Introduction
Optimal foraging theory suggests that animals prefer to choose food that is high in energy while minimising effort (Charnov & Orians Reference Charnov and Orians1973; Pyke et al. Reference Pyke, Pulliam and Charnov1977; Stephens & Krebs Reference Stephens and Krebs1986). Similarly, standard learning theory argues that subjects learn behaviour from maximising rewards while minimising costs (Skinner Reference Skinner1938; Hull Reference Hull1943). These theories predict that when animals choose between two tasks to get the same food reward, they will choose the easier one. However, many studies have shown that animals prefer to work for their food rather than take free food when faced with an equal amount of food resources. This phenomenon, which contradicts optimal foraging and learning theories, is known as contrafreeloading (CFL) (Jensen Reference Jensen1963; Inglis et al. Reference Inglis, Forkman and Lazarus1997).
CFL has been widely reported in laboratory, captive, and domesticated animals, such as laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus; Inglis et al. Reference Inglis, Forkman and Lazarus1997), laboratory pigeons (Columba livia; Neuringer Reference Neuringer1969), captive maned wolves (Chrysocyon brachyurus; da Silva Vasconcellos et al. Reference da Silva Vasconcellos, Adania and Ades2012), captive rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta; Reinhardt Reference Reinhardt1994), red jungle fowl (Gallus gallus; Lindqvist & Jensen Reference Lindqvist and Jensen2008), and pigs (Sus scrofa; De Jonge et al. Reference De Jonge, Tilly, Baars and Spruijt2008). Some wild animals in captivity also show a tendency for CFL. For example, McGowan et al. (Reference McGowan, Robbins, Alldredge and Newberry2010) found that captive wild grizzly bears (Ursus arctos horribilis) spent more time opening food boxes to get food rather than taking free food. CFL has also been reported in humans. Tarte (Reference Tarte1981) found that young people preferred to press a lever to receive candy or cash rewards rather than take them directly. Meanwhile, various researchers have explored this behaviour from different perspectives based on the animals’ living environment and physiological characteristics, aiming to provide reasonable explanations for this phenomenon.
Inglis et al. (Reference Inglis, Forkman and Lazarus1997) summarised five possible explanations for this seemingly counterintuitive behaviour. Prior training theory suggests that the training process to obtain food serves as a stimulus and becomes a secondary reinforcer, resulting in animals preferring more challenging foraging behaviour even when free food is available (Alferink et al. Reference Alferink, Crosssman and Cheney1973). Neophobia theory suggests that the training for a task causes animals to fear the novel free food provided (Mitchell & White Reference Mitchell and White1977). Stimulus change theory proposes that any form of sensory change is beneficial, and animals will seek to alter their environment by searching for food, creating novelty (Osborne & Shelby Reference Osborne and Shelby1975). Information primacy theory argues that working for food provides useful information for future foraging (Singh Reference Singh1970; Inglis & Ferguson Reference Inglis and Ferguson1986). Self-reinforcing theory suggests that the effort animals put into obtaining food is self-satisfying (Jensen Reference Jensen1963).
From the above explanations, several potential factors might affect CFL levels. First, according to prior training theory and neophobia theory, foraging training may affect animals’ CFL levels. The training process can serve as a stimulus reinforcer or make animals fear free food, leading to higher CFL levels. However, few experimental studies have compared the impact of foraging training on CFL levels. Second, according to information primacy theory, working for food provides useful information for future foraging. However, when the motivation to gather information is replaced by a stronger feeding motivation (such as hunger), information-gathering behaviour decreases (Inglis & Ferguson Reference Inglis and Ferguson1986; Lindqvist et al. Reference Lindqvist, Schütz and Jensen2002). This suggests that animals may show different levels of CFL relative to different food deprivation levels. Finally, self-reinforcing theory predicts that the effort required to obtain food can also affect the level of CFL. On the one hand, animals gain satisfaction from the effort of obtaining food, increasing their motivation to persist in challenging foraging tasks (Sasson-Yenor & Powell Reference Sasson-Yenor and Powell2019). On the other hand, if the difficulty level is too high and animals cannot gain satisfaction from overcoming it, they will stop foraging, leading to the disappearance of CFL (Carder & Berkowitz Reference Carder and Berkowitz1970).
Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) are small, climbing birds belonging to the order Psittaciformes, family Psittacidae (BirdLife International 2012), and genus Melopsittacus. They are common subjects in animal cognition research. Previous studies have shown that trained budgerigars can complete complex tasks to obtain food, and their problem-solving ability is related to their exploratory behaviour (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Zou, Sun and ten Cate2019). Therefore, budgerigars are ideal subjects for studying CFL and its influencing factors. The aims of this study were: (1) to assess the existence of CFL in budgerigars by investigating their feeder selection between two food containers (free food container and moderately challenging food container; free food container and highly challenging food container; moderately food container and highly challenging food container); (2) to investigate the potential factors affecting budgerigars’ feeder choices by dividing subjects into two groups: trained and untrained; moreover, all subjects were treated with three levels of food deprivation in order: no food deprivation, moderate food deprivation, and high food deprivation. Thus, our study aimed to analyse the effects of prior training, food deprivation, and effort required on the level of CFL. Studies have shown CFL to be correlated with foraging styles, and budgerigars used extended search to obtain food, similar to pigeons, rats, and gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus; Inglis et al. Reference Inglis, Forkman and Lazarus1997). Furthermore, because prior training theory suggests training would serve as a secondary reinforcement and neophobia theory would cause animals to fear the free food, we predicted that budgerigars would exhibit CFL, preferring to select the challenging food containers rather than those with free food. Finally, research indicates that increased food deprivation leads to a reduction in CFL tendencies (Inglis & Ferguson Reference Inglis and Ferguson1986), we predict that budgerigars will exhibit higher CFL levels when not subjected to food deprivation. Conversely, high levels of food deprivation are expected to result in lower CFL levels.
Materials and methods
Ethical approval
All the procedures described in this article were approved by the Institutional Committee for Animal Care and Use at Hefei Normal University (see Supplementary material).
Subjects and apparatus
Subjects
This study involved 12 adult budgerigars (six males and six females). All budgerigars were procured from the Hefei Yufeng Market, Yaohai District, China. Healthy and active budgerigars were selected, and none had prior experience with foraging task training. The budgerigars were housed in a well-ventilated and well-lit animal room with a natural light cycle. Four birds were placed in each wire cage, with ad libitum access to food (dry, shelled grains) and water. Researchers identified individuals based on external characteristics such as size and feather colour.
Apparatus
We conducted the experiments in a birdcage (32.5 cm × 30.5 cm × 34 cm; length × width × height), which included a choice area and two food containers (one being less challenging and the other more challenging in terms of obtaining food) (Figure 1). The budgerigars entered the cage via an entrance into an adaptation buffer area between the entrance and a transparent partition (made of glass). This partition prevented agitated animals from entering the food area and affecting the accuracy of the experiment while allowing the birds to observe the food area. When the budgerigars reached the perch, they could observe the two food containers (made of transparent plastic) and make choices based on the foraging challenge.

Figure 1. Birdcage used in the experimental set-up to test contrafreeloading (CFL) in budgerigars (n = 12). Image is to scale.
Three devices were set for animals to obtain food:
-
• Free food container (Free) – the top of the food container was uncovered, allowing the budgerigars to access the food freely;
-
• Moderately challenging food container (MC) – the food in the container was covered with wood-shavings, requiring the budgerigars to use their claws or beaks to move the shavings aside to access the food; and
-
• Highly challenging food container (HC) – the food in the container was covered with wood-shavings, and the top was sealed with plastic wrap (weak enough to be pierced by the beaks of the budgerigars). The budgerigars needed to peck open the plastic wrap and move the shavings aside to access the food.
Pre-experiment
Habituation
The budgerigars were placed in the experimental preparation room prior to the onset of the formal experiment, preventing the researcher’s actions, sounds, and experimental apparatus from unsettling the animals. Researchers fed the birds by hand and placed the required experimental apparatus around them, allowing the budgerigars to become familiar with the experimental environment. As the budgerigars had been artificially raised (by humans), it only took four days for them to no longer display any signs of fearful behaviour and move freely throughout the cage, thereby indicating habituation (Bean et al. Reference Bean, Mason and Bateson1999). For the duration of the study, only our study group’s researchers were allowed access to the room housing the birds.
