Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T10:55:06.747Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of adolescent social relationships in promoting alcohol resistance: Interrupting the intergenerational transmission of alcohol misuse

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 August 2022

Mallory Stephenson*
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Fazil Aliev
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Sally I-Chun Kuo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, USA
Alexis C. Edwards
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Gayathri Pandey
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Downstate Medical Center, State University of New York, Brooklyn, NY, USA
Jinni Su
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
Chella Kamarajan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Downstate Medical Center, State University of New York, Brooklyn, NY, USA
Danielle Dick
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Jessica E. Salvatore
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, USA
*
Corresponding author: Mallory Stephenson, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Genetic factors contribute to the intergenerational transmission of alcohol misuse, but not all individuals at high genetic risk develop problems. The present study examined adolescent relationships with parents, peers, and romantic partners as predictors of realized resistance, defined as high biological risk for disorder combined with a healthy outcome, to alcohol initiation, heavy episodic drinking, and alcohol use disorder (AUD). Data were from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (N = 1,858; 49.9% female; mean age at baseline = 13.91 years). Genetic risk, indexed using family history density and polygenic risk scores for alcohol problems and AUD, was used to define alcohol resistance. Adolescent predictors included parent-child relationship quality, parental monitoring, peer drinking, romantic partner drinking, and social competence. There was little support for the hypothesis that social relationship factors would promote alcohol resistance, with the exception that higher father-child relationship quality was associated with higher resistance to alcohol initiation ( $$\hat \beta $$ = −0.19, 95% CI = −0.35, −0.03). Unexpectedly, social competence was associated with lower resistance to heavy episodic drinking ( $$\hat \beta $$ = 0.10, 95% CI = 0.01, 0.20). This pattern of largely null effects underscores how little is known about resistance processes among those at high genetic risk for AUD.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Family history and genetic loading for alcohol use disorder (AUD) are well-established risk factors for the development of alcohol use and problems among adolescents and young adults (Chassin et al., Reference Chassin, Flora and King2004; Verhulst et al., Reference Verhulst, Neale and Kendler2015). Parental AUD is associated with early initiation of alcohol use, elevated risk for binge drinking in adolescence, and more rapid progression from alcohol use initiation to AUD diagnosis (Chassin et al., Reference Chassin, Pitts and Prost2002, Reference Chassin, Flora and King2004; Hussong & Bauer, Reference Hussong and Bauer2008). Twin and adoption studies have explored the heritability of alcohol problems, demonstrating that approximately 50% of the variation in AUD is attributable to genetic influences (Verhulst et al., Reference Verhulst, Neale and Kendler2015). Further, molecular genetic studies suggest that AUD is highly polygenic, driven by hundreds or thousands of genetic variants of very small individual effect (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Daly and O’Donovan2012).

Nonetheless, many individuals with high familial or polygenic loading do not develop alcohol problems and may be considered “resistant” to AUD. The substance use literature has traditionally adopted a risk perspective, identifying environmental factors that increase risk for alcohol use and misuse. As a result, the mechanisms through which individuals at high familial or genetic risk may develop resistance to AUD remain poorly understood. Vanyukov et al. (Reference Vanyukov, Tarter, Conway, Kirillova, Chandler and Daley2016) identify several targets to further understand alcohol resistance: (1) outset resistance, defined as low biological and environmental risk from birth; (2) realized resistance, described as high biological risk or childhood behavioral risk for a disorder combined with a healthy adult outcome; (3) resistance to continued drug use after initiation; (4) resistance to addiction; (5) ability to recover after becoming addicted; and (6) resistance to relapse after recovery.

Previous studies have focused on addiction resistance (Hoffmeister et al., Reference Hoffmeister, Cohoon, Sorocco, Acheson and Lovallo2019; Kendler & Myers, Reference Kendler and Myers2015), the fourth facet of the alcohol resistance phenotype. Individuals with high levels of addiction resistance report fewer substance use disorder (SUD) symptoms than would be expected based on their maximal drug consumption. Conversely, individuals with low levels of addiction resistance report more SUD symptoms than expected. In one study, Kendler and Myers (Reference Kendler and Myers2015) found that parental history of alcohol or drug use disorders, low parental warmth, and co-morbid internalizing and externalizing disorders predicted lower levels of addiction resistance. Similarly, Hoffmeister et al. (Reference Hoffmeister, Cohoon, Sorocco, Acheson and Lovallo2019) identified behavioral disinhibition, poor emotional stability, and early life adversity as negative predictors of addiction resistance (Hoffmeister et al., Reference Hoffmeister, Cohoon, Sorocco, Acheson and Lovallo2019).

In contrast with addiction resistance, the remaining facets of alcohol resistance are relatively unexplored. Given that approximately 7.5 million children in the United States live with at least one parent affected by AUD (Lipari & Van Horn, Reference Lipari and Van Horn2017), efforts to characterize the development of realized resistance, defined as lower levels of substance use or fewer SUD symptoms than would be expected based on one’s biological or childhood behavioral risk for disorder (Vanyukov et al., Reference Vanyukov, Tarter, Conway, Kirillova, Chandler and Daley2016), have the opportunity to benefit a substantial proportion of the population and provide insight into potential targets for the prevention of alcohol use and problems. In the present study, we adopt an organizational-developmental perspective to understand realized resistance. Specifically, we evaluate the contributions of adolescent social relationships with parents, peers, and romantic partners to delayed onset of alcohol use, later initiation or absence of heavy episodic drinking, and a lack of AUD diagnosis among individuals with high genetic risk (Vanyukov et al., Reference Vanyukov, Tarter, Conway, Kirillova, Chandler and Daley2016).

The organizational-developmental model

An organizational-developmental perspective posits that development is defined by changes in the organization of behavior over time. Within each developmental period, behavior must shift to accommodate salient developmental issues (Sroufe et al., Reference Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson and Collins2005), which are guided by sociocultural and historically embedded expectations for successful adaptation (Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Masten, Coatsworth and Tellegen2004). During adolescence in a Western context, the primary developmental task is individuation, or cultivating a sense of being a unique person. This maturing awareness of identity is facilitated by successfully balancing autonomy with connectedness, developing peer network competence, and coordinating school, work, and social activities (Sroufe et al., Reference Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson and Collins2005).

Early adolescence signifies a critical shift in the parent-child relationship, as parents must monitor and remain close to their child while also promoting their expanding autonomy. Adolescents increasingly develop social competence, drawing on experiences with their parents to approach relationships with a complex network of same- and other-gender friends and romantic partners (Sroufe et al., Reference Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson and Collins2005). Therefore, the organizational-developmental model posits that features of the parent-child relationship facilitate the development of social competence and inform adolescents’ relationships with their peers and romantic partners, which in turn contribute to healthy (or unhealthy) adaptation and functioning. If we consider alcohol resistance to be a healthy developmental outcome, the organizational-developmental model implicates two non-mutually exclusive pathways to alcohol resistance in adolescence. First, features of the parent-child relationship, including relationship quality and parental monitoring, may influence whether adolescents select into higher versus lower drinking peer groups and romantic partnerships, affecting alcohol resistance. Second, positive parent-child relationship characteristics may bolster adolescent social competence and enhance alcohol resistance. We review the potential contributions of parenting, alcohol use among peers and romantic partners, and social competence in turn. In view of the limited work on alcohol resistance, we focus on prior studies of the associations between adolescent social relationship factors and alcohol-related outcomes more broadly.

Parenting and adolescent drinking behavior

Parental monitoring and parent-child relationship quality have well-established protective effects on alcohol use onset, levels of alcohol use, and initiation of binge drinking (Handren et al., Reference Handren, Donaldson and Crano2016; Rusby et al., Reference Rusby, Light, Crowley and Westling2018; Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Jorm and Lubman2010; Van Ryzin et al., Reference Van Ryzin, Fosco and Dishion2012). For example, Yap et al. (Reference Yap, Cheong, Zaravinos-Tsakos, Lubman and Jorm2017) explored the joint and separate effects of parenting by mothers and fathers on adolescent alcohol use in a systematic review and meta-analysis. When they examined 12 dimensions of parenting relevant to offspring drinking patterns, parental monitoring emerged as the strongest protective factor, accounting for 3% of the variance in alcohol use initiation and 5% of the variance in subsequent use and misuse. Maternal support was related to delayed alcohol use initiation, and both maternal and paternal support predicted lower alcohol use and misuse. Parental involvement, mother-child relationship quality, and father-child relationship quality similarly predicted later alcohol use onset and lower levels of consumption (Yap et al., Reference Yap, Cheong, Zaravinos-Tsakos, Lubman and Jorm2017).

In addition to effects on alcohol use initiation, prior studies suggest that parental monitoring and parent-child relationship quality reduce risk for heavy episodic drinking and alcohol problems. Parental monitoring is negatively associated with alcohol misuse, binge drinking, and the development of SUDs across adolescence and young adulthood (Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Reifman, Farrell and Dintcheff2000; Bountress et al., Reference Bountress, Chassin and Lemery-Chalfant2017; Donaldson et al., Reference Donaldson, Handren and Crano2016), and these protective effects are particularly pronounced for those at high genetic risk (Bountress et al., Reference Bountress, Chassin and Lemery-Chalfant2017; Dick, Viken, et al., Reference Dick, Viken, Purcell, Kaprio, Pulkkinen and Rose2007; Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Aliev, Edwards, Evans, Macleod, Hickman, Lewis, Kendler, Loukola, Korhonen, Latvala, Rose, Kaprio and Dick2014). Similarly, maternal positive parenting is negatively associated with heavy episodic drinking in adolescence (Su et al., Reference Su, Kuo, Aliev, Guy, Derlan, Edenberg, Nurnberger, Kramer, Bucholz, Salvatore and Dick2018), and poor parent-child relationships are related to increased risk for heavy episodic drinking, as well as AUD onset and persistence (Berg et al., Reference Berg, Kiviruusu, Bean, Huurre, Lintonen and Hammarström2018; Foster et al., Reference Foster, Hicks, Iacono and McGue2014).

