), metabias, metatrim (Steichen, Reference Steichen1998) and metareg (Sharp, Reference Sharp1998). Cohen's d was used to estimate the effect sizes and is reported for all studies together with 95% CIs. The effect sizes were interpreted according to Cohen's (Reference Cohen1988) definitions of small (⩾0.20 ⩽0.50), medium (⩾0.50 ⩽0.80), large (⩾0.80 ⩽1.30) and very large (⩾1.30). Positive effect size indicates that the clinical group (ASD or AN) made more perseverative errors than HCs. A p value of <0.05 indicates significant difference between groups.

Risk of bias across studies

Publication bias was assessed by visually evaluating funnel plots of each study's standardized mean differences against its precision and formally by Egger's test (Egger et al. Reference Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider and Minder1997) to see if a study's precision was related to effect size (Borenstein et al. Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009).

Additional analysis

Between-study heterogeneity was measured by calculating I 2 (Higgins et al. Reference Higgins, Thompson, Deeks and Altman2003) based on Cochran's Q test: measure of heterogeneity, I 2 = 100% × (Q – df)/Q, where df is degrees of freedom. I 2 ranges between 0%, indicating no heterogeneity and 100%, indicating high heterogeneity. Child/adolescent studies were analysed separately as well as overall effect size being calculated to explore whether effect was related to participant age. In addition, meta-regression was used to test for differences between ASD and AN participants using diagnosis (AN or ASD) as the moderator.

Results

Study selection

In all, 24 studies in ASD and 22 in AN were included in the meta-analyses consisting of a total of 815 AN participants matched to 956 HCs and 739 ASD participants matched to 760 HCs. The selection process for both AN and ASD studies is shown in Fig. 1.

Study characteristics

All extracted information for AN and ASD studies included in the systematic reviews and meta-analyses is presented in Tables 1 and 2, respectively. Quality ratings for each study ranged from 16 (highest possible score) to 8 (Li et al. Reference Li, Zhu, Liu and Li2014). This study included very little information on the recruitment of clinical participants or HCs but the results were sufficiently detailed for it to be included in the current review. Of the AN studies, the lowest score was 9 (McAnarney et al. Reference McAnarney, Zarcone, Singh, Michels, Welsh, Litteer, Wang and Klein2011). This was presented as a brief report and thus methodological detail was missing. The results of this study were also unclear so the authors were contacted by K.T. to clarify before data were included in the meta-analysis. The full version of the paper by Koba et al. (Reference Koba, Shrie and Nabeta2002) was unavailable to the authors at the time of this review; however, as it has been included in previous reviews (Roberts et al. Reference Roberts, Tchanturia, Stahl, Southgate and Treasure2007) it was included in the current analysis.

One ASD study (Goldstein et al. Reference Goldstein, Johnson and Minshew2001) included both child and adult participants. Although the authors did split the ASD participants into ‘old’ and ‘young’ for analysis, the control group was not split so the pooled results of children and adults, with a mean age of 18 years, were included in the adult meta-analysis. The majority of ASD studies reported IQ which ranged from 77 to 113.1 in the ASD group and 89 to 118 in HCs, indicating no intellectual disability. Nine AN studies also reported IQ, all of which fell within the normal range (97.8–119.5 for AN and 104.9–119.7 for HCs). Of the AN studies, all but two included only female participants, whereas one study included only males (Goddard et al. Reference Goddard, Carral-Fernández, Denneny, Campbell and Treasure2014) and one included over 90% of females (Andres-Perpina et al. Reference Andres-Perpina, Lozano-Serra, Puig, Lera-Miguel, Lazaro and Castro-Fornieles2011). In the ASD studies three studies included only males, with the other studies ranging from 10 to 48% female participants.

Within both clinical groups, diagnosis varied. For example, within the ASD studies eight papers reported using a diagnosis of ASD, seven used high functioning autism, seven used Asperger's syndrome and one used pervasive developmental disorder. In the AN studies, 17 used a diagnosis of AN while five only included participants with restrictive subtype AN, one of which had a separate binge-purge subtype AN group. Data on mean BMI were also extracted from AN studies and ranged from 14.4 to 19.0 in the AN group. In the ASD studies, diagnosis was given using a variety of clinical measures; eight used both the Autism Diagnostic Interview and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; 12 used diagnostic criteria taken from the DSM and several self-report measures including the Childhood Autism Rating Scale, the Autism Spectrum Quotient and the Social Responsiveness Scale were also used. In the AN studies, all studies used diagnostic criteria from the DSM, either assessed directly or by using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Disorders (SCID; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). One study used the Eating Disorder Examination (Fairburn & Cooper, Reference Fairburn, Cooper, Fairburn and Wilson1993). Several ASD and AN studies excluded HCs who had a history of psychiatric disorder but few studies reported co-morbidities, although several AN studies assessed for depression, anxiety and/or obsessive–compulsive disorder.

