Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T12:45:59.254Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Letter to the Editor: Comments on ‘Is there an excess of significant findings in published studies of psychotherapy for depression?’

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 September 2014

G. M. Goodwin*
Affiliation:
University Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
*
Address correspondence to: Professor G. M. Goodwin, University Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Flint and colleagues make use of a powerful statistical approach to suggest that there is significant positive bias in the expected distribution of published trials of psychotherapy (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Cuijpers, Horder, Koole and Munafò2014). They draw a parallel with the bias we know exists in the drug treatment literature and imply that it is again publication bias that explains the finding. In other words, withholding publication has deflated the number of non-significant results.

Indeed, for drug trials conducted by companies (the majority), we know this is the case because the results of negative trials are submitted to the regulatory authorities and can be identified if they are not published. Much has been made of this in previous reviews (Turner, Reference Turner2013).

However, publication bias is not necessarily the explanation for the findings in psychotherapy trials. We know that the quality of most psychotherapy trials falls far short of what is normal in drug trials (Huhn et al. Reference Huhn, Tardy, Spineli, Kissling, Förstl, Pitschel-Walz, Leucht, Samara, Dold, Davis and Leucht2014). Thus, a variety of biases operate to inflate the number of significant results in psychotherapy trials before publication. These include strong allegiance bias, single blind designs and a flexible approach to statistical treatment of outcomes. The result may well be the distorted distribution of results that Flint and colleagues identify in the published literature. A simple prediction is that significant numbers of unpublished negative studies simply do not exist for psychotherapies.

In conclusion, if bias distorts the generation of data rather than simply its reporting, we are presumably uncertain what a true effect size is for psychotherapy.

Declaration of Interest

Guy Goodwin has held grants from Servier, received honoraria for speaking or chairing educational meetings from Abbvie, AstraZeneca, GSK, Lilly, Lundbeck, Medscape, Servier and advised AstraZeneca, Cephalon/Teva, Lundbeck, Merck, Otsuka, P1Vital, Servier, Shire, Sunovion, and Takeda, holds shares in P1vital and has acted as expert witness for Lilly.

References

Flint, J, Cuijpers, P, Horder, J, Koole, SL, Munafò, MR (2014). Is there an excess of significant findings in published studies of psychotherapy for depression. Psychological Medicine. Published online: 25 July 2014 . doi:10.1017/S0033291714001421.Google Scholar
Huhn, M, Tardy, M, Spineli, LM, Kissling, W, Förstl, H, Pitschel-Walz, G, Leucht, C, Samara, M, Dold, M, Davis, JM, Leucht, S (2014). Efficacy of pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy for adult psychiatric disorders: a systematic overview of meta-analyses. Journal of the American Medical Association Psychiatry 71, 706715.Google ScholarPubMed
Turner, EH (2013). Publication bias, with a focus on psychiatry: causes and solutions. CNS Drugs 27, 457468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed