1 Introduction
French vowels are reputedly pure, that is, non-diphthongal (Delattre Reference Delattre1963). In their IPA illustration, Fougeron & Smith (Reference Fougeron and Smith1993) list 14 vowels – 11 oral and 3 nasal – all of them monophthongs. At the acoustic level, Gottfried (Reference Gottfried1984) observes that French vowels in consonant–vowel–consonant (CVC) syllables are characterized by very short flanking consonantal transitions and a stable plateau in-between, as opposed to English vowels that present extensive formant dynamics (e.g. Nearey & Assmann Reference Nearey and Assmann1986). Additionally, according to Fougeron & Smith (Reference Fougeron and Smith1993: 74), length contrasts in vowels are not phonologically relevant for ‘most speakers’ of French, including the Parisian speaker their illustration is based on. For example, the majority of French speakers the authors refer to would produce the following pairs of words identically, that is, with a monophthongal and short [ɛ]:
However, as briefly acknowledged by Fougeron & Smith (Reference Fougeron and Smith1993), variation does exist. Speakers from eastern France, Belgium and Switzerland may produce the second word of these pairs with a long vowel: [ɛː] (see e.g. Walter Reference Walter1982, Avanzi Reference Avanzi2018).
When it comes to Quebec French, which will be the variety studied in this article, speakers also differentiate between these pairs of words, but there is no consensus on the actual basis of the contrast. Some researchers attribute this to length, like in the French spoken in eastern France, Belgium and Switzerland, though vowel quality and the diphthongal quality of the second vowel of these pairs (henceforth the fête vowel) have also been suggested. Indeed, one of the main features that distinguish Quebec French from European varieties is the possibility for some vowels to be produced with a complex nucleus (see e.g. Dumas Reference Dumas1974, Reference Dumas1987; Santerre & Millo Reference Santerre, Millo and David1978; Walker Reference Walker1984), a process that fête might be particularly inclined to undergo (Leblanc Reference Leblanc2012). The first goal pursued in this article is thus to determine, with the help of up-to-date empirical data, which cue – length or quality – makes fête most different from faite (the first vowel of the pairs), but also from the other vowels of Quebec French.
An important issue we have come across when conducting this research has to do with the multiplicity of symbols used to transcribe fête. Phonetic transcriptions include [ɛː], [Œː], [aɛ], [ai], [ae], [ej], [aj], [ɜ], [ɜe] and [ɜj], to name but a few (Dumas Reference Dumas1974; Santerre Reference Santerre1974; Walker Reference Walker1984; Martin Reference Martin1995, Reference Martin2002; Morin Reference Morin2009; Côté Reference Côté, Carmen, France and Yves2010, Reference Côté, Randall, Chantal and Trudel2012). However, this vowel has also been transcribed phonologically as /ɛ/ (identically to faite), /ɜ/ or /ɛː/ (e.g. Dumas Reference Dumas1974; Santerre Reference Santerre1974; Martin Reference Martin1995, Reference Martin1996). The second goal we pursue is thus to propose a transcription of fête that is consistent with both our empirical data and the International Phonetic Alphabet.
In Section 2, we provide the readers with further details regarding diphthongization and vocalic length in Quebec French, as well as the phonological status of fête. We also use this section to present the main conclusions drawn by a few authors who analyzed fête acoustically (Santerre Reference Santerre1974, Martin Reference Martin1995, Leblanc Reference Leblanc2012, Côté & Lancien Reference Côté, Lancien, Sasha, Paola, Marija and Paul2019), as well as summarize the rationale for the most widely used phonological transcriptions of this vowel. Section 3 describes the methods used to reach our goals. Results are detailed in Section 4, while Section 5 puts them into perspective, in regards to both the literature and our goals.
2 fête in Quebec French
As briefly mentioned in the Introduction, the diphthongal quality Quebec French vowels may acquire is a distinguishing feature of this variety (see Paradis & Dolbec Reference Paradis and Dolbec1998 for an extensive listing of phonetic phenomena taking place in Quebec French). In fact, diphthongization had already been mentioned in early descriptions of Quebec French, for example by Squair (Reference Squair1888: 162–165), who alternatively comments on ‘a tendency to drawl’ and ‘real diphthong[s]’. While the reasons behind the emergence of this phenomenon in Quebec though not in Europe are unclear (Paradis & Dolbec Reference Paradis and Dolbec1998), researchers agree that diphthongization is closely tied to duration.
First, let us recall that duration is an acoustic property of vowels correlated to their perceived length. In some languages and dialects, duration is used as a phonological contrast between same-quality vowels, while it may also vary contextually and depend on such factors as neighboring consonants, syllable type, stress, word or syntactic structure or speech rate. As for diphthongization, it basically consists of producing a vocalic nucleus with a double quality (or triple, in the case of triphthongs). Like duration, it may be involved in phonological contrasts or simply be a contextually driven event. In Quebec French, vowels can be diphthongized when stressed (i.e. in the last syllable of a word) and of relatively long duration. The main factors leading to such a longer duration in stressed vowels are their own nature and that of the syllabic coda.