Sources of participant recruitment also differed across studies; in AN studies, 10 studies recruited their AN sample from specialist eating disorder services consisting of a mixture of out-, day- and in-patient treatment. Four studies did not state what type of treatment the participants were receiving; one recruited from an out-patient service; one from an in-patient service and three from psychiatric hospital settings. HCs were recruited from universities, schools or the local community. In nine of the ASD studies, ASD participants were recruited from a variety of specialist, out-patient or in-patient services for people with ASD; four from autism societies, national registers or research recruitment programmes and one from a specialist school. HCs were recruited from recruitment programmes, schools or the community. Of the ASD studies, 10 were conducted in the USA; four in Japan; three in the UK; two in China and one in the Netherlands, Denmark, Hong Kong, Iran and Taiwan, respectively. Six of the AN studies were conducted in Italy; six in the UK; three in the USA; two in Germany, Japan and Spain and one in Belgium.

Risk of bias

There was good symmetry within all funnel plots indicating no relationship between effect and study size and thus no publication bias. Egger's test indicated no evidence of publication bias (p's > 0.21). Analysis for correction of publication bias (trim-and-fill method) estimated two missing studies but this did not change the effect size considerably, so uncorrected data are presented here.

Synthesis of results

The forest plot of AN studies included in the meta-analysis is shown in Fig. 2. The random-effects analysis with a total sample size of 1731 (AN = 815, HC = 916) revealed a significant difference between AN and HC groups on the number of perseverative errors on the WCST (d = 0.43, 95% CI 0.26–0.61) z = 4.83, p ⩽ 0.001). When adult AN studies were analysed separately, there was a significant difference between AN and HC groups with a medium effect size (d = 0.48, 95% CI 0.28–0.68, z = 4.70, p ⩽ 0.001). When the four child and adolescent studies were analysed separately, there was no significant difference between AN and HC groups (d = 0.25, 95% CI −0.05 to 0.55, z = 1.66, p = 0.096).

Fig. 2. Forest plot of mean perseverative error score on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: standardized mean effect size for differences (SMD) between anorexia nervosa and healthy controls. CI, Confidence interval.

The forest plot of ASD studies included in the meta-analysis is displayed in Fig. 3. The random-effects analysis with a total sample size of 1499 (ASD = 739, HC = 760) revealed a significant difference between ASD and HC groups on the number of perseverative errors obtained during the WCST (d = 0.65, 95% CI 0.51–0.80, z = 9.91, p ⩽ 0.001). For adult studies, there was a medium effect size (d = 0.68, 95% CI 0.44–0.91, z = 5.68, p ⩽ 0.001). When adolescent studies were analysed separately, the effect size was also medium (d = 0.51, 95% CI 0.21–0.82, z = 3.28, p = 0.001). Child studies produced a medium pooled effect size (d = 0.70, 95% CI 0.46–0.92, z = 5.86, p ⩽ 0.001).

Fig. 3. Forest plot of mean perseverative error score on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: standardized mean effect size for differences (SMD) between autism spectrum disorder and healthy controls. CI, Confidence interval.

Additional analysis

There was evidence of moderate heterogeneity in both AN (I 2 = 63.1%) and ASD (I 2 = 37.0%) studies. To examine the difference between ASD and AN groups, meta-regression was conducted with diagnosis as the moderator. Within adult studies, there was no effect of diagnosis and thus no significant difference between how ASD and AN groups performed on the WCST (n = 28, mean difference = 0.20, 95% CI −0.17 to 0.56, t = 1.12, p = 0.274). There was an almost significant difference between the children and adolescent ASD and AN groups, with the ASD group scoring, on average, worse on the WCST than children and adolescents of the AN groups; however, this difference did not reach significance (mean difference = 0.44, 95% CI −0.007 to 0.89, n = 14, t = 2.13, p = 0.053).

Discussion

The aim of this review was to synthesize the literature on the use of the WCST in both ASD and AN to examine whether there are differences between children and adults both within and between these clinical populations. There were a number of differences between studies including how a diagnosis of either AN or ASD was given, the service and country in which participants were recruited from, the experimental paradigm used and how HCs were matched to the clinical groups. These differences may have made an impact on the individual outcomes of each study; however, generally the quality of the studies was deemed sufficient for meaningful synthesis and analyses. Studies also differed on the version of the WCST used, for example 128 v. 64 cards and computerized v. pen-paper versions. This may have had an impact on the results of each study. Children with ASD have been found to perform better on the computerized version of the test, suggesting that the versions are not equivalent and may measure slightly different constructs (Ozonoff, Reference Ozonoff1995).

The meta-analysis indicated significant differences between participants with AN and HCs, with the AN group making significantly more perseverative errors during the WCST. When children and adults data were analysed separately, the difference between children with AN and HCs was not significant, while in adults the difference remained significant, with a medium effect size. This indicates that adults with AN may have greater difficulty with set-shifting than young people with the disorder. This finding supports and extends a previous systematic review and meta-analysis (Roberts et al. Reference Roberts, Tchanturia, Stahl, Southgate and Treasure2007) which showed that adults with AN had significant difficulties with set-shifting relative to HCs. The addition of thirteen studies since Roberts et al.'s (2007) meta-analysis has confirmed the original finding of a significant difference between adults with AN and HCs.

