Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T16:22:13.748Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Predictors of efficacy in depression prevention programmes

Meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Eva Jané-Llopis*
Affiliation:
Prevention Research Centre, Department of Clinical Psychology and Personality Nijmegen University as Personality, Nijmegen University, The Netherlands
Clemens Hosman
Affiliation:
Prevention Research Centre, Department of Clinical Psychology and Personality Nijmegen University as Personality, Nijmegen University, The Netherlands
Rachel Jenkins
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, London
Peter Anderson
Affiliation:
Department of Primary Care, University of Oxford, UK
*
Dr Eva Jané-Llopis, Department of Clinical Psychology and Personality, University of Nijmegen, PO Box 9104, 6500HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Worldwide, 340 million people are affected by depression, with high comorbid, social and economic costs.

Aims

To identify potential predictors of effect in prevention programmes.

Method

A meta-analysis was made of 69 programmes to reduce depression or depressive symptoms.

Results

The weighted mean effect size of 0.22 was effective for different age groups and different levels of risk, and in reducing risk factors and depressive or psychiatric symptoms. Programmes with larger effect sizes were multi-component, included competence techniques, had more than eight sessions, had sessions 60–90 min long, had a high quality of research design and were delivered by a health care provider in targeted programmes. Older people benefited from social support, whereas behavioural methods were detrimental.

Conclusions

An 11% improvement in depressive symptoms can be achieved through prevention programmes. Single trial evaluations should ensure high quality of the research design and detailed reporting of results and potential predictors.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2003 

Unipolar major depression is predicted to become the second leading cause of disease burden worldwide by the year 2020, accounting for 12% of disability-adjusted life-years (Reference Murray and LopezMurray & Lopez, 1996). Depression is comorbid with other psychiatric harm, including substance use disorders (Reference Rohde, Lewinsohn and SeeleyRohde et al, 1991; Reference GilvarryGilvarry, 2000), and anxiety and personality disorders (Reference Andrews, Henderson and HallAndrews et al, 2001). Personality, low competence, vulnerability to stress, and marital, occupational, financial and neighbourhood stressors are some of the risk factors for depression, and can be used to identify potential target groups for prevention (Reference Gillham, Shatte and FreresGillham et al, 2000).

Can depression be prevented?

Since the 1980s there has been increased development and implementation of universal, selective and indicated programmes (Reference Mrazek and HaggertyMrazek & Haggerty, 1994) that aim to reduce risk factors for depression, depressive symptoms and depressive disorders. Universal prevention interventions target a whole population group that has not been identified on the basis of increased risk; selective prevention targets subgroups of population whose risk of developing a mental disorder is significantly higher than average, as evidenced by biological, psychological or social risk factors; indicated prevention targets high-risk persons who are identified as having minimal symptoms foreshadowing mental disorder or biological markers indicating predisposition for mental disorder but do not meet diagnostic criteria for disorder at that time. Although there is substantial evidence that depressive symptoms can be reduced (Reference Munoz, Ying and Perez-StableMuñoz et al, 1993; Reference Gillham, Shatte and FreresGillham et al, 2000), only a few programmes have shown that depression can be prevented (Clarke et al, Reference Clarke, Hawkins and Murphy1995,Reference Clarke, Hawkins and Murphy1995, Reference Clarke, Hornbrook and Lynch2001). This meta-analysis aims to identify some of the population, programme and research design characteristics that predict the effect of primary prevention programmes targeting depression.

METHOD

Search procedures and inclusion criteria

Trials were identified through systematic literature searches in four databases (Current Contents, ERIC, Medline, PsycINFO), published meta-analyses, review articles, reference lists and through contact with members of the Society for Prevention Research. Key words for the literature searches were divided into five groups (details available from the author upon request). Two groups aimed to identify studies dealing with depression and mental health: resilience and other protective mental health factors related to depression (e.g. MASTERY, SELF-ESTEEM), and specific risk factors for depression (e.g. NEGATIVE THINKING, DYSTHYMIA). Three groups were defined to restrict the hits to the inclusion criteria: prevention and outcome measures (e.g. UNIVERSAL, SELECTIVE, DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS), evaluation (e.g. EFFICACY, CONTROL GROUP) and research design (e.g. RANDOMISED CONTROLLED TRIAL).

Studies were selected within the definitions of universal, selective and indicated prevention, excluding any pharmacological intervention, if they included the prevention of depression as a primary or secondary goal or outcome, the improvement of protective factors for depression or mental health (e.g. self-esteem) or the reduction of risk factors related to depression (e.g. negative thinking). Selection was restricted to English-language publications between the years 1985 and 2000 that could be retrieved through the library system, had either a randomly allocated control or equivalent comparison group, had pre–post measures, had objective outcome measures and had sufficient statistical information to calculate an effect size. From the selected trials, only those that had depressive symptoms or incidence of depression as an outcome measure were included for analysis.

Coding system and procedures

A coding instrument was developed to operationalise programme outcomes and hypothesised effect predictors. The coding instrument included trial descriptors, target group characteristics, programme characteristics, programme development characteristics, implementation characteristics, quality of the research design, and outcome indicators. The coding system comprised a code book with codes for each variable, a coding sheet to operationalise the information, and a coding instructions book with definitions and instructions (details available from the author upon request).

A trained coder and E.J.-L. undertook the coding process. Measures to minimise bias were taken into account, such as coders’ training (Reference Cooper and HedgesCooper & Hedges, 1994). A random sample of one in five trials was double-coded to assess interrater reliability. The kappa coefficient averaged across codes was 0.91, indicating excellent agreement beyond chance between the two coders (Reference Cooper and HedgesCooper & Hedges, 1994). Data were entered into a Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (Version 10) data file, checked and cleaned to control for data entry and coding errors.

Calculation of effect sizes and weighted effect sizes

An effect size estimate using the standardised mean difference (Reference Hedges and OlkinHedges & Olkin, 1985: p. 79, formula 3) was calculated from the published data for every outcome measure reported, corrected for pre-test measures and small sample sizes (Reference Lipsey and WilsonLipsey & Wilson, 2001). Positive effect sizes indicate improvement for the intervention group. Each effect size was weighted based on the inverse of its variance (Reference Hedges and OlkinHedges & Olkin, 1985: p. 86, formula 15). For multiple programmes within one study, weights were calculated according to Gleser & Olkin (Reference Gleser, Olkin, Cooper and Hedges1994: p. 346, formula 22.13). Weighted mean effect sizes (Reference Hedges and OlkinHedges & Olkin, 1985: p. 111, formula 6) and 95% confidence intervals were calculated (Reference Hedges and OlkinHedges & Olkin, 1985: p. 86, formula 16).