Pre-training
We selected six budgerigars (three males and three females) for pre-training. For each bird, we provided two food containers, one allowed free access to food, and the other was an MC/HC container. The birds need to use both containers to get their 4-h food rations (normal feeding cycles). Each container had the same amount of food, and both together provided the 4-h food ration, encouraging birds to complete the tasks, and meanwhile preventing the influence of stimulus reinforcement. First, for the Free/MC, the budgerigars could see the covered MC food through the transparent plastic food container. If, initially, budgerigars were unable to obtain the food provided (dry, shelled grains) researchers would remove the wood-shavings to encourage the budgerigars to see and eat the food from the top. Once they had successfully obtained the food from MC, researchers replaced the wood-shavings and observed whether the budgerigars could remove the shavings to eat. This was repeated until the budgerigars were able to effectively remove the wood-shavings and consume the food. Second, for the Free/HC, researchers pierced the plastic wrap covering of HC to encourage the budgerigars to pierce the wrap and move the wood-shavings to eat, and the unpierced plastic wrap was presented until the budgerigars could pierce the wrap and move the shavings to eat. Training ended when all six budgerigars could complete these two steps and successfully obtain the food at least three times. These pre-training procedures lasted seven days.
Procedure
To prevent interference with sex factors, six males and six females were utilised in our experiment. Before the formal experiment, researchers identified individual budgerigars based on size, feather colour, and specific markings. The experiments were conducted between 0900h and 1500h to avoid any influence of natural light changes. Each budgerigar was placed in a 32.5 cm × 30.5 cm × 34 cm cage for the duration of the experiment. We conducted 10-min videotaped trials, randomly alternating the food containers between the left and right sides to prevent any potential bias caused by the birds’ side preferences.
Six budgerigars (three males and three females) were pre-trained, while the others were not. Given that budgerigars’ feeding cycles are generally 3–5 h, we created three food deprivation levels: no deprivation (NDP, free feeding); moderate deprivation (MDP, 4 h without food); and high deprivation (HDP, 8 h without food) (Bean et al. Reference Bean, Mason and Bateson1999). All birds underwent these three deprivation levels. For each bird, we first tested their selection under no deprivation (NDP) level, and then the moderate deprivation (MDP) level on the second day. On the fourth day (one day was provided for adjustment), we tested their selection under a high deprivation (HDP) level. Two or more days later, another series of trials were started as above, i.e. six trials were conducted for each deprivation level.
For each trial, we recorded the budgerigars’ ID, sex, training status, and food deprivation levels, placing them in the experimental cage. Once the budgerigars were freely moving in the cage without showing any visible signs of stress, their food choices were observed and recorded. The study compared choices: (1) between Free and MC; (2) between Free and HC; and (3) between MC and HC. We determined container preferences by the first choice and the proportion of time spent at the more challenging food containers (Sasson-Yenor & Powell Reference Sasson-Yenor and Powell2019). The proportion of time spent at more challenging tasks was calculated as follows:
time spent at the more challenging food containers/(time spent at the less challenging food containers + time spent at the more challenging food containers).
Data analysis
The study first compared the proportions of the first choice for budgerigars with NDP. Additionally, we used paired t-tests to compare the time spent choosing more challenging versus less challenging food containers for budgerigars with NDP. Osborne (Reference Osborne1977) proposed that if the proportion of choosing challenging food containers exceeded 50%, the subjects were considered to show CFL tendencies (Osborne Reference Osborne1977). However, as optimal foraging theory and standard learning theory predict a total preference for free food, any deviation from such an outcome should be explained by other theories (Inglis et al. Reference Inglis, Forkman and Lazarus1997; da Silva Vasconcellos et al. Reference da Silva Vasconcellos, Adania and Ades2012). As a result of such controversies, here the decision was taken to classify the budgerigars’ CFL tendencies into four levels based upon the proportion of time spent at more challenging food containers: strong (> 0.5); moderate (0.3–0.5); weak (0.1–0.3); and null (< 0.1) (Delgado et al. Reference Delgado, Han and Bain2022).
To analyse the factors that affected CFL levels, three generalised linear mixed models (GLMMs) were constructed using the binomial error structure and logit-link function in R 3.6.3. The dependent variables were the first choice of less challenging and more challenging food containers. Prior training and food deprivation levels were fixed factors, while individual identity and sex were random variables. The GLMM analysis was performed using the glmer function from the lmerTest package in R (Bates et al. Reference Bates, Maechler and Bolker2012). Another three GLMMs with Gaussian error structure and identity link function were used to analyse the proportion of time spent at more challenging food containers, with fixed factors and random variables as above. The GLMM analyses were performed using the lmer function from the lmerTest package in R (Bates et al. Reference Bates, Maechler and Bolker2012). All data were analysed with the significance level set at 0.05 (two-tailed).
