Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T23:40:13.504Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of acute citalopram on self-referential emotional processing and social cognition in healthy volunteers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 October 2020

Catherine Hobbs*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, UK
Susannah E. Murphy
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, and NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Lucy Wright
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, and NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
James Carson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, and NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Indra Van Assche
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, and NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Jessica O'Brien
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, and NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Mayowa Oyesanya
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, and NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Jie Sui
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, UK
Marcus R. Munafò
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit at the University of Bristol, and National Institute of Health Research Biomedical Research Centre at the University Hospitals Bristol NHS Foundation Trust and the University of Bristol, UK
David Kessler
Affiliation:
Population Health Sciences, University of Bristol
Catherine J. Harmer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, and NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Katherine S. Button
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, UK
*
Correspondence: Catherine Hobbs. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Depression is characterised by negative views of the self. Antidepressant treatment may remediate negative self-schema through increasing processing of positive information about the self. Changes in affective processing during social interactions may increase expression of prosocial behaviours, improving interpersonal communications.

Aims

To examine whether acute administration of citalopram is associated with an increase in positive affective learning biases about the self and prosocial behaviour.

Method

Healthy volunteers (n = 41) were randomised to either an acute 20 mg dose of citalopram or matched placebo in a between-subjects double-blind design. Participants completed computer-based cognitive tasks designed to measure referential affective processing, social cognition and expression of prosocial behaviours.

Results

Participants administered citalopram made more cooperative choices than those administered placebo in a prisoner's dilemma task (β = 20%, 95% CI: 2%, 37%). Exploratory analyses indicated that participants administered citalopram showed a positive bias when learning social evaluations about a friend (β = 4.06, 95% CI: 0.88, 7.24), but not about the self or a stranger. Similarly, exploratory analyses found evidence of increased recall of positive words and reduced recall of negative words about others (β = 2.41, 95% CI: 0.89, 3.93), but not the self, in the citalopram group.

Conclusions

Participants administered citalopram showed greater prosocial behaviours, increased positive recall and increased positive learning of social evaluations towards others. The increase in positive affective bias and prosocial behaviours towards others may, at least partially, be a mechanism of antidepressant effect. However, we found no evidence that citalopram influenced self-referential processing.

Type
Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Royal College of Psychiatrists

Addressing negative perceptions of the self is believed to be central to the successful treatment of depression. According to cognitive theories, individuals with depression hold negative views and expectations about the self, developed as an internalised response to repeated negative social experiences (e.g. peer victimisation). When activated by external stressors, these negative self-schema dominate information processing, increasing automatic processing of negative information about the self. Deliberative cognitive processing is attenuated, preventing reappraisal of these automatic biases.Reference Beck1 Supportive of this theory, peer victimisation in childhood is associated with increased negative and reduced positive perceptions of the self.Reference Cole, Dukewich, Roeder, Sinclair, McMillan and Will2 Furthermore, negative affective biases are more likely to be observed in depression if stimuli are encoded in reference to the self.Reference Cole, Dukewich, Roeder, Sinclair, McMillan and Will2Reference Hertel and El-Messidi5 Changes in self-referential affective processing may therefore be a key mechanism of treatments for depression.

The role of antidepressants in addressing negative self-schema

Depression is commonly treated using antidepressant medication.Reference Kendrick, Dowrick, McBride, Howe, Clarke and Maisey6 Antidepressants are believed to operate by remediating negative affective biases early in treatment.Reference Harmer, Goodwin and Cowen7 Supportive of this, short-term administration of antidepressants is associated with increased processing of positive stimuli in both depressed and healthy volunteers.Reference Walsh and Harmer8 Negative self-schemas may be addressed through these changes in automatic affective processing.Reference Roiser, Elliott and Sahakian9 Individuals with depression preferentially process negative information about the self.Reference Phillips, Hine and Thorsteinsson10 Shifting affective processing through antidepressant treatment may expose individuals to more positive information about the self, remediating negative schema. In keeping with this argument, antidepressants have been found to increase recall of positive characteristics encoded to the self in healthy and depressed individuals.Reference Harmer, O'Sullivan, Favaron, Massey-Chase, Ayres and Reinecke11,Reference Harmer, Shelley, Cowen and Goodwin12

Importance of social cognition

In order for changes in affective processing from antidepressant treatment to alter mood, it has been suggested that individuals must engage with their social environment to relearn associations in a more positive context.Reference Godlewska and Harmer13 However, this aspect of the model is yet to be fully explored. Greater depression severity is associated with poorer-quality social interactionsReference Teo, Choi and Valenstein14 and increased expectations of rejection.Reference Caouette and Guyer15 Raised expectations of rejection may evoke hostile or non-responsive social behaviours, increasing the likelihood of reciprocal negative behaviours from others and reinforcing negative expectations of social interactions.Reference Lewinsohn16,Reference Tse and Bond17 In keeping with previous evidence of increases in positive affective biases and behaviours following antidepressant administration,Reference Harmer, O'Sullivan, Favaron, Massey-Chase, Ayres and Reinecke11,Reference Harmer, Shelley, Cowen and Goodwin12,Reference Tse and Bond18 antidepressant treatment is likely to strengthen positive learning and prosocial behaviours during social interactions. Repeated social interactions with remediated positive affective biases may therefore reinforce engagement in future social interactions, potentially addressing the issues of social withdrawal that are characteristic of depression.