Unit of analysis and sample heterogeneity

The unit of analysis in this study is at the programme level, where a programme was defined as an intervention with a preventive goal and a measure of depressive symptoms. When the efficacy of more than one programme within a study was compared with a control condition, the different interventions were treated independently and an effect size was calculated for each (Reference Gleser, Olkin, Cooper and HedgesGleser & Olkin, 1994: p. 346, formula 22.13). Effect sizes were averaged within each programme across outcome measures and follow-up times, resulting in one effect size per programme.

The Q statistic was calculated to test for heterogeneity (Reference Hedges and OlkinHedges & Olkin, 1985). The sample was heterogeneous (Q=474.72, d.f.=69, P<0.001) and characteristics of the programmes, target groups and research methodology were examined as independent variables to account for this heterogeneity (Reference Lipsey and WilsonLipsey & Wilson, 2001).

Predictors in the study

Gender was coded as a continuous variable, as the percentage of male participants in the programmes. Initial level of risk was defined as universal, selective or indicated (Reference Mrazek and HaggertyMrazek & Haggerty, 1994). The duration of programmes was coded in months, and the length of individual programme sessions in minutes. The quality of the research design was assessed with the Cochrane nine-item dichotomous scale (items scores 1 or 0); high-quality programmes were considered to be those with a score of 8 or above (Reference Brown, Berndt and BrinalesBrown et al, 2000). Intervention methods were classified into one of five groups: behaviour (e.g. behaviour change, pleasant activities, modelling); cognition (e.g. cognitive restructuring, counselling, explanatory-style training); competence (e.g. broad skill training, social resistance skills); education (e.g. direct instruction, lectures and workshops); and social support (e.g. network building, fostering socialisation). Programme providers were divided into healthcare personnel (physical and mental health professionals) and lay personnel (peers, family members, schoolteachers).

Statistical analyses to test hypotheses

The z-scores were used as significance tests to compare the weighted mean effect sizes of the different values of the categorical independent variables. Weighted least squares regression analyses were used to identify possible relationships and interactions between predictors in explaining the variation in effect sizes between studies for continuous and categorical independent variables, recoded to dummy variables. Each unweighted programme effect size was corrected by its weight as defined above. Regression coefficients were obtained and the adjusted R 2 was used to measure the proportion of variance accounted for by an independent variable. The standard errors of the regression coefficients (B) were corrected according to Hedges & Olkin (Reference Hedges and Olkin1985: p. 174) and used in a z test. Separate regression models were built, first testing for main effects and then for interaction effects of the target group characteristics gender, age and level of risk, which had been identified in previous research as possible moderators of effect (Reference Price, Van Ryn and VinokurPrice et al, 1992,Reference Price, Van Ryn and VinokurPrice et al, 1992; Reference Gillham, Shatte and FreresGillham et al, 2000). Scatter plots and plots of residuals found no evidence for violation of regression model assumptions. All the tests for statistical significance were based on two-tailed tests.

RESULTS

Trial flow and distribution of effect sizes

The searches identified 1474 publications. Screening on the basis of titles and abstracts reduced the number of publications according to the inclusion criteria to 201. Detailed inspection of the retrieved 201 publications reduced the sample to 108 studies that reported sufficient information to calculate an effect size (excluding those with missing data) and that had either a randomly allocated control or equivalent comparison group. From the 108 studies, only 54 trials reported an outcome measure for depressive symptoms, 11 of which compared more than one type of programme, resulting in 69 programmes for analysis. Table 1 provides a description of the included programmes. The effect sizes of the 69 programmes were normally distributed. The unweighted mean effect size was 0.25 (range 71.08 to 1.8; 95% CI 0.16–0.35) and the weighted mean effect size was 0.22 (95% CI 0.14–0.30).