All stressed vowels see their duration lengthened when followed by the so-called ‘lengthening’ consonants /v z ʒ ʁ/ and /vʁ/,Footnote 1 as opposed to other codas or in open syllables (V#). For example, /a/ is long in lave [laːv] ‘lava’, but not in lac [lak] ‘lake’ or la [la] ‘the’. As for the naturally or historically long vowels, it is beyond the scope of this study to get into the in-depth discussions many authors have nurtured in the 20th century regarding the origins, evolution and distribution of vocalic length in (Quebec) French (e.g. Straka Reference Straka1959, Martinet Reference Martinet1969, Morin & Ouellet Reference Morin and Ouellet1991, Yaeger-Dror Reference Yaeger-Dror and Keating1994, Yaeger-Dror & Kemp Reference Yaeger-Dror and Kemp1992), so let us simply mention that in closed syllables (e.g. CVC), /o ø ɑ ɑ˜ ɔ˜ ɛ˜ œ˜/ and fête have a longer duration than other vowels (Côté Reference Côté, Carmen, France and Yves2010). For example, a word like saute ‘jump’ is pronounced with a long vowel, [soːt], while sotte [sɔt] ‘stupid’ or sept [sɛt] ‘seven’ are not.
Once conditions conducive to diphthongization are created (i.e. stress and increased duration), complex vocalic nuclei that Quebec French speakers produce always rise and always remain on the same frontness/backness axis (Dumas Reference Dumas1974, Côté Reference Côté, Carmen, France and Yves2010). For example, saute ‘jump’ can be pronounced [sɔ͜ot] or [so͜ut], but not *[so͜ɔt] or *[so͜yt]. The diphthongization process admits few exceptions: only /a/ and some lexical items featuring the faite vowel cannot be diphthongized even if contextually lengthened (e.g. sage [saːʒ] ‘wise’ or chèvre [ʃɛːvʁ] ‘goat’). Despite the apparent regularity of the diphthongization rule and the fact that it seems like a well-established practice in Quebec French (Squair Reference Squair1888), it is usually not regarded as systematic. In particular, external factors such as socioeconomic background, geographical origin, age or formality make some speakers or communication situations more or less subject to diphthongization (Santerre Reference Santerre1974, Dumas Reference Dumas1987, Yaeger-Dror & Kemp Reference Yaeger-Dror and Kemp1992, Martin Reference Martin1995, Reinke Reference Reinke2005).
With regard to the foregoing, fête seems to stand out. First, it is the only historically long vowel that does not have a short variant. Indeed, /o ø ɑ ɑ˜ ɔ˜ ɛ˜ œ˜/ are long in closed syllables, but short in open syllables (e.g. saute [soːt] ‘jump’, seau [so] ‘bucket’; jeûne [ʒØːn] ‘fast’, jeu [ʒØ] ‘game’). fête simply cannot appear in open syllables; it is therefore always long and it always has the potential to be diphthongized. Second, for all vowels but fête, quality is definitely seen as prevailing over length. For example, the phonological contrast between vowels like /a/ and /ɑ/ in patte [pat] ‘paw’ and pâtes [pɑːt] ‘pasta’ is attributed to a difference in backness, the longer duration of /ɑ/ being treated as incidental. However, for Martin (Reference Martin1995), fête is first and foremost longer than faite, which makes it the only vowel of Quebec French for which length has been posited as the dominant contrastive cue. Third, in syllables closed by a lengthening coda, fête and faite should be neutralized, as faite theoretically has the potential to be diphthongized now that it is longer. As alluded to above, it is not always what happens with /v/ or /vʁ/ (Santerre Reference Santerre1981). In words like lève [lɛːv] ‘rise’ or chèvre [ʃɛːvʁ] ‘goat’, the faite vowel is never diphthongized, despite being stressed and followed by a lengthening coda. When it comes to faite–fête, a longer duration is thus necessary, but not sufficient, for diphthongization to happen. This is one of the facts that had initially led Santerre (Reference Santerre1981) to suggest that underlying phonological representations were at play, unlike for other contextually diphthongized vowels.