Alcohol use by peers and romantic partners, social competence, and drinking behavior

Affiliations with drinking peers may contribute to lower levels of alcohol resistance. Perceived peer substance use is associated with increased risk for alcohol use initiation (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer, Kuperman, Nurnberger, Salvatore, Schuckit, Bierut, Foroud, Chan, Hesselbrock, Meyers, Edenberg and Porjesz2017), and affiliating with a greater number of friends who drink predicts more rapid increases in alcohol consumption across adolescence (Simons-Morton & Chen, Reference Simons-Morton and Chen2005). Substance use by close friends is also related to higher levels of heavy episodic drinking in adolescence, a greater increase in heavy episodic drinking from mid-adolescence to young adulthood (Li et al., Reference Li, Cho, Salvatore, Edenberg, Agrawal, Chorlian, Porjesz, Hesselbrock and Dick2017), and higher likelihood of meeting criteria for substance abuse or dependence prior to age 21 (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Hill, Epstein, Lee, Hawkins and Catalano2016). Moreover, studies of gene-environment interaction suggest that genetic influences on alcohol use increase with increases in friends’ alcohol use, suggesting that peer drinking may be particularly detrimental to the development of realized resistance (Dick, Pagan, et al., Reference Dick, Pagan, Viken, Purcell, Kaprio, Pulkkinen and Rose2007; Harden et al., Reference Harden, Hill, Turkheimer and Emery2008).

Fewer studies have evaluated connections between adolescent romantic relationships and drinking behavior. Yet, romantic relationships are an emerging developmental task in adolescence (Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Masten, Coatsworth and Tellegen2004) and may have particular significance for the development of alcohol resistance. Romantic relationships share many features with friendships in adolescence (Furman & Shaffer, Reference Furman and Shaffer2003; Gudonis et al., Reference Gudonis, Lewis, Tong, Tu and Aalsma2012). However, adolescent romantic relationships rarely emerge from friendships or proximal peer groups, instead facilitating access to a new peer environment (Kreager et al., Reference Kreager, Molloy, Moody and Feinberg2016). Further, adolescent romantic relationships are characterized by greater intensity, increased expressions of affection (Collins et al., Reference Collins, Welsh and Furman2009), higher support, and greater conflict when compared to peer relationships (Furman & Shomaker, Reference Furman and Shomaker2008). Due to the emotional intensity and frequency of contact, adolescent romantic relationships may exert behavioral influence in spite of their brevity. Consistent with this idea, drinking by romantic partners has been prospectively associated with adolescent alcohol use, particularly among individuals with initially low levels of drinking (Gudonis et al., Reference Gudonis, Lewis, Tong, Tu and Aalsma2012). The effects of romantic partners’ drinking may be especially relevant for female adolescents in heterosexual relationships, as relationships with older boyfriends provide increased access to alcohol (Bremner et al., Reference Bremner, Burnett, Nunney, Ravat and Mistral2011; Brown et al., Reference Brown, McGue, Maggs, Schulenberg, Hingson, Swartzwelder, Martin, Chung, Tapert, Sher, Winters, Lowman and Murphy2009).

Finally, adolescent social competence, defined by the quality of one’s social interactions and relationships, group status, and social self-efficacy (Rose-Krasnor, Reference Rose-Krasnor1997), may contribute to the development of resistance to alcohol use onset, initiation of binge drinking, and AUD. The organizational-developmental model (Sroufe et al., Reference Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson and Collins2005) proposes that successful adaptation in adolescence is evidenced by competently navigating a complex network of same- and other-gender friends, as well as romantic partners. Assuming that alcohol resistance can be considered a healthy developmental outcome, the organizational-developmental perspective underscores the potential importance of adolescent social competence in the development of alcohol resistance among individuals with high genetic risk.

Evaluating pathways to alcohol resistance as a function of race/ethnicity and sex

Though prior research highlights parental monitoring, parent-child relationship quality, drinking by peers and romantic partners, and social competence as potential predictors of alcohol resistance, it is plausible that pathways to alcohol resistance may differ according to adolescents’ racial/ethnic background and sex. Prior research has shown differences in the patterns and correlates of drinking behavior by race/ethnicity (Bersamin et al., Reference Bersamin, Paschall and Flewelling2005; Chartier et al., Reference Chartier, Hesselbrock and Hesselbrock2009) and sex/gender (Dir et al., Reference Dir, Bell, Adams and Hulvershorn2017; Foster et al., Reference Foster, Hicks, Iacono and McGue2015; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Edwards and Gardner2015; Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Cho and Dick2017). For example, Black adolescents tend to initiate alcohol use later than White adolescents, are less likely to continue to progress to regular use, and exhibit lower rates of heavy episodic drinking (Malone et al., Reference Malone, Northrup, Masyn, Lamis and Lamont2012; Patrick & Schulenberg, Reference Patrick and Schulenberg2010). Further, affiliating with friends who get drunk is a stronger predictor of heavy episodic alcohol use among White adolescents when compared to Black adolescents (Patrick & Schulenberg, Reference Patrick and Schulenberg2010). Though some studies suggest that the association between parental monitoring and substance use is stronger for White adolescents when compared to Black adolescents (Blustein et al., Reference Blustein, Munn-Chernoff, Grant, Sartor, Waldron, Bucholz, Madden and Heath2015; Bohnert et al., Reference Bohnert, Ríos-Bedoya and Breslau2009), a study conducted by Su et al. (Reference Su, Kuo, Aliev, Guy, Derlan, Edenberg, Nurnberger, Kramer, Bucholz, Salvatore and Dick2018) found that the effects of parenting behavior on risky drinking were similar among European American and African American individuals. Thus, despite well-established differences in patterns of drinking behavior between White and Black adolescents, potential differences in the pathways to alcohol resistance remain unclear.

Differences in adolescent drinking patterns have also been observed between males and females. Though male and female adolescents report similar rates of alcohol use initiation (Swendsen et al., Reference Swendsen, Burstein, Case, Conway, Dierker, He and Merikangas2012), males exhibit greater risk for heavy drinking, binge drinking, and alcohol dependence (Chassin et al., Reference Chassin, Pitts and Prost2002; Stone et al., Reference Stone, Becker, Huber and Catalano2012). Further, there is some evidence that family relationships and parenting behaviors are more influential for females when compared to males (Beyers & Seiffge-Krenke, Reference Beyers and Seiffge-Krenke2007), though many studies have not found evidence for disparate effects of parenting according to child sex. For example, Rusby et al. (Reference Rusby, Light, Crowley and Westling2018) found that the effects of parent-child relationship quality and parental monitoring on binge drinking onset were similar for males and females.

The current study

In the present study, we examined the contributions of adolescent social relationships with parents, peers, and romantic partners to realized alcohol resistance. Realized alcohol resistance was operationalized as delayed alcohol use initiation, later initiation or absence of heavy episodic drinking, and a lack of AUD diagnosis among individuals with high familial or polygenic loading for AUD. Our hypotheses were as follows:

  1. 1. Adolescents who report higher quality relationships with their mother and father, greater parental monitoring, and higher levels of social competence will exhibit higher levels of alcohol resistance.

  2. 2. Conversely, individuals who report affiliations with drinking peers and romantic partners will demonstrate lower levels of resistance.

  3. 3. Consistent with the organizational-developmental model, mother-child relationship quality, father-child relationship quality, and parental monitoring will be positively associated with adolescent social competence, which in turn will predict higher alcohol resistance. In addition, mother-child relationship quality, father-child relationship quality, and parental monitoring will be negatively associated with peer and romantic partner drinking. Fewer affiliations with drinking peers and romantic partners will then predict higher levels of alcohol resistance.

Finally, we conducted exploratory analyses to test whether predictors of alcohol resistance differ as a function of adolescent race/ethnicity (operationalized as ancestry) and sex. The hypotheses and analytic plan for this study were preregistered using the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/rkbc5).

Method

Participants

Participants were 1,858 adolescents and young adults (49.9% female) from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA) Prospective Sample (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer, Kuperman, Nurnberger, Salvatore, Schuckit, Bierut, Foroud, Chan, Hesselbrock, Meyers, Edenberg and Porjesz2017), a large-scale longitudinal study of approximately 2,255 families densely affected by alcoholism (Begleiter et al., Reference Begleiter, Reich, Hesselbrock, Porjesz, Li, Schuckit, Edenberg and Rice1995). Data collection began in 2004 and concluded in 2019, with participants assessed biennially. The current analyses were limited to individuals who completed the adolescent version of the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism for Children (C-SSAGA-A-IV) during their baseline assessment (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, Cadoret, Cloninger, Dinwiddie, Hesselbrock, Nurnberger, Reich, Schmidt and Schuckit1994; Hesselbrock et al., Reference Hesselbrock, Easton, Bucholz, Schuckit and Hesselbrock1999). The self-reported race/ethnicity of participants was 28.5% Black/African American, 49.7% White/Caucasian, and 21.8% Native American, Asian, Pacific Islander, or Other. Participants were an average age of 13.91 years (SD = 1.78 years, range = 11–17 years) at baseline and were re-assessed every two years. Approximately 62% of participants completed at least one follow-up assessment, and participants completed a maximum of four assessments (i.e., one baseline assessment and three follow-up assessments). Parenting measures were drawn from the baseline assessment; measures of peer drinking, romantic partner drinking, and social competence were from the first follow-up assessment; and all available assessments were used to derive the alcohol resistance measures.

Measures

Parent-child bonding

Father- and mother-child bonding were assessed using the 12-item care scale of the Parental Bonding Instrument (Parker, Reference Parker1989; Parker et al., Reference Parker, Tupling and Brown1979) (∝ = 0.89 for father-child bonding; ∝ = 0.89 for mother-child bonding). Items referred to parents’ behavior in the past six months (e.g., “spoke to me in a warm and friendly voice”). Response options were “usually or always,” “often/a lot,” “not very often,” and “rarely or never.” Responses were summed across items, such that higher scores indicate greater bonding.