Table 1 Description of the 69 programmes included in the meta-analysis

Study Intervention provided Age group and age range (years) Male participants (%) Level of risk Programme duration (months) Number of sessions Length of session (min) Intervention methods Programme providers Quality of research design (score) Mean effect size
Emery et al (Reference Emery, Schein and Hauck1998) Education and stress management Elderly 47 Selective 2-3 > 8 60-90 Educational High (8) -0.05
Emery et al (Reference Emery, Schein and Hauck1998) Exercise, education and stress management Elderly 47 Selective 2-3 > 8 60-90 Behavioural High (8) 0.33
Muñoz et al (Reference Muñoz, Ying and Bernal1995) Coping with depression, primary care Adults 36 Selective 2-3 ≤ 8 > 90 Cognitive and competence HP only High (9) 0.28
Price et al (Reference Price, Van Ryn and Vinokur1992,Reference Price, Van Ryn and Vinokur1992) Job search intervention Adults Selective <2 ≤ 8 > 90 Behavioural, cognitive, competence and social support Lay only High (8) 0.20
Aronen & Kurkela (Reference Aronen and Kurkela1996) Home-based counselling Children Universal > 3 > 8 Cognitive, competence and social support HP only Low (7) 0.40
Eggert et al (Reference Eggert, Thompson and Herting1995) Personal growth class (one semester) Adolescents 42 Indicated > 3 > 8 <60 Behavioural, competence and social support HP only Low (7) 0.11
Eggert et al (Reference Eggert, Thompson and Herting1995) Personal growth class (two semesters) Adolescents 42 Indicated > 3 > 8 <60 Behavioural, competence and social support HP only Low (7) -0.10
Stolberg & Mahler (Reference Stolberg and Mahler1994) Skills and support Children 8-12 45 Selective > 3 > 8 Behavioural, educational and social support Both High (8) 0.4
Stolberg & Mahler (Reference Stolberg and Mahler1994) Support Children 8-12 45 Selective > 3 > 8 Behavioural, educational and social support Both High (8) 0.08
Stolberg & Mahler (Reference Stolberg and Mahler1994) Transfer skills, support Children 8-12 45 Selective > 3 > 8 Behavioural, educational and social support Both High (8) 0.26
Clarke et al (Reference Clarke, Hawkins and Murphy1995,Reference Clarke, Hawkins and Murphy1995) Coping with depression course Adolescents 30 Indicated <2 > 8 <60 Behavioural, cognitive, educational and social support HP only High (9) 0.18
Jaycox et al (Reference Jaycox, Reivich and Gillham1994) Penn prevention programme Children 10-13 54 Selective 2-3 > 8 60-90 Behavioural cognitive and educational HP only High (8) 0.38
Short (Reference Short1998) Stress management and alcohol awareness programme Children 10-13 47 Selective > 8 Behavioural, cognitive, competence, social support HP only Low (5) 0.22
Seligman et al (Reference Seligman, Schulman and DeRubeis1999,Reference Seligman, Schulman and DeRubeis1999) Workshop based on cognitive therapy Adults 48 Indicated 2-3 ≤ 8 > 90 Behavioural, educational and social support HP only High (9) 0.16
Cuijpers (Reference Cuijpers1998) Coping with chronic illness Adults 30-60 24 Indicated > 8 Cognitive Low (7) 1.81
Barth (Reference Barth1991) Child—parent enrichment project Adults 0 Selective > 3 > 8 Behavioural, educational and social support Lay only High (9) 0.01
Foa et al (Reference Foa, Hearst-Ikeda and Perry1995) PTSD intervention Adults 0 Indicated <2 ≤ 8 > 90 Behavioural, cognitive and competence Lay only High (8) 0.84
DiLorenzo et al (Reference DiLorenzo, Bargman and Stucky-Ropp1999) Aerobic fitness programme Adults 18-39 32 Universal 2-3 > 8 <60 Behavioural Both Low (7) 0.74
Haight (Reference Haight1992) Life review Elderly 61-99 22 Universal <2 ≤ 8 60-90 Cognitive and social support Lay only High (9) 0.32
Hains (Reference Hains1992) Anxiety management training Adolescents 15-16 100 Universal > 8 <60 Behavioural, cognitive and competence Both High (8) 0.75
Hains (Reference Hains1992) Cognitive intervention Adolescents 15-16 100 Universal > 8 <60 Behavioural, cognitive and social support Both High (8) 0.66
Zubernis et al (Reference Zubernis, Cassidy and Gillham1999) Depression prevention programme for children Children 51 Selective 2-3 > 8 60-90 Cognitive and competence HP only Low (7) 0.61
Heller et al (Reference Heller, Thompson and Trueba1991) Peer support telephone dyads: telephone contact discontinued Elderly 0 Selective 2-3 > 8 Behavioural Lay only Low (7) -0.15
Heller et al (Reference Heller, Thompson and Trueba1991) Peer support telephone dyads: dyad initiators Elderly 0 Selective 2-3 > 8 Behavioural Lay only Low (7) -0.23
Heller et al (Reference Heller, Thompson and Trueba1991) Peer support telephone dyads: dyad receivers Elderly 0 Selective 2-3 > 8 Behavioural Lay only Low (7) -0.21
Heller et al (Reference Heller, Thompson and Trueba1991) Peer support telephone dyads: dyad refusers Elderly 0 Selective 2-3 > 8 Behavioural Lay only Low (7) -0.12
Heller et al (Reference Heller, Thompson and Trueba1991) Peer support telephone dyads: telephone staff contact continues Elderly 0 Selective 2-3 > 8 Behavioural Lay only Low (7) -0.14
Barth et al (Reference Barth, Hacking and Ash1988) Child—parent enrichment project Adults 0 Selective > 3 > 8 Behavioural, educational and social support Lay only High (8) 0.08
Skitka & Frazier (Reference Skitka and Frazier1995) Support group for children of divorce Children 6-12 42 Selective 2-3 > 8 <60 Educational and social support Lay only Low (6) -0.09
Astin (Reference Astin1997) Stress reduction Adolescents 5 Universal 2-3 ≤ 8 > 90 Behavioural, competence and social support Lay only Low (5) -1.09
Gelfand et al (Reference Gelfand, Teti and Seiner1996) Home-based intervention Adults 19-45 0 Indicated > 3 > 8 Behavioural, cognitive, competence, educational and social support HP only Low (7) 0.3
Haight (Reference Haight1988) Life review Elderly 73-79 22 Universal <2 ≤ 8 60-90 Cognitive and social support Lay only High (8) 0.54
Irvine et al (Reference Irvine, Biglan and Smolkowski1999) Parenting skills intervention Children 61 Selective 2-3 > 8 > 90 Behavioural, competence and social support Lay only High (9) 0.22
Klein et al (Reference Klein, Wing and Simkin-Silverman1997) Lifestyle intervention Adults 44-50 0 Universal > 3 > 8 Behavioural and educational High (8) 0.22
Levy et al (Reference Levy, Derby and Martinkowski1993) Bereavement support group Adults 28 Selective Social support Low (6) 0.06
Ostwald et al (Reference Ostwald, Hepburn and Caron1999) Minnesota family workshop Adults 44 Selective <2 ≤ 8 > 90 Behavioural, competence, educational and social support Both High (9) 0.25
Elderen-van-Kemenade et al (Reference Elderen-van-Kemenade, Maes and Van den Broek1994) Health education Adults 33-69 82 Selective > 8 Behavioural, cognitive and educational HP only Low (7) 0.22
Dracup et al (Reference Dracup, Moser and Taylor1997) CPR training Adults 17 Selective ≤ 8 60-90 Behavioural and competence HP only High (8) -0.05
Dracup et al (Reference Dracup, Moser and Taylor1997) CPR training and education Adults 17 Selective ≤ 8 > 90 Behavioural, competence, educational and social support HP only High (8) 0.06
Dracup et al (Reference Dracup, Moser and Taylor1997) CPR training and social support Adults 17 Selective Behavioural, competence and social support HP only High (8) 0.10
Mignone & Guidotti (Reference Mignone and Guidotti1999) Support group Adults 20-69 38 Selective 2-3 ≤ 8 > 90 Social support Low (7) -0.03
Grey et al (Reference Grey, Boland and Davidson1998) Coping skills training and diabetes management Adolescents 13-20 43 Selective 2-3 ≤ 8 60-90 Behavioural, cognitive and competence HP only High (8) 0.48
McCallion et al (Reference McCallion, Toseland and Freeman1999) Family visit education plan Elderly 21 Indicated 2-3 ≤ 8 60-90 Behavioural, competence and social support HP only High (8) 0.07
Goodkin et al (Reference Goodkin, Blaney and Feaster1999) Bereavement intervention Adults 18-65 100 Indicated 2-3 > 8 60-90 Competence and social support HP only Low (7) 0.26
Worrall et al (Reference Worrall, Angel and Chaulk1999) Educational intervention Adults Universal ≤ 8 > 90 Behavioural, educational and social support HP only High (8) 0.34
Burns et al (Reference Burns, Nichols and Martindale-Adams2000) Geriatric evaluation and management Elderly 66-95 97 Indicated HP only High (8) 0.48
Bisson et al (Reference Bisson, Jenkins and Alexander1997) Psychological debriefing Adults 16-65 75 Indicated ≤ 8 <60 Behavioural, cognitive and educational HP only High (8) -0.26
Phillips (Reference Phillips2000) Education group Elderly 67-75 40 Selective 2-3 > 8 60-90 Competence, educational and social support HP only High (8) 1.44
Trzcieniecka-Green & Steptoe (Reference Trzcieniecka-Green and Steptoe1996) Stress management Adults 87 Selective 2-3 > 8 Behavioural, competence, educational and social support HP only Low (7) 0.40
Chang (Reference Chang1999) Nurse line video-assisted modelling programme Adults 0 Selective 2-3 ≤ 8 Behavioural, cognitive, competence, educational and social support HP only High (9) 0.05
Partonen et al (Reference Partonen, Leppaemaeki and Hurme1998) Exercise training Adults 22-57 22 Selective 2-3 > 8 60-90 Behavioural HP only High (8) 0.22
Partonen et al (Reference Partonen, Leppaemaeki and Hurme1998) Exercise training and bright light Adults 22-57 22 Selective 2-3 > 8 60-90 Behavioural HP only High (8) 0.73
Lorig et al (Reference Lorig, Gonzalez and Ritter1999) Arthritis self-management programme Adults 17 Indicated <2 ≤ 8 > 90 Behavioural and educational Lay only High (9) 0.33
Harris et al (Reference Harris, Bausell and Scott1998) Peer counselling and leadership training Adults 0 Selective > 3 > 8 60-90 Behavioural, cognitive, competence and educational HP only High (9) 0.31
Allart et al (Reference Allart, Hosman and Hoogduin2003) ‘Coping with depression’ course Adults 18-65 39 Indicated 2-3 > 8 > 90 Behavioural, cognitive, competence and educational HP only High (9) 0.54
Kahan et al (Reference Kahan, Kemp and Staples1985) Education, caregiver intervention Adults 16-77 Selective 2-3 ≤ 8 > 90 Behavioural, competence, educational and social support HP only Low (6) 0.57
Lovett & Gallagher (Reference Lovett and Gallagher1988) Life satisfaction Adults 17 Selective 2-3 > 8 > 90 Behavioural and cognitive HP only High (9) 0.29
Lovett & Gallagher (Reference Lovett and Gallagher1988) Problem solving Adults 17 Selective 2-3 > 8 > 90 Cognitive HP only High (9) 0.22
LaFromboise & Howard-Pitney (Reference LaFromboise and Howard-Pitney1995) Skills curriculum Adolescents 14-19 36 Selective > 3 > 8 Behavioural, competence and educational Lay only High (8) 0.44
Gross & McCaul (Reference Gross and McCaul1992) Psychoeducational, for children of substance misusers Children 11-18 47 Selective > 3 > 8 60-90 Behavioural, cognitive, competence, educational and social support HP only Low (7) 0.59
King & Kirschenbaum (Reference King and Kirschenbaum1990) Wisconsin early intervention — full service Children 61 Indicated > 3 > 8 <60 Behavioural, cognitive competence and educational Lay only High (8) 0.42
King & Kirschenbaum (Reference King and Kirschenbaum1990) Wisconsin early intervention — partial service Children 61 Indicated Behavioural and educational Lay only High (8) 0.02
Gwynn & Brantley (Reference Gwynn and Brantley1987) Divorce group intervention Children 9-11 50 Selective 2-3 ≤ 8 Behavioural, cognitive, educational and social support Low (4) -0.52
Hains & Ellmann (Reference Hains and Ellmann1994) Stress inoculation training Adolescents 76 Universal > 8 <60 Behavioural, cognitive, competence and social support HP only Low (7) 0.28
Peters & Carlson (Reference Peters and Carlson1999) Stress management training Adults 60 Selective > 3 > 8 60-90 Behavioural, cognitive, competence and educational Both High (8) 0.58
Cunningham et al (Reference Cunningham, Bremner and Boyle1995) Clinic-based parent training Children Selective 2-3 > 8 60-90 Behavioural, cognitive, competence and social support HP only High (8) 0.06
Cunningham et al (Reference Cunningham, Bremner and Boyle1995) Community-based parent training Children Selective 2-3 > 8 60-90 Behavioural, cognitive, competence and social support HP only High (8) -0.02
Sheeber & Johnson (Reference Sheeber and Johnson1994) Parent training Children 3-5 60 Selective 2-3 > 8 > 90 Behavioural, competence, educational, social support Lay only High (8) 0.7
Wolfe et al (Reference Wolfe, Edwards and Manion1988) Parent intervention Adults 16-25 0 Selective 60-90 Behavioural, cognitive competence, educational and social support Both High (9) 0.34