Results
A total of 216 trials were conducted, with 18 trials per individual in this study. Among them, the budgerigars did not choose in 21 trials, and these instances occurred 16 times under conditions with NDP, five times under conditions with MDP, and never under conditions with HDP. In the remaining 195 trials, the less challenging food containers were chosen first 111 times while the more challenging containers were chosen first 84 times. Table 1 shows the detailed information regarding specific choices. Primarily, for the trials including HC food containers (n = 144), the pre-trained budgerigars spent time at the HC food containers in 62 trials and failed to access food in 29 trials. The untrained budgerigars spent time at the HC food containers in 37 trials and failed to access food in 27 trials.
Table 1. Proportion of time each budgerigar (n = 12) chose the challenging food containers in each trial of the study. The larger the value, the higher the proportion of time budgerigars chose the more challenging food container. Numbers in bold indicate that the more challenging food container was the first choice. Blank means no choice

ID: Three-letter identification of each budgerigar. NDP: no food deprivation; MDP: moderate food deprivation; HDP: high food deprivation. Free: freely accessible food container; MC: moderately challenging food container; HC: highly challenging food container; L (left) and R (right) indicate the side of the more challenging food container in a trial.
Budgerigars’ CFL tendencies
Given that the identification of CFL tendency is based upon the choices made by animals not subject to food deprivation, this study compared the first choice and the time proportion of the food containers chosen by the budgerigars with NDP. In the 56 experiments without food deprivation, the budgerigars chose the less challenging food containers first 25 times and the more challenging food containers 31 times (binomial test; P = 0.504). Additionally, a paired t-test showed no significant difference in the time spent between less challenging and more challenging food containers (t = 0.982, n = 56; P = 0.330). Further, we calculated each budgerigar’s CFL level and based on these results, we would classify half of the budgerigars (n = 6) as strong contrafreeloaders (CFL levels: 0.62–0.98), three budgerigars as moderate (CFL levels: 0.33–0.46) and one budgerigar as weak (0.21) contrafreeloaders, and one budgerigar as freeloader (CFL level: 0.0). One bird did not choose.
Factors affecting budgerigars’ first choice of food containers
For the trials set on the first choice, out of 62 first choices between MC and Free food containers, the budgerigars chose the Free food containers 30 times and the MC food containers 32 times. Out of 65 first choices between HC and Free food containers, the budgerigars chose the Free food containers 41 times and the HC food containers 24 times. Out of 68 first choices between HC and MC food containers, the budgerigars chose the MC food containers 40 times and the HC food containers 28 times. We used GLMM binomial regression to assess the factors affecting the budgerigars’ first choice for food containers. The results showed that pre-training (Yes/No) and food deprivation (MDP vs NDP, HDP vs NDP) did not significantly affect the budgerigars’ first choice between the less challenging and more challenging food containers (see Table 2).
Table 2. GLMM binomial regression results for the factors on the first choice made by budgerigars (n = 12) in contrafreeloading (CFL) tests. Estimates, SE, Z value, and P-values for the three GLMMs run to test the influence of pre-training and food deprivation on first choice between two food containers: MC vs Free, HC vs Free, and HC vs MC

Free: free to access food container; MC: Moderately challenging food container; HC: Highly challenging food container.
NDP: no deprivation; MDP: moderate deprivation; HDP: high deprivation.
P-values < 0.05 are indicated in bold
Factors affecting proportion of time at more challenging food container
We used GLMM Gaussian regression to assess the factors affecting the budgerigars’ proportion of time spent at more challenging food containers. The results in Table 3 (MC vs Free) showed that both pre-training (Yes/No) and food deprivation (MDP vs NDP, HDP vs NDP) did not have a significant effect on budgerigars’ proportion of time spent at challenging containers. However, for the trials between HC and Free food containers, the results (Table 3; HC vs Free) show that budgerigars with MDP and HDP both tend to select the Free food containers rather than highly challenging food containers while pre-training (Yes/No) did not significantly affect the proportion of time spent at highly challenging food containers. Furthermore, the GLMM results for the trials between MC and HC food containers showed that pre-trained budgerigars were more likely to select HC food containers, and budgerigars with HDP spent a greater proportion of time at the MC than HC food containers.
Table 3. GLMM gaussian regression results for the factors on proportion of time spent at more challenging food container by budgerigars (n = 12) in contrafreeloading (CFL) tests. Estimates, SE, t value, and P-values for the three GLMMs run to test the influence of pre-training, and food deprivation on proportion of time spent on the more challenging of two food containers: MC vs Free, HC vs Free, and HC vs MC

Free: free to access food container; MC: Moderately challenging food container; HC: Highly challenging food container.