Parent-child closeness

Two items from the C-SSAGA-A-IV were used to measure father- and mother-child closeness: “how well do you get along with your [father/mother] figure most of the time?” (1 = poor, 2 = fair, 3 = good, 4 = excellent), and “how close do you feel to your [father/mother] figure?” (1 = not at all close, 2 = somewhat close, 3 = very close). Responses were standardized and averaged across the two items (Su et al., Reference Su, Kuo, Aliev, Guy, Derlan, Edenberg, Nurnberger, Kramer, Bucholz, Salvatore and Dick2018). The values of Cronbach’s alpha were ∝ = 0.72 for father-child closeness and ∝ = 0.69 for mother-child closeness.

Parent-child communication

Father- and mother-child communication were assessed using three items from the C-SSAGA-A-IV, which asked adolescents whether they discussed the news, their friends and activities, and their problems with their parents (∝ = 0.59 for father-child communication; ∝ = 0.56 for mother-child communication). Responses were coded as 0 (“no”) and 1 (“yes”) before calculating a sum score (Su et al., Reference Su, Kuo, Aliev, Guy, Derlan, Edenberg, Nurnberger, Kramer, Bucholz, Salvatore and Dick2018).

Parental involvement

Paternal and maternal involvement were assessed using six items from the C-SSAGA-A-IV, which evaluated fathers’ and mothers’ involvement with schoolwork or projects, chores, fun activities, shopping, and making plans (∝ = 0.64 for paternal involvement; ∝ = 0.64 for maternal involvement). Response options were coded as 0 (“no”) and 1 (“yes”) before calculating a sum score (Su et al., Reference Su, Kuo, Aliev, Guy, Derlan, Edenberg, Nurnberger, Kramer, Bucholz, Salvatore and Dick2018).

Parental monitoring

Three items from the C-SSAGA-A-IV measured parental monitoring (∝ = 0.73): “my parent figures know about my plans,” “my parent figures have a pretty good idea of my interests, activities, and whereabouts,” and “my parent figures know where I am and who I am with when I am not at home” (1 = always, 2 = usually, 3 = sometimes, 4 = rarely). Responses were reverse coded and summed across items (Chassin et al., Reference Chassin, Pillow, Curran, Molina and Barrera1993).

Peer drinking

The C-SSAGA-A-IV measured peer drinking with one item: “how many of your best friends use alcohol?” Response options were “none of them” (1), “a few of them” (2), “most of them” (3), and “all of them” (4) (Kaprio et al., Reference Kaprio, Pulkkinen and Rose2002).

Romantic partner drinking

Drinking by romantic partners was assessed with one item from the C-SSAGA-A-IV: “have you had any boyfriends or girlfriends who used alcohol?” Partner drinking was a dichotomous variable, with responses coded as “no” (0) or “yes” (1).

Social competence

Social competence was measured using the six-item social competence scale from the Achenbach Youth Self-Report, which assesses adolescents’ number of group activities, number of friends, frequency of social interactions, and quality of social interactions (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001; Ivanova et al., Reference Ivanova, Achenbach, Rescorla, Dumenci, Almqvist and Bilenberg2007; Song et al., Reference Song, Sing and Singer1994).

Polygenic risk scores (PRS)

Three genome-wide genotyping arrays were used in COGA Phase IV: data for individuals of European ancestry (EA) were genotyped on the Illumina Human OmniExpress 12V1 array (Illumina, San Diego, CA); data for individuals of African ancestry (AA) were genotyped on the Illumina 2.5M array (Illumina, San Diego, CA); and all remaining samples were genotyped on the Smokescreen genotyping array (BioRealm LLC, Walnut, CA). Samples were imputed to 1000 Genomes using SHAPEIT2 (Delaneau et al., Reference Delaneau, Howie, Cox, Zagury and Marchini2013) and Minimac3 (Das et al., Reference Das, Forer, Schönherr, Sidore, Locke, Kwong, Vrieze, Chew, Levy, McGue, Schlessinger, Stambolian, Loh, Iacono, Swaroop, Scott, Cucca, Kronenberg, Boehnke and Fuchsberger2016) within each array. Variants with non-A/T or C/G alleles, missing rates less than 5%, minor allele frequencies greater than 3%, and Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium p-values more than 0.0001 were used for imputation. Imputed single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) with information scores less than 0.30, individual genotype probability scores less than 0.90, a missing rate greater than 25%, or a minor allele frequency less than 1% were excluded, as were SNPs that did not pass Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (p < 1 × 10−6). In total, 6,832,792 SNPs passed quality control thresholds and were available for analysis (Lai et al., Reference Lai, Wetherill, Bertelsen, Carey, Kamarajan, Kapoor, Meyers, Anokhin, Bennett, Bucholz, Chang, Jager, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer, Kuperman, Nurnberger, Raj, Schuckit and Foroud2019).

PRS were constructed using PRS-CSx (Ruan et al., Reference Ruan, Lin, Feng, Chen, Lam, Guo, He, Sawa, Martin, Qin, Huang and Ge2022). This procedure uses ancestry-specific discovery sample genome-wide association study (GWAS) weights, paired with linkage disequilibrium information from an ancestry-matched external reference panel, to estimate the posterior effect size for each SNP. By integrating GWAS summary statistics from multiple populations, PRS-CSx improves polygenic prediction in non-European samples. Reference panels from the 1000 Genomes Phase III European or African subsamples were used for the EA and AA groups, respectively. For participants of European ancestry, the discovery sample consisted of a meta-analysis of summary statistics from the following GWAS performed in individuals of European ancestry: DSM-IV alcohol dependence from the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (COGA sample removed; N = 45,622) (Walters et al., Reference Walters, Polimanti, Johnson, McClintick, Adams, Adkins, Aliev, Bacanu, Batzler, Bertelsen, Biernacka, Bigdeli, Chen, Clarke, Chou, Degenhardt, Docherty, Edwards, Fontanillas and Agarwal2018), AUDIT-P from the UKBiobank (N = 141,932) (Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Fontanillas, Elson, Gray, de Wit, Davis, MacKillop and Palmer2019), and DSM-5 Alcohol Use Disorder from the Million Veteran Program (N = 202,004) (Kranzler et al., Reference Kranzler, Zhou, Kember, Smith, Justice, Damrauer, Tsao, Klarin, Baras, Reid, Overton, Rader, Cheng, Tate, Becker, Concato, Xu, Polimanti, Zhao and Gelernter2019). For participants of African ancestry, GWAS summary statistics were drawn from the meta-analyzed European ancestry summary statistics described above, in tandem with summary statistics from the following GWAS performed in individuals of African ancestry: DSM-IV alcohol dependence from the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (COGA sample removed; N = 6,280) (Walters et al., Reference Walters, Polimanti, Johnson, McClintick, Adams, Adkins, Aliev, Bacanu, Batzler, Bertelsen, Biernacka, Bigdeli, Chen, Clarke, Chou, Degenhardt, Docherty, Edwards, Fontanillas and Agarwal2018) and DSM-5 Alcohol Use Disorder from the Million Veteran Program (N = 56,648) (Kranzler et al., Reference Kranzler, Zhou, Kember, Smith, Justice, Damrauer, Tsao, Klarin, Baras, Reid, Overton, Rader, Cheng, Tate, Becker, Concato, Xu, Polimanti, Zhao and Gelernter2019).

Family history density for AUD

Family history density for AUD was calculated based on log-transformed DSM-5 AUD maximum symptom counts for all first- and second-degree non-descendants, including fathers, mothers, full siblings, grandparents, parental siblings, and half siblings. Maximum AUD symptom counts were weighted according to degree of relatedness (Pandey et al., Reference Pandey, Seay, Meyers, Chorlian, Pandey, Kamarajan, Ehrenberg, Pitti, Kinreich, Subbie-Saenz de Viteri, Acion, Anokhin, Bauer, Chan, Edenberg, Hesselbrock, Kuperman, McCutcheon, Bucholz and Porjesz2020).

Age of alcohol use initiation

Age of alcohol use onset was measured within the C-SSAGA-A-IV: “how old were you the first time you had your very first whole drink?”

Heavy episodic drinking onset

Initiation of heavy episodic drinking was assessed using one item from the C-SSAGA-A-IV and from the adult version of the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA-IV): “did you ever have five or more drinks in 24 hours?” Age of heavy episodic drinking onset was coded as the participant’s age at the first assessment in which they report heavy episodic drinking.

AUD diagnosis

AUD diagnoses were evaluated using the C-SSAGA-A-IV and Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism. Individuals aged 15 years or older who met diagnostic criteria for DSM-IV alcohol dependence were coded as affected (0). Drinkers who were assessed through age 23 and did not meet diagnostic criteria at any evaluation were coded as 1, and individuals who did not drink or who were not assessed through age 23 and did not meet criteria for AUD at any evaluation were coded as missing. By removing individuals who were not assessed through age 23, this coding scheme ensures that individuals who have not passed the peak period of vulnerability for AUD are not improperly identified as unaffected (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Foroud, Hinrichs, Le, Bertelsen, Budde, Harari, Koller, Wetherill, Agrawal, Almasy, Brooks, Bucholz, Dick, Hesselbrock, Johnson, Kang, Kapoor, Kramer and Goate2013).

Covariates

Age at the baseline assessment, sex (0 = male, 1 = female), 10 ancestral principal components, presence of data for maternal AUD, and presence of data for paternal AUD were included as covariates in Cox proportional hazard models and in models involving resistance to AUD. The latter two covariates were included to correct for our earlier observation that missing parental information is often associated with increased risk for externalizing behaviors and substance use disorders in this sample (Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Aliev, Bucholz, Agrawal, Hesselbrock, Hesselbrock, Bauer, Kuperman, Schuckit, Kramer, Edenberg, Foroud and Dick2015).