Altering self-schema through changes in social cognition

The self is a social construct, shaped by our perceptions of others’ evaluations of us.Reference Shrauger and Schoeneman19 During social interactions, healthy individuals preferentially incorporate positive evaluations into their self-concept.Reference Korn, Prehn, Park, Walter and Heekeren20 By contrast, individuals with greater depression expect more negative evaluations,Reference Moritz and Roberts21 selectively engage in negative feedbackReference Giesler, Josephs and Swann22 and show reduced learning of positive evaluationsReference Hobbs, Faraway, Kessler, Munafò and Sui23 about the self. Preferential engagement with negative social evaluations about the self may reinforce negative self-schema and increase social withdrawal in a vicious cycle. Increasing positive affective biases through antidepressant treatment may increase learning of positive social evaluations from others, altering the affective content of self-schema and breaking the pattern of maladaptive learning in social interactions. Changes in self-referential affective learning within social contexts may be an important pathway in antidepressant action.

Aims

In this study, we examined the influence of acute administration of citalopram on affective self-referential cognition and social behaviours in healthy volunteers using a double-blind placebo-controlled design. We hypothesised that acute administration of citalopram would be associated with an increase in positive affective biases about the self and increased prosocial behaviour.

Method

This study was pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/nhjvs/), where study materials are also available. The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in the University of Bath Research Data Archive at https://doi.org/10.15125/BATH-00891.

Subjects

Participants aged 18–45 years and fluent in English, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, were recruited through advertisement to the local community. We excluded participants meeting diagnostic criteria for past or current axis 1 DSM-V psychiatric disorder identified using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-V axis I disorders (SCID-VReference First, Williams, Karg and Spitzer24). Other exclusion criteria were current use of psychoactive medication (excluding contraceptive medication), current or past drug or alcohol dependency, a current or past significant neurological condition, known hypersensitivity to the study drug, current pregnancy or breast feeding, current significant medical condition, consumption of more than five cigarettes or more than six caffeinated drinks per day, lactose intolerance, previous participation in a study using similar cognitive tasks, previous participation in a study involving medication within the past 3 months, or recreational psychoactive drug use within the past 3 months.

Ethical approval

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation, and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human subjects were approved by the University of Oxford Medical Sciences Interdivisional Research Committee (R64589). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Design

This study used a between-subject, double-blind, placebo controlled design. Participants were randomised to receive a single acute oral dose of 20 mg citalopram or lactose placebo encapsulated in identical white capsules. Blocked randomisation, stratified by gender, was generated using an online randomisation tool.25

Procedure

Participants first completed self-report questionnaires on mood and personality (baseline) and were administered the study medication. Citalopram is rapidly absorbed, with peak concentrations reached within 2–4 h.Reference Kragh-Sørensen, Overø, Petersen, Jensen and Parnas26 Cognitive testing therefore started following a 3 h rest period after drug administration, in order to maximise drug levels during testing. Participants repeated state measures of mood (post-drug timepoint) and completed the following cognitive tests in a fixed order: social evaluation learning; associative learning; prisoner's dilemma; go/no-go self-esteem; referential categorisation and recall. Participants then repeated the state measures of mood (post-testing timepoint), before completing the Oxford Cognition Stress Task (reported elsewhere). Testing lasted approximately 1.5–2 h. Prior to participating, participants were asked to eat a light meal and were provided with light refreshments after the rest period. Participants were informed that the study aimed to examine how citalopram alters processing of emotional and social information about the self and others, but they were blinded to the specific study hypotheses.

Materials

Questionnaires

Depression was measured using the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9)Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams27 and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II).Reference Beck, Steer and Brown28 Anxiety was measured using the Generalised Anxiety Disorder Scale (GAD-7),Reference Spitzer, Kroenke, Williams and Löwe29 the Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation ScaleReference Leary30 and the Trait Anxiety Inventory.Reference Spielberger31 Personality traits were measured using the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Abbreviated.Reference Francis, Brown and Philipchalk32 State mood was measured using the State Anxiety Inventory,Reference Spielberger31 Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS),Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen33 and visual analogue scales (VAS) of sadness, disgust, anger, fear, anxiety and alertness. State mood measures were completed at baseline, post-drug and post-testing timepoints.

Blinding

To assess the effectiveness of blinding, at the end of testing participants and the administrating researcher guessed the study drug administered, and indicated their certainty regarding this guess using a VAS. Side-effects were also monitored using participant self-reports of nausea, dizziness, dry mouth, headaches, alertness and agitation (absent to severe) at each timepoint.

Cognitive tasks

For brevity, a short description of each task is provided below; full details can be found in the supplementary material, available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2020.107.

Prisoners’ dilemma: Antidepressants may promote positive social relationships with others by increasing prosocial behaviours. We therefore measured cooperative behaviours using a prisoners’ dilemma task. Participants won points based on their decision to cooperate or defect in combination with the computer-simulated opponent's decision (Fig. 1(a)). If both chose to cooperate then the points were equally shared, if one defected and the other cooperated then the defector gained all the points, and if both defected neither player gained any points. Participants were unaware of the other player's decision when making their choice. Social context was manipulated so that the other player could initially choose to cooperate (positive) or defect (negative). The proportion of cooperative choices was recorded.

Fig. 1 Cognitive task procedures.

Fig. 2 Mean proportion of cooperative behaviours according to drug group and social context. Error bars represent standard deviations.