The addition of Lang et al.'s (2015a) empirical study to the child literature indicates a small non-significant effect of AN on set-shifting, in line with a previous systematic review (Lang et al. Reference Lang, Stahl, Espie, Treasure and Tchanturia2014). This non-significant difference between AN and HC groups may be driven by a lack of statistical power with a limited number of studies conducted with small numbers of participants. The Lang et al. (Reference Lang, Lloyd, Khondoker, Simic, Treasure and Tchanturia2015a ) study was designed to specifically replicate the methods used within adult studies and to address previous methodological shortcomings in the child literature and therefore adds strength to the current literature, despite the result remaining non-significant. The analysis presented here shows that young people with AN may have difficulties with set-shifting, but further studies are needed to determine whether these difficulties are significant. The effect size in adult studies is larger than that in child/adolescent studies and longitudinal studies are required to properly test whether there is a magnification of the effect over time, whereby poor set-shifting is a risk factor for AN, but is also intensified by the illness, thus acting as a precipitating and perpetuating factor within the disorder. Another possibility is that more severe set-shifting problems during childhood are predictors of the persistence of these problems into adulthood. Alternatively, the WCST may be more reliable and sensitive to problems when used with adults, thus yielding bigger, significant effect sizes in adults.

Meta-analysis of ASD studies indicated a different pattern to the AN studies. Adults with ASD made significantly more perseverative errors on the WCST in comparison with HCs, indicating significant difficulties with set-shifting with a medium effect size. When child studies were analysed separately, the effect size was also medium, demonstrating that difficulties with set-shifting in ASD appear to be stable across the lifespan. This is a novel finding, as little attention has been paid in the ASD literature to WCST performance across the lifespan; however, these findings support previous work that suggests that deficits on the WCST are stable over time (Huizinga & Van Der Molen, Reference Huizinga and Van Der Molen2007) and studies of executive functioning deficits in children with ASD (Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2007; Kimhi et al. Reference Kimhi, Shoam-Kugelmas, Agam Ben-Artzi, Ben-Moshe and Bauminger-Zviely2014). The recent meta-analysis by Landry & Al-Taie (Reference Landry and Al-Taie2016) found age to be negatively correlated with set-shifting ability. While planned statistical analysis of our review did not specifically test this, CIs for the child and adult studies overlap so we are unable to conclude that there is a significant difference between children and adults with ASD.

Despite difficulties in set-shifting being widely reported within the ASD literature, preserved performance relative to HCs has also been documented in some studies (Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Russell & Hill, Reference Russell and Hill2001). Hill (Reference Hill2004) attributes this discrepancy to methodological inconsistencies amongst studies; however, as the meta-analysis performed here only included studies using the WCST, it appears to provide strong evidence than individuals with ASD have problems in set-shifting, at least as assessed using the WCST. This is the first meta-analysis to statistically compare both adult and child ASD studies using the WCST and suggests very little difference between the two groups.

The meta-regression, analysing the impact of diagnosis on set-shifting ability, found no significant difference between ASD and AN, in either the adult or child/adolescent groups, although there was a non-significant trend for children with ASD to perform worse than children with AN. This suggests that there may be differences in the development of set-shifting difficulties across ages between AN and ASD, although larger, stronger-powered longitudinal studies would be needed to confirm this.

It is important to consider the implications of these meta-analyses for both AN and ASD populations. Within AN, interventions like cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) which targets cognitive processes have been developed and empirically tested (Tchanturia et al. Reference Tchanturia, Lloyd and Lang2013a , Reference Tchanturia, Lounes and Holttum2014, Reference Tchanturia2015), with improvements in set-shifting as well as high acceptability and engagement in the treatment being reported. In spite of this success, the use of executive function training in ASD is limited, with a paucity of research in this area (De Vries et al. Reference De Vries, Prins, Schmand and Geurts2015). The few studies that have focused on flexibility training in ASD have yielded mixed results, with one study reporting improvements in flexibility (Kenworthy et al. Reference Kenworthy, Anthony, Naiman, Cannon, Wills, Luong-Tran, Werner, Alexander, Strang, Bal, Sokoloff and Wallace2014), one showing no improvement (Fisher & Happe, Reference Fisher and Happe2005) and one suggesting a trend towards improvement with intention-to-treat analysis (De Vries et al. Reference De Vries, Prins, Schmand and Geurts2015). These mixed findings may suggest that the type of flexibility training currently offered to individuals with ASD is not suitable for this population. Trials in CRT for ASD are in their infancy but the success of CRT in other disorders along with the observation that children with ASD show development in executive functioning (Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2010) makes it a worthwhile focus for future research.

While this review suggests that difficulties with set-shifting are not directly comparable in ASD and AN, its impact on symptoms and wider functioning is still unknown. One of the main cognitive theories of ASD proposes that deficits in executive function underlie the disorder (Russell, Reference Russell1997) and that these difficulties may also cause problems with social interaction and inflexible behaviour (Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2007; Geurts et al. Reference Geurts, De Vries, Van Den Beurgh, Goldstein and Nagleiri2014). In AN, the cognitive–interpersonal maintenance model of AN (Treasure & Schmidt, Reference Treasure and Schmidt2013) postulates that difficulty with flexibility is a pre-disposing factor for AN, making individuals more susceptible to societal rules and physical appearance. Thus, set-shifting may underlie other symptoms associated with the disorder. The observation that difficulties with set-shifting may underlie other symptoms in both ASD and AN suggests that transdiagnostic interventions which target cognitive style rather than direct symptoms could improve functioning and engagement in other therapies, such as cognitive–behavioural approaches which require a more flexible thinking style (Treasure et al. Reference Treasure, Cardi, Lappanen and Turton2015).