If we now turn to acoustic descriptions of faite–fête, one of the earliest is provided by Santerre (Reference Santerre1974), who first notes that the duration of fête is twice as long as that of faite. The first two formants always diverge over the course of the fête vowel (F1 decreases and/or F2 increases), while they tend to converge over the course of faite. Drawing from his dissertation (Santerre Reference Santerre1971), in which articulatory and acoustic measurements were taken from two speakers of different socioeconomic backgrounds, Santerre (Reference Santerre1974) also highlights that fête is only diphthongized by the lower class speaker. Nonetheless, the higher class speaker produces fête vowels that are acoustically distinct from faite: even when diphthongization is not perceived, fête keeps its signature longer duration and diverging F1 and F2. Finally, Santerre (Reference Santerre1974: 123) briefly alludes to a perception experiment where fête was deliberately shortened and faite lengthened – listeners were never misled by these manipulations and always correctly identified the original vowel. For this author then, fête is mainly characterized by its quality, not duration, and he transcribes it as /ɜ/. This choice of symbol seems purely arbitrary, since at no point does Santerre (Reference Santerre1974) mention some sort of centralization, which would be expected if /ɜ/ had been drawn from the IPA vowel chart. A quick parenthesis should be made regarding the fact that, more recently, Côté (Reference Côté, Randall, Chantal and Trudel2012: 240–241) justifies using that same symbol given the central quality of the fête vowel, with references to Santerre (Reference Santerre1974, Reference Santerre1981). As mentioned above, Santerre himself does not allude to centralization in 1974 while in his 1981 phonological reflection, he claims that fête has the same starting point as faite and is subsequently fronted (‘articulation … avançante’ (Santerre Reference Santerre1981: 377)), which we do not think can be taken as evidence of centralization.
Martin (Reference Martin1995) also provides an acoustic description of the faite and fête vowels produced by undergraduate students aged from 19 to 21 years. First, the author proceeds to an auditory categorization and comes up with the three following groups:
A. short and monophthongal faite
B. long and diphthongal fête
C. long and monophthongal fête
The few vowels belonging to group C likely correspond to those produced by Santerre’s (Reference Santerre1974) upper class speaker, but all in all, Martin (Reference Martin1995) considers that in the 1990s, diphthongization of fête has become the norm among young university students. In terms of duration, faite vowels can be half as long as fête vowels (groups B and C), a tendency also observed by Santerre (Reference Santerre1974). The acoustic distance between F1 and F2 slightly shrinks over the course of faite and group C fête, whereas for group B fête, F1 and F2 grow farther apart. These results are slightly different from Santerre’s (Reference Santerre1974), whose group C fête vowels showed formant trajectories similar to group B. Furthermore, Martin (Reference Martin1995: 44) highlights the ‘compact’ onset of diphthongal fête vowels, that is, high F1 and relatively low F2. The diphthong would thus start as [a] and further rise and front. Martin (Reference Martin1995) nonetheless maintains /ɛː/ for his phonological transcriptions, thereby implying that a longer duration is the dominant characteristic of fête.
Let us now say a few words about a recent study by Leblanc (Reference Leblanc2012), whose main goal is to identify the acoustic properties of diphthongal vowels in Quebec French. Three trained phoneticians first transcribed 2760 naturally uttered words – only those that contained vowels that the majority perceived as diphthongal were kept for further analyses, that is, 17.7% of the initial corpus. Leblanc (Reference Leblanc2012) reports that the fête vowel is by far the most frequently uttered diphthongal, at 92%. The author also shows, with Bark transformed F1–F0 and F2–F1 distances, that the onset of fête shares properties with /a/, after which rising and fronting towards /ɛ/ and /e/ occur.
Even more recently, Côté & Lancien (Reference Côté, Lancien, Sasha, Paola, Marija and Paul2019) used recordings from the PFC project (Phonologie du français contemporain; Durand, Laks & Lyche Reference Durand, Laks, Lyche, Durand, Laks and Lyche2009) to follow up on Santerre’s (Reference Santerre1974) work. In addition to the usual faite and fête, they analyze words morphologically derived from fête, in which the vowel of interest is not stressed (fêtard, fêter, fêteriez). The authors find that the length distinction between faite and fête is only maintained under stress. In an F1/F2 plane, fête rises and fronts, although the trajectory of the stressed vowel is greater than that of the unstressed vowels, while faite follows the opposite trajectory (reduced lowering and backing). Finally, the authors set up a lexical decision task where 44 listeners categorized (stressed) fête and faite tokens that naturally varied in length. The results suggest that relatively short fête and long faite were rarely misidentified by the participants. This led Côté & Lancien (Reference Côté, Lancien, Sasha, Paola, Marija and Paul2019) to conclude that the faite–fête distinction is driven by vowel quality.