Social evaluation learning: It is possible that antidepressants may alter negative self-schema by increasing learning of positive evaluations about the self. We therefore measured learning of social evaluations within a reinforcement learning task.Reference Button, Browning, Munafò and Lewis34,Reference Button, Kounali, Stapinski, Rapee, Lewis and Munafò35 Participants learned how much the computer ‘liked’ the self, a friend and a stranger based on feedback to a forced choice selection between positive and negative social evaluation pairings (Fig. 1(b)). Participants learned two rules based on the probability of the positive evaluations being ‘correct’ (‘like’ 60–80%, ‘dislike’ 20–40%). The number of errors made before reaching the criterion of eight consecutive rule-congruent responses was recorded. Bias scores were calculated by subtracting errors to criterion made when learning the dislike rule from the like rule.

Referential categorisation and recall: Previous research has indicated increases in recall of positive characteristics following antidepressant administration. However, the specificity of these effects to the self are unclear. To measure recall of characteristics about the self and others, participants first categorised whether 40 presented positive and negative traits described themselves or a familiar other (yes/no). Participants were then given 2 min to recall those personality traits, using the keyboard to enter their responses (Fig. 1(c)). Separate blocks were completed for each referential condition in a randomised order. The total numbers of words categorised and correctly recalled were recorded.

Self-esteem go/no-go: To measure self-referential processing occurring in interaction with affective processing we used a go/no-go task. This task measured inhibitory control when responding to affective words in relation to the self and others.Reference Gregg and Sedikides36 Participants categorised words relating to the self or others, and positive or negative traits, by pressing the space bar if a presented word belonged to a specified paired referential-emotion category (Fig. 1(d)). Discriminative accuracy (d′) was calculated according to the referential-emotion condition.

Associative learning: To measure self, emotion and reward processing occurring independently, we used three simple associative learning tasks.Reference Sui, Ohrling and Humphreys37,Reference Stolte, Humphreys, Yankouskaya and Sui38 In each task, participants were presented with a combination of stimulus-shape pairings, related to the relevant area of processing, and used the keyboard to indicate whether the presented pairings matched previously learnt associations (Fig. 1(e)). Stimuli varied according to the area of processing examined. Accuracy (percentage correct) and reaction times were recorded.

Statistical analyses

Analyses were conducted in R 3.6.

Sample size calculation

We aimed to recruit 44 participants to provide 90% power to detect changes in emotion processing previously observed in healthy volunteer studies (drug mean 10.64 (s.d. 9.77), placebo mean 3.36 (s.d. 5.96)).Reference Harmer, Shelley, Cowen and Goodwin12 However, owing to COVID-19, recruitment was terminated at 41 participants in March 2020. With the recruited sample, we were able to detect an effect size of d = 1.04 at 90% power and an alpha of 0.05.

State mood and side-effects

The influence of citalopram on state mood and side-effects were tested using mixed-effects linear regression models. Separate models were conducted for each measure, with drug group, timepoint and the interaction between these as predictors. Participant was entered as a random effect to account for the effect of time.

Cognitive tasks

A series of mixed-effect linear regression models were used to assess the influence of citalopram on task performance. For all models, participant was entered as a random effect to account for the repeated measures elements of tasks, drug group was entered as a predictor, and the task outcome as the outcome. For tasks including a referential and/or valence (e.g. emotion or rule) condition, these were entered into the models as additional categorical predictors, independently and in interaction with drug group. Full model details are available in the supplementary material. For exploratory analyses, P-values are not reported owing to undetermined inflation of the alpha rate.Reference Nosek and Lakens39

Drug group guess and certainty

Differences in group assignment guesses according to drug group were assessed using χ2-tests. Differences in certainty of group assignment according to drug group were evaluated using t-tests.

Results

Sample

Participants (n = 41) were randomly allocated to the citalopram (n = 20) or placebo group (n = 21). Sample characteristics, according to drug group, are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Sample demographic characteristics and baseline trait mood and personality self-report measures

PHQ-9, Patient Health Questionnaire; BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory II; GAD-7, Generalised Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire; BFNE, Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale; STAI-T, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Trait Subscale; EPQR-A, Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Abbreviated.

a Data were missing for one participant for mood and personality measures in the citalopram group (N = 19) owing to a technical error.

Self-reported state mood

We found no evidence that citalopram altered mood. Participants showed some evidence of a decrease in positive mood (PANAS positive) between baseline and post-drug (β = −1.90, 95% CI: −3.77, −0.02, P = 0.051), and post-testing (β = −2.47, 95% CI: −4.35, −0.60, P = 0.012), but this did not differ between groups (post-drug: β = −0.25, 95% CI: −2.84, 2.34, P = 0.852, post-testing: β = −1.00, 95% CI: −3.59, 1.59, P = 0.450). Participants showed a slight increase in state anxiety (State-Trait Anxiety Inventory State Subscale; STAI-S) between baseline and post-testing (β = 2.31, 95% CI: 0.00, 4.63, P = 0.054), but this did not differ by drug group (β = 0.64, 95% CI: −2.56, 3.83, P = 0.697). No significant changes over time or differences between groups were observed for the PANAS negative subscale or for ratings of disgust, anger, fear, anxiety or alertness.

Differences were observed between drug groups at baseline in VAS ratings of happiness and sadness, with the placebo group showing higher levels of sadness (β = 8.86, 95% CI: 1.50, 16.22, P = 0.021) and lower levels of happiness (β = −8.22, 95% CI: −15.93, −0.51, P = 0.041). However, exploratory follow-up pairwise comparisons of drug group according to timepoint found no group differences at future timepoints (Supplementary Table 1).