Statistical analysis

Preliminary analyses

To construct measures of father-child and mother-child relationship quality, separate one-factor confirmatory factor analyses were conducted using the “cfa” function in the R {lavaan} package (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). Parent-child bonding, closeness, communication, and involvement were included as indicators. A Comparative Fit Index (CFI) > 0.90 and Standardized Root Mean Squared Residual (SRMR) < 0.08 were considered as criteria for acceptable model fit (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). The “lavPredict” function was then used to extract factor scores for mother-child and father-child relationship quality.

Operationalizing realized alcohol resistance

To calculate the alcohol resistance phenotype, two mixed effects Cox proportional hazard models were conducted using the “coxph” function in the R {survival} package (Therneau, Reference Therneau2015). Mixed effects models were used to account for non-independence of data from the same family. Family history density and polygenic risk for AUD were included as predictors, and age at the baseline assessment, sex, 10 ancestral principal components, presence of maternal data for AUD, and presence of paternal data for AUD were included as covariates. The first Cox proportional hazard model evaluated resistance to alcohol use onset; only individuals who had not initiated alcohol use at the baseline assessment (N = 1,141) were included. The second Cox proportional hazard model assessed resistance to heavy episodic drinking, and analyses were limited to individuals who did not report heavy episodic drinking at baseline (N = 1,350). Individuals who had initiated alcohol use or heavy episodic drinking at baseline were excluded to ensure that predictor variables measured at the baseline assessment temporally preceded the alcohol resistance outcome. Hazard ratios (HRs) were considered statistically significant if the 95% confidence interval (CI) did not include 1.

The resistance phenotypes were derived separately within each ancestral group to avoid population stratification. To assess ancestry, principal components (PCs) were calculated using Eigenstrat (Price et al., Reference Price, Patterson, Plenge, Weinblatt, Shadick and Reich2006) and 1000 Genomes. Each individual was assigned an ancestry classification (EA, AA, or Other) based on the first two PCs. Final participant classification as EA versus AA was then derived from family-based analyses: families were assigned an ancestry according to the majority of individual-based ancestry classifications (Lai et al., Reference Lai, Wetherill, Bertelsen, Carey, Kamarajan, Kapoor, Meyers, Anokhin, Bennett, Bucholz, Chang, Jager, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer, Kuperman, Nurnberger, Raj, Schuckit and Foroud2019).

Deviance residuals, which represent the difference between actual age of onset and expected age of onset based on family history density for AUD and PRS, were then extracted from each model. Positive values for the deviance residual indicate that the participant’s age of onset occurred sooner than expected based on their level of genetic risk, denoting lower levels of realized alcohol resistance. Conversely, negative values indicate that the participant’s age of onset occurred later than expected, denoting higher levels of resistance.

Univariable associations

First, univariable models were constructed to investigate the influence of adolescent relationships with parents, peers, and romantic partners on resistance to alcohol use onset, initiation of heavy episodic drinking, and AUD diagnosis. Features of the parent-child relationship were measured at the baseline assessment, and measures of adolescent relationships with peers and romantic partners were drawn from the first follow-up assessment. Each predictor was included in a separate model to avoid issues with multicollinearity or suppression effects. For initiation of alcohol use and heavy episodic drinking, resistance was operationalized as the deviance residual. Linear mixed models were tested using the R {plm} package (Croissant & Millo, Reference Croissant and Millo2008) to account for non-independence of data from individuals within the same family.

For AUD, age at baseline, sex, ancestry, presence of data for maternal AUD, and presence of data for paternal AUD were included as covariates. Resistance was operationalized as the absence of an AUD diagnosis. Resistance to AUD was operationalized differently from resistance to alcohol use initiation and heavy episodic drinking for two reasons. First, any individual who did not meet criteria for AUD by the end of the study (∼90% of the sample) would be right-censored in a Cox proportional hazard model. The deviance residual, by default, is negative for right-censored observations, which likely would have led to a skewed distribution of deviance residuals with restricted variance. Second, though later initiation of alcohol use may be considered a “healthy” developmental outcome, AUD is associated with clinically significant problems and functional impairment, regardless of the age at diagnosis. Because the definition of realized resistance presupposes high biological risk (Vanyukov et al., Reference Vanyukov, Tarter, Conway, Kirillova, Chandler and Daley2016), only participants who had a non-zero value on the measure of family history density or were in the top quartile for PRS were included in analyses for resistance to AUD (N = 1,725). Probit regression models were conducted using the R {pglm} package (Croissant, Reference Croissant2017), which allows for estimation of panel models with a dichotomous outcome and accounts for non-independence of data from individuals within the same family.

Structural equation modeling

Next, consistent with the proposition that familial relationships form the basis for relationships with peers and romantic partners (Sroufe et al., Reference Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson and Collins2005), structural equation models were constructed to evaluate the associations between father-child relationship quality, mother-child relationship quality, and parental monitoring at the baseline assessment and peer drinking, romantic partner drinking, and social competence at the first follow-up assessment. In addition, direct associations of father-child relationship quality, mother-child relationship quality, parental monitoring, peer drinking, romantic partner drinking, and social competence with alcohol resistance were tested (Figure 1). Separate models were constructed for each alcohol resistance outcome using the R {lavaan} package (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). Full information maximum likelihood was used to account for missing data.

Figure 1. Structural equation model examining the influences of mother-child relationship quality, father-child relationship quality, parental monitoring, peer drinking, partner drinking, and social competence on alcohol resistance. The direct effects of parenting variables on alcohol resistance are shown in red. Variances are not displayed.

Potential differences in the development of alcohol resistance by ancestry and sex

To test for differences in the pathways to alcohol resistance by participant ancestry, separate models were constructed for individuals of European and African ancestry. Parameter estimates were then constrained to equality across groups, and change in model fit was assessed using the likelihood ratio test. We note that ancestral group and self-reported race/ethnicity are not perfectly correlated but generally correspond to one another (> 96% concordance in the COGA sample). The same procedure was used to test for potential differences in the pathways to alcohol resistance by participant sex.

In view of our preregistered hypotheses, we used p < .05 (two-tailed) as the criterion for statistical significance.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Descriptive statistics for each of the key study variables are presented in Table 1. One-factor models of parent-child relationship quality were constructed with bonding, closeness, communication, and involvement included as indicators. Confirmatory factor analyses showed acceptable model fit for father-child (χ2(2) = 42.71, p < .001, CFI = 0.98, SRMR = 0.02) and mother-child (χ2(2) = 45.87, CFI = 0.98, SRMR = 0.03) relationship quality.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for key study variables

Notes. Subsamples for analyses of resistance to alcohol initiation, heavy episodic drinking, and AUD differed in size because participants who endorsed the outcome of interest (initiation of alcohol use, initiation of heavy episodic drinking, and AUD diagnosis, respectively) at the baseline assessment were excluded. Analyses of resistance to AUD were further limited to participants who had a non-zero family history density score or were in the top quartile for PRS. Abbreviations. AA = African ancestry; HED = heavy episodic drinking; AUD = alcohol use disorder; PRS = polygenic risk score; F-C = father-child; M-C = mother-child.

Next, mixed effects Cox proportional hazard models were conducted to evaluate associations of family history density and polygenic risk for AUD with age of onset for alcohol use and heavy episodic drinking. Age at the baseline assessment, sex, 10 ancestral principal components, presence of maternal data for AUD, and presence of paternal data for AUD were included as covariates, and analyses were conducted separately by ancestry. In these models, family history density and polygenic risk for AUD were not significantly associated with age of alcohol use initiation among EA (family history: HR = 1.12, 95% CI = 0.24, 5.17; polygenic risk: HR = 0.99, 95% CI = 0.46, 2.13) or AA (family history: HR = 1.30, 95% CI = 0.30, 5.61; polygenic risk: HR = 0.92, 95% CI = 0.46, 1.81) individuals. Family history density was associated with earlier onset of heavy episodic drinking among EA individuals (HR = 4.64, 95% CI = 1.10, 19.67) but not AA individuals (HR = 2.35, 95% CI = 0.52, 10.63). Therefore, among EA individuals, a one-unit increase in the family history density score increased the hazard of transition to heavy episodic drinking by a factor of 4.64, on average. Polygenic risk for AUD was not related to age of onset for heavy episodic drinking in either ancestral group (EA individuals: HR = 0.71, 95% CI = 0.33, 1.55; AA individuals: HR = 0.88, 95% CI = 0.39, 2.00). Deviance residuals, which represent the difference between actual age of onset and expected age of onset based on family history density and PRS, were extracted from each model as a measure of realized resistance to alcohol initiation and onset of heavy episodic drinking.

Univariable associations between adolescent social relationships and alcohol resistance

A series of univariable models were conducted to investigate associations of adolescent relationships with parents, peers, and romantic partners with realized alcohol resistance. Parameter estimates and 95% CIs are presented in Table 2. It should be noted that positive values for the deviance residual indicate that the participant’s age of onset occurred sooner than expected based on their level of genetic risk. Therefore, positive beta estimates indicate that the predictor of interest is related to lower levels of realized alcohol resistance, and negative beta estimates indicate that the predictor of interest is related to higher levels of resistance. Resistance to AUD, however, was not operationalized using a deviance residual, and parameter estimates may be interpreted in the usual way (i.e., a positive beta estimate indicates that the predictor is related to higher levels of resistance to AUD).

Table 2. Univariable associations between predictor variables and resistance to alcohol initiation, heavy episodic drinking, and alcohol use disorder

Notes. Statistically significant associations are shown in bold font. Abbreviations. F-C = father-child; M-C = mother-child; HED = heavy episodic drinking; AUD = alcohol use disorder.