To sum up, Santerre (Reference Santerre1974) underscores the divergent trajectory of the first two formants of fête, in addition to the obviously long duration of the diphthong. Contrastively, Martin (Reference Martin1995) focuses on the compact onset of the vowel. Leblanc’s (Reference Leblanc2012) data clearly shows a vowel that rises and fronts from the /a/ area, which is corroborated by Côté & Lancien’s (Reference Côté, Lancien, Sasha, Paola, Marija and Paul2019) analysis, as well as Santerre’s (Reference Santerre1981) and Martin’s (Reference Martin1995) comments. Occasionally, fête is long but monophthongal. In such cases, Santerre (Reference Santerre1974) considers it to share the acoustic properties of diphthongal fête, though Martin (Reference Martin1995) finds it is more similar to monophthongal faite, a discrepancy that partly explains why the former uses /ɜ/ and the latter /ɛː/ in their transcriptions. Leblanc (Reference Leblanc2012) exclusively focuses on diphthongal vowels in the acoustic part of his study, but his descriptive statistics show that instances of monophthongal fête are marginal (8%) in the 2010s, a tendency that also arises from Martin’s (Reference Martin1995) classification. We will elaborate on this further below with our own empirically based acoustic description of fête.
3 Methods
3.1 Speech material
In order to best describe fête, we chose to compare it with faite, along with the entire vocalic system of Quebec French. Note that this strategy is intended to locate fête in the acoustic space and highlight its characteristics, and not to exhaustively describe Quebec French (we refer those interested in a more complete description to Martin Reference Martin1996, Paradis & Dolbec Reference Paradis and Dolbec1998, Côté Reference Côté, Randall, Chantal and Trudel2012). On account of the technical difficulties involved in the acoustic analysis of nasal vowels, our study focuses exclusively on oral vowels.
We set up reading tasks that would bring speakers to utter all Quebec French oral vowels in stressed syllables, specifically: [i I y ʏ u ʊ e Ø o œ ɔ a ɑ], faite and fête. The only historically long vowels taken into consideration are thus /Ø o ɑ/ and fête. Note that schwa is not included in the list as it is usually not stressed. It is also worth mentioning that in Quebec French, the high vowels /i y u/ split into mutually exclusive ‘tense’ and ‘lax’ variants, respectively [i y u] and [I ʏ ʊ], the latter appearing in stressed syllables closed by a non-lengthening coda, and the former elsewhere. Moreover, we chose to avoid lengthening codas, since these can significantly modify vowel duration and the potential for contextual diphthongization, as well as partly neutralize the distinction between faite and fête (see Section 2). The 15 aforementioned vowels were thus produced in syllables closed by a non-lengthening coda or in open syllables, for a total of 57 different lexical items, as shown in Appendix A.
Each of these 57 lexical items was produced over the course of three reading tasks (every lexical item uttered three times). Task 1 consisted of participants reading out of context, but meaningful, carrier sentences ending with a target lexical item. For example, the following sentences featured the target words bête ‘grumpy’ (fête vowel) and belle ‘beautiful’ (faite vowel):
– Le matin, j’ai l’air bête.
‘In the morning, I am (or look) grumpy.’
– La vie est belle.
‘Life is beautiful.’
Task 2 featured the same target words within a fixed carrier sentence. Appearing at the end of an accentual phrase, they were stressed:
– Je pense au mot bête très fort.
‘I am thinking very hard about the word “grumpy”.’
– Je pense au mot belle très fort.
‘I am thinking very hard about the word “beautiful”.’
Task 3 again featured the 57 target lexical items, but this time in isolation:
– Bête
‘Grumpy’
– Belle
‘Beautiful’
The main reason for designing three different tasks was to avoid a task effect. Additionally, each task had its own strengths and weaknesses. Task 1 avoided all possible lexical confusion, but frequently gave way to phenomena like breathy and creaky voice. This was avoided in Task 2, but coarticulation effects might have been more prevalent. Task 3 may have provoked unnatural prosodic patterns, but isolated words could be easily reused in other studies, for example perception experiments. We are aware that slight cross-task variation may arise, but the relative characteristics of the vowels remain unchanged. As explained in Section 3.3, this has been taken into account in our statistical analyses.
The words presented to the speakers over the three reading tasks correspond to a standard but common register in Quebec French and comprise some dialectal particularities (e.g. gougoune ‘plastic sandal/thong’). Lexical frequency was not explicitly controlled for. The participants were instructed to read, in an as natural a manner as possible, the sentences or words appearing individually on the screen of an electronic tablet at a rate controlled by the experimenter. A short training phase helped them familiarize themselves with the three tasks. They were recorded individually in a sound-attenuated booth, using a Zoom H4n (digital format, 44,100 Hz, 16 bits).
3.2 Speakers
Fifty-four participants were recruited for this study; all were undergraduate students enrolled at Université Laval in September 2016. Data from two speakers were excluded as their parents were from France, they had lived abroad for several years and their pronunciation comprised features of European French, such as three nasal vowels instead of four and tense variants of /i y u/ in syllables closed by a non-lengthening coda. The remaining 52 participants, 37 women and 15 men aged from 18 to 23 years, were native speakers of Quebec French. Twenty were born and raised in Quebec City or Levis (a city on the south shore of the St. Lawrence River across from Quebec City), while the others came from various towns and cities across the province and had just moved to Quebec City to start university. None of them had lived outside the province of Quebec, except for short intervals (e.g. a six-month student exchange in South America to learn Spanish or a summer spent in British Columbia). They were compensated $ 10 for their time.