Participant characteristics

About a quarter of the programmes (16) targeted children, 9 targeted adolescents, almost a half (32) were aimed at adults and 12 were for older people (Table 2). There was no significant difference in effect size between the different age groups. There was also no significant difference in effect size between universal, selective and indicated programmes. Of the 63 programmes in which gender distribution was specified, weighted least squares regression analyses indicated a direct positive relationship between percentage of male participants and effect size (Table 3). There was an interaction between percentage of male participants and level of risk, so that the relationship between percentage of males in the programme and effect size was present for universal and selective programmes, but not for indicated programmes (Table 3).

Table 2 Participant characteristics

Variable Number of programmes Weighted mean effect size 95% CI
Age
     Children (0-14 years) 16 0.21 0.09 to 0.32
     Adolescents (15-18 years) 9 0.19 0.007 to 0.38
     Adults (19-65 years) 32 0.21 0.15 to 0.28
     Elderly (> 65 years) 12 0.24 0.12 to 0.37
Gender
     Male only 3 0.38 0.01 to 0.75
     Female only 13 0.08 -0.01 to 0.18
     Both 47 0.26 0.20 to 0.32
     Not reported 6 0.24 0.10 to 0.38
Programme
     Universal 10 0.30 0.18 to 0.43
     Selective 44 0.19 0.12 to 0.25
     Indicated 15 0.23 0.12 to 0.34

Table 3 Weighted least squares regression analysis for gender and its interaction with level of risk

Independent variables Model 1: percentage of male participants Model 2: percentage of male participants, level of risk
Percentage of male participants
    β′ 0.244
     B 0.0030
     s.e. 0.0010
     z-score 2.99
     P 0.002
Universal programme × percentage of male participants
    β′ 0.181
     B 0.0064
     s.e. 0.0031
     z-score 2.04*
     P 0.045
Selective programme × percentage of male participants
    β′ 0.421
     B 0.0059
     s.e. 0.0014
     z-score 4.14***
     P 0.000
Indicated programme × percentage of male participants
    β′ -0.110
     B -0.0015
     s.e. 0.0019
     z-score -0.806
     P 0.423
Adjusted R 2 0.044 0.082

Programme characteristics

Time descriptors

Programmes with more than eight sessions were significantly better than those with eight sessions or fewer. Programmes with session lengths of 60–90 min were significantly better than those with sessions lasting less than 60 min or longer than 90 min. No significant difference was found for duration of programmes or distribution of sessions (Table 4).

Table 4 Programme time descriptors

Number of programmes Weighted mean effect size 95% Cl z 1 P
Programme duration (months)
    <2 7 0.27 0.13 to 0.41
     2-3 32 0.22 0.15 to 0.30
    > 3 15 0.23 0.13 to 0.33
     Total 54
Number of sessions
     1-8 19 0.14 0.05 to 0.23
    > 8 45 0.26 0.19 to 0.32 2.05* 0.045
     Total 64
Session distribution
     More than once a week 7 0.31 0.13 to 0.48
     Once a week or less 37 0.30 0.23 to 0.37
     Total 44
Length of session (min)
    <60 10 0.14 -0.01 to 0.31 2.40* 0.016
     60-90 19 0.38 0.27 to 0.49
    > 90 16 0.24 0.15 to 0.33 2.00* 0.045
     Total 45

Programme providers

There were 102 programme providers reported in 62 programmes (Table 5). Programmes that used a combination of health care professionals and lay personnel had the largest effect sizes. Programmes provided by health care professionals (physical and mental health personnel) and those provided by both health care and lay personnel yielded significantly larger effect sizes than programmes provided by lay personnel alone. Programmes provided by health care professionals had larger effect sizes than programmes run by lay personnel only for selective (z=2.04, P=0.045) and indicated populations (z=2.37, P=0.016), but the difference was not significant for universal populations (z=1.90, P=0.057).