As shown in Table 2, greater father-child relationship quality promoted resistance to alcohol initiation. Conversely, peer and romantic partner drinking were associated with lower levels of resistance to alcohol initiation, and higher levels of peer drinking were also associated with lower resistance to heavy episodic drinking. The remaining associations were not statistically significant, and the observed effect sizes were small. Only the magnitude of the association between romantic partner drinking and resistance to AUD exceeded 0.20; specifically, for individuals who reported that their romantic partner had used alcohol, their z-score for resistance to AUD decreased by 0.31 compared to individuals whose romantic partners had never used alcohol. However, this relationship was not statistically significant.

Structural equation modeling

Three models were specified to, firstly, evaluate the degree to which family relationships form the basis for relationships with peers and romantic partners and, secondly, to test effects of father- and mother-child relationship quality, parental monitoring, peer drinking, romantic partner drinking, and social competence on resistance to alcohol initiation, onset of heavy episodic drinking, and AUD. Standardized factor loadings, path coefficients, and correlation coefficients for each model are reported in Table 3.

Table 3. Parameter estimates for models of realized alcohol resistance

Notes. Statistically significant associations are shown in bold font. Abbreviations. F-C = father-child; M-C = mother-child; HED = heavy episodic drinking; AUD = alcohol use disorder.

Associations of parenting variables with features of peer and romantic relationships

Models of resistance to alcohol initiation, onset of heavy episodic drinking, and AUD included different analytic subsamples: analyses of resistance to alcohol initiation included 1,141 individuals who had not initiated alcohol use at the baseline assessment, analyses of resistance to heavy episodic drinking included 1,350 individuals who did not report heavy episodic drinking at baseline, and analyses of resistance to AUD included participants who had a non-zero value on the measure of family history density or were in the top quartile for PRS (N = 1,725). As a result, each model yielded similar, but slightly varying patterns of associations between features of the parent-child relationship and features of peer and romantic relationships in adolescence.

Associations with peer drinking

Across all three models, father-child relationship quality was not significantly associated with peer drinking. Effects of father-child relationship quality on peer drinking were consistently negative and small in magnitude (| $$\hat \beta $$ | = 0.03–0.07). Greater mother-child relationship quality was associated with lower levels of peer drinking in models of resistance to alcohol initiation and heavy episodic drinking, though this association was non-significant and smaller in magnitude within the model of resistance to AUD. Similarly, higher levels of parental monitoring were associated with lower levels of peer drinking in models of resistance to heavy episodic drinking and AUD, but this association was non-significant and smaller in magnitude within the model of resistance to alcohol initiation.

Associations with romantic partner drinking

Across models, father-child relationship quality was not significantly associated with partner drinking. Effects of father-child relationship quality on partner drinking were consistently negative and small in magnitude (| $$\hat \beta $$ | = 0.10–0.14). Adolescents who reported higher quality relationships with their mothers were less likely to indicate that their romantic partner consumed alcohol across all three models tested (| $$\hat \beta $$ | = 0.04–0.16), though the relationship was statistically significant in the model of resistance to AUD only. Parental monitoring was not significantly related to romantic partner drinking across models, though the direction of effect was consistently negative (| $$\hat \beta $$ | = 0.04–0.07).

Associations with social competence

Father-child relationship quality was not significantly related to social competence and showed varying directions of effect across models. By contrast, higher mother-child relationship quality promoted greater social competence in models of resistance to heavy episodic drinking and AUD, though the association was non-significant and smaller in magnitude within the model of resistance to alcohol initiation. Moreover, higher levels of parental monitoring were related to greater social competence in models of resistance to alcohol initiation and heavy episodic drinking, but the association was non-significant and smaller in magnitude within the model of resistance to AUD.

Resistance to alcohol initiation

Greater father-child relationship quality promoted higher resistance to alcohol initiation. Mother-child relationship quality was not significantly associated with resistance to alcohol initiation, though the effect size was non-trivial (| $$\hat \beta $$ | = 0.16). Effects of parental monitoring, peer drinking, partner drinking, and social competence on resistance were not statistically significant and were extremely small in magnitude (| $$\hat \beta $$ | = 0.00–0.08).

Resistance to heavy episodic drinking

Greater social competence in adolescence was related to lower levels of resistance to heavy episodic drinking. Effects of father-child relationship quality, mother-child relationship quality, parental monitoring, peer drinking, and romantic partner drinking were non-significant and small in magnitude (| $$\hat \beta $$ | = 0.02–0.07).

Resistance to AUD

No statistically significant predictors of resistance to AUD were identified, and effects were small in magnitude ( $\left| \hat{\beta }\right|$ = 0.00–0.09).

Potential differences in the development of alcohol resistance by ancestry and sex

To test for differences in the pathways to alcohol resistance by participant ancestry, separate models were constructed for participants of European and African ancestries. When comparing a freely estimated model to a model with all paths constrained to equality, there was not a significant change in model fit for models of resistance to alcohol initiation (χ 2(15) = 10.91, p = .759) and heavy episodic drinking (χ 2(15) = 15.21, p = .436). However, there was a statistically significant decrement in model fit when regression coefficients were constrained to be equal within the model of resistance to AUD (χ 2(15) = 26.21, p = .036). Parameter estimates for the freely estimated model are shown in Table 4. To provide further insight into differences in the development of resistance to AUD based on ancestry, each regression path was individually constrained to equality. There was a significant decrease in model fit when the association between mother-child relationship quality and social competence was constrained to be equal across EA and AA groups, χ 2(1) = 5.46, p = .019. Likelihood ratio tests were not statistically significant for the remaining paths, χ 2(1) = 0.00–3.45, ps = .063–.950.

Table 4. Parameter estimates for ancestry-stratified model of resistance to alcohol use disorder

Notes. Statistically significant associations are shown in bold font. Abbreviations. F-C = father-child; M-C = mother-child.

Next, separate models were specified for males and females, and regression coefficients were constrained to be equal across groups. There was not a significant change in model fit across models of resistance to alcohol initiation (χ 2(15) = 7.23, p = .951), heavy episodic drinking (χ 2(15) = 13.30, p = .579), and AUD (χ 2(15) = 7.85, p = .930), suggesting that pathways to alcohol resistance were similar for male and female participants.

Discussion

In this study, we used an organizational-developmental perspective (Sroufe et al., Reference Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson and Collins2005) to examine the contributions of adolescent social relationships with parents, peers, and romantic partners to realized alcohol resistance across a series of key drinking milestones – alcohol initiation, the onset of heavy episodic drinking, and diagnosis with alcohol dependence. We focus on four key takeaways from the results of this preregistered study.

First, using conventional thresholds for statistical significance (p < .05), we found mixed support for the hypothesis that features of the parent-child relationship would be associated with features of subsequent peer and romantic partner relationships. Father-child relationship quality was not significantly associated with peer drinking, romantic partner drinking, and social competence. Higher mother-child relationship quality was prospectively associated with lower levels of peer drinking in the models of resistance to alcohol initiation and heavy episodic drinking, lower likelihood of romantic partner drinking in the model of resistance to AUD, and higher levels of social competence in the models of resistance to heavy episodic drinking and AUD. Finally, parental monitoring was related to lower levels of peer drinking in the models of resistance to heavy episodic drinking and AUD, and higher levels of social competence in the models of resistance to alcohol initiation and heavy episodic drinking.

It is worth noting that, in instances where a predictor was statistically significant in one of the resistance models but not others (e.g., mother-child relationship quality was associated with lower peer drinking in models of resistance to alcohol initiation and heavy episodic drinking, but not in the model of resistance to AUD), the non-significant effects were in the same direction but less precisely known (as indicated by wider confidence intervals). Thus, taken as a whole, the pattern of results observed here is somewhat consistent with prior theory and evidence regarding the familial antecedents of social competence (Laible & Carlo, Reference Laible and Carlo2004) and affiliations with antisocial peers (Dishion et al., Reference Dishion, Patterson, Stoolmiller and Skinner1991). Our results also underscore the need to measure and examine the separate influences of mothers’ and fathers’ parenting, as mother-child relationship quality appeared to have more consistent associations with features of adolescent peer and romantic relationships compared to father-child relationship quality.

Second, we found little evidence to support the hypothesis that features of adolescent social relationships with parents, peers, and romantic partners would promote resistance to alcohol initiation, heavy episodic drinking, and the development of disorder. There were several exceptions: higher father-child relationship quality was associated with higher resistance to alcohol initiation, peer and romantic partner drinking were associated with lower resistance to alcohol initiation (univariable model only), more peer drinking was associated with lower resistance to heavy episodic drinking (univariable model only), and adolescent social competence was associated with lower resistance to heavy episodic drinking. As noted by others (Bell et al., Reference Bell, Herring and Annand2020), research on the impact of parenting on adolescent substance use outcomes has historically focused on mothers or used non-specific language when referring to parents. In an illustration of this, only two father-specific effects for parent-child relationship quality and alcohol initiation were identified as part of a recent meta-analysis of the relationship between parenting factors and adolescent alcohol misuse (Yap et al., Reference Yap, Cheong, Zaravinos-Tsakos, Lubman and Jorm2017). Thus, our finding that father-child relationship quality influences resistance to alcohol initiation adds to an emerging area of research on the developmental significance of fathers for adolescent alcohol outcomes and merits replication in other samples.

Although the association between adolescent social competence and lower resistance to heavy episodic drinking was unexpected, we speculate that this effect may reflect the benefits and liabilities associated with social competence in this period of the life span. On the one hand, the ability to establish high quality relationships with friends and successfully navigate the larger peer group is a hallmark of adolescent social functioning (Brown, Reference Brown2004). Yet, this level of social integration can also render one vulnerable to risky behaviors, such as alcohol use, that often take place in group contexts. Indeed, given that adolescent drinking is associated with higher quality relationships with friends and largely takes places in social contexts (Hoel et al., Reference Hoel, Eriksen, Breidablik and Meland2004; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Power and Francis1992), some developmentalists have noted that adolescent alcohol use can be considered a marker of social integration and adaptation (Jessor, Reference Jessor1991; Vaillant, Reference Vaillant1995).