3.3 Analysis
The recorded speech was analyzed acoustically using Praat (Boersma & Weenink Reference Boersma and Weenink2019). The 8892 tokens (57 words × 3 tasks × 52 speakers) were first segmented manually by a trained acoustician, who identified vocalic boundaries based on at least one of the following cues: periodicity, intensity, formant structure, voice bars, high-frequency noise, bursts, etc. Twenty-six tokens were discarded when segmentation or further analysis was deemed impossible, leaving 8866. Duration was measured, as well as frequency of the first two formants at three time points: at 25%, 50% and 75% of the vowel duration. This part of the analysis was also performed manually, with the formant detection ceiling and number of formants to be detected in the frequency interval optimized for each speaker and each vocalic category – when needed, they were adjusted per token.
Taking into account more than one formant measurement set was motivated by the fact that we are dealing with diphthongal vowels, which typically have a complex vocalic core and formants that do not truly stabilize (Lehiste & Peterson Reference Lehiste and Peterson1961). Our choice of measuring formants near the onset (25%) and offset (75%) of the vowel is inspired by the ‘dual target’ parameterization proposed in the literature focusing on spectral changes (Nearey & Assmann Reference Nearey and Assmann1986, Morrison & Assmann Reference Morrison and Assmann2013). However, we used them to compute a single metric that accounts for the temporal evolution of a given formant (Fx), as follows:
– Fx dynamics = Fx at 75% – Fx at 25%
Here is a concrete example. If the F1 of a given vowel has a frequency of 500 Hz at 25%, its dynamics will be of 10 Hz if its frequency rises to 510 Hz at 75%, while its dynamics will be of –10 Hz if its frequency lowers to 490 Hz at 75%. A negative value thus indicates that Fx has had its frequency lowered over the course of the vowel, while the opposite is true for a positive value. We computed the formant dynamics for every vowel, not exclusively for fête, in order to highlight its specific properties.
Linear mixed effects models were fit to the data using the lme4 package (Bates et al. Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Walker2015) in the R environment (R Core Team 2019). Duration and the dynamics of F1 and F2 were set as response variables in separate models. Initially, random effects included the speakers, as well as the words nested within the tasks. A factor combining the vowel and type of syllable it was produced in (open or closed) was created and set as fixed effect, in addition to the speakers’ sex and their interaction. The step() function of the lmerTest package (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff & Christensen Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen2017) then made it possible to eliminate non-significant interactions or factors. We visually assessed whether variance was homogeneous and residuals normally distributed. As duration presented a heteroscedasticity problem, the model was rerun on log-transformed duration. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons were conducted using the emmeans package (Lenth, Love & Herve Reference Lenth, Love and Herve2018), with degrees of freedom calculated with the Satterthwaite equation and adjusted p-values with the Tukey method. The significance level has been set to 5%. Model summaries are displayed in Appendix B.
4 Results
In this section, we first present results on duration, followed by those on formant frequencies. Duration is presented with the help of boxplots, hence the data is not averaged. On the contrary, formant frequencies have been averaged, first per speaker, then across speakers. The data has not been normalized. Where appropriate, the results have been split per syllable type (open or closed, henceforth OS and CS), as this can lead to substantial differences, especially in duration. This is also how the statistical analyses were performed. The results of the three tasks are presented jointly. Note that in the graphs displayed hereafter, fête is symbolized by ‘ $\hat{\varepsilon}$ ’ and faite by ‘ $\varepsilon$ ’.
Figure 1 suggests that fête has the longest duration of all, but also the widest distribution, as shown by its spread whiskers. The vowels /ɑ o Ø/ have comparably long durations when produced in CS. This is coherent with historical length only emerging in CS, as explained in Section 2. The short variants of /ɑ o Ø/ produced in OS have durations comparable to those of the intrinsically short vowels, the durations of which do not seem much influenced by the type of syllable they were produced in.
If we observe the duration ratio between faite and fête, we find it to be somewhat smaller than that reported by Santerre (Reference Santerre1974) and Martin (Reference Martin1995), who both observe that fête is twice as long as faite. In our own data, the mean duration of faite (0.136 s across syllable types) represents two thirds of the duration of fête (0.220 s). Nonetheless, fête is statistically significantly different from faite in both OS and CS (OS: β = –0.532, s.e. = 0.104, t(1.41) = –5.101, p <.001; CS: β = –0.282, s.e. = 0.031, t(2.41) = –8.909, p <.001). However, the estimated duration of fête does not differ from that of /ɑ o Ø/ in CS (/ɑ/: β = 0.066, s.e. = 0.059, t(1.68) = 1.108, p = .2692; /o/: β = 0.006, s.e. = 0.052, t(2.08) = 0.115, p = .9082; /Ø/: β = –0.063, s.e. = 0.074, t(2.67) = –0.852, p = .3952), while it does from all the rest (see Table B2 of Appendix B for more details).