Table 5 Programme providers

Programme providers Number of programmes Weighted mean effect size 95% Cl z P
Health professionals only 34 0.28 0.20-0.35 2.88** 0.003
Lay personnel only 19 0.10 0.01-0.20
Both (lay and health personnel) 9 0.42 0.23-0.62 2.85** 0.004

Methods and techniques

Programmes that involved a competence enhancement component yielded the largest effect sizes, whereas programmes including behavioural methods yielded the lowest effect sizes (Table 6). When analysed by age group, the worse performance of programmes that included behavioural methods was present for all age groups, although it was significant only for the older population (with a behavioural component, the weighted effect size (WES) was –0.10; without, WES=0.95; z=7.14, P<0.001). Programmes that included competence enhancement techniques did significantly better than those that did not include them. Programmes that included social support did generally worse than those that did not, except for the older group, for which social support programmes yielded larger weighted effect sizes (with social support, WES=0.92; without, WES=–0.12; z=7.13, P<0.001). Programmes that included three or more different types of methods were significantly better than those that included only one or two.

Table 6 Comparisons between programmes including one of the prevention methods

Programme content Number of programmes1 Weighted mean effect size 95% CI z P
Behavioural techniques 55 0.17 0.12-0.23 3.46*** 0.000
No behavioural techniques 13 0.42 0.29-0.55
Cognitive techniques 30 0.26 0.17-0.34 1.28 0.193
No cognitive techniques 38 0.19 0.13-0.25
Competence techniques 34 0.29 0.22-0.36 3.15** 0.001
No competence techniques 34 0.13 0.06-0.20
Education methods 32 0.27 0.20-0.34 2.47* 0.012
No education methods 36 0.14 0.07-0.22
Social support methods 38 0.23 0.16-0.29 0.57 0.548
No social support methods 30 0.19 0.12-0.27
One or two methods 25 0.14 0.06-0.22 2.17* 0.027
Three or more methods 43 0.26 0.19-0.32

Research methodological characteristics

The programmes with a high quality of research design were significantly more effective than those of low quality (Table 7). Programmes that reported attrition rates were significantly better than those that did not. Programmes rated as having a well-defined intervention were better than those that did not.

Table 7 Quality of the research design and presence of independent quality items in the Cochrane scale

n 1 Weighted mean effect size 95% CI z 2 P 3
Quality of research design
High-quality design (65%) 45 0.26 0.20 to 0.31 2.60** 0.009
Low-quality design (35%) 24 0.11 0.01 to 0.20
Items in Cochrane scale
Defined aims
     Reported 66 0.22 0.17 to 0.27
     Not reported 3 -0.08 -0.38 to 0.21
Well-defined intervention
     Reported 54 0.24 0.19 to 0.30 2.17* 0.027
     Not reported 15 0.10 -0.007 to 0.21
Randomly allocated control
     Reported 52 0.21 0.15 to 0.26 0.40 0.689
     Not reported 17 0.24 0.12 to 0.35
Equivalent comparison group
     Reported 51 0.21 0.16 to 0.26 0.25 0.764
     Not reported 18 0.23 0.11 to 0.34
Number of subjects in trial
     Reported 69 0.21 0.16 to 0.26
Pre-intervention data
     Reported 65 0.19 0.14 to 0.24
     Not reported 4 0.45 0.28 to 0.62
Post-intervention data
     Reported 69 0.21 0.16 to 0.26
Attrition rates
     Reported 41 0.29 0.23 to 0.36 3.65*** 0.000
     Not reported 28 0.11 0.03 to 0.18
Findings for all outcomes
     Reported 69 0.21 0.16 to 0.26

Changes in risk factors and symptoms

The outcome measures of each programme were subsequently divided into an averaged measure per programme indicating changes in risk factors (n=49), changes in depressive symptoms (n=69), and changes in psychiatric symptoms other than depression, such as anxiety (n=51). Measures for each group were averaged across programmes to obtain three mean effect sizes, one per group. There was no significant difference in effect size between depressive symptoms (WES=0.24, 95% CI 0.13–0.35), risk factors (WES=0.28, 95% CI 0.15–0.41) 0.15–0.41) and other psychiatric symptoms (WES=0.18, 95% CI 0.09–0.27), all three of which had significant and independent positive outcomes. Weighted mean effect sizes were further subdivided within these three groups into universal, selective and indicated approaches (Fig. 1). Comparisons of means indicated no significant difference between the type of preventive approach and changes in depressive symptoms, risk factors and changes in other psychiatric symptoms.

Fig. 1 Weighted mean effect sizes for changes in depressive symptoms, risk and protective factors and related psychiatric symptoms for universal, selective and indicated programmes.

DISCUSSION

Limitations of the study: sampling bias

Caution is needed in interpreting metaanalytical findings because of the potential upward bias of the mean effect size (Reference Williams and GarnerWilliams & Garner, 2002), which can be examined on a funnel plot (Reference Begg, Cooper and HedgesBegg, 1994). The graph indicated no evidence of publication bias (Fig. 2). The fail-safe N is an estimate of the number of unpublished studies reporting null results needed to reduce the cumulated effect to the point of non-significance (Reference WolfWolf, 1986). The calculation of the fail-safe N to a criterion level of 0.1 resulted in a figure of 104 studies that would need to be included to reduce the effects to this criterion level. As the availability of such a large number of studies with null effects is unlikely, we assume that the studies included in the analysis are reasonably representative of the mean effect size.

Fig. 2 Funnel graph to estimate possible sampling bias.

Effective prevention and variation in outcome

Consistent with earlier meta-analyses for mental health promotion (Reference Durlak and WellsDurlak & Wells, 1997; Reference Tobler and StrattonTobler & Stratton, 1997; Reference Brown, Berndt and BrinalesBrown et al, 2000), our meta-analysis found a weighted mean effect size of 0.22. This is equivalent to an 11% improvement in the intervention groups compared with the control groups. Effect sizes for prevention programmes tend to be smaller than those of treatment, largely because prevention applies the same strategies to a population group that might or might not be at risk for a later mental health problem. However, from a public health perspective the prevention strategy can be cost-effective, as a small effect size in a large number of people can lead to a greater population gain than a large effect size in a small number of people (Reference RoseRose, 1993).

What leads to increased effects in depression prevention?

There was a large variation in programme outcomes. Subsequent analyses aimed to identify what might predict this variation.