Cognitive tasks

Prisoner's dilemma

An effect of the drug was observed on the proportion of cooperative choices; participants in the placebo group made 20% fewer cooperative choices compared with the citalopram group (95% CI: −37%, −2%, P = 0.030). There was some suggestion of a main effect of social context in the expected direction, although the confidence interval included the null; on average, participants made 13% fewer cooperative choices when the other player had initially defected versus cooperated (95% CI: −26%, 1%, P = 0.073). We did not find evidence of an interaction between drug group and social context, indicating that participants in the placebo group made fewer cooperative choices irrespective of whether the other player initially cooperated or defected (β = 5%, 95% CI: −14%, 24%, P = 0.580; Figure 2).

Social evaluation learning

There were some indications that participants in the citalopram group showed a greater positive bias overall, as indicated by bias scores. Participants in the citalopram group, on average, made 4.8 (s.d. 6.64) more errors when learning negative relative to positive evaluations. By comparison, participants in the placebo group made 2.25 (s.d. 5.37) more errors on average when learning negative relative to positive evaluations. This group effect was particularly heightened for the friend condition (citalopram: −5.70, s.d. 5.45; placebo: −1.64, s.d. 4.59; Supplementary Fig. 1). However, in our initial confirmatory model examining main and interaction effects of the referential condition and drug group, we found no evidence of group differences (Supplementary Table 2).

To examine whether effects of bias scores were obscured by learning within a particular rule (e.g. better learning of ‘dislike’ or worse learning of ‘like’), we examined the effects of drug group on errors to criterion according to the referential condition and rule. Participants made a greater number of errors before learning the negative ‘dislike’ rule (β = 4.53, 95% CI: 2.21, 6.84, P < 0.001). However, this effect did not vary according to drug group or referential condition. No main effects of drug, referential condition, or an interaction between these were observed (Supplementary Table 2).

Given our previous findings of increased prosocial behaviours towards others in the citalopram group in the prisoners’ dilemma task, and given that the sample size may be small for testing interaction effects, and that the descriptive statistics suggested a drug group effect in the friend condition, we conducted additional exploratory analyses focusing on the effects of drug group in the friend condition. We therefore examined the effects of drug group on bias scores separately for each referential condition. Participants in the citalopram group showed a greater positive bias in the friend condition (β = 4.06, 95% CI: 0.88, 7.24), but there was little evidence of a difference between drug groups in the self (β = 1.95, −2.25, 6.16) or stranger conditions (β = 1.63, −2.46, 5.72).

To further explore the effect of drug group in the friend condition, we conducted additional exploratory analyses examining the effects of rule and drug group on errors to criterion for each referential condition separately. For the friend condition, an interaction between drug group and rule was observed (β = −4.06, 95% CI: −7.14, −0.98). Participants in the citalopram group on average made 3.55 (s.d. 2.97) errors before learning the ‘like’ rule and 9.25 (s.d. 4.42) errors before learning the ‘dislike’ rule. A smaller difference was observed between rules for the placebo group, with participants on average making 5.19 (s.d. 3.16) errors before learning the ‘like’ rule and 6.38 (s.d. 3.58) errors before learning the ‘dislike’ rule (Fig. 3). The greater positive bias towards the friend, observed with bias scores, was therefore driven by both reduced learning of the friend being disliked and increased learning of the friend being liked. We found no evidence for an interaction between drug group and rule when learning about the self (β = −1.95, 95% CI: −5.73, 1.83) or the stranger (β = −1.63, 95% CI: −5.16, 1.91).

Fig. 3 Mean errors to criterion in the friend condition according to drug group and rule. Error bars represent standard deviations.

However, these results are exploratory and require further replication to determine the reliability of the effects.

Referential emotional categorisation and recall

Participants endorsed a greater number of positive words as descriptive (β = −11.70, 95% CI: −13.50, −9.90, P <0.001), for both the self and other. There was no evidence that this effect differed between drug groups (Supplementary Table 3).

When examining the number of correctly recalled words, we found that participants recalled more positive than negative words (β = −1.30, 95% CI: −2.37, −0.23, P = 0.019). There was weak evidence that this differed according to referential condition, with participants recalling fewer dislikeable words in the other versus self condition, although the confidence interval included the null (β = −1.30, 95% CI: −2.82, 0.22, P = 0.096). We found no evidence of an interaction between valence and drug group, or referential condition and drug group (Supplementary Table 3).

There was weak evidence of an interaction between drug group, referential condition and valence (β = 1.92, 95% CI: −0.20, 4.04, P = 0.079), although the confidence interval included the null. To explore this effect further, we conducted additional exploratory analyses examining the interaction between drug group and valence for each referential condition separately. When recalling words about the self, participants recalled fewer dislikeable words (β = −1.30, 95% CI: −2.34, −0.27), but this did not vary by drug group (β = 0.49, 95% CI: −0.96, 1.94). Conversely, when recalling words about others, we observed an interaction between drug group and valence (β = 2.41, 95% CI: 0.89, 3.93). Participants in the citalopram group showed a positive bias towards others, recalling more likeable words and fewer dislikeable words, compared with the placebo group (Fig. 4). However, these findings are exploratory and require further replication.

Fig. 4 Mean number of correctly recalled words in the other condition according to drug group and valence. Error bars represent standard deviations.

The citalopram group had a higher proportion of participants that did not speak English as a first language (Table 1), which may have affected recall. Sensitivity analyses indicated that effects were consistent when this was taken into account. English as a first language was not associated with recall (β = −1.13, 95% CI: −2.48, 0.23, P = 0.112).