Figure 2 illustrates how formant frequencies evolve from 25% to 75% of the vowel duration. We observe that only /a/ and /ɑ/ in OS, respectively located in the first (Q1) and second (Q2) quadrants, see their first formant rise over time. For the other vowels, F1 drops to various degrees or stays roughly the same. Vowels in Q2 and Q3 have their second formant lowered, while F2 rises in Q1 and Q4 vowels. Except for /i/, male and female productions are located in the same quadrants. However, some of the vowels produced by the male speakers have slightly reduced trajectories compared to those produced by the female speakers, for example /y/, /œ/ in OS (which overlaps with /œ/ in CS) or fête. This is likely due to the fact that male formant frequencies are usually lower, and that smaller differences in low frequencies have similar perceptual consequences as larger differences in higher frequencies. While certain vowels produced in both types of syllables are located in the same quadrant, for example /o/ and /Ø/, it is not always the case. In addition to /a/ and /ɑ/ that were already mentioned, faite is located in Q3 when in CS and Q4 in OS. There is no clear tendency for front and back vowels to follow a particular direction: their second formant may as well rise or drop.
Perhaps the most striking observation emerging from Figure 2 is the location of fête in Q4, far from the other vowels. Both F1 and F2 undergo extensive change over the course of fête, including a considerable mean rise of 497 Hz on F2 in female speakers and 357 Hz in male speakers. If anything, this suggests that if only one vowel has been diphthongized, it has to be fête. Qualitatively, these findings confirm those of Santerre (Reference Santerre1974) and Martin (Reference Martin1995), in that the first two formants of fête diverge over time (F1 drops while F2 rises). Côté & Lancien (Reference Côté, Lancien, Sasha, Paola, Marija and Paul2019) report the opposite trajectory for faite (F1 rising and F2 dropping), a trend that is only partly confirmed here. In CS, the F2 of faite does drop, while its F1 does not rise; in OS (not analyzed by Côté & Lancien Reference Côté, Lancien, Sasha, Paola, Marija and Paul2019), the formants of faite follow the same trajectory as fête, although less markedly. Statistically speaking, the estimated F1 and F2 dynamics for fête are significantly different from those of all the other vowels for female speakers (see Appendix B). For male speakers, fête and /Ø/ in CS do not exhibit significantly different F1 dynamics (β = –73.62, s.e. = 26.34, t(452) = –2.795, p = .7148). Also for male speakers, the difference between the F2 dynamics of fête and that of /e/, /u/ and /Ø/ in both types of syllables only shows a trend towards significance (/e/: β = 269.98, s.e. = 71.9, t(184) = 3.758, p = .0992; /u/: β = 277.38, s.e. = 71.9, t(185) = 3.857, p = .0736; /Ø/ CS: β = 218.08, s.e. = 56, t(405) = 3.893, p = .0589; /Ø/ OS: β = 272.09, s.e. = 71.9, t(184) = 3.787, p = .0910). This might be due to the relatively low number of male speakers in our study (only 15), or to a tendency for either female speakers to diphthongize fête particularly strongly, or male speakers to produce vowels other than fête with substantial spectral changes.
Figure 3 presents a more familiar illustration of the mean formant frequencies for the vowels across syllable types. The vowel spaces represented offer no surprise, although two characteristics are worth mentioning. One is the location of /e/, very close to /i/, whereas the other mid-close vowels, /Ø/ and /o/, are well distinguished from the high vowels on the F1 axis. It is probable that the third and fourth formants, not represented here, contribute to further distinguishing /i/ and /e/ acoustically, the former reputedly having high frequency focalized F3 and F4 (Ménard Reference Ménard2002). The second characteristic is the location of /ɔ/ along the F2 axis. This tendency for /ɔ/ to centralize, well documented in European French since Martinet (Reference Martinet1958), has also been attested in some studies on Quebec French (e.g. Martin Reference Martin2002, St-Gelais Reference St-Gelais2019). Finally, the female and male vowel spaces displayed in Figure 3 are visually similar, although the /œ/–/Ø/–/ɔ/ triangle seems slightly more compressed in female speakers.