Gender differences

There was a relationship between the percentage of male participants in universal and selective programmes and effect size, but not in indicated programmes. This finding is consistent with some within-trial findings (Reference Gillham, Reivich and JaycoxGillham et al, 1995) but not with others (Reference Seligman, Schulman and DeRubeisSeligman et al, 1999,Reference Seligman, Schulman and DeRubeisSeligman et al, 1999). It is possible that indicated programmes, which target specific problems with focused techniques, are more tailored to depressive symptoms and disorder than universal and selective programmes, and also take into account gender differences in the development of the programme. The results should be interpreted with caution, because the relationship is between the proportion of male participants in the programme and the effect size, not the actual effect size for each gender subgroup, which unfortunately is rarely reported. The results stress the importance of analysing and reporting gender differences in single trial evaluations to understand gender-specific programme effectiveness.

Initial level of risk

No difference was found between universal, selective and indicated programmes. There has been a marked preference for targeted interventions for depression prevention, because of evidence in reducing symptoms and incidence (Reference Clarke, Hawkins and MurphyClarke et al, 1995,Reference Clarke, Hawkins and MurphyClarke et al, 1995) and because subgroups identified at increased risk have seemed to benefit the most (Reference Price, Van Ryn and VinokurPrice et al, 1992,Reference Price, Van Ryn and VinokurPrice et al, 1992; Reference Gillham, Reivich and JaycoxGillham et al, 1995). However, evidence has also accumulated that universal preventive interventions can be beneficial for those at risk, because of lowered stigma and better socialisation (Reference Kellam, Ling and MeriscaKellam et al, 1998; Reference Reid, Eddy and FetrowReid et al, 1999). The results of our analysis have supported both these directions and there seems merit in interventions that combine both universal and targeted prevention (Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 2000).

Number and length of sessions

Research has focused on testing the efficacy of shortened versions of existing prevention programmes (Reference Munoz, Ying and Perez-StableMuñoz et al, 1993). The results of our analysis indicated that programmes with more than eight sessions and programmes with session lengths of 60–90 min yielded the larger effect sizes. The number of sessions is relevant for participants’ ability to internalise methods and processes offered by the interventions; fewer than nine sessions might not be enough. The length of sessions is important because of the group focus of prevention, where sufficient time needs to be allocated for interaction and group processes; less than an hour might not allow participants to feel engaged in a group process.

The promise of competence enhancement techniques

In addition to cognitive techniques (Reference Price and Bennett JohnsonPrice & Bennett Johnson, 1999; Reference Seligman, Schulman and DeRubeisSeligman et al, 1999,Reference Seligman, Schulman and DeRubeisSeligman et al, 1999; Reference Gillham, Shatte and FreresGillham et al, 2000; Reference Clarke, Hornbrook and LynchClarke et al, 2001), competence methods were also found to be effective across different age groups. Programmes that included behavioural techniques were detrimental for the elderly and were not superior for the other age groups. Programmes that combined three or more intervention methods were more effective than those that did not, suggesting the importance of multi-component programmes.

Provider qualifications

Lay personnel have been proposed as potential efficient programme providers for preventing depression (Reference Munoz, Ying and Perez-StableMuñoz et al, 1993). However, our meta-analysis found that lay personnel alone were not the best providers for selective and indicated programmes. The specificity and severity of depression in targeted populations who are already experiencing risk factors or symptoms may require trained personnel who are aware of and skilled in dealing with depressive symptoms.

Quality of the research design

Consistent with earlier findings (Reference Tobler and StrattonTobler & Stratton, 1997; Reference Brown, Berndt and BrinalesBrown et al, 2000) high-quality research trials were predictive of better outcomes. Well-defined intervention aims and accounting for attrition rates were independent predictors of effect size. Well-defined aims have already been identified as effect predictors in health promotion (Reference Kok, van den Borne and MullenKok et al, 1997). Reporting attrition rates might indicate a deeper analysis of intervention effects, and studies that do so might be more likely to have accounted for patient withdrawal at the outset, and to have provided the target group with incentives to continue in the programme.

Changes in depressive outcomes, risk factors and other psychiatric symptoms

Simultaneous positive changes in risk and protective factors and in related psychiatric symptoms (e.g. anxiety) were found in addition to the reductions in depressive symptoms, indicating the multiple outcome potential of prevention programmes. However, despite the evidence that prevention programmes can reduce depressive symptoms for both universal and targeted populations, few have demonstrated that the incidence of depression can be reduced (Clarke et al, Reference Clarke, Hawkins and Murphy1995,Reference Clarke, Hawkins and Murphy1995, Reference Clarke, Hornbrook and Lynch2001). There is an urgent need for further trials of sufficient power to study the impact of preventing the onset of depression and the role of moderating and mediating variables.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Prevention programmes to reduce depressive symptoms can lead to an 11% improvement in the intervention groups compared with control groups. However, the large variation in outcome stresses the importance of implementing only practices for which there is evidence of effect.

  2. Health and mental health care providers should be informed and provided with training in interventions to reduce and prevent depressive symptoms for targeted populations.

  3. Programmes that do not primarily target depression can lead to reductions in depressive symptoms, although unfortunately this is not often measured. When making choices for implementation, programmes targeting common risk and protective factors in addition to those focusing on depressive symptoms could lead to larger gains in other associated symptoms and disorders.

LIMITATIONS

  1. The inclusion criteria set for this study and insufficient information reported in single trial evaluations might have excluded other programmes that have targeted depressive symptoms.

  2. Although there was no evidence of publication bias, as with all meta-analyses, caution should be taken into account when interpreting the results because of non-included studies and non-reported findings.

  3. The gender result should be treated with caution because the finding is related to the proportion of male participants and the programme effect size, not the actual effect sizes of the two gender subgroups, which unfortunately are rarely reported.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Dutch Health Research and Development. Council (ZON), grant number 2200.0020.

We express our deep appreciation to Dr Hendricks Brown for his statistical. and methodological support and feedback during the different research phases. of the project. We also thank Sietske van Haren, the second coder, for her. dedicated input during the coding process and Rianne Kassander for her support. during the revision of the paper.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