This comparison between AN and ASD studies should, however, be interpreted in light of study limitations. The majority of ASD studies only included male participants whereas most AN studies included only females. Thus, there may be important gender differences within each disorder which make an impact on set-shifting ability. For example, in one study adolescent females with ASD outperformed their male counterparts on a test of set-shifting ability (Bolte et al. Reference Bolte, Duketis, Poustka and Holtmann2011); so if females with ASD were directly compared with females with AN, the results may be different. As starvation is thought to confound set-shifting ability in AN, it is also important to consider variables such as BMI and illness duration on WCST performance, which were not analysed in this review. A study in healthy female participants found set-shifting ability to be impaired after fasting for a 16 h period (Bolton et al. Reference Bolton, Burgess, Gilbert and Serpell2014), supporting the notion that in females at least, set-shifting is worsened following starvation. Thus, the question of whether poor set-shifting ability is a trait or state characteristic remains unanswered, despite the evidence from this review suggesting that this difficulty is present in young people with AN. In the future, population-based data, rather than data from clinical samples regarding set-shifting difficulties in those with either subclinical AN or ASD traits, may be useful to avoid bias. The impact of diagnosis was not investigated here and as the diagnostic criteria of ASD and AN have become broader under the revised DSM-V criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), it is possible that greater heterogeneity between studies exists, making studies less comparable.

Acknowledgements

H.W. and K.T. would like to thank the Medical Research Council, Swiss Anorexia Foundation and the Psychiatry Research Trust for their support. D.S. received financial support from the National Institute of Health Research Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London.