Third, we identified very few differences in pathways to alcohol resistance based on participant sex and ancestry. For models of resistance to alcohol initiation and heavy episodic drinking, regression coefficients could be constrained to equality without a substantial change in model fit. For resistance to AUD, only the association between mother-child relationship quality and social competence was significantly different among European and African ancestry individuals. Specifically, higher mother-child relationship quality promoted greater social competence among individuals of European, but not African, ancestry.

Finally, given the relative novelty of the resistance framework, it is worth commenting more broadly on the opportunities and challenges of this approach as it relates to alcohol outcomes. Vanyukov et al. (Reference Vanyukov, Tarter, Conway, Kirillova, Chandler and Daley2016) have argued that resistance factors are not merely risk factors with the opposite sign. Just as trauma exposure may elevate risk for alcohol misuse, but a lack of trauma exposure is not necessarily protective, resistance factors likely encompass a number of environmental variables whose presence enhances resistance but whose absence may be considered neutral. As an illustrative example, consider the Icelandic Prevention Model for adolescent substance use prevention. One of the most highly utilized components of the prevention program was the “parental prowl,” in which parents joined together to walk around their neighborhood and monitor youth during Friday and Saturday evenings. Though the lack of a community monitoring approach is not necessarily risky, the “parental prowl” enhanced neighborhood social capital and contributed to a substantial decline in adolescent substance use following the implementation of the Icelandic Prevention Model (Sigfúsdóttir et al., Reference Sigfúsdóttir, Thorlindsson, Kristjánsson, Roe and Allegrante2009). Such findings also bolster Vanyukov and colleagues’ (2016) argument that resistance factors may have stronger translational impact than risk factors, given their potentially broader application and direct relationship with healthy outcomes. Yet, the largely null effects observed across the candidate resistance factors examined here underscores the need for further theoretical articulation of the factors that contribute to realized resistance. Answering these questions is critical for understanding the factors that can promote healthy outcomes among those at the greatest genetic risk for AUD.

Limitations

Our results should be interpreted within the context of the following limitations. First, COGA is a high-risk sample with most participants from extended families enriched for AUDs. Findings may not be generalizable to other populations or samples ascertained with different risk profiles. Second, individuals who drank earlier than their first assessment were coded as missing from the analyses of resistance to alcohol initiation to avoid a situation where the parenting, peer, and romantic partner predictor variables were measured after the age at initiation. As a consequence, very early drinkers were removed from the analysis, and our results are not informative about resistance to alcohol initiation in especially young age groups. Third, although the identification of potential resistance factors for the present study was grounded in an organizational-developmental perspective, it is also acknowledged they were selected from a study that (like many) was originally conceived within a risk framework (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer, Kuperman, Nurnberger, Salvatore, Schuckit, Bierut, Foroud, Chan, Hesselbrock, Meyers, Edenberg and Porjesz2017). Fourth, we recognize that our measure of parental monitoring reflects not only parental behavior, but also adolescents’ disclosure of their activities and whereabouts (Stattin & Kerr, Reference Stattin and Kerr2000).

Fifth, we relied on genome-wide polygenic scores and family history measures to calculate the measures of realized resistance. Although this approach was chosen to align with Vanyukov et al.’s (Reference Vanyukov, Tarter, Conway, Kirillova, Chandler and Daley2016) model, we recognize that polygenic scores and family history measures represent imprecise measures of genetic risk, and also capture some environmental risk (Selzam et al., Reference Selzam, Ritchie, Pingault, Reynolds, O’Reilly and Plomin2019). Further, polygenic scores were not statistically significantly associated with age at initiation of alcohol use and heavy episodic drinking in the present study, which limits the utility of deviance residuals and may contribute to the pattern of largely null effects. As GWAS discovery sample sizes continue to increase, the predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores (Dudbridge, Reference Dudbridge2013) and utility of deviance residuals should correspondingly improve, enabling future studies of realized resistance. Sixth, polygenic scores for participants of African ancestry were based on summary statistics from both European ancestry and African ancestry discovery samples (with appropriate adjustments to the European ancestry summary statistics). This method was selected to improve polygenic prediction in individuals of African ancestry, as existing GWAS of alcohol-related outcomes in African ancestry individuals have very small sample sizes relative to GWAS in European ancestry individuals. Continued efforts to improve the representation of diverse populations in genetic research may facilitate the use of ancestrally matched discovery samples and further strengthen polygenic prediction in African ancestry individuals.

Seventh, genetic influences on features of the environment, including parenting, were not addressed in the present work. It is certainly plausible that familial or polygenic loading for AUD is correlated with parent-child relationship quality and parental monitoring (see Kendler & Baker, Reference Kendler and Baker2007 for a review of genetic influences on the environment). However, within the context of the current study, we anticipate that the presence of gene-environment correlation would attenuate, rather than strengthen, the magnitude of associations. In the presence of gene-environment correlation, parenting behaviors should contribute to greater correspondence between the adolescent’s genetic risk and alcohol-related outcome, rather than facilitating resistance to genetic or familial risk for AUD. Eighth, we focused on realized resistance in this study. Whether the candidate resistance factors examined here are associated with other dimensions of resistance, such as resistance to relapse following recovery, remains to be seen. Finally, although many of our hypothesized effects were not statistically significant, we caution that these null effects do not imply that relationships with parents, peers, and romantic partners do not contribute to alcohol resistance.

Conclusions

Historically, efforts to understand the etiology of health and disorder have focused on identifying risk and protective factors. A resistance framework (Vanyukov et al., Reference Vanyukov, Tarter, Conway, Kirillova, Chandler and Daley2016) represents a shift in that paradigm to instead ask what promotes a healthy outcome among individuals with a high liability to disorder. In the present study, results were largely inconsistent with our hypotheses that parenting, peer, and romantic partner factors would contribute to resistance to key drinking milestones, with the exception that father-child relationship quality promoted resistance to alcohol initiation. This pattern of null effects illustrates how little is known about resistance processes among those at high genetic risk for AUD. Identifying variables that increase risk, then seeking to reduce or eliminate those risk factors, has provided only partial insight into how individuals remain healthy despite high biological risk for disorder. To further our understanding of healthy developmental outcomes, searching for and bolstering resistance-enhancing factors is an important next step.

Acknowledgements

The Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA), Principal Investigators B. Porjesz, V. Hesselbrock, T. Foroud; Scientific Director, A. Agrawal; Translational Director, D. Dick, includes eleven different centers: University of Connecticut (V. Hesselbrock); Indiana University (H.J. Edenberg, T. Foroud, Y. Liu, M. Plawecki); University of Iowa Carver College of Medicine (S. Kuperman, J. Kramer); SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University (B. Porjesz, J. Meyers, C. Kamarajan, A. Pandey); Washington University in St. Louis (L. Bierut, J. Rice, K. Bucholz, A. Agrawal); University of California at San Diego (M. Schuckit); Rutgers University (J. Tischfield, R. Hart, J. Salvatore); The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania (L. Almasy); Virginia Commonwealth University (D. Dick); Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (A. Goate, P. Slesinger); and Howard University (D. Scott). Other COGA collaborators include: L. Bauer (University of Connecticut); J. Nurnberger Jr., L. Wetherill, X., Xuei, D. Lai, S. O’Connor, (Indiana University); G. Chan (University of Iowa; University of Connecticut); D.B. Chorlian, J. Zhang, P. Barr, S. Kinreich, G. Pandey (SUNY Downstate); N. Mullins (Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai); A. Anokhin, S. Hartz, E. Johnson, V. McCutcheon, S. Saccone (Washington University); J. Moore, Z. Pang, S. Kuo (Rutgers University); A. Merikangas (The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia and University of Pennsylvania); F. Aliev (Virginia Commonwealth University); H. Chin and A. Parsian are the NIAAA Staff Collaborators. We continue to be inspired by our memories of Henri Begleiter and Theodore Reich, founding PI and Co-PI of COGA, and also owe a debt of gratitude to other past organizers of COGA, including Ting- Kai Li, P. Michael Conneally, Raymond Crowe, and Wendy Reich, for their critical contributions. This national collaborative study is supported by NIH Grant U10AA008401 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA).

Funding statement

This project was supported by grant U10AA008401 from the National Institutes of Health.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Footnotes