Studies Included in The Meta-Analysis

Allart, E., Hosman, C., Hoogduin, C., et al (2003) Short-term results of the Coping with Depression course as an indicated preventive intervention: a randomized controlled trial. Behavior Therapy, 34, 381396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aronen, E. T. & Kurkela, S. A. (1996) Long-term effects of an early home-based intervention. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 16651672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Astin, J. A. (1997) Stress reduction through mindfulness meditation: effects on psychological symptomatology, sense of control, and spiritual experiences. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 66, 97106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barth, R. P. (1991) An experimental evaluation of in-home child abuse prevention services. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 363375.Google Scholar
Barth, R. P., Hacking, S. & Ash, J. R. (1988) Preventing child abuse: an experimental evaluation of the Child–Parent Enrichment Project. Journal of Primary Prevention, 8, 201217.Google Scholar
Bisson, J. I., Jenkins, P. L., Alexander, J., et al (1997) Randomised controlled trial of psychological debriefing for victims of acute burn trauma. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 7881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, R., Nichols, L. O., Martindale-Adams, J., et al (2000) Interdisciplinary geriatric primary care evaluation and management: two-year outcomes. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 48, 813.Google Scholar
Chang, B. L. (1999) Cognitive–behavioral intervention for homebound caregivers of persons with dementia. Nursing Research, 48, 173182.Google Scholar
Clarke, G., Hawkins, W., Murphy, M., et al (1995) Targeted prevention of unipolar depressive disorder in an at-risk sample of high school adolescents: a randomized trial of a group cognitive intervention. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 312321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuijpers, P. (1998) Prevention of depression in chronic general medical disorders: a pilot study. Psychological Reports, 82, 735738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, C. E., Bremner, R. & Boyle, M. (1995) Large group community-based parenting programs for families of preschoolers at risk for disruptive behaviour disorders: utilization, cost effectiveness, and outcome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 36, 11411159.Google Scholar
DiLorenzo, T. M., Bargman, E. P., Stucky-Ropp, R., et al (1999) Long-term effects of aerobic exercise on psychological outcomes. Preventive Medicine, 28, 7585.Google Scholar
Dracup, K., Moser, D. K., Taylor, S. E., et al (1997) Psychological consequences of cardiopulmonary resuscitation training for family members of patients at risk for sudden death. American Journal of Public Health, 87, 14341439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eggert, L., Thompson, E., Herting, J., et al (1995) Reducing suicide potential among high-risk youth: tests of a school-based prevention program. Suicide and Life Threatening Behavior, 25, 276296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elderen-van-Kemenade, T., Maes, S. & Van den Broek, Y. (1994) Effects of a health education programme with telephone follow-up during cardiac rehabilitation. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 367378.Google Scholar
Emery, C. F., Schein, R. L., Hauck, E. R., et al (1998) Psychological and cognitive outcomes of a randomized trial of exercise among patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Health Psychology, 17, 232240.Google Scholar
Foa, E. B., Hearst-Ikeda, D. & Perry, K. J. (1995) Evaluation of a brief cognitive–behavioral program for the prevention of chronic PTSD in recent assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 948955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelfand, D. M., Teti, D. M., Seiner, S. A., et al (1996) Helping mothers fight depression: evaluation of a home-based intervention program for depressed mothers and their infants. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25, 406422.Google Scholar
Goodkin, K., Blaney, N. T., Feaster, D. J., et al (1999) A randomized controlled clinical trial of a bereavement support group intervention in human immunodeficiency virus type 1-seropositive and-seronegative homosexual men. Erratum. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 693.Google Scholar
Grey, M., Boland, E. A., Davidson, M., et al (1998) Short-term effects of coping skills training as adjunct to intensive therapy in adolescents. Diabetes Care, 21, 902908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. & McCaul, M. E. (1992) An evaluation of a psychoeducational and substance abuse risk reduction intervention for children of substance abusers. Journal of Community Psychology, 20 (OSAP special issue), 7587.Google Scholar
Gwynn, C. A. & Brantley, H. T. (1987) Effect of a divorce group intervention for elementary school children. Psychology in the Schools, 24, 161164.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haight, B. K. (1988) The therapeutic role of a structured life review process in homebound elderly subjects. Journal of Gerontology, 43, 4044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haight, B. K. (1992) Long-term effects of a structured life review process. Journal of Gerontology, 47, 312315.Google Scholar
Haight, B. K., Michel, Y. & Hendrix, S. (1998) Life review: preventing despair in newly relocated nursing home residents: short-and long-term effects. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 47, 119142.Google Scholar
Hains, A. (1992) Comparison of cognitive–behavioral stress management techniques with adolescent boys. Journal of Counseling and Development, 70, 600605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hains, A. & Ellmann, S. W. (1994) Stress inoculation training as a preventative intervention for high school youths. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 8, 219232.Google Scholar
Harris, R. M., Bausell, R. B., Scott, D. E., et al (1998) An intervention for changing high-risk HIV behaviors of African American drug-dependent women. Research in Nursing Health, 21, 239250.Google Scholar
Heller, K., Thompson, M. G., Trueba, P. E., et al (1991) Peer support telephone dyads for elderly women: was this the wrong intervention? American Journal of Community Psychology, 19, 5374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irvine, A. B., Biglan, A., Smolkowski, K., et al (1999) The effectiveness of a parenting skills program for parents of middle school students in small communities. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 811825.Google Scholar
Jaycox, L. H., Reivich, K. J., Gillham, J., et al (1994) Prevention of depressive symptoms in school children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 801816.Google Scholar
Kahan, J., Kemp, B., Staples, F. R., et al (1985) Decreasing the burden in families caring for a relative with a dementing illness: a controlled study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 33, 664670.Google Scholar
King, C. A. & Kirschenbaum, D. S. (1990) An experimental evaluation of a school-based program for children at risk: Wisconsin Early Intervention. Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 167177.Google Scholar
Klein, M. L., Wing, R. R., Simkin-Silverman, I., et al (1997) The psychological consequences of weight gain prevention in healthy, premenopausal women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 21, 167174.Google Scholar
LaFromboise, T. & Howard-Pitney, B. (1995) The Zuni life skills development curriculum: description and evaluation of a suicide prevention program. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 479486.Google Scholar
Levy, L. H., Derby, J. F. & Martinkowski, K. S. (1993) Effects of membership in bereavement support groups on adaptation to conjugal bereavement. American Journal of Community Psychology, 21, 361381.Google Scholar
Lorig, K., Gonzalez, V. M. & Ritter, P. (1999) Community-based Spanish language arthritis education program: a randomized trial. Medical Care, 37, 957963.Google Scholar
Lovett, S. & Gallagher, D. (1988) Psychoeducational interventions for family caregivers: preliminary efficacy data. Behavior Therapy, 19, 321330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCallion, P., Toseland, R. W. & Freeman, K. (1999) An evaluation of a family visit education program. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47, 203214.Google Scholar
Mignone, J. & Guidotti, T. L. (1999) Support groups for injured workers: process and outcomes. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 41, 10591064.Google Scholar
Muñoz, R. F., Ying, Y., Bernal, G., et al (1995) Prevention of depression with primary care patients: a randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 199222.Google Scholar
Ostwald, S. K., Hepburn, K. W., Caron, W., et al (1999) Reducing caregiver burden: a randomized psychoeducational intervention for caregivers of persons with dementia. Gerontologist, 39, 299309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Partonen, T., Leppaemaeki, S., Hurme, J., et al (1998) Randomized trial of physical exercise alone or combined with bright light on mood and health-related quality of life. Psychological Medicine, 28, 13591364.Google Scholar
Peters, K. K. & Carlson, J. G. (1999) Worksite stress management with high-risk maintenance workers: a controlled study. International Journal of Stress Management, 6, 2144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, R. S. C. (2000) Preventing depression: a program for African American elders with chronic pain. Family and Community Health, 22, 5765.Google Scholar
Price, R., Van Ryn, M. & Vinokur, A. (1992) Impact of a preventive job search intervention on the likelihood of depression among the unemployed. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 33, 158167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seligman, M. E. P., Schulman, P., DeRubeis, R. J., et al (1999) The prevention of depression and anxiety. Prevention & Treatment, 2, article 8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheeber, L. B. & Johnson, J. H. (1994) Evaluation of a temperament focused parent training program. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 23, 249259.Google Scholar
Short, J. (1998) Evaluation of a substance abuse prevention and mental health promotion program for children of divorce. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 28, 139155.Google Scholar
Skitka, L. J. & Frazier, M. (1995) Ameliorating the effects of parental divorce: do small group interventions work? Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 24, 159179.Google Scholar
Stolberg, A. & Mahler, J. (1994) Enhancing treatment gains in a school-based intervention for children of divorce through skill training, parental involvement and transfer procedures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 147156.Google Scholar
Trzcieniecka-Green, A. & Steptoe, A. (1996) The effects of stress management on the quality of life of patients following acute myocardial infarction or coronary bypass surgery. European Heart Journal, 17, 16631670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfe, D. A., Edwards, B., Manion, I., et al (1988) Early intervention for parents at risk of child abuse and neglect: a preliminary investigation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 4047.Google Scholar
Worrall, G., Angel, J., Chaulk, P., et al (1999) Effectiveness of an educational strategy to improve family physicians' detection and management of depression: a randomized controlled trial. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 161, 3740.Google Scholar
Zubernis, L. S., Cassidy, K. W., Gillham, J. E., et al (1999) Prevention of depressive symptoms in preadolescent children of divorce. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 30, 1136.Google Scholar