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Abbate-Daga, G, Buzzichelli, S, Amianto, F, Rocca, G, Marzola, E, McClintock, SM, Fassiono, S (2011). Cognitive flexibility in verbal and nonverbal domains and decision making in anorexia nervosa patients: a pilot study. BMC Psychiatry 11, 162.Google Scholar
Abbate-Daga, G, Buzzichelli, S, Marzola, E, Amianto, F, Fassino, S (2014). Clinical investigation of set-shifting subtypes in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research 219, 592597.Google Scholar
Allely, C (2013). Anorexia nervosa – on the autistic spectrum? Psychologist 26, 656658.Google Scholar
Aloi, M, Rania, M, Caroleo, M, Bruni, A, Palmieri, A, Cauteruccio, M, De Fazio, P, Segura-Garcia, C (2015). Decision making, central coherence and set-shifting: a comparison between binge eating disorder, anorexia nervosa and healthy controls. BMC Psychiatry 15, 6.Google Scholar
Ambery, FZ, Russell, AJ, Perry, K, Morris, R, Murphy, DG (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism 10, 551564.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Andres-Perpina, S, Lozano-Serra, E, Puig, O, Lera-Miguel, S, Lazaro, L, Castro-Fornieles, J (2011). Clinical and biological correlates of adolescent anorexia nervosa with impaired cognitive profile. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 20, 541549.Google Scholar
Atkinson, TM, Ryan, JP (2008). The use of variants of the trail making test in serial assessment: a construct validity study. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 26, 4254.Google Scholar
Austin, MP, Mitchell, P, Wilhelm, K, Parker, G, Hickie, I, Brodaty, H, Chan, J, Eyers, K, Milic, M, Hadzi-Pavlovic, D (1999). Cognitive function in depression: a distinct pattern of frontal impairment in melancholia? Psychological Medicine 29, 7385.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S, Jaffa, T, Davies, S, Auyeung, B, Allison, C, Wheelwright, S (2013). Do girls with anorexia nervosa have elevated autistic traits? Molecular Autism 4, 24.Google Scholar
Bolte, S, Duketis, E, Poustka, F, Holtmann, M (2011). Sex differences in cognitive domains and their clinical correlates in higher-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism 15, 497511.Google Scholar
Bolton, HM, Burgess, PW, Gilbert, SJ, Serpell, L (2014). Increased set shifting costs in fasted healthy volunteers. PLOS ONE 9, e101946.Google Scholar
Borenstein, M, Hedges, LV, Higgins, JPT, Rothstein, HR (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Wiley: Chichester.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradburn, MJ, Deeks, JJ, Altman, DG (1998). sbe24: metan – an alternative meta-analysis command. Stata Technical Bulletin, 44, 415. Reprinted in Stata Technical Bulletin Reprints 8, 86–100. College Station, TX: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, J (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edn. Laurence Erlbaum Associates: London.Google Scholar
Courty, A, Maria, AS, Lalanne, C, Ringuenet, D, Vindreau, C, Chevallier, C, Pouga, L, Pinabel, F, Philippe, A, Adrien, JL, Barry, C, Berthoz, S (2013). Levels of autistic traits in anorexia nervosa: a comparative psychometric study. BMC Psychiatry 13, 222.Google Scholar
Critical Appraisal Skills Programme (2013). CASP Case Control Checklist. Critical Appraisal Skills Programme. CASP: Oxford (http://media.wix.com/ugd/dded87_63fb65dd4e0548e2bfd0a982295f839e.pdf). Accessed March 2016.Google Scholar
Danner, UN, Sanders, N, Smeets, PA, Van Meer, F, Adan, RA, Hoek, HW, Van Elburg, AA (2012). Neuropsychological weaknesses in anorexia nervosa: set-shifting, central coherence, and decision making in currently ill and recovered women. International Journal of Eating Disorders 45, 685694.Google Scholar
De Vries, M, Prins, PJ, Schmand, BA, Geurts, HM (2015). Working memory and cognitive flexibility-training for children with an autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 56, 566576.Google Scholar
Eack, SM, Bahorik, AL, McKnight, SA, Hogarty, SS, Greenwald, DP, Newhill, CE, Phillips, ML, Keshavan, MS, Minshew, NJ (2013). Commonalities in social and non-social cognitive impairments in adults with autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 148, 2428.Google Scholar
Egger, M, Davey Smith, G, Schneider, M, Minder, C (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal 315, 629634.Google Scholar
Fagundo, AB, De La Torre, R, Jimenez-Murcia, S, Aguera, Z, Granero, R, Tarrega, S, Botella, C, Banos, R, Fernandez-Real, JM, Rodriguez, R, Forcano, L, Fruhbeck, G, Gomez-Ambrosi, J, Tinahones, FJ, Fernandez-Garcia, JC, Casanueva, FF, Fernandez-Aranda, F (2012). Executive functions profile in extreme eating/weight conditions: from anorexia nervosa to obesity. PLOS ONE 7, e43382.Google Scholar
Fairburn, CG, Cooper, Z (1993). The eating disorder examination. In Binge Eating: Nature, Assessment and Treatment, 12th edn. (ed. Fairburn, C.G. and Wilson, G.T.), pp. 317360. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Fassino, S, Piero, A, Daga, GA, Leombruni, P, Mortara, P, Rovera, GG (2002). Attentional biases and frontal functioning in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders 31, 274283.Google Scholar
Fisher, N, Happe, F (2005). A training study of theory of mind and executive function in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35, 757771.Google Scholar
Fitzpatrick, KK, Darcy, A, Colborn, D, Gudorf, C, Lock, J (2012). Set-shifting among adolescents with anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders 45, 909912.Google Scholar
Galimberti, E, Fadda, E, Cavallini, MC, Martoni, RM, Erzegovesi, S, Bellodi, L (2013). Executive functioning in anorexia nervosa patients and their unaffected relatives. Psychiatry Research 208, 238244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geurts, HM, Corbett, B, Solomon, M (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Science 13, 7482.Google Scholar
Geurts, HM, De Vries, M, Van Den Beurgh, SFWM (2014). Executive functioning theory of autism. In Handbook of Executive Functioning (ed. Goldstein, S. and Nagleiri, J.A.), pp. 121141. Springer: New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giel, KE, Wittorf, A, Wolkenstein, L, Klingberg, S, Drimmer, E, Schoenenberg, M, Rapp, AM, Fallgatter, AJ, Hautzinger, M, Zipfel, S (2012). Is impaired set-shifting a feature of “pure” anorexia nervosa? Investigating the role of depression in set-shifting ability in anorexia nervosa and unipolar depression. Psychiatry Research 200, 538543.Google Scholar