Mallory Stephenson and Fazil Aliev contributed equally to this work and joint first authors.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Barnes, G. M., Reifman, A. S., Farrell, M. P., & Dintcheff, B. A. (2000). The effects of parenting on the development of adolescent alcohol misuse: A six-wave latent growth model. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62(1), 175186. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00175.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begleiter, H., Reich, T., Hesselbrock, V., Porjesz, B., Li, T. K., Schuckit, M. A., Edenberg, H. J., & Rice, J. P. (1995). The collaborative study on the genetics of alcoholism. Alcohol Health & Research World, 19(3), 228236.Google Scholar
Bell, L., Herring, R., & Annand, F. (2020). Fathers and substance misuse: A literature review. Drugs and Alcohol Today, 20(4), 353369. https://doi.org/10.1108/DAT-06-2020-0037 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berg, N., Kiviruusu, O., Bean, C. G., Huurre, T., Lintonen, T., & Hammarström, A. (2018). Social relationships in adolescence and heavy episodic drinking from youth to midlife in Finland and Sweden—Examining the role of individual, contextual and temporal factors. BMC Public Health, 18(1), 1000. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-018-5885-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bersamin, M., Paschall, M. J., & Flewelling, R. L. (2005). Ethnic differences in relationships between risk factors and adolescent binge drinking: A national study. Prevention Science, 6(2), 127136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-005-3411-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beyers, W., & Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2007). Are friends and romantic partners the “best medicine?” How the quality of other close relations mediates the impact of changing family relationships on adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31(6), 559568. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025407080583 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blustein, E. C., Munn-Chernoff, M. A., Grant, J. D., Sartor, C. E., Waldron, M., Bucholz, K. K., Madden, P. A. F., & Heath, A. C. (2015). The association of low parental monitoring with early substance use in European American and African American adolescent girls. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 76(6), 852861. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2015.76.852 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohnert, K. M., Ríos-Bedoya, C. F., & Breslau, N. (2009). Parental monitoring at age 11 and smoking initiation up to age 17 among Blacks and Whites: A prospective investigation. Nicotine & Tobacco Research: Official Journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco, 11(12), 14741478. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntp160 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bountress, K., Chassin, L., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2017). Parent and peer influences on emerging adult substance use disorder: A genetically informed study. Development and Psychopathology, 29(1), 121142. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941500125X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, P., Burnett, J., Nunney, R., Ravat, M., & Mistral, W. (2011). Young people, alcohol, and influences: A study of young people and their relationship with alcohol. Joseph Rowntree Foundation.Google Scholar
Brown, B. B. (2004). Adolescents’ relationships with peers. In Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 363394). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, S. A., McGue, M., Maggs, J., Schulenberg, J., Hingson, R., Swartzwelder, S., Martin, C., Chung, T., Tapert, S. F., Sher, K., Winters, K. C., Lowman, C., Murphy, S. (2009). Underage alcohol use. Alcohol Research & Health, 32(1), 4152.Google ScholarPubMed
Bucholz, K. K., Cadoret, R., Cloninger, C. R., Dinwiddie, S. H., Hesselbrock, V. M., Nurnberger, J. I., Reich, T., Schmidt, I., & Schuckit, M. A. (1994). A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: A report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55(2), 149158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucholz, K. K., McCutcheon, V. V., Agrawal, A., Dick, D. M., Hesselbrock, V. M., Kramer, J. R., Kuperman, S., Nurnberger, J. I., Salvatore, J. E., Schuckit, M. A., Bierut, L. J., Foroud, T. M., Chan, G., Hesselbrock, M., Meyers, J. L., Edenberg, H. J., & Porjesz, B. (2017). Comparison of parent, peer, psychiatric, and cannabis use influences across stages of offspring alcohol involvement: Evidence from the COGA Prospective Study. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 41(2), 359368. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13293 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chartier, K. G., Hesselbrock, M. N., & Hesselbrock, V. M. (2009). Ethnicity and adolescent pathways to alcohol use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 70(3), 337345. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2009.70.337 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chassin, L., Flora, D. B., & King, K. M. (2004). Trajectories of alcohol and drug use and dependence from adolescence to adulthood: The effects of familial alcoholism and personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(4), 483498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chassin, L., Pillow, D. R., Curran, P. J., Molina, B. S., & Barrera, M. (1993). Relation of parental alcoholism to early adolescent substance use: A test of three mediating mechanisms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(1), 319. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.102.1.3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chassin, L., Pitts, S. C., & Prost, J. (2002). Binge drinking trajectories from adolescence to emerging adulthood in a high-risk sample: Predictors and substance abuse outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(1), 67. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.67 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, W. A., Welsh, D. P., & Furman, W. (2009). Adolescent romantic relationships. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 631652. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163459 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croissant, Y. (2017, November 2). pglm: Panel generalized linear models. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=pglm Google Scholar
Croissant, Y., & Millo, G. (2008). Panel data econometrics in R: The plm package. Journal of Statistical Software, 27(1), 143. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v027.i02 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Das, S., Forer, L., Schönherr, S., Sidore, C., Locke, A. E., Kwong, A., Vrieze, S. I., Chew, E. Y., Levy, S., McGue, M., Schlessinger, D., Stambolian, D., Loh, P.-R., Iacono, W. G., Swaroop, A., Scott, L. J., Cucca, F., Kronenberg, F., Boehnke, M., …Fuchsberger, C. (2016). Next-generation genotype imputation service and methods. Nature Genetics, 48(10), 12841287. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3656 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delaneau, O., Howie, B., Cox, A. J., Zagury, J.-F., & Marchini, J. (2013). Haplotype estimation using sequencing reads. American Journal of Human Genetics, 93(4), 687696. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2013.09.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., Pagan, J. L., Viken, R., Purcell, S., Kaprio, J., Pulkkinen, L., & Rose, R. J. (2007). Changing environmental influences on substance use across development. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10(2), 315326. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.10.2.315 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., Viken, R., Purcell, S., Kaprio, J., Pulkkinen, L., & Rose, R. J. (2007). Parental monitoring moderates the importance of genetic and environmental influences on adolescent smoking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 213218. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.213 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dir, A. L., Bell, R. L., Adams, Z. W., & Hulvershorn, L. A. (2017). Gender differences in risk factors for adolescent binge drinking and implications for intervention and prevention. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 289. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00289 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dishion, T. J., Patterson, G. R., Stoolmiller, M., & Skinner, M. L. (1991). Family, school, and behavioral antecedents to early adolescent involvement with antisocial peers. Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 172180. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.1.172 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donaldson, C. D., Handren, L. M., & Crano, W. D. (2016). The enduring impact of parents’ monitoring, warmth, expectancies, and alcohol use on their children’s future binge drinking and arrests: A longitudinal analysis. Prevention Science : The Official Journal of the Society for Prevention Research, 17(5), 606614. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-016-0656-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dudbridge, F. (2013). Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genetics, 9(3), e1003348. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1003348 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foster, K. T., Hicks, B. M., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2014). Alcohol use disorder in women: Risks and consequences of an adolescent onset and persistent course. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors : Journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, 28(2), 322335. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035488 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foster, K. T., Hicks, B. M., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2015). Gender differences in the structure of risk for alcohol use disorder in adolescence and young adulthood. Psychological Medicine, 45(14), 30473058. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715001014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furman, W., & Shaffer, L. (2003). The role of romantic relationships in adolescent development. In Adolescent romantic relations and sexual behavior: Theory, research, and practical implications (pp. 322). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Furman, W., & Shomaker, L. B. (2008). Patterns of interaction in adolescent romantic relationships: Distinct features and links to other close relationships. Journal of Adolescence, 31(6), 771788. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.10.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gudonis, L. C., Lewis, L., Tong, Y., Tu, W., & Aalsma, M. C. (2012). Adolescent romantic couples influence on substance use in young adulthood. Journal of Adolescence, 35(3), 638647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.08.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Handren, L. M., Donaldson, C. D., & Crano, W. D. (2016). Adolescent alcohol use: Protective and predictive parent, peer, and self-related factors. Prevention Science : The Official Journal of the Society for Prevention Research, 17(7), 862871. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-016-0695-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harden, K. P., Hill, J. E., Turkheimer, E., & Emery, R. E. (2008). Gene-environment correlation and interaction in peer effects on adolescent alcohol and tobacco use. Behavior Genetics, 38(4), 339347. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-008-9202-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hesselbrock, M., Easton, C., Bucholz, K. K., Schuckit, M., & Hesselbrock, V. (1999). A validity study of the SSAGA–A comparison with the SCAN. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 94(9), 13611370. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.94913618.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoel, S., Eriksen, B. M., Breidablik, H.-J., & Meland, E. (2004). Adolescent alcohol use, psychological health, and social integration. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 32(5), 361367. https://doi.org/10.1080/14034940410027894 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffmeister, J. R., Cohoon, A. J., Sorocco, K. H., Acheson, A., & Lovallo, W. R. (2019). Addiction resistance to alcohol: What about heavy drinkers who avoid alcohol problems? Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 204, 107552. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2019.107552 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, S. O., Power, T. G., & Francis, D. J. (1992). Defining patterns of drinking in adolescence: A cluster analytic approach. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 53(1), 4047. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.1992.53.40 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hussong, A., & Bauer, D. (2008). Telescoped trajectories from alcohol initiation to disorder in children of alcoholic parents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(1), 6378. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.63 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ivanova, M. Y., Achenbach, T. M., Rescorla, L. A., Dumenci, L., Almqvist, F., & Bilenberg, N. (2007). The generalizability of the Youth Self-Report syndrome structure in 23 societies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(5), 729738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessor, R. (1991). Risk behavior in adolescence: A psychosocial framework for understanding and action. Journal of Adolescent Health, 12(8), 597605. https://doi.org/10.1016/1054-139X(91)90007-K CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, T. M., Hill, K. G., Epstein, M., Lee, J. O., Hawkins, J. D., & Catalano, R. F. (2016). Understanding the interplay of individual and social-developmental factors in the progression of substance use and mental health from childhood to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 28(3), 721741. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000274 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaprio, J., Pulkkinen, L., & Rose, R. J. (2002). Genetic and environmental factors in health-related behaviors: Studies on Finnish twins and twin families. Twin Research: The Official Journal of the International Society for Twin Studies, 5(5), 366371. https://doi.org/10.1375/136905202320906101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Baker, J. H. (2007). Genetic influences on measures of the environment: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 37(5), 615626. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291706009524 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Edwards, A. C., & Gardner, C. O. (2015). Sex differences in the pathways to symptoms of alcohol use disorder: A study of opposite-sex twin pairs. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 39(6), 9981007. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.12694 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Myers, J. (2015). Addiction resistance: Definition, validation and association with mastery. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 154, 236242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.06.043 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kranzler, H. R., Zhou, H., Kember, R. L., Smith, R. V., Justice, A. C., Damrauer, S., Tsao, P. S., Klarin, D., Baras, A., Reid, J., Overton, J., Rader, D. J., Cheng, Z., Tate, J. P., Becker, W. C., Concato, J., Xu, K., Polimanti, R., Zhao, H., & Gelernter, J. (2019). Genome-wide association study of alcohol consumption and use disorder in 274,424 individuals from multiple populations. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1499. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09480-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreager, D. A., Molloy, L. E., Moody, J., & Feinberg, M. E. (2016). Friends first? The peer network origins of adolescent dating. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26(2), 257269. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12189 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, D., Wetherill, L., Bertelsen, S., Carey, C. E., Kamarajan, C., Kapoor, M., Meyers, J. L., Anokhin, A. P., Bennett, D. A., Bucholz, K. K., Chang, K. K., Jager, P. L. D., Dick, D. M., Hesselbrock, V., Kramer, J., Kuperman, S., Nurnberger, J. I., Raj, T., Schuckit, M., …Foroud, T. (2019). Genome-wide association studies of alcohol dependence, DSM-IV criterion count and individual criteria. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 18(6), e12579. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12579 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laible, D. J., & Carlo, G. (2004). The differential relations of maternal and paternal support and control to adolescent social competence, self-worth, and sympathy. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19(6), 759782. https://doi.org/10.1177/0743558403260094 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, J. J., Cho, S. B., Salvatore, J. E., Edenberg, H. J., Agrawal, A., Chorlian, D. B., Porjesz, B., Hesselbrock, V., & Dick, D. M. (2017). The impact of peer substance use and polygenic risk on trajectories of heavy episodic drinking across adolescence and emerging adulthood. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 41(1), 6575. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13282 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipari, R. N., & Van Horn, S. L. (2017). Children living with parents who have a substance use disorder. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. https://www.samhsa.gov/data/sites/default/files/report_3223/ShortReport-3223.html Google ScholarPubMed
Malone, P. S., Northrup, T. F., Masyn, K. E., Lamis, D. A., & Lamont, A. E. (2012). Initiation and persistence of alcohol use in United States Black, Hispanic, and White male and female youth. Addictive Behaviors, 37(3), 299305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.11.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pandey, G., Seay, M. J., Meyers, J. L., Chorlian, D. B., Pandey, A. K., Kamarajan, C., Ehrenberg, M., Pitti, D., Kinreich, S., Subbie-Saenz de Viteri, S., Acion, L., Anokhin, A., Bauer, L., Chan, G., Edenberg, H., Hesselbrock, V., Kuperman, S., McCutcheon, V. V., Bucholz, K. K., …Porjesz, B. (2020). Density and dichotomous family history measures of alcohol use disorder as predictors of behavioral and neural phenotypes: A comparative study across gender and race/ethnicity. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 44(3), 697710. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.14280 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, G. (1989). The Parental Bonding Instrument: Psychometric properties reviewed. Psychiatric Developments, 7(4), 317335.Google ScholarPubMed
Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1979.tb02487.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, M. E., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2010). Alcohol use and heavy episodic drinking prevalence and predictors among national samples of American eighth-and tenth-grade students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 71(1), 4145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, A. L., Patterson, N. J., Plenge, R. M., Weinblatt, M. E., Shadick, N. A., & Reich, D. (2006). Principal components analysis corrects for stratification in genome-wide association studies. Nature Genetics, 38(8), 904909. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng1847 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roisman, G. I., Masten, A. S., Coatsworth, J. D., & Tellegen, A. (2004). Salient and emerging developmental tasks in the transition to adulthood. Child Development, 75(1), 123133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose-Krasnor, L. (1997). The nature of social competence: A theoretical review. Social Development, 6(1), 111135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(1), 136. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruan, Y., Lin, Y.-F., Feng, Y.-C. A., Chen, C.-Y., Lam, M., Guo, Z., Stanley Global Asia Initiatives He, L., Sawa, A., Martin, A. R., Qin, S., Huang, H., & Ge, T. (2022). Improving polygenic prediction in ancestrally diverse populations. Nature Genetics, 54(5), 573580. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-022-01054-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rusby, J. C., Light, J. M., Crowley, R., & Westling, E. (2018). Influence of parent-youth relationship, parental monitoring, and parent substance use on adolescent substance use onset. Journal of Family Psychology, 32(3), 310320. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000350 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, S. M., Jorm, A. F., & Lubman, D. I. (2010). Parenting factors associated with reduced adolescent alcohol use: A systematic review of longitudinal studies. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 44(9), 774783. https://doi.org/10.1080/00048674.2010.501759 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvatore, J. E., Aliev, F., Bucholz, K., Agrawal, A., Hesselbrock, V., Hesselbrock, M., Bauer, L., Kuperman, S., Schuckit, M. A., Kramer, J., Edenberg, H. J., Foroud, T. M., Dick, D. M. (2015). Polygenic risk for externalizing disorders: Gene-by-development and gene-by-environment effects in adolescents and young adults. Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 3(2), 189201. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614534211 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvatore, J. E., Aliev, F., Edwards, A. C., Evans, D. M., Macleod, J., Hickman, M., Lewis, G., Kendler, K. S., Loukola, A., Korhonen, T., Latvala, A., Rose, R. J., Kaprio, J., Dick, D. M. (2014). Polygenic scores predict alcohol problems in an independent sample and show moderation by the environment. Genes, 5(2), 330346. https://doi.org/10.3390/genes5020330 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salvatore, J. E., Cho, S. B., & Dick, D. M. (2017). Genes, environments, and sex differences in alcohol research. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 78(4), 494501. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2017.78.494 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchez-Roige, S., Fontanillas, P., Elson, S. L., 23andMe Research Team, Gray, J. C., de Wit, H., Davis, L. K., MacKillop, J., & Palmer, A. A. (2019). Genome-wide association study of alcohol use disorder identification test (AUDIT) scores in 20 328 research participants of European ancestry. Addiction Biology, 24(1), 121131. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12574 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selzam, S., Ritchie, S. J., Pingault, J.-B., Reynolds, C. A., O’Reilly, P. F., & Plomin, R. (2019). Comparing within- and between-family polygenic score prediction. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 105(2), 351363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2019.06.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigfúsdóttir, I. D., Thorlindsson, T., Kristjánsson, Á.L., Roe, K. M., & Allegrante, J. P. (2009). Substance use prevention for adolescents: The Icelandic Model. Health Promotion International, 24(1), 1625. https://doi.org/10.1093/heapro/dan038 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons-Morton, B., & Chen, R. (2005). Latent growth curve analyses of parent influences on drinking progression among early adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66(1), 513. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.2005.66.5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, L. Y., Sing, J., & Singer, M. (1994). The Youth Self-Report Inventory: A study of its measurement fidelity. Psychological Assessment, 6(3), 236245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development, 71(4), 10721085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, A. L., Becker, L. G., Huber, A. M., & Catalano, R. F. (2012). Review of risk and protective factors of substance use and problem use in emerging adulthood. Addictive Behaviors, 37(7), 747775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.02.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, J., Kuo, S. I.-C., Aliev, F., Guy, M. C., Derlan, C. L., Edenberg, H. J., Nurnberger, J. I., Kramer, J. R., Bucholz, K. K., Salvatore, J. E., Dick, D. M. (2018). Influence of parental alcohol dependence symptoms and parenting on adolescent risky drinking and conduct problems: A family systems perspective. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 42(9), 17831794. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13827 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, P. F., Daly, M. J., & O’Donovan, M. (2012). Genetic architectures of psychiatric disorders: The emerging picture and its implications. Nature Reviews. Genetics, 13(8), 537551. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg3240 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swendsen, J., Burstein, M., Case, B., Conway, K. P., Dierker, L., He, J., & Merikangas, K. R. (2012). Use and abuse of alcohol and illicit drugs in US adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(4), 390398. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1503 Google ScholarPubMed
Therneau, T. (2015). A package for survival analysis in S. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=survival Google Scholar
Vaillant, G. E. (1995). The natural history of alcoholism revisited. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Ryzin, M. J., Fosco, G. M., & Dishion, T. J. (2012). Family and peer predictors of substance use from early adolescence to early adulthood: An 11-year prospective analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 37(12), 13141324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanyukov, M., Tarter, R. E., Conway, K. P., Kirillova, G. P., Chandler, R. K., & Daley, D. C. (2016). Risk and resistance perspectives in translation-oriented etiology research. Translational Behavioral Medicine, 6(1), 4454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13142-015-0355-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhulst, B., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2015). The heritability of alcohol use disorders: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Medicine, 45(5), 10611072. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714002165 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walters, R. K., Polimanti, R., Johnson, E. C., McClintick, J. N., Adams, M. J., Adkins, A. E., Aliev, F., Bacanu, S.-A., Batzler, A., Bertelsen, S., Biernacka, J. M., Bigdeli, T. B., Chen, L.-S., Clarke, T.-K., Chou, Y.-L., Degenhardt, F., Docherty, A. R., Edwards, A. C., Fontanillas, P., …Agarwal, A. (2018). Transancestral GWAS of alcohol dependence reveals common genetic underpinnings with psychiatric disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 21(12), 16561669. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0275-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, J.-C., Foroud, T., Hinrichs, A. L., Le, N. X. H., Bertelsen, S., Budde, J. P., Harari, O., Koller, D. L., Wetherill, L., Agrawal, A., Almasy, L., Brooks, A. I., Bucholz, K., Dick, D., Hesselbrock, V., Johnson, E. O., Kang, S., Kapoor, M., Kramer, J., …Goate, A. M. (2013). A genome-wide association study of alcohol-dependence symptom counts in extended pedigrees identifies C15orf53. Molecular Psychiatry, 18(11), 12181224. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2012.143 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yap, M. B. H., Cheong, T. W. K., Zaravinos-Tsakos, F., Lubman, D. I., & Jorm, A. F. (2017). Modifiable parenting factors associated with adolescent alcohol misuse: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Addiction, 112(7), 11421162. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13785 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Structural equation model examining the influences of mother-child relationship quality, father-child relationship quality, parental monitoring, peer drinking, partner drinking, and social competence on alcohol resistance. The direct effects of parenting variables on alcohol resistance are shown in red. Variances are not displayed.

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for key study variables

Figure 2

Table 2. Univariable associations between predictor variables and resistance to alcohol initiation, heavy episodic drinking, and alcohol use disorder

Figure 3

Table 3. Parameter estimates for models of realized alcohol resistance

Figure 4

Table 4. Parameter estimates for ancestry-stratified model of resistance to alcohol use disorder