Go/no-go self-esteem

Ten participants were excluded from analyses for the go/no-go association self-esteem task as their responses indicated non-compliance according to a priori data exclusion criteria

Participants showed reduced discriminative accuracy for words relating to others versus self (β = −0.47, 95% CI: −0.84, −0.11, P = 0.012), and for negative versus positive words (although the confidence interval included the null; β = −0.36, 95% CI: −0.73, 0.00, P = 0.052). An interaction between referential condition and valence was observed (β = 0.65, 95% CI: 0.14, 1.16, P = 0.015). Participants showed higher discriminative accuracy for positive versus negative words in the self condition, but the opposite pattern in the other condition, suggesting a positive self-bias. However, there was no evidence that these effects varied by drug group (Supplementary Table 4).

Additional analyses suggested that these effects were driven by hits rather than false alarms (Supplementary Table 4).

When participants excluded according to a priori data exclusion criteria were included in the sensitivity analyses, the main and interactive effects of the referential condition and valence were further strengthened, but the lack of an effect of drug group remained.

Associative learning

A main effect of stimuli was observed for the self and emotion tasks, with participants showing greater accuracy and faster reaction times when matching shapes with the self and a happy face (Supplementary Table 5). For the reward task, there was no evidence of differences in accuracy according to level of reward, although participants were slightly slower to match shapes with the medium (£3) compared with the high (£9) level of reward. There was no evidence that performance on any of the associative learning tasks varied by drug group, either as a main effect or in interaction with stimuli (Supplementary Table 5).

Blinding

A greater proportion of participants in the citalopram group (74%) believed that they had taken citalopram compared with the placebo group (14%), at a greater level of certainty (t(30.09) = 3.47, P = 0.002). Conversely, researchers administering the drug did not significantly differ in their group guesses and certainty according to drug group (Supplementary Table 6).

Failure of participant blinding was possibly attributable to the side-effects of citalopram, with the citalopram group only showing increases in nausea (P < 0.001) and dizziness (P = 0.012) over time. An increase in agitation (P = 0.011) and some evidence of an increase in headaches (P = 0.078) were also observed over time, but this varied little by group (P = 0.100 and P = 0.474, respectively). There was no evidence of changes for dry mouth or alertness in either group (Supplementary Table 7).

Discussion

Our results tentatively support the theory that antidepressants increase positive affective biases and prosocial behaviours in healthy volunteers, in the absence of change in mood. Participants administered citalopram cooperated more in a prisoners’ dilemma game. There was also some evidence, based on exploratory analyses, that participants administered citalopram showed a greater positive bias when recalling words about others and when learning social evaluations about a friend.

In support of our pre-registered hypothesis, participants administered citalopram cooperated on a greater proportion of trials in a prisoners’ dilemma task. Our findings add to the literature indicating that serotonin is instrumental in modulating social behaviour. Previous research has indicated that temporary reduction of serotonin through tryptophan depletion reduces cooperative behaviours in a prisoners’ dilemma game.Reference Wood, Rilling, Sanfey, Bhagwagar and Rogers40 In line with our findings, increasing serotonin through administration of citalopram over a 2 week period has previously been found to increase cooperative communication and behaviour towards others.Reference Tse and Bond18 One week administration of an selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor has also been found to reduce self-reported hostile behaviours.Reference Knutson, Wolkowitz, Cole, Chan, Moore and Johnson41 Increases in prosocial behaviour resulting from antidepressant treatment may increase quality of social interactions, positively reinforcing engagement in interpersonal communications and increasing social support.

However, our findings of an increase in cooperative behaviours following a single dose of citalopram differs from those of previous research. In a previous study, a single dose of citalopram was not associated with greater cooperation in healthy volunteers.Reference Tse and Bond42 Disagreement between these findings may be attributable to variations in the prisoners’ dilemma task. In Tse and Bond'sReference Tse and Bond42 study, participants controlled the number of points allocated to the other player. In this study, allocation of points was dependent on both the participant's and the other player's decisions. Prosocial behaviours are believed to be motivated by the aim of eliciting reciprocal altruistic behaviours from others.Reference Trivers43 In this study, participants may have therefore been more motivated to engage in cooperative actions, providing a more sensitive marker of change following serotonin modulation.

We also found some evidence that antidepressants increase prosocial affective biases towards familiar others. Participants administered citalopram showed better learning of friends being liked and reduced learning of friends being disliked by a computer persona. Affective recall was also altered. Participants administered citalopram recalled more likeable characteristics and fewer dislikeable characteristics of others. However, these were exploratory analyses and require further replication. One potential mechanism of antidepressants may involve blunting perceptions of negative characteristics and increasing sensitivity to positive characteristics in others, although this requires further exploration in clinical samples. Increasing positive perceptions of others through antidepressant treatment may increase engagement in social interactions, thereby addressing issues of social withdrawal and anhedonia associated with depression.

In contrast to our expectations, there was no evidence to support our hypothesis that the effects of antidepressants on affective processing were strongest for self-referential stimuli. This contrasts with previous research indicating a change in positive self-referential biases following antidepressant administration.Reference Harmer, O'Sullivan, Favaron, Massey-Chase, Ayres and Reinecke11,Reference Harmer, Shelley, Cowen and Goodwin12 However, in these studies, participants only encoded information in reference to the self. The specificity of this effect to self-related information is therefore unclear. In a study including both a self and friend condition, participants administered escitalopram endorsed fewer negative characteristics about themselves but also more positive characteristics about others.Reference Matthews, Simmons, Strigo, Arce, Stein and Paulus44 In this study, we found no evidence to suggest that citalopram selectively affected positive learning about the self. If anything, our exploratory analyses indicated that citalopram produced the largest group effects in the friend condition, with increased positive learning of social evaluations of friends following antidepressant treatment. However, our confirmatory test did not support a referential condition by drug group interaction.