Gillberg, IC, Billstedt, E, Wentz, E, Anckarsater, H, Rastam, M, Gillberg, C (2010). Attention, executive functions, and mentalizing in anorexia nervosa eighteen years after onset of eating disorder. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology 32, 358365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goddard, E, Carral-Fernández, L, Denneny, E, Campbell, IC, Treasure, J (2014). Cognitive flexibility, central coherence and social emotional processing in males with an eating disorder. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 15, 317326.Google Scholar
Goldstein, G, Johnson, CR, Minshew, NJ (2001). Attentional processes in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31, 433440.Google Scholar
Greve, KW, Ingram, F, Bianchini, KJ (1998). Latent structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in a clinical sample. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 13, 597609.Google Scholar
Greve, KW, Stickle, TR, Love, JM, Bianchini, KJ, Stanford, MS (2005). Latent structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: a confirmatory factor analytic study. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20, 355364.Google Scholar
Griebling, J, Minshew, NJ, Bodner, K, Libove, R, Bansal, R, Konasale, P, Keshavan, MS, Hardan, A (2010). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex magnetic resonance imaging measurements and cognitive performance in autism. Journal of Child Neurology 25, 856863.Google Scholar
Harrison, A, Tchanturia, K, Naumann, U, Treasure, J (2012). Social emotional functioning and cognitive styles in eating disorders. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 51, 261279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, RK, Chelune, GJ, Talley, JL, Kay, GG, Curtiss, G (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.: Odessa, FL.Google Scholar
Higgins, JP, Thompson, SG, Deeks, JJ, Altman, DG (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal 327, 557560.Google Scholar
Hill, EL (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Science 8, 2632.Google Scholar
Hill, EL, Bird, CM (2006). Executive processes in Asperger syndrome: patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia 44, 28222835.Google Scholar
Holliday, J, Tchanturia, K, Landau, S, Collier, D, Treasure, J (2005). Is impaired set-shifting an endophenotype of anorexia nervosa? American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 22692275.Google Scholar
Holmén, A, Juuhl-Langseth, M, Thormodsen, R, Sundet, K, Melle, I, Rund, BR (2012). Executive function tests in early-onset psychosis: which one to choose? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 53, 200225.Google Scholar
Huizinga, M, Van Der Molen, MW (2007). Age-group differences in set-switching and set-maintenance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task. Developmental Neuropsychology 31, 193215.Google Scholar
Huke, V, Turk, J, Saeidi, S, Kent, A, Morgan, JF (2013). Autism spectrum disorders in eating disorder populations: a systematic review. European Eating Disorders Review 21, 345351.Google Scholar
Huke, V, Turk, J, Saeidi, S, Kent, A, Morgan, JF (2014). The clinical implications of high levels of autism spectrum disorder features in anorexia nervosa: a pilot study. European Eating Disorders Review 22, 116121.Google Scholar
Kado, Y, Sanada, S, Yanagihara, M, Ogino, T, Ohno, S, Watanabe, K, Nakano, K, Morooka, T, Oka, M, Ohtsuka, Y (2012). Executive function in children with pervasive developmental disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder assessed by the Keio version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Brain and Development 34, 354359.Google Scholar
Kaland, N, Smith, L, Mortensen, EL (2008). Brief report: cognitive flexibility and focused attention in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism as measured on the computerized version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38, 11611165.Google Scholar
Kenworthy, L, Anthony, LG, Naiman, DQ, Cannon, L, Wills, MC, Luong-Tran, C, Werner, MA, Alexander, KC, Strang, J, Bal, E, Sokoloff, JL, Wallace, GL (2014). Randomized controlled effectiveness trial of executive function intervention for children on the autism spectrum. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 55, 374383.Google Scholar
Kenworthy, L, Yerys, BE, Anthony, LG, Wallace, GL (2008). Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychology Review 18, 320338.Google Scholar
Kimhi, Y, Shoam-Kugelmas, D, Agam Ben-Artzi, G, Ben-Moshe, I, Bauminger-Zviely, N (2014). Theory of mind and executive function in preschoolers with typical development versus intellectually able preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 44, 23412354.Google Scholar
Koba, T, Shrie, S, Nabeta, Y (2002). Impaired performance of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in patients with eating disorders: a preliminary study. Seishin Igaku 44, 681683.Google Scholar
Lam, YG, Yeung, SSS (2012). Cognitive deficits and symbolic play in preschoolers with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6, 560564.Google Scholar
Landry, O, Al-Taie, S (2016). A meta-analysis of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 46, 12201235.Google Scholar
Lang, K, Lloyd, S, Khondoker, M, Simic, M, Treasure, J, Tchanturia, K (2015 a). Do children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa display an inefficient cognitive processing style? PLOS ONE 10, e0131724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, K, Stahl, D, Espie, J, Treasure, J, Tchanturia, K (2014). Set shifting in children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa: an exploratory systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders 47, 394399.Google Scholar
Lang, K, Treasure, J, Tchanturia, K (2015 b). Is inefficient cognitive processing in anorexia a familial trait? A neuropsychological pilot study of mothers of offspring with a diagnosis of anorexia nervosa. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry. Published online 1 December 2015. doi:10.3109/15622975.2015.1112035.Google Scholar
Leopold, R, Backenstrass M, (2015). Neuropsychological differences between obsessive–compulsive washers and checkers: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 30, 4858.Google Scholar