NDP: no deprivation; MDP: moderate deprivation; HDP: high deprivation.
P-values < 0.05 are indicated in bold
Discussion
This study represents the first investigation into contrafreeloading behaviour in budgerigars. The results revealed that, at the group level, budgerigars did not exhibit a statistically significant preference for more challenging food containers. Moreover, only half of the budgerigars exhibited strong CFL levels, and one was even classified as freeloader. Additionally, when examining the influencing factors of CFL levels, we found that pre-training and food deprivation had no significant effect on budgerigars’ first choice between less challenging and more challenging food containers. While food deprivation, especially the HDP, significantly decreased the budgerigars’ proportion of time spent at HC food containers in the trials ‘Free vs HC’ and ‘MC vs HC’, and pre-training increased their proportion of time spent at HC food containers in the trials ‘MC vs HC.’ Our results indicated that budgerigars exhibit a CFL level that is intermediate, lying somewhere between a strong tendency and the absence of such behaviour. However, the specific level is influenced by a variety of factors.
In previous studies, domestic cats (Felis catus) were considered the only species that did not show CFL (Delgado et al. Reference Delgado, Han and Bain2022). Researchers believe that cats are predatory animals, and their sit-and-wait predation is a low-energy and widespread hunting method (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Wolfe, Davis, Kendall, Richter, Wang, Bryce, Elkaim and Wilmers2014). This foraging style of domestic cats makes them more inclined towards low energy feeding methods, showing lower CFL tendencies (Delgado et al. Reference Delgado, Han and Bain2022). Animals such as domestic pigeons, mice (Mus musculus), and giraffes (Giraffa camelopardalis) belong within the category of foraging animals, requiring energy expenditure during the search for food (Inglis et al. Reference Inglis, Forkman and Lazarus1997). They are more likely, therefore, to exhibit CFL tendencies under captive or caged conditions. Budgerigars in this study belong to exploratory foraging animals but did not exhibit strong CFL tendencies, demonstrating that foraging style can only partly explain the phenomenon of such behaviour.
The definition of CFL is the criterion used by researchers to determine whether animals exhibit CFL tendencies. Some researchers believe that when animals choose to make an effort to obtain food in more than 50% of cases, they are exhibiting CFL tendencies (Osborne Reference Osborne1977). Other researchers propose that according to the optimal foraging theory and standard learning theory, animals should choose free food over effortful food. Therefore, as long as animals select to make an effort to obtain food, they can be considered as exhibiting CFL tendencies (Inglis et al. Reference Inglis, Forkman and Lazarus1997; McGowan et al. Reference McGowan, Robbins, Alldredge and Newberry2010; Ogura Reference Ogura2011; da Silva Vasconcellos et al. Reference da Silva Vasconcellos, Adania and Ades2012). In this study, at the group level, the budgerigars’ first choices and the proportion of time spent at more challenging food containers did not differ significantly, results that were similar to those observed in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Bastos, Chodorow, Taylor and Pepperberg2022). Although they selected the more challenging food containers more than 50% of the time, half of the budgerigars did not exhibit strong contrafreeloading. Thus, we conclude that the CFL levels of budgerigars falls within an intermediate range, situated between a strong tendency and the absence of such behaviour.
In studying CFL, researchers may construct different forms of experimental apparatus relative to each species’ characteristics (such as body size, foraging method, food type, etc). For example, when studying starlings (Sturnus vulgaris), researchers provided food dishes covered with transparent and opaque plastic membranes for selection (Bean et al. Reference Bean, Mason and Bateson1999); when studying maned wolves, researchers set up food scattered areas and tray areas for selection (da Silva Vasconcellos et al. Reference da Silva Vasconcellos, Adania and Ades2012); when studying domestic cats, researchers set up plates for direct access to food and food puzzles that required effort for food to be accessed (Delgado et al. Reference Delgado, Han and Bain2022). Therefore, the inconsistent and diverse experimental settings may also affect animals’ choices. In this study, two food containers were set up in the birdcage for the budgerigars to select, the more challenging food container required the animals to move aside the wood-shavings covering the food (MC) or open a transparent plastic membrane before moving the wood-shavings to access the food (HC). In this way, animals may have their vision obstructed by the wood-shavings during feeding. However, since the food containers used in this study were transparent, the birds could see the food in both containers from a straight and downward slanting angle upon entering the feeding area of the cage. Therefore, we believed that the budgerigars’ vision would not affect the results of this study.