Self-schemas are pervasive, dominate information processing and are resistant to disconfirmatory evidence.Reference Beck1 A single dose of an antidepressant may not be sufficient to address entrenched self-referential negative biases. Affective processing of information related to others may be more flexible and therefore more sensitive to change by acute administration of antidepressants. In support of this theory, pharmacological induction of anxiety was found to influence other-referential processing, whereas self-referential processing was preserved.Reference Button, Karwatowska, Kounali, Munafò and Attwood45 We may therefore only see changes in self-referential affective biases with longer periods of antidepressant treatment. Alternatively, addressing negative self-schema may also require remediation of top-down, deliberative biases through treatments such as cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT).Reference Roiser, Elliott and Sahakian9 Further longitudinal studies are required to examine changes in self-referential affective biases during long-term use of antidepressants, to assess their effectiveness in remediating negative self-schema.

We found no evidence that acute citalopram influenced inhibitory control, measured using an affective go/no-go association task, or simple associative learning of emotional, self and reward stimuli. Previous research has reported inconsistent findings regarding the relationship between depressive symptoms and performance on affective inhibitory control tasks.Reference Lewis, Button, Pearson, Munafò and Lewis46,Reference Kilford, Foulkes, Potter, Collishaw, Thapar and Rice47 Similarly, we have previously found no association between simple associative learning and depression severity.Reference Hobbs, Kessler, Munafo, Sui and Button48 Serotonin therefore appears to have little influence over these particular cognitive processes.

Clinical implications

Variation in individual treatment response and delays in therapeutic action currently expose patients to considerable periods of potentially ineffective antidepressant treatment.Reference Warden, Rush, Trivedi, Fava and Wisniewski49 Our findings suggest that changes in prosocial behaviours and, more tentatively, positive affective biases towards others may be sensitive markers of early changes in response to antidepressant treatment. Extension of our findings to clinical samples may offer a promising marker of treatment response that could allow clinicians to identify effective treatments for individuals experiencing depression at earlier timepoints.

Limited change in negative self-schema from antidepressant treatment, as we observed in this study, may provide an explanation for the relatively high relapse rates following treatment discontinuation.Reference Geddes, Carney, Davies, Furukawa, Kupfer and Frank50 At present, it is unclear whether change in affective processing is sustained after antidepressant treatment has been discontinued. If increased positive affective biases do not translate to a sustained change in self-schema, depressive symptoms are likely to return following antidepressant discontinuation. Sustained changes in self-schema through treatments such as CBT may explain the substantially lower relapse rates compared with antidepressant treatment.Reference Butler, Chapman, Forman and Beck51

Limitations

This study had lower statistical power than planned, as recruitment was terminated prematurely owing to COVID-19. We were therefore powered to detect large effects and may have been underpowered to detect smaller effects such as the hypothesised interaction effects for social evaluation learning. To address this limitation, we have made all materials and data publicly available for further replication and extension of our work.

In addition, despite strict blinding procedures, there was evidence that blinding was not effective at a participant level. It is possible that our results may have been partially driven by response biases. However, mitigating this possibility, we predominantly used implicit affective tasks, where the main purpose of the tasks was not explicitly stated, and participants were blinded to the study hypotheses.

This study used a placebo drug as the comparator condition. However, high placebo response rates have been observed in antidepressant trials,Reference Timothy Walsh, Seidman, Sysko and Gould52 suggesting that placebo expectations may influence similar cognitive processes. Future studies including a no-treatment-control arm condition or a placebo lead-in phase would allow more precise evaluation of the effects of citalopram on social cognition.

Finally, this study examined changes in social cognition and behaviour following antidepressant administration in healthy volunteers. The use of healthy volunteers offers the opportunity to study affective biases unconfounded by ‘cold’ cognitive biases.Reference Roiser and Sahakian53 However, it limits our insight into the mechanisms of antidepressants in improving mood. It is also possible that our findings may not directly translate to clinically depressed patients. Further research would benefit from extending our findings to longer-term changes in affective processing in depressed individuals.

Summary

Overall, our findings suggest that acute administration of citalopram in healthy volunteers is associated with increased prosocial behaviour towards others. In contrast to our expectations, we found little evidence that the effect of acute citalopram on affective processing was heightened for information related to the self. Exploratory analyses instead suggested increased positive affective biases towards others. Changes in affective processing and prosocial behaviours towards others may, at least partially, be a mechanism of antidepressant effect. Further research in clinical samples is required to examine this possibility.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2020.107.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in the University of Bath Research Data Archive at https://doi.org/10.15125/BATH-00891, reference number 891.

Acknowledgements

We thank the participants who took part in the study.

Author contributions

C.H., S.E.M., C.J.H. and K.S.B. conceptualised the study aims and design, and acquired the funding for this study. J.S. and K.S.B. provided materials for the study. C.H. programmed the cognitive tasks. K.S.B., C.J.H. and S.E.M. supervised the study. C.H., K.S.B., J.S., M.R.M. and D.K. obtained funding and provided supervisory support for the wider PhD project this study is part of. C.H., L.W., J.C., I.V.A. and J.O. recruited participants and collected data. M.O. provided medical supervision. C.H. curated, analysed and visualised the data. C.H. wrote the original draft of this manuscript, which was reviewed and edited by S.E.M., J.S., M.R.M., D.K., C.J.H. and K.S.B.