Now let us consider fête. If we first concentrate on values at 50% duration (illustrated by the small squares in Figure 3), we observe that when the vowel reaches its midpoint, it is approximately halfway between /a/ and faite in the F1/F2 planes, in a position that roughly corresponds to /Œ/ in the IPA vowel chart. However, the most interesting part is the trajectory of fête. At 25% duration, its first two formants present values similar to those of /a/, while they reach the faite area at 75% duration. Some fronting and extensive rising thus characterize the temporal evolution of fête, which is consistent with Leblanc’s (Reference Leblanc2012) data and Martin’s (Reference Martin1995) remark. We are less inclined, however, to acknowledge the latter’s claim that the compact onset of fête is a key feature. With average values of 778 Hz × 1760 Hz for female speakers and 640 Hz × 1474 Hz for male speakers, the first two formants of fête are not especially close, notably when compared to, say, /o/ (456 Hz × 895 Hz for females, 389 Hz × 787 Hz for males). Neither do our results support Santerre’s (Reference Santerre1981) comment about fête and faite starting alike and then differing. In fact, the limited trajectory of faite evidenced in Figure 2 makes it very unlikely it starts as low as fête. While both vowels may well end similarly, they have radically different starting points.
In conclusion, our results show that fête has the longest duration among the vocalic categories analyzed, although it is closely followed by the other historically long oral vowels /Ø o ɑ/ in CS (Figure 1), from which it does not differ statistically. fête is clearly distinct when we observe formant trajectories over time. F1 and F2 change considerably: they start with values similar to those of /a/ at 25%, reach the /Œ/ area at 50% and faite at 75% of the duration of fête (Figure 3). The magnitude of this temporal evolution is matched in no other vowel produced by the female speakers, and in very few vowels produced by the male speakers (Figure 2).
5 Discussion and conclusion
In this article, we have sought to describe the Quebec French vowel fête. In this variety of French, a stressed long vowel is frequently diphthongized, no matter if its length is a consequence of the following coda or an intrinsic property of the vowel itself. fête, however, has retained the attention of numerous authors over the years due to its unparalleled acoustic characteristics and a particularly strong tendency to be produced with a complex nucleus. Regardless, its status has so far remained imprecise, especially regarding its dominant distinguishing feature and the way it should be transcribed. Our main goals were thus to determine whether length or quality made fête stand out and propose a phonetic transcription that would fit both recently gathered empirical data and the International Phonetic Alphabet.
To do so, we have acoustically analyzed 8866 vowels produced by 52 native speakers of Quebec French aged from 18 to 23 years. Our results first show that fête has one of the longest durations of the vocalic categories analyzed, but that it is not statistically different from the other historically long vowels considered, /ɑ o Ø/ in CS. At 50% duration, the first two formants of fête have values that locate it halfway between /a/ and faite in F1/F2 planes. Taking into account more than one formant measurement set reveals tremendous spectral changes in fête. Near the onset, it is similar to /a/, whereas it finishes near faite, indicating that the location captured at 50% duration is just an intermediate step in its long fronting and rising trajectory. The vowels /ɑ o Ø/ in CS, despite being as long as fête, do not show such extensive dynamics.
In light of these findings, what can we conclude about the dominant cue of fête? Note that Santerre (Reference Santerre1974) and Côté & Lancien (Reference Côté, Lancien, Sasha, Paola, Marija and Paul2019) put forward quality rather than length. In the first study, this view was justified by the divergent F1 and F2 trajectories of fête, while the latter pointed to length reduction in unstressed fête, a trajectory opposite to faite in the vocalic space and right identification of short fête by listeners. Contrastively, Martin (Reference Martin1995) comes to the conclusion that fête is first and foremost longer. In a way, they are all right: if we only focus on the faite–fête distinction, we might as well consider length or quality to be the most distinguishing feature. This is where looking at the whole vocalic system of Quebec French, like we have done in the current study, brings crucial depth. If fête is undoubtedly longer than faite, its duration is very similar to that of /ɑ Ø o/, while none of these intrinsically long vowels presents as substantial formant dynamics. We argue that what truly characterizes fête and makes it different from the other vowels of Quebec French is its quality, more precisely its diphthongal quality.
We will now meet our objective of proposing a transcription of fête that we consider appropriate. Let us recall that Santerre (Reference Santerre1974, Reference Santerre1981) suggests using /ɜ/. However, none of the uncovered acoustic properties of fête matches what this symbol represents in the IPA. We understand from Santerre’s papers that, originally, /ɜ/ was not meant to reflect some sort of central quality. Since it has come to be used in that way in recent years (see Côté Reference Côté, Randall, Chantal and Trudel2012), we think /ɜ/ is ambiguous and should be dismissed.
Martin (Reference Martin1995) uses /ɛː/. The duration of fête is indeed long, but it is no different from the other historically long vowels. Moreover, given that /ɛ/ is used to represent faite, /ɛː/ would mean that fête is just a longer faite. Now, what we think distinguishes fête is the trajectory of its first two formants. F1 and F2 shift considerably more over the course of fête than of the other vowels, including faite, and as suggested in Figure 3, span two vocalic qualities of Quebec French. Since we argue that this characterizes the fête vowel, it should be reflected more in its transcription than duration; therefore, we also put /ɛː/ aside. Given the evidence we have just given, we conclude that an appropriate symbol for fête would be /a͜ɛ/. This is, of course, a phonemic transcription, while the phonetic realizations may differ and be transcribed otherwise, for example [a͜I] or [a͜e].