Leung, RC, Zakzanis, KK (2014). Brief report: cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disorders: a quantitative review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 44, 26282645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, MD (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd edn. Oxford University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Li, J, Zhu, L, Liu, J, Li, X (2014). Social and non-social deficits in children with high-functioning autism and their cooperative behaviors. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8, 16571671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindner, SE, Fichter, MM, Quadflieg, N (2014). Set-shifting and its relation to clinical and personality variables in full recovery of anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review 22, 252259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez, BR, Lincoln, AJ, Ozonoff, S, Lai, Z (2005). Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35, 445460.Google Scholar
Maes, JHR, Eling, PATM, Wezenberg, E, Vissers, CTWM, Kan, CC (2011). Attentional set shifting in autism spectrum disorder: differentiating between the role of perseveration, learned irrelevance, and novelty processing. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 33, 210217.Google Scholar
McAnarney, ER, Zarcone, J, Singh, P, Michels, J, Welsh, S, Litteer, T, Wang, H, Klein, JD (2011). Restrictive anorexia nervosa and set-shifting in adolescents: a biobehavioral interface. Journal of Adolescent Health 49, 99101.Google Scholar
Milner, B (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Archives of Neurology 9, 90100.Google Scholar
Minshew, NJ, Goldstein, G, Siegel, DJ (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society 3, 303316.Google Scholar
Minshew, NJ, Meyer, J, Goldstein, G (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: a dissociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology 16, 327334.Google Scholar
Minshew, NJ, Muenz, LR, Goldstein, G, Payton, JB (1992). Neuropsychological functioning in nonmentally-retarded autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 14, 749761.Google Scholar
Moher, D, Liberati, A, Tetzlaff, J, Altman, DG, Group, P (2009). Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: the PRISMA statement. British Medical Journal 339, b2535.Google Scholar
Nakazato, M, Hashimoto, K, Schmidt, U, Tchanturia, K, Campbell, IC, Collier, DA, Iyo, M, Treasure, J (2010). Serum glutamine, set-shifting ability and anorexia nervosa. Annals of General Psychiatry 9, 29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, HE (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex 12, 313314.Google Scholar
Nielsen, S, Anckarsätar, H, Gillberg, C, Rastam, M, Wentz, E (2015). Effects of autism spectrum disorders on outcome in teenage-onset anorexia-nervosa evaluated by the Morgan–Russell Outcome Assessment Schedule: a controlled community-based sample. Molecular Autism 6, 14.Google Scholar
Odent, M (2010). Autism and anorexia nervosa: two facets of the same disease? Medical Hypotheses 75, 7981.Google Scholar
Ohrmann, P, Kersting, A, Suslow, T, Lalee-Mentzel, J, Donges, US, Fiebich, M, Arolt, V, Heindel, W, Pfleiderer, B (2004). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in anorexia nervosa: correlations with cognition. Neuroreport 15, 549553.Google Scholar
Oldershaw, A, Hambrook, D, Tchanturia, K, Treasure, J, Schmidt, U (2010). Emotional theory of mind and emotional awareness in recovered anorexia nervosa patients. Psychosomatic Medicine 72, 7379.Google Scholar
Oldershaw, A, Treasure, J, Hambrook, D, Tchanturia, K, Schmidt, U (2011). Is anorexia nervosa a version of autism spectrum disorders? European Eating Disorders Review 19, 462474.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S (1995). Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology 9, 491500.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S, Jensen, J (1999). Brief report: specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29, 171177.Google Scholar
Pantelis, C, Barber, FZ, Barnes, TR, Nelson, HE, Owen, AM, Robbins, TW (1999). Comparison of set-shifting ability in patients with chronic schizophrenia and frontal lobe damage. Schizophrenia Research 37, 251270.Google Scholar
Pascualvaca, DM, Fantie, BD, Papageorgiou, M, Mirsky, AF (1998). Attentional capacities in children with autism: is there a general deficit in shifting focus? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 28, 467478.Google Scholar
Pellicano, E (2007). Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: clues to developmental primacy. Developmental Psychology 43, 974990.Google Scholar
Pellicano, E (2010). The development of core cognitive skills in autism: a 3-year prospective study. Child Development 81, 14001416.Google Scholar
Pellicano, L, Hiller, R (2013). Anorexia and autism: a cautionary note. Psychologist 26, 780.Google Scholar
Pender, S, Gilbert, SJ, Serpell, L (2014). The neuropsychology of starvation: set-shifting and central coherence in a fasted nonclinical sample. PLOS ONE 9, e110743.Google Scholar
Pignatti, R, Bernasconi, V (2013). Personality, clinical features, and test instructions can affect executive functions in eating disorders. Eating Behaviour 14, 233236.Google Scholar
Pooragha, F, Kafi, SM, Sotodeh, SO (2013). Comparing response inhibition and flexibility for two components of executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder and normal children. Iranian Journal of Pediatrics 23, 309314.Google ScholarPubMed
Rhind, C, Bonfioli, E, Hibbs, R, Goddard, E, Macdonald, P, Gowers, S, Schmidt, U, Tchanturia, K, Micali, N, Treasure, J (2014). An examination of autism spectrum traits in adolescents with anorexia nervosa and their parents. Molecular Autism 5, 56.Google Scholar
Roberts, ME, Tchanturia, K, Stahl, D, Southgate, L, Treasure, J (2007). A systematic review and meta-analysis of set-shifting ability in eating disorders. Psychological Medicine 37, 10751084.Google Scholar
Roberts, ME, Tchanturia, K, Treasure, JL (2010). Exploring the neurocognitive signature of poor set-shifting in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatric Research 44, 964970.Google Scholar
Rumsey, JM (1985). Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 15, 2336.Google Scholar
Russell, J (1997). Autism as an Executive Disorder. Oxford University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Russell, J, Hill, EL (2001). Action-monitoring and intention reporting in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 42, 317328.Google Scholar
Sanders, J, Johnson, KA, Garavan, H, Gill, M, Gallagher, L (2008). A review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research in autistic spectrum disorders: attention, inhibition and cognitive flexibility. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 2, 116.Google Scholar
Sato, Y, Saito, N, Utsumi, A, Aizawa, E, Shoji, T, Izumiyama, M, Mushiake, H, Hongo, M, Fukudo, S (2013). Neural basis of impaired cognitive flexibility in patients with anorexia nervosa. PLOS ONE 8, e61108.Google Scholar
Sharp, S (1998). sbe23: meta-analysis regression. Stata Technical Bulletin, 42, 1622. Reprinted in Stata Technical Bulletin Reprints 7, 148–155. College Station, TX: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Shu, BC, Lung, FW, Tien, AY, Chen, BC (2001). Executive function deficits in non-retarded autistic children. Autism 5, 165174.Google Scholar
Steichen, TJ (1998). sbe19: tests for publication bias in meta-analysis. Stata Technical Bulletin, 41, 915. Reprinted in Stata Technical Bulletin Reprints 7, 125–133. College Station, TX: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Steinglass, JE, Walsh, BT, Stern, Y (2006). Set shifting deficit in anorexia nervosa. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society 12, 431435.Google Scholar
Sumiyoshi, C, Kawakubo, Y, Suga, M, Sumiyoshi, T, Kasai, K (2011). Impaired ability to organize information in individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their siblings. Neuroscience Research 69, 252257.Google Scholar
Tchanturia, K (editor) (2015). Cognitive Remediation Therapy (CRT) for Eating and Weight Disorders. Routledge: Hove, East Sussex.Google Scholar
Tchanturia, K, Davies, H, Roberts, M, Harrison, A, Nakazato, M, Schmidt, U, Treasure, J, Morris, R (2012). Poor cognitive flexibility in eating disorders: examining the evidence using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task. PLOS ONE 7, e28331.Google Scholar
Tchanturia, K, Lloyd, S, Lang, K (2013 a). Cognitive remediation therapy for anorexia nervosa: current evidence and future directions. International Journal of Eating Disorders 46, 492495.Google Scholar
Tchanturia, K, Lounes, N, Holttum, S (2014). Cognitive remediation in anorexia nervosa and related conditions: a systematic review. European Eating Disorders Review 22, 454462.Google Scholar
Tchanturia, K, Morris, RG, Anderluh, MB, Collier, DA, Nikolaou, V, Treasure, J (2004). Set shifting in anorexia nervosa: an examination before and after weight gain, in full recovery and relationship to childhood and adult OCPD traits. Journal of Psychiatric Research 38, 545552.Google Scholar
Tchanturia, K, Smith, E, Weineck, F, Fidanboylu, E, Kern, N, Treasure, J, Baron Cohen, S (2013 b). Exploring autistic traits in anorexia: a clinical study. Molecular Autism 4, 44.Google Scholar
Thompson, SBN (1993). Implications of neuropsychological test results of women in a new phase of anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review 1, 152165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treasure, J (2013). Coherence and other autistic spectrum traits and eating disorders: building from mechanism to treatment. The Birgit Olsson lecture. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry 67, 3842.Google Scholar
Treasure, J, Cardi, V, Lappanen, J, Turton, R (2015). New treatment approaches for severe and enduring eating disorders. Physiology and Behavior 152, 456465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treasure, J, Schmidt, U (2013). The cognitive–interpersonal maintenance model of anorexia nervosa revisited: a summary of the evidence for cognitive, socio-emotional and interpersonal predisposing and perpetuating factors. Journal of Eating Disorders 1, 13.Google Scholar
Tsuchiya, E, Oki, J, Yahara, N, Fujieda, K (2005). Computerized version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in children with high-functioning autistic disorder or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain and Development 27, 233236.Google Scholar
Van Autreve, S, De Baene, W, Baeken, C, Van Heeringen, C, Vervaet, M (2013). Do restrictive and bingeing/purging subtypes of anorexia nervosa differ on central coherence and set shifting? European Eating Disorders Review 21, 308314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westwood, H, Eisler, I, Mandy, W, Leppanen, J, Treasure, J, Tchanturia, K (2016). Using the autism-spectrum quotient to measure autistic traits in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 46, 964977.Google Scholar
Williams, D, Jarrold, C (2013). Assessing planning and set-shifting abilities in autism: are experimenter-administered and computerised versions of tasks equivalent? Autism Research 6, 461467.Google Scholar
Winsler, A, Abar, B, Feder, MA, Schunn, CD, Rubio, DA (2007). Private speech and executive functioning among high-functioning children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37, 16171635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, M, Brockmeyer, T, Hartmann, M, Skunde, M, Herzog, W, Friederich, HC (2014). Set-shifting ability across the spectrum of eating disorders and in overweight and obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 44, 33653385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, J, Zhou, S, Yao, S, Linyan, S, McWhinnie, C (2009). The relationship between theory of mind and executive function in a sample of children from mainland China. Child Psychiatry and Human Development 40, 169182.Google Scholar
Yasuda, Y, Hashimoto, R, Ohi, K, Yamamori, H, Fujimoto, M, Umeda-Yano, S, Fujino, H, Takeda, M (2014). Cognitive inflexibility in Japanese adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorders. World Journal of Psychiatry 4, 4248.Google Scholar
Zucker, N, Losh, M, Bulik, CM, LaBar, KS, Piven, J, Pelphrey, KA (2007). Anorexia nervosa and autism spectrum disorders: guided investigation of social cognitive phenotypes. Psychological Bulletin 133, 9761006.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic information for AN studies

Figure 1

Table 2. Demographic information for ASD studies

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Systematic review search process. WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; PE, perseverative error; PR, perseverative response; M, mean; SD, standard deviation; HC, healthy control; AN, anorexia nervosa.

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Forest plot of mean perseverative error score on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: standardized mean effect size for differences (SMD) between anorexia nervosa and healthy controls. CI, Confidence interval.

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Forest plot of mean perseverative error score on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: standardized mean effect size for differences (SMD) between autism spectrum disorder and healthy controls. CI, Confidence interval.