Funding

This study was funded by the UK National Productivity Investment Fund awarded to C.H. through the GW4 BioMed Medical Research Council Doctoral Training Partnership. This study was supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the National Health Service, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Declaration of interest

C.J.H. has received consultancy fees from P1vital Ltd., Janssen Pharmaceuticals, Sage Therapeutics, Pfizer and Lundbeck. S.E.M. has received consultancy fees from P1vital Ltd. and Janssen Pharmaceuticals. C.J.H. and S.E.M. hold grant income from UCB Pharma, Janssen Pharmaceuticals, Pfizer and Zogenix.

ICMJE forms are in the supplementary material, available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2020.107.

References

Beck, AT. The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165(8): 969–77.Google ScholarPubMed
Cole, DA, Dukewich, TL, Roeder, K, Sinclair, KR, McMillan, J, Will, E, et al. Linking peer victimization to the development of depressive self-schemas in children and adolescents. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2014; 42(1): 149–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaddy, MA, Ingram, RE. A meta-analytic review of mood-congruent implicit memory in depressed mood. Clin Psychol Rev 2014; 34(5): 402–16.Google ScholarPubMed
Ji, JL, Grafton, B, MacLeod, C. Referential focus moderates depression-linked attentional avoidance of positive information. Behav Res Ther 2017; 93: 4754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertel, PT, El-Messidi, L. Am I blue? Depressed mood and the consequences of self-focus for the interpretation and recall of ambiguous words. Behav Ther 2006; 37(3): 259–68.Google ScholarPubMed
Kendrick, T, Dowrick, C, McBride, A, Howe, A, Clarke, P, Maisey, S, et al. Management of depression in UK general practice in relation to scores on depression severity questionnaires: analysis of medical record data. BMJ 2009; 338: b750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, CJ, Goodwin, GM, Cowen, PJ. Why do antidepressants take so long to work? A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195(2): 102–8.Google Scholar
Walsh, AEL, Harmer, CJ. The cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant response. Curr Opin Psychol 2015; 4: 124–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roiser, JP, Elliott, R, Sahakian, BJ. Cognitive mechanisms of treatment in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2012; 37(1): 117–36.Google ScholarPubMed
Phillips, WJ, Hine, DW, Thorsteinsson, EB. Implicit cognition and depression: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 2010; 30(6): 691709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, CJ, O'Sullivan, U, Favaron, E, Massey-Chase, R, Ayres, R, Reinecke, A, et al. Effect of acute antidepressant administration on negative affective bias in depressed patients. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166(10): 1178–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, CJ, Shelley, NC, Cowen, PJ, Goodwin, GM. Increased positive versus negative affective perception and memory in healthy volunteers following selective serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibition. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161(7): 1256–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Godlewska, BR, Harmer, CJ. Cognitive neuropsychological theory of antidepressant action: a modern-day approach to depression and its treatment. Psychopharmacology [Epub ahead of print] 15 Jan 2020. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-019-05448-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teo, AR, Choi, HJ, Valenstein, M. Social relationships and depression: ten-year follow-up from a nationally representative study. PLoS One 2013; 8(4): e62396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caouette, JD, Guyer, AE. Cognitive distortions mediate depression and affective response to social acceptance and rejection. J Affect Disord 2016; 190: 792–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, PM. The behavioral study and treatment of depression. In Progress in Behavior Modification: 1964. Elsevier, 1975.Google Scholar
Tse, WS, Bond, AJ. Consequences of displaying abnormal social behaviour: avoidance and reduction of social reinforcement. J Affect Disord 2003; 75(1): 4958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tse, WS, Bond, AJ. Serotonergic intervention affects both social dominance and affiliative behaviour. Psychopharmacology 2002; 161(3): 324–30.Google ScholarPubMed
Shrauger, JS, Schoeneman, TJ. Symbolic interactionist view of self-concept: through the looking glass darkly. Psychol Bull 1979; 86(3): 549.Google Scholar
Korn, CW, Prehn, K, Park, SQ, Walter, H, Heekeren, HR. Positively biased processing of self-relevant social feedback. J Neurosci 2012; 32(47): 16832–44.Google ScholarPubMed
Moritz, D, Roberts, JE. Self-other agreement and metaperception accuracy across the Big Five: examining the roles of depression and self-esteem. J Pers 2018; 86(2): 296307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giesler, RB, Josephs, RA, Swann, WB. Self-verification in clinical depression: the desire for negative evaluation. J Abnorm Psychol 1996; 105(3): 358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobbs, C, Faraway, J, Kessler, D, Munafò, MR, Sui, J, Button KS. Social evaluation learning in social anxiety disorder and depression: a mega-analysis presented at the Summer Meeting of the British Association for Psychopharmacology. J Psychopharmacol 2019; Supplement(8): A33.Google Scholar
First, M, Williams, J, Karg, R, Spitzer, R. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association, 2015.Google Scholar
Sealed Envelope Ltd. Simple randomisation service. 2019 (https://www.sealedenvelope.com/simple-randomiser/v1/ [cited 21 May 2020]).Google Scholar
Kragh-Sørensen, P, Overø, KF, Petersen, OL, Jensen, K, Parnas, W. The kinetics of citalopram: single and multiple dose studies in man. Acta Pharmacol Toxicol 1981; 48(1): 5360.Google ScholarPubMed
Kroenke, K, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBW. The Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) – overview. J Gen Intern Med 2001; 16: 606–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Brown, GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. TX Psychol Corp, 1996.Google Scholar
Spitzer, RL, Kroenke, K, Williams, JBW, Löwe, B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med 2006; 166 (10):1092–7. doi:10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092.Google ScholarPubMed
Leary, MR. A brief version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 1983; 9(3): 371–5.Google Scholar
Spielberger, CD. Assessment of state and trait anxiety: conceptual and methodological issues. South Psychol 1985; 2(4): 616.Google Scholar
Francis, LJ, Brown, LB, Philipchalk, R. The development of an abbreviated form of the revised Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQR-A): its use among students in England, Canada, the U.S.A. and Australia. Pers Individ Dif 1992; 13(4): 443–9.Google Scholar
Watson, D, Clark, LA, Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54(6): 1063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Button, KS, Browning, M, Munafò, MR, Lewis, G. Social inference and social anxiety: evidence of a fear-congruent self-referential learning bias. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2012; 43(4): 1082–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Button, KS, Kounali, D, Stapinski, L, Rapee, RM, Lewis, G, Munafò, MR. Fear of negative evaluation biases social evaluation inference: evidence from a probabilistic learning task. PLoS One 2015; 10(4): e0119456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregg, AP, Sedikides, C. Narcissistic fragility: rethinking its links to explicit and implicit self-esteem. Self Identity 2010; 9(2): 142–61.Google Scholar
Sui, J, Ohrling, E, Humphreys, GW. Negative mood disrupts self- and reward-biases in perceptual matching. Q J Exp Psychol 2016; 69(7): 1438–48.Google ScholarPubMed
Stolte, M, Humphreys, G, Yankouskaya, A, Sui, J. Dissociating biases towards the self and positive emotion. Q J Exp Psychol 2017; 70(6): 1011–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosek, BA, Lakens, D. Registered reports: a method to increase the credibility of published results. Soc Psychol 2014; 45(3): 137–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, RM, Rilling, JK, Sanfey, AG, Bhagwagar, Z, Rogers, RD. Effects of tryptophan depletion on the performance of an iterated Prisoner's Dilemma game in healthy adults. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006; 31(5): 1075–84.Google ScholarPubMed
Knutson, B, Wolkowitz, OM, Cole, SW, Chan, T, Moore, EA, Johnson, RC, et al. Selective alteration of personality and social behavior by serotonergic intervention. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155(3): 373–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Tse, WS, Bond, AJ. Difference in serotonergic and noradrenergic regulation of human social behaviours. Psychopharmacology 2002; 159(2): 216–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trivers, RL. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 1971; 46(1): 3557.Google Scholar
Matthews, SC, Simmons, AN, Strigo, IA, Arce, E, Stein, MB, Paulus, MP. Escitalopram attenuates posterior cingulate activity during self-evaluation in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Res 2010; 182(2): 81–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Button, KS, Karwatowska, L, Kounali, D, Munafò, MR, Attwood, AS. Acute anxiety and social inference: an experimental manipulation with 7.5% carbon dioxide inhalation. J Psychopharmacol 2016; 30(10): 1036–46.Google ScholarPubMed
Lewis, G, Button, KS, Pearson, RM, Munafò, MR, Lewis, G. Inhibitory control of positive and negative information and adolescent depressive symptoms: a population-based cohort study. Psychological Medicine [Epub ahead of print] 17 Jul 2020. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720002469.Google Scholar
Kilford, EJ, Foulkes, L, Potter, R, Collishaw, S, Thapar, A, Rice, F. Affective bias and current, past and future adolescent depression: a familial high risk study. J Affect Disord 2015; 174: 265–71.Google ScholarPubMed
Hobbs, C, Kessler, D, Munafo, MR, Sui, J, Button, KS. Biased self-referential processing in depression. OSF, 2020 (osf.io/34ma2).Google Scholar
Warden, D, Rush, AJ, Trivedi, MH, Fava, M, Wisniewski, SR. The STAR*D project results: a comprehensive review of findings. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2007; 9(6): 449–59.Google Scholar
Geddes, JR, Carney, SM, Davies, C, Furukawa, TA, Kupfer, DJ, Frank, E, et al. Relapse prevention with antidepressant drug treatment in depressive disorders: a systematic review. Lancet 2003; 361(9358): 653–61.Google ScholarPubMed
Butler, AC, Chapman, JE, Forman, EM, Beck, AT. The empirical status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses. Clin Psychol Rev 2006; 26(1): 1731.Google ScholarPubMed
Timothy Walsh, B, Seidman, SN, Sysko, R, Gould, M. Placebo response in studies of major depression: variable, substantial, and growing. J Am Med Assoc 2002; 287(14): 1840–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roiser, JP, Sahakian, BJ. Hot and cold cognition in depression. CNS Spectr 2013; 18(3): 139–49.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Cognitive task procedures.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Mean proportion of cooperative behaviours according to drug group and social context. Error bars represent standard deviations.

Figure 2

Table 1 Sample demographic characteristics and baseline trait mood and personality self-report measures

Figure 3

Fig. 3 Mean errors to criterion in the friend condition according to drug group and rule. Error bars represent standard deviations.

Figure 4

Fig. 4 Mean number of correctly recalled words in the other condition according to drug group and valence. Error bars represent standard deviations.

Supplementary material: File

Hobbs et al. supplementary material

Hobbs et al. supplementary material 2

Download Hobbs et al. supplementary material(File)
File 50.6 KB
Supplementary material: File

Hobbs et al. supplementary material

Hobbs et al. supplementary material 2

Download Hobbs et al. supplementary material(File)
File 14 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.