According to the theory of self-reinforcement mentioned above, if animals cannot achieve satisfaction in overcoming difficulties to obtain food, they will stop feeding, leading to the disappearance of CFL (Carder & Berkowitz Reference Carder and Berkowitz1970). In this study, the untrained budgerigars had a relatively low success rate (27%) in opening the highly challenging containers to access food, resulting in a loss of interest in HC food containers and a preference for less challenging food containers. In contrast, the pre-trained budgerigars had a relatively higher success rate (53%) in opening the highly challenging containers to access food, and they exhibited higher CFL tendencies than untrained budgerigars, supporting the theory of self-reinforcement.
According to the information primacy theory, hungry animals are more inclined towards free food and exhibit lower CFL levels (Singh Reference Singh1970; Inglis & Ferguson Reference Inglis and Ferguson1986). In the ‘Free vs HC’ and ‘MC vs HC’ food container selection trials here, budgerigars with food deprivation, especially the HDP (8 h without food) showed lower CFL levels, similar to the results of red jungle fowl and starlings, indicating that animals exhibit lower CFL levels when hungry, supporting this theory (Inglis & Ferguson Reference Inglis and Ferguson1986; Lindqvist et al. Reference Lindqvist, Schütz and Jensen2002).
Besides, many other factors have been suggested as affecting CFL levels, such as age (McGowan et al. Reference McGowan, Robbins, Alldredge and Newberry2010), sex (Andrews et al. Reference Andrews, Viviani, Egan, Bedford, Brilot, Nettle and Bateson2015), physical state (Sasson-Yenor & Powell Reference Sasson-Yenor and Powell2019), food competition (Andrews et al. Reference Andrews, Viviani, Egan, Bedford, Brilot, Nettle and Bateson2015), play opportunity (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Greene, Hartsfield and Pepperberg2021), behavioural pathology (van Zeeland et al. Reference van Zeeland, Schoemaker and Lumeij2023), and so on. The affecting factors from our study come from the five summarised explanations of Inglis et al. (Reference Inglis, Forkman and Lazarus1997) and apply to most animals that show CFL tendencies. Future studies might investigate the species-specific influencing factors on CFL levels.
Wild budgerigars are ground foragers that primarily seek cereals through extended searches. Contrary to their natural behaviour of CFL which offers adaptive advantages in the wild (Singh Reference Singh1970; Inglis & Ferguson Reference Inglis and Ferguson1986), captive budgerigars are typically provided with abundant food, rendering foraging behaviours seemingly unnecessary. Consequently, caregivers often assume that simply supplying sufficient food is adequate. However, this study suggests that caged budgerigars maintain a certain level of internal motivation to explore and engage with their environment. The absence of opportunities to express these instincts may result in negative emotional consequences and health issues (Clubb & Mason Reference Clubb and Mason2003). Therefore, we would recommend that budgerigar caregivers offer appropriate challenges and choices for obtaining food within their living environment, thereby promoting opportunities for CFL behaviours to be expressed. Our results also have implications for the welfare of other caged or domesticated animals.
Animal welfare implications
Budgerigars display certain tendencies for CFL. Budgerigars have bright feather colours, lively temperaments, and ease of domestication. They are highly favoured by bird lovers and kept as captive animals. Additionally, they are often used as experimental model animals due to their good learning abilities (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Zou, Sun and ten Cate2019). The results of this study indicate that budgerigars tend to engage in some foraging behaviour rather than simple feeding activities with no food deprivation, having implications for the feeding of budgerigars by appropriately setting foraging environments to ensure their animal welfare. For example, budgerigars can be presented with two food containers: one that allows direct access to food, and another where the food is mixed with non-edible items (such as small stones or wood-shavings, as used in this experiment), requiring a degree of effort to locate and retrieve the food. This approach not only satisfies their basic feeding needs but also stimulates their natural inclination to explore and engage with their environment. Furthermore, the training experience, food deprivation, and effort required can affect their CFL levels, which lays a theoretical foundation for the subsequent study of vertebrate CFL.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2025.15.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Ming Geng and Jing Chen for their help and support throughout our study. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript. This study was made possible with the help of a grant received from the Research Foundation for Talented Scholars of Hefei Normal University (2020rcjj46 and 2022rcjj47).
Competing interest
None.