References

Andrews, G., Henderson, S. & Hall, W. (2001) Prevalence, comorbidity, disability and service utilisation: overview of the Australian National Mental Health Survey. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 145153.Google Scholar
Begg, C. (1994) Publication bias. In The Handbook of Research Synthesis (eds Cooper, H. & Hedges, L. V.), pp. 399409. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Brown, C. H., Berndt, D., Brinales, J., et al (2000) Evaluating the evidence of effectiveness for preventive interventions: using a registry system to influence policy through science. Addictive Behaviors, 25, 955964.Google Scholar
Clarke, G., Hawkins, W., Murphy, M., et al (1995) Targeted prevention of unipolar depressive disorder in an at-risk sample of high school adolescents: a randomized trial of a group cognitive intervention. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 312321.Google Scholar
Clarke, G., Hornbrook, M., Lynch, F., et al (2001) A randomized trial of a group cognitive intervention for preventing depression in adolescent offspring of depressed parents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 11271134.Google Scholar
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group (2000) Merging universal and indicated prevention programs: the Fast Track model. Addictive Behaviors, 25, 913929.Google Scholar
Cooper, H. & Hedges, L. V. (1994) The Handbook of Research Synthesis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Durlak, J. & Wells, A. (1997) Primary prevention mental health programs for children and adolescents: a meta-analytic review. American Journal of Community Psychology, 25, 115152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillham, J., Reivich, K., Jaycox, L., et al (1995) Prevention of depressive symptoms in school children: two year follow-up. Psychological Science, 6, 343351.Google Scholar
Gillham, J., Shatte, A. & Freres, D. (2000) Preventing depression: a review of cognitive–behavioral and family interventions. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 9, 6388.Google Scholar
Gilvarry, E. (2000) Substance abuse in young people. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41, 5580.Google Scholar
Gleser, L. & Olkin, I. (1994) Stochastically dependent effect sizes. In The Handbook of Research Synthesis (eds Cooper, H. & Hedges, L. V.), pp. 339356. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Hedges, L. & Olkin, I. (1985) Statistical Methods for Meta-analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kellam, S., Ling, X., Merisca, R., et al (1998) The effect of the level of aggression in the first grade classroom on the course and malleability of aggressive behavior into middle school. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 165185.Google Scholar
Kok, G., van den Borne, B. & Mullen, P. (1997) Effectiveness of health education and health promotion: meta-analyses of effect studies and determinants of effectiveness. Patient Education and Counseling, 30, 1927.Google Scholar
Lipsey, M. & Wilson, D. (2001) Practical Meta-analysis (vol. 49). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Mrazek, P. & Haggerty, R. (eds) (1994) Reducing Risks for Mental Disorders: Frontiers for Preventive Intervention Research. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Munoz, R., Ying, Y., Perez-Stable, E., et al (1993) The Prevention of Depression: Research and Practice. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Murray, C. & Lopez, A. (1996) The Global Burden of Disease. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Price, R. & Bennett Johnson, S. (1999) Cognitive behavioral approaches to the prevention of depression, anxiety and health problems: new results and future prospects. Prevention and Treatment, 2, 7.Google Scholar
Price, R., Van Ryn, M. & Vinokur, A. (1992) Impact of a preventive job search intervention on the likelihood of depression among the unemployed. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 33, 158167.Google Scholar
Reid, J., Eddy, M., Fetrow, R., et al (1999) Description and immediate impacts of a preventive intervention for conduct problems. American Journal of Community Psychology, 27, 483517.Google Scholar
Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. & Seeley, J. (1991) Comorbidity of unipolar depression: II. Comorbidity with other mental disorders in adolescents and adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 214222.Google Scholar
Rose, G. (1993) The Strategy of Preventive Medicine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P., Schulman, P., DeRubeis, R. J., et al (1999) The prevention of depression and anxiety. Prevention & Treatment, 2, article 8.Google Scholar
Tobler, N. & Stratton, H. (1997) Effectiveness of school-based drug prevention programs: a meta-analysis of the research. Journal of Primary Prevention, 18, 71128.Google Scholar
Williams, D. D. R. & Garner, J. (2002) The case against ‘the evidence’: a different perspective on evidence-based medicine. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 812.Google Scholar
Wolf, F. (1986) Meta-Analysis: Quantitative Methods for Research Synthesis. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Description of the 69 programmes included in the meta-analysis

Figure 1

Table 2 Participant characteristics

Figure 2

Table 3 Weighted least squares regression analysis for gender and its interaction with level of risk

Figure 3

Table 4 Programme time descriptors

Figure 4

Table 5 Programme providers

Figure 5

Table 6 Comparisons between programmes including one of the prevention methods

Figure 6

Table 7 Quality of the research design and presence of independent quality items in the Cochrane scale

Figure 7

Fig. 1 Weighted mean effect sizes for changes in depressive symptoms, risk and protective factors and related psychiatric symptoms for universal, selective and indicated programmes.

Figure 8

Fig. 2 Funnel graph to estimate possible sampling bias.

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.