On another matter, we wish to call the attention on the fact that the studies summarized in Section 2 cover four decades of spoken Quebec French. One of the things that seem to have evolved is the in-between category of long but monophthongal fête (group C). While such realizations make for a large part of Santerre’s (Reference Santerre1974) description, notably in his arguing that however it is produced, fête has a distinctive acoustic signature, Martin (Reference Martin1995) has to deal with this to a lesser extent. In Leblanc’s (Reference Leblanc2012) study, a very large majority (92%) of the expected fête tokens were diphthongal, but the author simply discarded the remaining 8% without describing them further. We cannot conclude, then, that they are residual instances of long but monophthongal fête; they might as well have been short and similar to faite. As for our own data, the following remark may seem anecdotal, but it is nonetheless of importance for this discussion. The first author, who analyzed the data and is a native speaker of Quebec French, is under the impression that the participants did not produce long but monophthongal fête vowels. Diphthongization was occasionally less prominent, but never to the point that an intermediate category like Martin’s (Reference Martin1995) group C was needed. Tokens were either long and diphthongal, or short and monophthongal. Some inter-speaker variation did occur for certain words (e.g. plaide ‘plead’), but it was always clear whether the vowel produced was faite- or fête-like. It is thus a possibility that faite and fête have become increasingly distinct over the years. If it were the case that such a change has happened, our results would suggest that it is led by female speakers, for whom the acoustic dissimilarity between fête and the other categories analyzed is statistically and visually significant and greater than for male speakers (Figure 2). One question that remains open is whether fête is still subject to social conditioning as it used to be in Santerre’s (Reference Santerre1971) days (see also Yaeger-Dror & Kemp Reference Yaeger-Dror and Kemp1992). Words like baleine ‘whale’ and arrête ‘stop’ tend to act as shibboleths (supposedly pronounced /ɛ/ in Quebec City and /a͜ɛ/ in Montreal; Dumas Reference Dumas1987). Yet, beyond the odd stereotypical lexical items, how conscious are listeners of diphthongal fête and how do they judge it?
Our study has limitations that must be mentioned. First, we have analyzed read speech produced by 52 speakers aged from 18 to 23 years. It does not reflect all possible Quebec French usages. Moreover, our conclusions are based on a handful of acoustic parameters. Given the complexity of speech, however, it could have been described in a much richer way, including in terms of spectral changes. Also, we have used a limited lexical set that was identical across speakers, but not otherwise controlled. Additionally, our participants were from various cities across the province and were equally considered ‘Quebec French speakers’, but we do not discount the possibility of regional variation being present in the data. Beyond baleine and arrête, Dolbec & Ouellon (Reference Dolbec and Ouellon1999) point out that speakers from Western Quebec (Montreal and the surrounding area) reputedly diphthongize vowels more strongly than speakers from Eastern Quebec (Quebec City and the surrounding area), which might be worth further investigation in future studies about diphthongal fête.Footnote 2
Finally, we are conscious that we have offered a very incomplete picture of diphthongization of Quebec French vowels by choosing to exclude lengthening codas. Future work will address this issue in due form, more specifically through a comparison of the acoustic profile of /a͜ɛ/ and short vowels in lengthening codas. Given our findings with regard to /ɑ o Ø/, it is not unlikely /a͜ɛ/ will remain acoustically distinct. After decades of researchers highlighting the specific nature of /a͜ɛ/, we felt it was high time this vowel had its own representative symbol, and in that regard, we have achieved the goals we set for ourselves in this study.
Acknowledgements
We wish to thank the editor and three anonymous reviewers for their useful suggestions. This work has been funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC).
Appendix A. Target lexical items
Appendix B. Summaries of the linear mixed effects models
VowSyl = vowel, type of syllable
Sig(nificance) codes: 0 ‘***’, .001 ‘**’, .01 ‘*’, .05 ‘.’, .1 ‘ ’, 1
log(duration) ~ VowSyl + (1 | Speaker) + (1 | Task:Word)
VowSyl = vowel, type of syllable; M = male
Sig(nificance) codes: 0 ‘***’, .001 ‘**’, .01 ‘*’, .05 ‘.’, .1 ‘ ’, 1
F1_dyn ~ VowSyl * Sex + (1 | Speaker) + (1 | Task:Word)
VowSyl = vowel, type of syllable; M = male
Sig(nificance) codes: 0 ‘***’, .001 ‘**’, .01 ‘*’, .05 ‘.’, .1 ‘ ’ 1
F2_dyn ~ VowSyl * Sex + (1 | Speaker) + (1 | Task:Word)